The Tonal Structure of Nkoroo Nominal Constructions1
Akinbiyi Akinlabi, Bruce Connell and Ebitare Obikudo
Rutgers University, York University and Delta State University.
1. Introduction
1.1 The Language
The Nkoroo people are located in Opobo/Nkoroo local government area of Rivers State,
Nigeria. Nkoroo is an endangered minority language spoken in Nkoroo town alongside Defaka
(another severely endangered language) and in some other smaller villages. In official records,
Nkoroo is the name of the people and the dialect they speak but the people refer to themselves
and their dialect as Kirika, so named after their ancestor who led the migration to their present
location. Kirika has no semblance with Okrika, which is the anglicised form of wa kirik
meaning 'we are the same' in Okrika dialect. Linguistically, Nkoroo has been classified as an
Eastern Ijoid dialect (Jenewari, 1989; Williamson and Blench, 2000) alongside the dialect cluster
of Kalabari, Ibani and Okrika.
1.2 The Key Claims
This paper presents a comparison of the tone patterns found in Nkoroo nominal
compounds and proclitic plus host structures with the tone patterns found in other types of Noun
Phrases. We reveal that a crucial output tone pattern separates Nkoroo nominal compounds and
proclitic+host structures from these Noun Phrases.
Viewed from a processorial perspective, the output tone patterns of Nkoroo nominal
compounds and proclitic+host structures reveal a cocktail of processes, including the possible
postulation of a floating tone, tone spreading processes, and tonal metathesis. However the result
of each process is always the same (a fixed output pattern), regardless of input tone structure.
This paper argues that an adequate account, whether rule based or constraint based, must capture
this apparent conspiracy.
We define tonal conspiracy as a phenomenon in which several independent tonal
processes in a language result in the same tonal outcome. It is as if the language requires a
specific tonal melody on the output, and it does not matter what the input is.
Nkoroo is a two-tone plus downstep language. The examples in (1) illustrate this lexical
contrast.
(1) Tonal contrast in Nkoroo
oki
(HH) ‘take’
oki (H↓H) ‘swim’
oki
(HL) ‘swordfish’
Hoewever, the overall pitch pattern in nominal compounds shows that Nkoroo may be
moving in the direction of an accentual system, with fixed tonal melodies associated with
particular morpho-syntactic categories. Other scholars, including Jenewari (1977) and
Williamson (1978, 1986) have made similar “accentual” observations about Ijoid languages in
general, and Harry (1987) has made this observation about Nkoroo.
1
2.0 Noun + Noun Compounds
In this section, we present a representative set of examples of nominal compounds. Our
only interest is in the tone structure of the compounds. Therefore the nouns are arranged based
on the underlying (or isolation) tone patterns. In the examples in 2-6 the tone of the first noun of
the compound is H (or HH if we take the H tone of each syllable as separate). The tone of the
second noun in the compound is varied. In (2) the second noun is HH; in (3) it is LL; in (4) it is
HL; in (5) it is H↓H; and finally in (6) it is LH. This exhausts the possible patterns that can occur
in the second noun position.
2.1 HH as Noun 1
(2)
HH + HH
u
body
apa
shoulder
foni
bird
apara
skin
mba
bone
ee
mouth
u
apara
skin (of body)
apa mba
shoulder blade
foni ee
beak
→
→
→
The generalization from the above examples is that the compound takes the tone pattern
HL. Fall occurs after the first syllable of the second noun, as the form u apara “skin”
indicates. Essentially input HH + HH becomes output HH HL. Two things are important, (i) the
overall output pattern (HL), and (ii) the alignment of the pattern (the fall from H occurs after the
first syllable of the second noun). Note that the H tone of the first noun remains unchanged.
Processorially, we can assume a floating L (?) or a default L that associates with all syllables of
the second noun, except the first syllable.
(3)
HH + LL
kini
person
ndo
breast
i
leg
okpo
back
mindi
water
tou
edge
kini okpo
human back
ndo mindi
milk
i
tou
heel
→
→
→
When the second noun is LL, the same generalization holds of the output tone pattern of the
compound, as when the second noun is HH. The compound still takes the tone pattern HL, and
what needs to be accounted for is the alignment of this pattern (or melody). Notice that in this
case we need to account for the sudden emergence of an H tone on the first syllable of the first
noun. Processorially, we can postulate the spread of the H from the first noun.
(4)
HH + HL
i
leg
furo
belly
i
furo
thigh
→
2
i
leg
i
foot
kiri
→
land / ground
kiri
When the second noun is HL, the same generalization holds of the output tone pattern of the
compound, as when the second noun is HH (in 2 above) or LL (in 3 above). Again the compound
takes the output tone pattern HL. Interestingly, since the input is HH + HL, nothing needs to be
done in this case. But is this the result of a “null-process”, or the result of the fact that the output
pattern is fixed?
(5)
HH + H ↓H (same as HH)
u
dim
body
hair
nama
dim
animal
hair
u
dim
hair (of body)
nama dim
fur
→
→
When the second noun is H↓H, the same generalization holds of the output tone pattern of the
compound, as in all the cases previously seen. Again the compound must be HL in the output.
But this example is probably easy to account for. This is because the second noun is H↓H. If we
assume that the input tone of the second noun has a Low tone, that is H(L)H, we can say that the
final H is simply not realized. But note that we now have a third process that results in the same
output.
(6)
HHH + LH
anana
sheep
oori
goat
tku
child
tku
child
anana tku
lamb
oori tku
kid
→
→
Finally, when the second noun is LH, the same generalization holds of the output tone pattern of
the compound; it has to be HL. This is by far the most complicated structure to account for. The
examples in (6) show that the input tones LH of the second noun have suddenly become HL.
Now what is going on in (6)? Is this tone spreading, tonal metathesis, or just a pattern HL?
Notice that if we do not want to assume tonal metathesis we must assume that the final H of the
first noun spreads, and the final L tone is inserted (by default?).
We propose instead that there is a pattern HL in Nkoroo nominal compounds. This
pattern emerges as long as the first noun is all H, regardless of the tone of the second noun. But
in order to conclude whether or not the only pattern is HL, it is crucial to vary the tone of the first
noun. We now turn to this in the next two sections.
2.2 HL as Noun 1
Just as we did in the preceding section, we keep the tone pattern of the first noun as HL,
while varying the tone patterns of the nouns in Noun 2 position. In all of the examples in (7) –
(9), we have HL on Noun 1.
3
(7)
HL + HH
ara
arm/hand
ara
arm/hand
dim
palm
dim
palm
mba
bone
mmm
nail / claw
mbli
seed
apa
shoulder
ara mba
arm bone
ara mmm
fingernail
dim mbli
palm nut
dim apa
palm frond
→
→
→
→
These examples reveal that if the fall already occurs on the first noun, there cannot be a
second fall on the second noun. So, there are no HLHL compounds. But note that, taken
together, the entire compound constitutes a single HL pattern, with the H remaining only on the
first syllable of the first noun, and ALL of the remaining syllables of the second noun having a L
tone.
Therefore, when the first noun is HL, and the second noun is HH, the same generalization
holds of the output tone pattern of the compound as when both nouns are HH. The compound
still takes the overall tone pattern HL. The only difference is how the HL is mapped or aligned to
the compound. This shows that the tone of Noun 1 contributes to the alignment of the overall HL
of the compound.
Processorially, we must say that the final L of Noun 1 spreads to all the syllables of the
second noun. But by now it is becoming clear that the processorial way of looking at this is
complete waste of time, since the result is always HL, notwithstanding the process assumed to
result in it.
(8)
HL + LL
ara
arm/hand
ara
arm
k
neck
alki
upper
ara k
lower arm
ara li
lower arm
→
→
When the second noun is LL, the same HL generalization holds of the output tone pattern of the
compound, as when the second noun is HH (in 7 above). Interestingly, the tonal alignment is also
the same. But as in (4) above, one can assume that this is another case of “null-process”. That is,
the input tone pattern is the same as the output tone pattern.
If the forms results from “null-process”, and if we fail to assume a fixed pattern, then the
output result of the form in (9) must be a colossal accident, because an LH second noun is
realized as LL on the surface, just like the LL noun of (8).2
(9)
HL + LH
wari
house
tku
child
wari tku
domestic servant
→
4
The above compound is comparable to the one in (6), where tku “child” takes the tone pattern
HL after a Noun 1, which ends in H tone. Note however that in this case, as in all compounds in
which the first noun underlyingly HL, the entire compound is HL.
If we assume that the desired output tone of the compound is HL, all we need to account
for is how to align the surface HL pattern of the compounds. We can do by assuming that tone of
noun 1 always remains the same. That is, the fall from H to L takes place after the first syllable
of the second noun, except when the first noun is HL. In this case once there is a fall on the
second syllable, this continues throughout the compound.
However, whether the fall takes place on the first noun or the second noun, the overall
melody of the compound is HL.
Finally, does the compound always take the HL tone pattern? If the first noun always
retains its tone as stated here, then we expect that LL nouns in Noun position will also retain
their tone. This is confirmed in the following section.
2.3 LL as Noun 1
Finally, as in the preceding section, we keep the tone pattern of the first noun constant as
LL, while varying the tone patterns of the nouns in Noun 2 position. In all of the examples in
(10) – (13), the compound is all Low toned. This confirms two things. First, it confirms that the
tone of Noun 1 is always the same in isolation as in the compound. Secondly, it proves that once
there is a fall to the low tone or once the Low tone starts, the low remains until the end of the
compound. Essentially therefore, what we need to worry about is where the Fall begins, while
keeping the tone of Noun 1 constant.
A processorial account of the forms below (10) – (13) will need a Low tone spreading
process. The tone spreading must stipulate that it continues to the end of the second noun, to
account for the forms in (10). However, this is not always true. This is because sometimes we
must assume that it is the L tone of the second noun, and not that of the first noun, that spreads.
Proof of this is (13). In (13), it is last syllable of the second noun that is H. The initial syllable is
L. If there already a L tone on the second noun, why start the spreading from the first noun.
(10)
(11)
(12)
LL + HH
kukw
scrotum
okpo
back
mindi
water
mblu
seed
mba
bone
ikaki
tortoise
→
LL + HL
du
farm
kiri
ground/land
→
du kiri
village
LL + LL
mindi
water
abana
pit
→
mindi abana
well
kukw mblu
testicle
okpo mba
spine/backbone
mindi ikaki
turtle
→
→
5
(13)
ndi
fish
abana
pit
→
ndi abana
fish pond
LLL + LH
nambulo
cow
k
chicken
tku
child
tku
child
→
nambulotku
calf
k tku
chick
→
2.4 Generalizations from sections 2.1 – 2.3
The following generalizations emerge from the foregoing discussion.
(a)
The Nkoroo nominal compound has a HL pattern, unless Noun 1 begins with an L tone.
(b)
When Noun 1 begins with L tone, the entire compound is Low toned.
(c)
The tone of Noun1 never changes in the compound.
(d)
When Noun 1 ends in an H tone, the L tone of the HL melody is aligned with the second
syllable of Noun 2, otherwise the entire Noun 2 is L toned.
3.0 Alternative Analyses
In the foregoing we have considered an unattractive processorial alternative, suggesting
what amounts to the following processes:
(i) Floating L on last syllable
u
apara
(ii) H spread from Noun 1
kini
okpo
(iii) L spread from Noun 1
ara
mba
mindi
ikaki
k
tku
(iv) Tonal Metathesis in Noun 2
anana
tku
→
u
apara
→
kini
okpo
→
→
→
ara mba
mindi ikaki
k tku
→
anana tku
However, there is processorial alternative that assumes none of these3. This alternative also does
not assume that a fixed melody drives the tone of the compounds. Suppose we assume that in
Noun1 plus Noun2 compounds the tonal tier of the second noun is deleted wholesale. When
Noun 1 ends in a High tone, there is a one step spread of the High tone to the second noun,
otherwise there is no spreading. Apart from this one step spread, all remaining syllables receive a
Low tone by default.
There are several objections to this, but we will only discuss two. The first is the socalled “Duke of York” gambit. A pattern is undone, only to turn round and do it again. First we
delete a high tone only to replace that same High tone by spreading. This is true of all forms like
(4), where Noun2 is HL. The melody of Noun2 is HL, and it is deleted wholesale only to create
the same melody by spreading an H from the preceding noun and insert a Low tone by default.
6
The second objection is that any rule-based approach loses sight of the fact that the
language calls for a fixed melody, HL, in compounds, whose exception occurs only when Noun
1 begins in L. It is interesting that even in this case the compound has a fixed Low-tone melody,
regardless of the tone of the second noun.
In a constraints only framework, we do not need to assume deletions, spreading, etc. The
only focus is the tonal melody that is considered unmarked for compounds. We state this roughly
as a constraint in (14).
(14)
Compound Tone:
Compounds have the tone pattern HL.
This constraint drives the surface tone pattern of compounds. It certainly has to interact with
other constraints. That of faithfulness to the underlying tone patterns of the nouns; the alignment
of the surface HL, and the LL exceptions. But all these are secondary to the surface pitch pattern
that the compounds assume.
4.0 Nkoroo Noun Phrases
In this section we turn to the nominal structures consisting of short possessive pronouns
and following nouns. The tonal evidence leads us to conclude that these pronouns cliticize with
the following nouns and so form one prosodic word with the noun. This is because their output
tone pattern is the same as that of compounds. This tone structure separates them from other
noun phrases.
4.1 Short pronouns
In Nkoroo, subject short pronouns plus nouns have the same tone structure as
compounds. This suggests that the pronouns cliticize with, and form one prosodic word with the
nouns. A noun changes to L tone after the L tone clitic, and becomes HL after an H tone clitic,
regardless of its underlying tone.
As in the nominal compounds when the pronoun is High toned, the entire possessive
structure has the pitch pattern HL, with the L aligned with the second syllable of the noun,
regardless of the underlying tone pattern of the noun. However, when the short pronoun is L
toned the entire structure takes a surface L, just as we find in the compounds. The examples in
(15) – (18) illustrate this.
(15)
HH noun:
i tee
i tee
wa tee
tee head
my head
your head
our head
(16)
HL noun:
i tru
i tru
wa tru
tru eye
my eye
your eye
our eye
7
(17)
LL noun:
i kl
i kl
wa kl
kl walking stick
my walking stick
your walking stick
our walking stick
(18)
LLH noun:
ye mm
ye mm
wa mm
mm
my fly
your fly
our fly
fly
The form in (18) is especially instructive. It is always possible to claim that the L of the first
person pronoun spreads to the noun, and the H of the second and third person pronouns spread to
the noun. Note however that the isolation tone pattern of the noun in (18) is LLH. But in all
cases, it takes only two tone patterns: it is HLL or LLL. The question is, where is the final H of
the isolation form in all cases? Viewed from a processorial perspective, the loss of the final H is
a problem that calls for powerful maneuvers. Doing this, the overall picture gets lost. The
generalization is simply that the clitic+noun host tone pattern is HL, else LL if the clitic is L.
4.2 The Numerals
Nkoroo numerals behave differently from one another. The numeral “one” behaves like it
is forming compound with the noun. The tone pattern is the same HL that compounds have.
Compare all the forms meaning “one X” in (19a-d). Regardless of the underlying tone
pattern of the noun, the output tone pattern of “ri X” is HL, with the L aligned with the second
syllable of the noun as in compounds whose Noun1 is HH. We must conclude therefore that
based on tonal evidence the numeral “one” compounds with the noun.
(19a) HH noun:
ri tee
tee
head
one head
(19b) HL noun:
ri tru
tru
eye
one eye
(19c) LL noun:
ri kl
kl
walking stick
one walking stick
(19d) LLH noun:
ri mm
mm
fly
[r mm] one fly
The other numerals (“two”, “three”, “four”) do not behave like the numeral “one”. They
also do not have the same tonal effect that the possessive pronouns have have the same effect
that preceding clitics have on following hosts. Though the underlying tone pattern of the numeral
“three” (tara) is HH like that of the numeral “one”, the tones do not behave the same way. This
8
tonal evidence shows that there is no tone spreading in nominal phrases. The underlying tones of
the items are the same on the surface. The forms in (20a – d) show this.
(20a) HH noun:
maa tee
tara tee
tee
head
two heads
three heads
(20b) HL noun:
maa tru
tara tru
tru
eye
two eyes
three eyes
(20c) LL noun:
maa kl
tara kl
kl
walking stick
two walking sticks
three walking sticks
(20d) LLH noun:
mm
fly
maa mm
two flies
tara mm
three flies
Finally, The numerals 5-9 show behaviors that combine the above two. On the one hand
the numerals themselves form independent words within the phrase. But each of these numerals
comes with a clitic-like element, a Low-tone [a]. The vowel [a] seems to cliticize with the noun,
and therefore its Low tone forms the same output seen in compounds where Noun1 has a Low
tone (10 – 13), or where a Low-toned possessive pronoun is followed by a noun (15 – 18).
Compare all the forms meaning “eight X”. In each case the phrase can be separated into two
words: the numeral “eight” and the low tone [a] plus the noun. While the numeral “eight” retains
its underlying HL tone, the [a] cliticizes with the noun, and like the L tone possessive pronoun
“my” forms a L tone word with the noun, regardless of the input tone of the noun. The forms in
(21a – d) illustrate the points made above.
(21a) HH noun:
tee
sn a tee
niini a tee
head
five heads
eight heads
(21b) HL noun:
niini a tru
tru
eye
eight eyes
(21c) LL noun:
niini a kl
kl
walking stick
eight walking sticks
(21d) LLH noun:
mm
fly
9
niini a mm
eight flies
4.3 Nkoroo Demonstratives
Like the numerals, the demonstratives also split into two, in terms of tonal behavior.
Among demonstratives, “this” and “that” behave differently from “some” and “many”. While
“this” and “that” appear to share the tonal characteristics of the compound nouns, “some” and
“many” leave the underlying tone pattern of the noun unaffected. Based on tonal behavior, we
conclude that “this” and “that” compound with the following noun, while “some” and “many” do
not.
Compare the forms meaning “this X” and “that X” with the isolation forms of the nouns.
There is a minor tonotactic restriction involving voiceless consonants. Therefore we have used
both vowel and consonant initial nouns to illustrate the tonal structures here.
(22a) HH noun:
mi aru
ami aru
dia aru
suu(su) aru
aru
canoe
this canoe
that canoe
some canoes
many canoes
tee head
mi tee
ami tee
dia tee
suu(su) tee
this head
that heads
some heads
many heads
(23a) HL noun:
owu
mi owu
ami owu
dia owu
suu(su) owu
sand
this sand
that sand
some sands
many sands
tru eye
mi tru
ami tru
dia tru
suu(su) tru
this eye
that eye
some eyes
many eyes
(b)
(b)
(24a) LL noun:
mi akpa
ami akpa
dia akpa
akpa
eye
this eye
that eye
some eyes
10
(b)
(25)
suu(su) akpa
many eyes
kl walking stick
mi kl
ami kl
dia kl
suu(su) kl
this walking stick
that walking sticks
some walking sticks
many walking sticks
LLH noun:
mm
fly
mi mm
this fly
ami mm
that fly
dia mm
some flies
suu mm
many flies
5.0 Conclusions
Nkoroo noun compounds and pro-clitic+noun host combinations share a fixed tone
pattern: they take a uniform HL pattern, except the noun is L-toned. In this case the entire
compound is L. This output tone occurs without regard to the input tone of the noun. A
processorial account of this outcome fails because nothing in such an approach captures the
desired tonal output of the compounds.
Nkoroo phrases on the other hand have no fixed surface pitch patterns. Though there are
tonal processes not described here, the inputs to these tonal processes rely on the input tones of
the words in the phrase. This is unlike the output patterns described above, which have very liitle
to do with the input tonal structure of the words.
The overall pattern shows the gradual movement in the direction of an accentual system,
which is now widespread in Ijoid (Williamson 1978).
Endnotes
1
A version of this paper was presented at the 26th West African Languages Congress in Winneba, Ghana, in 2008,
and at the 6th World Congress of African Linguistics at Cologne, Germany, in 2009. We are grateful to Ben Elugbe
and Francis Egbokhare, for comments at WALC26, and to John Roberts and Peter Avery for comments at WOCAL
6. All addiional errors are our responsibility. Research resulting in this work was funded by an NSF grant 0553971
awarded to Akinbiyi Akinlabi and Bruce Connell to document Defaka and Nkoroo. We are grateful to the NSF for
supporting the work.
2
The pattern LH is very rare in Nkoroo nouns. In fact, we did not find any compound in which Noun1 is LH.
Therefore it is not possible to test the claim that once the L tone begins in compounds it continues to the end of the
compound.
3
This alternative is due to Peter Avery and John Roberts. They have the credit for the idea, but they are in no way
responsible for the way it is presented here.
11
References
Harry, Otelemate G. (1978). A Phonology of Nkoroo: A Dialect of Eastern Ijo. BA Long Essay.
University of Port Harcourt.
Jenewari, C. E. W. (1977). Studies in Kalabari Syntax. Ph. D Thesis, University of Ibadan.
Jenewari, C. E. W. (1983). Defaka: Ijo's closest linguistic relative. Port Harcourt: University of
Port Harcourt Press.
Jenewari, C. E. W. (1989). Ijoid. In Bendor-Samuel, J. (ed.) The Niger-Congo Languages.
Lanham: University Press of America, 105-118.
Williamson, K. (1978). From Tone to Pitch Accent: The case of Ijo. KIABARA 1: 116-122.
University of Port Harcourt.
Williamson, K. (1989). Niger-Congo Overview. In Bendor-Samuel, J. (ed.) The Niger-Congo
Languages. Lanham: University Press of America, 3-45.
Williamson, K. and R. Blench (2000). Niger-Congo. In Heine, B. & D. Nurse (eds.) African
Languages: An introduction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 11–42.
12