Studies in African Linguistics
Volume 32, Number 2,2003
TONAL ALTERNATIONS IN THE URHOBO NOUN PHRASE
Rose O. Aziza
Delta State University
Urhobo is an Edoid language spoken extensively in Delta State, Nigeria. This
paper examines tonal alternations in the Urhobo noun phrase with a view to providing more data that can lead to a better understanding of tonal behavior in
Edoid languages. The language has two basic tones, high and low, two gliding
tones, high-low (falling) and low-high (rising), plus a phenomenon of downstep,
both automatic and non-automatic. A number of noun phrases are examined here
and, as will become obvious later, the language relies heavily on tone to express
many grammatical categories. The main points of interest from this study include
the preservation of H at the expense of L, the fact that the loss of L does not result in downstep, that 'H plus following H can give H so the down step can be
deleted, and that there are syntactically conditioned floating H tones in certain
constructions.
1.
Introduction
Since Urhobo is not yet a well studied language, a brief mention of its sounds is
necessary here. The sounds ofUrhobo are given in Tables 1 and 2 below.
Table 1:
Urhobo Consonant Sounds
Bilabial
Plosive
Fricative
Nasal
Trill/tap
Flap
Approximant
p
Labiodental
b
t
f
<p
Alveolar
v
s
m
d
v
n
r
Palato- Palatal
alveolar
c
J
S
3
J1
Velar
k g
h Y
Labialvelar
kp gb
IJ
r
v
j
w
2
Studies in African Linguistics 32(2), 2003
The language also has seven phonetic vowels but nine phonological vowels. This is because the vowels lei and 101 behave sometimes as low vowels and
sometimes as high vowels in their vowel harmony requirements when they occur
in sequences. We shall not, however, discuss that here. Only the seven phonetic
vowels, which are also the orthographic vowels, will be presented. There is no
contrastive vowel length, although vowels appear to have greater duration when
they bear a contour tone.
Table 2:
Urhobo Vowel Sounds
u
e
o
8
:)
a
The Urhobo tone system has been studied by Kelly (1969), Welmers (1969,
1973), Elugbe (1973, 1989) and Aziza (1997). It has a terraced level tone system
with the following characteristics. Only vowels bear tones in this language. There
are two basic tones, low (L) and high (H). Consecutive tones are realized on the
same pitch level. There are also two gliding tones, HL (falling) and LH (rising)
which are generally derived from the basic tones in a relatively few syntactic
contexts outside the noun phrase. There are no lexical contour tones. However,
the digits in the numeral system display a LH contour in their citation forms, and
will be discussed in the appropriate section. There is non-automatic downstep
(,H) which Welmers regards as a mid tone. His analysis is not surprising since
this pitch acts phonologically "special" in ways that are somewhat suggestive of
being a M tone. It is found only after H, especially in the final position of some
nouns and numerals. It is not traceable to a lost L tone, but it has a lowering effect
on the pitch of a following H such that after it, a following H is realized on the
same pitch as 'H and no succeeding tone can rise significantly higher than its
level within the same tone phrase. Downstep can be deleted from the phonetic
string due to certain phonological processes, and when that happens, the lost !H
has no lowering effect on surviving tones (see e.g. section 2.1). There is phonetic
downdrift such that in a H-L-H sequence, the second H is realized on a pitch level
lower than the initial H, but for this to take place, the L tone must be phonetically
Tonal Alternations In The Urhobo Noun Phrase
3
realised. Where, due to phonological processes, it gets deleted from the phonetic
string, it does not lower a H tone.
Most nouns in Urhobo are either disyllabic or trisyllabic and usually have
the structure prefix + stem. The prefix is always a vowel while the stem always
begins with a consonant. Prefix vowels of nouns and adjectives usually alternate
to reflect number concord. The plural prefix appears in two phonological shapes
[i-] ~ [e-], the choice of which depends on the ATR vowel harmony requirements
of the stem: [+ ATR] vowels select [i-] while [-A TR] vowels select [e-]. Most Urhobo personal names are phrases or even sentences but they can be shortened, and
when this is done, they may appear with or without a prefix vowel. For example,
[5yenEtega] 'God is worthy to be worshipped' is usually realized simply as [tega]
'this is wealth' is usually realized as
'worthy to be worshipped' while [~naef]
[efe] 'wealth'. However, as we shall see later, when such forms occur in certain
phrases, a low-toned [i-] prefix is always introduced irrespective of the type of
vowel in the stem, an indication that the deletion of the prefix vowel may be only
a surface structure phenomenon. Many nouns differ in meaning only because they
differ in tone. On the basis of tone patterning, Urhobo disyllabic and trisyllabic
nouns in their citation form can be classified into five and ten tone groups respectively as in (1) and (2) below. (Note: in this paper, the tones will be marked
as follows: the low tone as [' ], the high tone [' ], and the non-automatic downstep as [-] on the vowel bearing the tone).
(1)
Disyllabic Nouns
a. low-low
USI
b. low-high
USI
c. high -high
uk6
d. high -low
ukpe
e. high - downstep
5g5
(L-L)
,
'line
(L-H)
'starch'
(H-H)
,
'cup
(L-H)
'bedlbedroom'
(H-!H)
,
'in-law
ukpe
'year'
::>g::>
'bottle'
kEUE
personal name 1
tega
personal name
,
-
lyO
'money'
1 This personal name means 'gave me' and comes from [SYEnEkEuE] spelt as Qgh~nkvw
meaning 'God gave me').
4
(2)
Studies in AIi"iean Linguistics 32(2), 2003
Trisyllabic Nouns
a. high-high-high
agada
b. high-high-downstep
orere
c. high-high-low
6n6gbo
d. high-low-high
6dlb6
e. high-low-low
egodo
f. low-high-high
lrlb6
g. low-high-downstep
likutii
h. low-high-low
erako
1. low-low-high
egbede
J. low-low-low
odibo
/
/
(H-H-H)
'matchet'
eh6h6
(H-H-'H)
'town, city'
uk6k6
(H-H-L)
'pussy cat'
Itaba
(H-L-H)
,
,
'banana
Jy£n£
(H-L-L)
'compound'
5Sare
(L-H-H)
,
'pepper
Isene
(L-H-'H)
,
'stone
eWer!
(L-H-L)
'dog'
IJere
(L-L-H)
'needle'
okpetU
(L-L-L)
'servant, slave' oh~re
/
/
'a hidden comer'
'association, meeting'
'tobacco'
'God'
'man
,
'camwood'
'monkey'
'road'
'trouble'
'neck'
It should be noted, though, that the group of nouns here labeled H-L-H are actually closer in pitch to H -M -M but since the basic tones of the language are Hand
L, we identify them as H-L-H rather than as H-'H-H.
Welmers (1969: 94) states that the mid tone, which we regard as a downstep, occurs only after a H tone and in final position of some nouns and numerals
and that it becomes low when anything at all follows it. His examples include:
(3)
a. 5s£ 'father'
b. ly6 'money'
/
JS£ w£
Iyo nana
'your father'
'this money'
While our data attest to the fact that 'H is found only after H and in final
position, 'H does not actually become L when followed by another tone: the examples in (4) show that HL and H!H contrast phrase-medially such that (4a) surfaces as H-M-M and (4b) as H-L-M since there is downdrift in the language.
Tonal Alternations In The Urhobo Noun Phrase
(4)
a.
-
[- -]
[5SE wE]
'your father'
H'H H
[HMM]
'your maize'
HL H
[H L M]
father your
b.
5
[-
-]
[5kll wE]
maize your
In this paper, our main focus is on the behavior of tone in the noun phrase.
2.
Tonal alternations in the noun phrase
In general, a simple noun phrase consists of a noun and a modifier, and the modifiers are of different types. From the way the NPs pattern, it is possible to classify
them into five broad groups: non-associative, associative, noun+relative, noun/
noun phrase doubling, and noun reduplication. In terms of tonal alternations, our
data reveals the following generalizations:
• that there are tone changes resulting from the merging of vowels which illustrate the principle that H tones are preserved at
the expense of L tones;
• that when a L tone between two H tones gets deleted, a downstep does not result, in contrast to what occurs in many languages that have downstep;
• that!H becomes H when merged with an immediately following
H tone in certain constructions; and
• that floating H tones exist in this language, which provides another way of illustrating these tone principles independent of
vowel deletion.
2.1 Non-associative noun phrases. The structure of the NPs in this group is an N
followed by its modifier(s). The modifiers, except the class of numerals, comprise
a prefix vowel (which alternates to reflect number concord) and a stem. The prefix vowels of the definite article, the demonstrative and the possessive pronouns
(first and second person singular forms only) bear L tones and when they follow
the noun, both the prefix vowel and its L tone are deleted; the result is that there
are no tone alternations in the noun. The independent forms of the modifiers are:
6
Studies in African Linguistics 32(2), 2003
(i) Definite article:
;na 'the (sing. )/this' ~ ena 'the (pI. )/these'
;je 'that' ~ eje 'those'
(ii) Demonstrative:
;nana 'this one' ~ enana 'these ones'
;jena 'that one' ~ ejena 'those ones'
(iii) Possessive Pronoun: (first and second person singular forms only)
.,
JmE" ~ ernE"', my, mme
,
, '"
,
JWE ~ewE
your, yours
'-
'-
/
These forms feature in the NPs in (5) below:
(5)
a. ukpe
bed
b. lk6
cups
c. ukpe
bed
d. lk6
cups
e. ukpe
bed
f. lk6
cups
Jna
the
ena
the
,
Jnana
this (one)
enana
these
JmE
my/mine
'
eWE
/
your/yours
[ukpe na]
'the bed'
na]
~k6
'the cups'
[ukpe nan a]
'this bed'
~k6
nana]
'these cups'
[ukpe mE]
'my bed'
~k6
wE]
'your cups'
Welmers (1969) rightly lists JrobO'z 'that one over there' as one of the demonstratives but this form actually belongs to the associative construction to be discussed
in 2.2.
The indefinite article Jva 'a, an, one' alternates with eva 'some'; it also
follows its noun and bears L tones but unlike in the constructions above, it is the
final vowel of the noun which either gets elided if it is [-high] or undergoes glide
formation if it is [+high]: however, its tone remains, again demonstrating the retention of H tone at the expense of L tone. The noun's tone either merges with the
tone of the prefix vowel of the modifier if it is also L as in (6a) and (6b) or replaces it if it is H or 'H as in (6c, d), evidence that the H tone is preserved at the
Tonal Alternations In The Urhobo Noun Phrase
7
expense of the L tone.
(6)
a. ukpe :,vo
[ukp:,vo]
'a/ one bed'
Ulbevo na]
'some (of the) hands, some occasions'
(ikevo]
'some cups'
[5s3vo]
'a, one father'
a,one
bed
, ,
ena
hands some the
c. iko evo
cups some
d.
JSE
;vo
father a, one
A wh- modifier also behaves in a similar way but since these are usually
questions, the L question tonal morpheme (hereafter referred to as "tomorph")
attaches to the final vowel which already bears a H tone, and a HL contour is
thereby created. In (7b) the tone immediately following !H is L and so 'H survives
while L is deleted. For example:
(7)
a. lwem
Ibro
houses
how many
/
eV;J
-
b. esc
fathers
which
(L)
Q.
(L)
Q.
[lweul bro]
'how many houses?'
[esev3]
'which fathers?'
Nouns may also be followed by numeral modifiers. Welmers (1969) gives
a good, broad account of the numeral system of Urhobo and, as he correctly observes, the system is basically vigesimal, i.e., based on 'twenty' with a subordinate decimal division based on 'ten'. Among the digits' one' through 'ten', only
/:,vo/ 'one' begins with a L tone. Welmers analyzes the initial tone of the digits
'two' through 'ten' as H but we believe that that tone is better analyzed as a LH
contour for two main reasons. First, in the citation form of these digits, an initial
LH contour is clearly perceived. The point must however be made that the tone
contour is quick such that it can easily be simplified to H. Moreover, the LH
contour appears to be a property only of the digits other than' one', as these constitute the only group of words found with a contour tone in the citation form in
this data. Other cases of tone contours are derived from the juxtaposition of the
two basic tones on one vowel in certain grammatical constructions.
Second, following Welmers' analysis where the numerals have initial H
tone, the forms in (8) and (9) below should have similar outputs in the surface but
they do not. Whereas in (8) the juxtaposition of L+H and 'H+H respectively at the
8
Studies in African Linguistics 32(2), 2003
boundary between the noun and its attributive modifier results in a H tone, in (9)
which involve numeral modifiers, a LH contour and a 'H respectively surface in
the outputs, an indication that there is a difference in the tone sequences at the
beginning of the modifier. As has been observed in our examples so far and from
the forms in (8) below, a basic tone principle in this language is that the H tone is
preserved at the expense of the L tone, such that the merging of L+H, H+L or
!H+H at grammatical boundaries usually produces a H tone and not a LH sequence or 'H (even if the L gets deleted from the phonetic string). Note that we
cannot have a sequence of H+!H because !H is never found in word initial position: it always follows H within a morpheme.
One way to account for the difference between the outputs in (8) and (9) is
to postulate a floating L construction-marking tomorph for (9) but, as we will
show later, Urhobo has a floating H construction marker in certain numeral combinations that would make postulating a floating L uneconomical.
(8)
a. lkpe 19rogro
,
b. e:se: eroaro
Gkp'igrogro]
[eserwaro]
'long beds'
'big fathers'
a. lkpe lVE
b. eSE era
Gkp'ivE]
[esera]
'two beds'
'three fathers'
/'
(9)
/
//
However, the digits are the only group of words in the language found with a
contour tone in the citation form, and this makes the tone sequence suspect as a
lexical property of the numerals. Rather, we regard it as a property of the construction so that it is the construction that assigns an initial LH contour to the digits. Evidence that this may actually be the case can be seen in the form for
'eleven' /uhw5vo/ in the data presented in (12) below where /?wo/ 'one', which is
the only digit with an initial L in isolation, surfaces with a LH contour when it
combines with the form for 'ten'.
The digits of Urhobo are:
(10)
:wo
,
IVe:
v ,
era
v
,
e:ne
"., ,
IJon
v
'one'
,
'two
,
'three
'four'
'five'
v
/
esa
v
W /
Iy rc:
£rere
v / _
Inn
'ihwe
, . ,
SIX
,
,
seven
,
eight'
,
nme
'ten'
,.
Tonal Alternations In The Urhobo Noun Phrase
9
In the NP, the numeral modifier follows the N but it is the final vowel of
the N that either undergoes glide formation if it is [+high] or gets elided if it is
[-high] thereby setting its tone afloat. The floating tone merges with the prefix
vowel of the numeral resulting in one of three tonal outputs: (i) a LH contour if
the combination is a final L + a numeral-initial LH as in (lla, b); (ii) a !H if the
combination is !H+LH as in (llc, d); or (iii) a H if the combination is H+LH as in
(lIe, f). Note that in (1 Ie, d) the 'H survives because the immediately following
tone is a sequence ofL+H. Where the tone immediately following 'H is L, 'H survives but where it is H, 'H deletes as in (8b) above. In (11 e, f), the expected HLH
triple sequence simplifies to just H as a result of the principle that H tones are
preserved. This principle of sacrificing L's in favor of H's will also be found in
subsequent sections.
(11 ) a. lkpe
v
,
IVE
beds
two
b. ab;
era
hands
/
c. eSE
fathers
d. eg5
in-laws
e. enE
yams
f. lko
cups
v
,
Gkp'ive]
'two beds'
[abera]
'three hands/ thrice'
[eslve]
'two fathers
[egErere]
'eight in-laws
[emjofl]
'five (tubers of) yams'
Gkesa]
'six cups'
three
v
,
IVE
,
nine
v
/
,
Erere
,
eight
V'.' ,
IJOfl
five
v
/
esa
six
,
The forms for' eleven' through 'nineteen are:
(12)
uhw5vo
lhw'ivE
lhwera
'ihw£ne
ihw'ij6rl
'eleven,2
,
'twelve
'thirteen'
'fourteen'
'fifteen'
lhwegbesa
lhwegb'i yW fE
lhwegbEnbfe
'ihwegblnfl
,
'sixteen
'seventeen'
'eighteen'
'nineteen'
After the form for 'ten', the only other unique forms are:
In this language, vowels frequently assimilate in roundness or backness to neighboring
vowels. This explains why the initial vowel in the form for' 11' is [u] rather than [i]. See also
the forms in example (16) below.
2
10
(13)
Studies in African Linguistics 32(2), 2003
'20'
'30'
'1,000'
'100,000'
, 1,000,000'
'1,000,000,000 '
, 1,000,000,000,000'
u3e
5gba
un
odu
odudufu
oduduma
Ima
As will be noticed from the data in (12), the forms for 'eleven' through
'fifteen' are derived from a combination of 'ten' followed immediately by the
forms for 'one' through 'five' with the final vowel of 'ten' elided. In terms of
tone, these combinations manifest important tonal modifications. First, the L tone
of the elided vowel of 'ten' merges with the L of the LH contour of the combining digit. Second, the initial LH of 'ten' simplifies to H while the L tone( s) of the
root in the non-initial number becomes H; this tone raising does not affect all numerals in the construction, just the second one. We can account for this modification by postulating a floating H tone construction marker for numerals formed
from a combination of two numerals in succession. This explains why '6ne appears as '6ne.
As part of the NP, the tone of the final vowel of the noun gets deleted with
its vowel if it is L or !H, leaving only the initial H tone of the numeral. In this
case, the pattern of tone retention on the noun is different than it was in other
noun + numeral cases, where those numerals began with a rising tone. In those
earlier cases, final 'H is kept. Now here, !H deletes while the initial H of the numeral is kept. This follows from the basic phonological distinction between the
numbers, where there is a level H in these complex number constructions involving two numerals.
The forms in (14) below are examples of noun plus numeral:
(14)
a.
lWU
~wlh'ivE]
1hw'i VE
' 12 garments'
garments twelve
b. lko
cups
/
-
c. esc
fathers
Ihw'ijon
~klhwjon]
fifteen
'15 cups'
[esihw£ne]
' 14 fathers'
ihw£ne
fourteen
Tonal Alternations In The Urhobo Noun Phrase
11
From 'sixteen' through 'nineteen', the numerals 'six' through 'nine' are
added to 'ten' with a conjunctive element [gbe] whose vowel and L tone are always elided. The only tonal modification in these combinations is on the initial
vowel of ' 10' which also bears H tone: units following [gb£] retain their original
tone(s). In the NP, tonal alternations are as in (11) above. Examples include:
(15)
~wlhegbsa
a. iwu ihwegbesa
b. es£ ihwegblrlrl
c. ik6 ihwegbErere
]
[esihwegblrlrl]
~klhwegbEr
]
, 16 garments'
, 19 fathers'
'18 cups'
The numerals 'twenty-one' through 'twenty-nine' and 'thirty-one' through
'thirty-nine' are formed by combining 'twenty' or 'thirty' and the relevant digit
with [gb£] between them. No new tonal alternation occurs in these. From 'forty'
onwards, counting is in multiples of 'twenty' and the numerals are formed by
combining 'twenty' with the forms for 'one' through 'nineteen'. In these multiples of 'twenty', a H tone emerges between 'twenty' and the combining numeral,
evidence of the floating H construction marker identified earlier which is found in
numerals made up of two numerals that follow each other directly without the intervention of[gb£].
The forms for 'forty', 'sixty', 'eighty' and 'one hundred' are:
(16)
U3uv£
u3 0ra
U3;)ne
U3 uon
,
v
,
[U3e + (H)
[U3e + (H)
[U3e + (H)
[U3e + (H)
+ lvE]
+ eni]
+ Ene]
+ Ijori]
'forty'
'sixty'
'eighty'
'one hundred'
As part of the NP, other tonal alternations result from the principle of H
preservation earlier identified so that even a L-toned number prefix deletes when
preceeded by a H or 'H. Compare the sequences in (17) with those in (18). Notice
that there is no rising tone at all in these "mUltiplicative" structures and there is
+
'twenty cups'.
no prefixal rising tone in
eg3 uje
(17)
a. iko + U3uv£
b. eg5 + u30ra
c. ab~
+ u35ne
~ku3UV£]
[eg u30ra]
[abu35ne]
'40 cups'
'60 in-laws'
'80 hands/times'
12
(18)
Studies in AFrican Linguistics 32(2), 2003
a.
b.
c.
d.
lko + u3egb'iv£
ab; + u3egbi:ne
eg5 + u3uv£gblhwenl
lko + u35negb'ihwegb'irlfl
~ku3egb'iv£
]
[abu3egbi:ne]
[egu3uve gblhwenl]
~kti35negb'hwrlf]
'22
'24
'53
'99
cups'
hands/times'
in-laws'
,
cups
The last group of non-associative NPs to be examined here is the Noun +
Attributive modifier NP. As Welmers (1969) correctly observes, Urhobo has no
word class which can be appropriately labeled 'adjective', rather there are attributive forms which function as adjectives and can be found in both the nonassociative and associative constructions. Some of the non-associative attributive
forms are derived from verbs through a process of reduplication and these usually
have three or four syllables: those with three syllables bear HHL tones while
those with four bear HHLL tones. As modifiers, they follow the noun and one of
the following alternations may take place: (i) if both tones at the boundary are
identical, they merely collapse into one with the final vowel of the noun deleting
as in (19a, b); (ii) if the final vowel of the noun bears a 'H or L and the prefix
vowel of the modifier bears H, both the final vowel of the noun and its tone are
deleted and replaced by the vowel and tone of the modifier as in (19c-f). If on the
other hand the tone of the noun vowel is H and that of the modifier is L, then the
noun vowel and its tone replace the modifier vowel and its tone as in (19g-i).
(19)
;)ruaro
a. uko
cup
big
'
b. aIJma
/
cloth
c. 5g5
in-law
d. ly6
5fuitfo
white
obiebi
black
obuebu
money
, ,
much/plenty
e. eru
6tete
cap
small
f. Ikpe
beds
IJ OJ oUi
fine (pI.)
[uk5rwitro]
'a big cup'
[aIJm5fwitfo]
'a white cloth'
[5gobjebl]
'a black in-law'
[lyobwebu]
'much/plenty of money'
[erwotete]
'a small cap'
[lkpljoj Oul]
'nice beds'
Tonal Alternations In The Urhobo NOlin Phrase
,
/
ekpo~
g. eIJma
cloths
in-laws
1.
[eIJmakpo~
]
'new (pieces of) cloth'
[eg5kpo~
]
'new in-laws'
[ukperode]
'a big bed'
new (pI.)
ekpo~
new (pi)
~rode
a big one
h. eg5
ukpe
bed
13
It is possible to combine some of these structures, but no new tone changes occur.
(20)
Ene ~na
a. 'ikpe 'itete
beds
small
four the
b. eg5 ekpo~
esa
in-laws new
c. 'iko 'irefe
six
Erere ewe
cups empty
eight
yours
d. eg5 'ibuebu :ma e30b'i :ma
in-laws many
the
all
the
0kp'itetenena]
'the four small beds'
[egekpokpesa]
'six new in-laws'
0kltefErerewe]
'your eight empty cups'
[eg'ibwebu ne30b'i na]
'all the many in-laws'
A close look at the preceding data reveal that the loss of 'H is not conditioned by the fact that the vowel bearing it is deleted, but rather by the type of
tone which immediately follows it: if the following tone is a L the 'H survives,
but if the following tone is a H, the 'H is lost. The crucial feature of down step
deletion in this language is that 'H immediately follows H. This also explains why
in (11 c, d) where 'H merges with LH, 'H survives since it is immediately followed
not by H but by the L part of the sequence.
In many languages that have downstep, sequences such as (19a) with consecutive H tones in a grammatical sequence (cf. Igbo (Clark 1980)), and (l9f)
where the L between two H tones is deleted (cf. Ghotuo (Elugbe 1985)) would
trigger downstep, but in Urhobo they do not. In fact, as examples (19c, d), as well
as many of the examples in the other sections show, the preservation of H will result in the replacement of 'H with regular H rather than the reverse. When 'H
H on one vowel, the result is H and not 'H.
merges with an immediately f~lowing
These facts are worthy of note since they contradict what is usually held in the
literature: that down step results from a lost L tone, or that it is syntactically or
grammatically conditioned. Even in the tradition where the downstep is regarded
as an entity separate from the H, when H gets deleted, one does not expect the
down step to also be deleted. Yet, in Urhobo, the deletion of a H also means the
deletion of the preceding downstep. Problems such as these have led some to re-
14
Studies in African Linguistics 32(2), 2003
gard the third pitch in Urhobo as a mid tone and not 'H, but its distribution is
definitely that of a downstep. Unlike the M tone in, e.g. Yoruba, no utterance begins with this supposed M, and it does not appear after L: no tone rises above it
within the phrase. As far as we can tell, the down step in Urhobo is phonemic and
is phonologically 'bound' to the H tone, and is found as part of the lexical tones
of some nouns and numerals. There are no processes in the tonology of this language that actively create downsteps.
2.2 Associative constructions. The associative construction is used to express
both a possession relation and an association relation. Its structure is that of a
noun and its modifier conjoined by a high-toned independent grammatical morpheme /cE/ which we refer to as the associative marker (AM). The vowel of AM
is always elided in normal conversational speech and, in fast speech, even the
consonant may be elided, although it is easy to recover both in slow deliberate
speech. Its tone is retained and surfaces on the following vowel. Two types of associative constructions are examined: Noun + Noun, and Modifier + Noun.
2.2.1 Noun + Noun (Nl + N2) Associative construction. This type of NP signals a possession relation, and the modifier is either another noun or a possessive
pronoun other than the first and second person singular forms. For our purposes
here, we shall regard these possessive pronouns as nouns. The modifier follows
the head noun with the high-toned AM between them. When the vowel of the AM
is elided, its high tone obligatorily relinks onto the prefix vowel of N2 and either
merges with the tone in that position if it is H, or replaces it if it is L. Thus, only
the prefix vowel of N2 in the output is affected by tone change: it is always H.
(Note that the tones involved in this alternation are those of the AM which is H
and the prefix vowel of N2 which can only be H or L; !H is excluded from these
positions). In (21 c), because the noun begins with a consonant, a vowel is inserted
in the output which then bears the H tone of AM after it is set afloat. As mentioned earlier, where the lost tone of N2 is a L, there is no lowering effect on any
surviving tone in the sequence. This is because, as has been observed elsewhere
(Aziza 2001), although there is downdrift in this language, the second H of a HL-H sequence is lowered only if the L is present in the output. If it is deleted before it gets to the surface, it simply disappears leaving no trace on surviving
tones. In (21 d) there are two levels of the downstep, the first lowers the pitch of
the following H while the second is lower than the first, producing a terracing ef-
Tonal Alternations In The Urhobo Noun Phrase
15
fect. Where the NP is a recursive one as in (2Ih, i), the same processes observed
above also take place with the high-toned AM preceding every additional noun.
/
(21 ) a. obJ
r8
hand
em
[obJ f(!nl]
'an elephant's hand'
[uko r5gba rltlv3]
'a cup belonging to a warlord
from where?'
Gy6 rltega]
'Tega's money'
Gy6 r5g5 v5]
'which in-law's money?'
[ukpe r5je]
'his bed'
[5g5 ravare]
,
'our in-law
Uu)ma rowllVa]
'your cloth'
AM elephant
/
/
Itlv5 (L)
Cup AM warlord AM where Q
b. uko
c.
/
5gba
r8
-
/
Tega
rE
lyO
r8
money AM name
d.
/
-
/
r8
lyO
5g5
in-law
money AM
/
e. ukpe
r8
bed
,
JJe
AM
/
f. 5g5
r8
his
avare
in-law AM
/
we/our
/
g. aDma
cloth
r8
owava
AM
you/your (pI.)
h. [[[aDma
cloth
(L)
which Q
JVJ
rE
J<pa]
rE
5Sare]
AM
bride
AM
man
f8
ode
na]
AM yesterday the
[aDma r5<pa r5Sare rodE na]
'cloth (belonging to) the yesterday's man's bride'
1.
[[obJ
rE
enl]
rE
IJen!]
[obJ renl rlJere]
AM elephant AM name of town
hand
'a hand (belonging to) Jesse's elephant'
2.2.2 Modifier + Noun. As mentioned in 2.1 above, there are attributive modifiers which signal an association relation which are actually nouns and feature in
structures similar to the N 1 + N2 phrase type described above except that in this
case, the modifier precedes the noun.
(22)
,
,
,
/
a. omamJ rE
good
b. erave
animal
ohuo
AM
person
r8
ayua
AM
bush
[omamJ rohwo]
'a good person'
[erave raywa]
'a wild animal'
16
Studies in African Linguistics 32(2), 2003
,
c. egodo
fS
uruoma=
[egodo fUfwoma]
'a peaceful compound'
[lnbo rinonE]
'today's pepper'
[ekpu raware]
'an old bag'
[ekpu riy6]
'a bag (containing) money'
compound AM peace
d. lnbo
pepper
e. ekpu
bag
f. ekpu
bag
g. [[[1y6
,
rs
AM
,
rs
, , ,
mons
today
aware
AM
,
rs
AM
rE
money AM
old
,
-
Iyo
money
uko]
rE
udl]
cup
AM
drink
rE
aJe
AM woman
mE]~
mine
=
[iy6 ruko rudl raje mE]
'money for my wife's cup of alcoholic drink'
There is also an associative construction that functions as an adverbial
which Welmers lists as a demonstrative, namely JrobOl 'that one over there', pI.
erobol 'those'. We treat them as associative constructions and not unanalyzable
words for two main reasons: first, although Urhobo has vowel sequences, there
are no [oi] sequences, and second, there is actually an approximant Iji between the
two vowels which may be elided. Examples of this form include the following.
(Only the first example has been broken into its components.)
(23) a. ~
[~robjl]
pI. [erobojl]
'that one over there' 'those ones over there'
is ) here'
pI. erobonE 'these'
'this one here'
pI. eretlnE 'these'
'that one there'
pI. eretljl 'those'
-rE obojl =
3rd pers.sg. AM there
~robnE
'the one (that
b.
c. retinE~
d. retlj~
It can also take a demonstrative as in (24) below:
(24)
a. al)ma robonE na
b. ewu retljl na
c. Iy6 roboji na
'the cloth here'
'that dress there'
'the money over there'
2.3 Noun + Relative Clause. In order to make this section clearer, some preliminary remarks about verbal constructions in Urhobo need to be made. Most of the
simple verbs are either monosyllabic or disyllabic and begin with a consonant.
Tonal Alternations In The Urhobo Noun Phrase
17
There are no minimal pairs in the verb class which differ in lexical meaning only
as a result of a difference in tone patterning, so that while nouns can be classified
according to their tone patterns, verbs cannot be so classified. In the citation form,
all verbs (as well as all singular subject pronouns) bear the low tone and, although
a number of tonal alternations are attested in verbal constructions, they are usually motivated by the different grammatical configurations. All verbs with the
same syllable structure will behave alike in a given verbal construction. Differences in tense and aspect are usually revealed through tonal alternations in the
subject NP, or the verb and/or the prefix of the object NP.
For instance, Urhobo marks only three tenses, the present, the past and the
future and, apart from the future tense which has an additional tense marker,
tenses are differentiated only by tone. There are floating Hs in all tenses, which
appear on the final vowel of the subject NP in the present tense, on the final
vowel of the verb stem in the past tense and on both the subject NP and the verb
in the future tense. The question morpheme is a floating L tone borne by the final
vowel of the utterance, while the negative morpheme is a floating LH tone also
segmentalized utterance finally. The segmentalization of these floating tones usually causes tonal modifications (often in favor of the H tone, including creating
contour tones) and, in some cases, vowel lengthening. The examples in (25) are
for illustration as a full examination of this topic is beyond the scope of this paper. Notice, however, that whereas segmentalization of the floating tones results
in the creation of contour tones in (25a, d), it results in the deletion of L tones in
(25b, c) while in (25e) it causes both vowel lengthening and tone contour.
(25)
a.
b.
,
:)
(H) de
3rd pers. sg.
pres. buy yam
,
:)
de
he/she buy
(H)
:)n£
past
yam
,
,
he/she fut. part. buy fut.
,
:)
dE (H)
he/she buy past
e.
,
:)
(H)
[;, d5n~]
'he/she bought yam'
/
de (H) :)n£
c. ;, (H) ca
d.
[5 d;,n~]
'he/she buys/is buying yam'
:)n£
dE
he/she pres. buy
yam
:)n£ (L)
yam
Q.
:)n£ (LH)
yam Neg.
[5 eli d5ni]
,
'he/she will buy yam
[;, d5ni:]
'did he/she buy yam?'
[5 d;,n~E]
,
'he/she doesn't buy yam
Studies in African Linguistics 32(2), 2003
18
The Noun + Relative Clause construction is marked on both the segmental
and tonal levels. It is marked by a low-toned relativizer Irf) which occurs between
the noun and the modifying relative clause and by a floating H tone which is
segmentalized on the final vowel of the subject of the relative clause. The relativizer should not be confused with the AM because, although they are similar in
terms of segments (both are irEI and in both cases the vowel is deleted), they bear
different tones: the AM bears a H tone while the relativizer bears a L tone. In addition, the tones of these morphemes behave differently. Whereas the H tone of
the AM does not disappear when its vowel deletes but surfaces on the following
vowel and replaces its inherent tone, the L of the relativizer disappears with its
vowel with no effect on surrounding tones.
The subject of the relative clause immediately precedes the verb and its final vowel does not undergo vowel elision or glide formation because the following segment is a consonant. When the H tomorph construction marker is segmentalized on it, the result is the deletion of the L or 'H inherent tone at the end of
the word in that position, so that this vowel always surfaces as H in the output. It
can be seen in the examples in (26) below that this H tone is there in connection
with relative clauses irrespective of whether the head noun is subject or object
within the relative clause. In our examples, we identify the relativizer as reI., the
subject concord marker as SCM and the floating construction marking tone as
RTM (relative tone marker). Note that where both the tense marker and the RTM
are segmentalized on the same vowel, the two merely contract into one with no
effect on vowel length. In (26c, g, j), the past tense suffix [re] found in (26a, b, e,
f) is deleted because the verb takes an object, the verb stem vowel then gets
elided resulting in the past tomorph surfacing on the prefix vowel of the object.
(26)
,
, ,
(H)
a. ob:, rE em
[ob:, rem rere]
rere
hand reI. elephant RTM ate
'a hand that an elephant ate'
,
b. ob:, :,na rE
(H)
:l
/
,
[ob:, na r5 rere]
rere
hand the reI. he/she RTM ate
'the hand that helshelit ate'
c. em
,
rE
elephant reI.
,
:)
(H)
re
(H)
SCM RTM eat past
'an elephant that ate a hand'
ob:,
hand
[enl r5 rjob:,]
Tonal Alternations In The Urhobo Noun Phrase
d. enl
rE:>
(H) (H) re
19
ob3 3na =
elephant reI. SCM pres. RTM eat hand the
'an elephant that eatslis eating the hand'
e. ure
rE 5s£
(H) torEre
[tIre r5sE torEre]
wood reI. father R TM burnt
'wood that father burnt'
f. tIre
rE 5s£
3na (H)
wood reI. father the
[tIre r5s£ na torEre]
torEre
RTM burnt
'wood that the father burnt'
g. 5s£
rE
3
(H) tore (H) uweOl
[5s£ r5 tOrUweUl]
father reI. SCM R TM bum past house
'a father that burnt a house'
h. 5g5
rE
5s£ (H) (H)
,
[5g5 r5sE se]
se
in-law reI. father pres. RTM call
'an in-law that father calls/ is calling'
1.
1Yo
rE 5g5
3na (H) (H)
money reI. in-law the
ca
ca hwii]
~yo
r5g5 na
~yo
remevo SE kEuE]
hwa (H) =
fut. RTM part. pay
fut.
'money that the in-law will pay'
J. 1Yo
rE em:>
eva (H) SE
(H) kE UE =
money reI. children some RTM sell past
'money that some children sold
for me
for me'
In (27) below, we present some examples in context. Notice that any word
before the relative clause verb has a final H and in (27c), we display the two types
of [rs] in the complex NP: the first one, the associative marker, which joins two
nouns bears a high tone while the second one, the relativizer, which joins the first
NP to the relative clause bears a low tone, although after relevant phonological
processes have taken place, both are indistinguishable
20
(27)
Studies in Afi'ican Linguistics 32(2), 2003
a. me mrE ob~
I
saw
rE em
(H) rere
= [me mrob~
rem rere]
hand reL elephant RTM ate
'I saw a hand that an elephant ate'
b. ob~
r£ eni
(H) rere 5je me mrEre
hand reL elephant RTM ate that I
= [ob~
rem rere 5je me mrEre]
saw
'a hand that an elephant ate was what I saw'
c. [[[[[5Sare rE ~<pa
~na]
man AM bride the
~
(H) dE ukpe] odE]
Seri] non£]
reL SCM RTM bought bed yesterday fell today
r£
= [5Sare r5<pa na r5 dukpe ode Seri non£]
'the husband of the bride who bought a bed yesterday fell down today'
From the foregoing, it is easy to observe that the tone change, even here, is not
due to vowel deletion, but rather to the affiliation of a floating tone.
2.4 Tonal alternation in Noun/NP doubling. Noun/noun phrase doubling is a
syntactic construction used in Urhobo to express some type of quantification
equivalent to the English noun phrases: 'every/each + noun' and 'only/so many
or much + noun'. The construction is marked on both the segmental and tonal
levels. A low-toned independent morpheme [k£] precedes the noun and both the
morpheme and the noun are duplicated to make up the noun phrase. The vowel of
[k£] together with its L tone are elided while the consonant joins the following
noun by resyllabification. A floating H tone construction marker (which we identify as the double construction tone marker (DTM) surfaces on the right end of
the first noun and the result is that an underlying L or 'H in that position becomes
H in the output. Only the right edge of the first noun is affected by tonal modification, all other tones remain intact. The NP may be further modified by an 'adjective' which is also doubled as in (28g, h). We have used this NP type in complete clauses in those last two examples to show its behavior in fuller sentential
context: the doubling of segments does not proceed beyond the 'adjective' and
the tone principles of the preservation of H remain the same, but note that the
floating H will appear on a postnominal adjective, if the noun phrase contains a
modi fer. Tense tones have not been further analyzed. In (28h) both the associative
and relative clause markers are involved and the results are as expected.
Tonal Alternations In The Urhobo Noun Phrase
(28)
a. k£ ob; (H)
k£ ob;
[kob5kob;]
'every/each hand'
part. hand RDTM part. hand
b. k£ 5Sare (H)
k£ 5Sare
[k5Sarek5Sare]
'every/each man'
part. man RDTM part. man
c. k£ uko (H)
k£ uko
[kukokuko]
'every leach cup'
part. cup RDTM part. cup
d. k£ Irlbo (H)
[klrlboklrlbo]
'every/each pepper'
k£ Irlbo
part. pepper RDTM part. pepper
e. k£ 5g5
(H)
[k5g5k5g5]
'every/each in-law'
k£ 5g5
part. in-law RDTM part. in-law
f. k£
ewerl (H)
k£ ewerl
[kewenkewerl]
'every/each monkey'
part. monkey RDTM part. monkey
g. k£ ukpe ;rode (H)
part. bed
big
k£
RDTM part.
21
ukpe ;rode r£
bed
big
;
(H) dEre ulrlrl
reI. he/she RTM bought broke
=[kukpe rode kukpe rode r5 dEre Ulrlrl]
'each/every large bed that he/she bought got broken'
h. k£ omam; rE ewerl (H)
k£ omam; rE ewerl r£ 5 (H)
ca
Part. good AM monkey RDTM part. good AM monkey reI. he fut. RTM
dE
ca JE
buy fut. run
ca ca
dE JE]
=[komam; rewen komam; rewer! r5
'each /every good monkey that he/she will buy will run (away).'
In these sequences too, Land 'H delete in the presence of floating H, also not involving vowel fusion
2.5 Tonal alternation in noun reduplication. This construction conveys the
meaning 'only/so many or much + noun' and affects mainly the plural form of the
noun. As mentioned earlier, pluralization is by prefix vowel alternation. This NP
type is not marked by any independent segment rather, all the sound segments of
the first occurrence of the noun except the prefix vowel and the initial consonant
segment(s) fuse with the duplicate. Other words that follow the noun within the
22
Studies in African Linguistics 32(2), 2003
phrase are not copied. In terms of tone, a floating H toned reduplication marker
(RM) surfaces on the prefix vowel of the noun duplicate resulting in the replacement of L or !H in that position. Examples include the following.
(29)
a. ab~
(H)
ab~
[ab~]
hands RDTM hands
b. es£
(H) es£
fathers RM fathers
c. lbro (H)
'ibro
rE enE
halves RDTM halves AM yams
'only/so many hands'
[eses£]
'only/so many fathers'
[lbiibro renE]
, only/so many half (tubers ot) yam'
3. Summary and conclusion
In this paper, we have described tonal alternations as they occur in different types
of noun phrases in Urhobo. The noun phrases were divided into four main groups:
the non-associative, the associative, the noun + Relative clause and noun reduplication constructions, each with a number of sub-types. One obvious fact is that
floating grammatical tones exist and are commonly used to mark various grammatical constructions in this language. In all the NPs examined, tonal alternations
affect only sequences at word boundaries and result from either the merging of
vowels or the segmentalization of floating grammatical tones. From the evidence
at our disposal, the behavior of tones in the NP illustrate a general principle that
H tones are preserved at the expense of L tones. In addition, when 'H merges with
an immediately following H on the same vowel, the result is H rather than !H, an
indication that !H is this language may have a phonemic status; more so that the
phenomenon is found only as part of the lexical make-up of some nouns. There is
also evidence that grammatical tones have more relevance in the grammar of this
language than lexical tones. so that Land !H delete in the presence of floating
grammatical H even where vowel fusion may not be involved.
Tonal Alternatiolls In The Urhobo
NOUIl
Phrase
23
REFERENCES
Aziza, Rose O. 1997. Urhobo Tone System. Ph.D Thesis, University ofIbadan.
Aziza, Rose O. 2001. "An overview of the tone system of Urhobo". Paper read at
the 32 nd ACAL, University of California, Berkeley, March 23-25,2001.
Clark, Mary M. 1980. "On the treatment of syntactically distributed downstep".
Studies in African Linguistics 11: 10 1-13 7.
E1ugbe, Ben O. 1973. A Comparative Edo Phonology. Ph.D Thesis, University of
Ibadan.
E1ugbe, Ben O. 1985. "The tone system of Ghotuo". Cambridge Papers in Phonetics and Experimental Linguistics 4: 1-21
E1ugbe, Ben O. 1989. Comparative Edoid: Phonology and Lexicon. Delta Series,
No.6. Port Harcourt: University of Port Harcourt Press.
Kelly, John. 1969. "Urhobo". Twelve Nigerian Languages, ed. by Elizabeth Dunstan. London: Longman. Pp. 153-161.
We1mers, William E. 1969. "Structural notes on Urhobo". Journal of West African Languages 1: 85-107.
Welmers, William E. 1973. African Language Structures. Los Angeles: University of California Press.
Department of Languages and Linguistics
Delta State University
Abraka, Nigeria
roaziza
[email protected]
[Received June 22, 2003
accepted June 15,2004]