Unawareness of motor phenoconversion
in Huntington disease
Elizabeth A. McCusker,
MBBS (Hons),
FRACP, FAAN
David G. Gunn, PhD
Eric A. Epping, MD, PhD
Clement T. Loy, MBBS,
FRACP, MPH
Kylie Radford, PhD
Jane Griffith, RN
James A. Mills, MS
Jeffrey D. Long, PhD
Jane S. Paulsen, PhD
On behalf of the
PREDICT-HD
Investigators of the
Huntington Study
Group
Correspondence to
Dr. Paulsen:
[email protected]
ABSTRACT
Objective: To determine whether Huntington disease (HD) mutation carriers have motor symptoms (complaints) when definite motor onset (motor phenoconversion) is diagnosed and document
differences between the groups with and without unawareness of motor signs.
Methods: We analyzed data from 550 HD mutation carriers participating in the multicenter
PREDICT-HD Study followed through the HD prodrome. Data analysis included demographics, the Unified Huntington’s Disease Rating Scale (UHDRS) and the Participant HD History of symptoms, selfreport of progression, and cognitive, behavioral, and imaging measures. Unawareness was identified
when no motor symptoms were self-reported but when definite motor HD was diagnosed.
Results: Of 38 (6.91%) with onset of motor HD, almost half (18/38 5 47.36%) had no motor
symptoms despite signs of disease on the UHDRS motor rating and consistent with unawareness.
A group with motor symptoms and signs was similar on a range of measures to the unaware
group. Those with unawareness of HD signs reported less depression. Patients with symptoms
had more striatal atrophy on imaging measures.
Conclusions: Only half of the patients with newly diagnosed motor HD had motor symptoms.
Unaware patients were less likely to be depressed. Self-report of symptoms may be inaccurate
in HD at the earliest stage. Neurologyâ 2013;81:1141–1147
GLOSSARY
ANCOVA 5 analysis of covariance; BDI-II 5 Beck Depression Inventory–II; CAP 5 CAG-Age Product; DCL 5 diagnostic
confidence level; FrSBe 5 Frontal Systems Behavior Scale; HD 5 Huntington disease; HDHX 5 Participant Huntington
Disease History; UHDRS 5 Unified Huntington’s Disease Rating Scale.
Patient unawareness of disease manifestations is documented in neurodegenerative diseases associated with cognitive impairment.1 Unawareness or anosognosia, the pathologic unawareness of a
neurologic or functional deficit,2 is measured as “the discrepancy between the patient’s self-report
and the report of a natural caregiver or the clinical rating of a health professional.”3 Clearly,
affected patients may have no complaints (symptoms), deny difficulty, and display lack of concern
(anosodiaphoria) and insight into disease impact and, importantly, the need for care.4,5
Evidence of unawareness in Huntington disease (HD) in the following studies focused
mainly on individuals with a clinical diagnosis. Patients with HD have difficulty identifying
their chorea, as contrasted with findings on an objective neurologic examination.6 Impaired
self-awareness of the movement disorder is greater in HD than in Parkinson disease.7 Patients
with HD have impaired awareness of their cognitive, emotional, and functional capacity compared with companion ratings and objective assessments.7,8
The PREDICT-HD Study is a longitudinal observational investigation of those with the HD
mutation but who do not meet criteria for a diagnosis of HD at study entry.9 In an earlier study
of a subset of the PREDICT-HD sample, mutation carriers had significantly more frontal
behaviors than noncarriers and a greater discrepancy in reporting frontal behaviors from the
companion report in those closest to phenoconversion, consistent with unawareness.10
Supplemental data at
www.neurology.org
From the Neurology Department (E.A.M., D.G.G., C.T.L., K.R., J.G.), Westmead Hospital, Sydney; Sydney Medical School (E.A.M., C.T.L.),
University of Sydney, Australia; and Department of Psychiatry (E.A.E., J.A.M., J.D.L, J.S.P.), University of Iowa, Iowa City.
PREDICT-HD coinvestigators are listed on the Neurology® Web site at www.neurology.org.
Go to Neurology.org for full disclosures. Funding information and disclosures deemed relevant by the authors, if any, are provided at the end of the article.
© 2013 American Academy of Neurology
1141
ª"NFSJDBO"DBEFNZPG/FVSPMPHZ6OBVUIPSJ[FESFQSPEVDUJPOPGUIJTBSUJDMFJTQSPIJCJUFE
Unawareness could influence accuracy of
information about disease course when there
is reliance on self-report alone. We analyzed
the PREDICT-HD data at the third annual
study visit to:
1. Examine whether patients with HD have
motor symptoms at the time the rater records a motor diagnosis (phenoconversion)
based on the findings of unequivocal signs
on motor examination.
2. Document differences between the groups
with unawareness of motor phenoconversion and without.
3. Determine whether there is a group with
increased vigilance for possible HD symptoms and signs.
METHODS Standard protocol approvals, registrations,
and patient consents. Five hundred fifty HD mutation carriers
and 163 noncarriers (controls) from the PREDICT-HD Study
(clinicaltrials.gov registry identifier NCT00051324) were
included. All participants provided written informed consent as
approved by their individual sites’ Institutional Review Boards.
Participants. The participants did not know of this study
hypothesis when they consented to join the PREDICT-HD
Study. The study was not conceived at the time of data collection
by motor raters. Observations at the third annual study visit provided a reasonable sample size for the grouping of interest. Demographics and data on a variety of domains were collected at each
visit, including the Unified Huntington’s Disease Rating Scale
(UHDRS) and the Participant HD History (HDHX). Table 1
shows the 4 groups (A–D) identified by responses to 1) the selfreport HDHX item 1 (“Since your last visit, have you noticed any
symptoms that you feel are suggestive of HD?”), and 2) the motor
examiner report UHDRS item 17 (“To what degree are you
confident that this participant meets the operational definition
of the unequivocal presence of an otherwise unexplained extrapyramidal movement disorder in a participant at risk for HD?”).
Motor diagnosis is defined as a rating of 4 ($99% rater confidence) on UHDRS item 17, known as the diagnostic confidence
level (DCL). Those diagnosed at baseline or visit 2 were excluded,
and because UHDRS item 17 refers to motor signs, participants
reporting symptoms other than “Motor,” “Oculomotor,” or
“Mixed” on HDHX item 3 (“Describe the symptom[s] that
Table 1
Definition of 4 awareness groups (A–D) for participants with HD
mutation
HDHX item 1: Since your last visit, have you
noticed any symptoms that you feel are
suggestive of HD?
UHDRS item 17
No symptoms
Symptoms
No Dx (rating of 0–3)
A (n 5 427)
B (n 5 85)
Dx (rating of 4)
C (n 5 18)
D (n 5 20)
DCL
Abbreviations: DCL 5 diagnostic confidence level; Dx 5 diagnosis; HD 5 Huntington disease; HDHX 5 Participant HD History; UHDRS 5 Unified Huntington’s Disease Rating Scale.
1142
Neurology 81
you noticed since your last visit: Motor, Cognitive, Psychiatric,
Oculomotor, Other, Mixed”) were also excluded. Unawareness
was identified when no motor symptoms were self-reported but
when a diagnosis of definite motor HD was made.
As seen in table 1, group A reported no symptoms and did not
have a motor diagnosis. Group B reported symptoms but did not
have a motor diagnosis. Group C reported no symptoms but had
a motor diagnosis. Finally, group D reported symptoms and had a
motor diagnosis.
Measures. Clinical. Trained examiners administered the
UHDRS motor examination,11 consisting of 15 primary items.
The examiner rates the DCL based on the UHDRS motor examination that conveys the degree of likelihood that the participant
has manifest HD ranging from 0 5 no abnormalities to 4 5
motor abnormalities that are unequivocal signs of HD ($99%
confidence). The UHDRS also includes the Total Functional
Capacity score and a participant assessment of HD progression
(UHDRS item 79: “Since your last assessment, do you feel
improved, worsened, or about the same?”). At the first study visit,
examiners record whether they are blinded to the participant’s
HD mutation status. For this sample, data were available for all
participants except for one mutation carrier. Motor raters knew
the gene status for 74% of the HD mutation carriers, and 71% of
the noncarriers.
Self-report questionnaires. Frontal/executive dysfunction was
rated using an adaptation of the Frontal Systems Behavior Scale
(FrSBe),12 a 24-item scale on which both participants and their
companions (informants) rate the participant’s severity and level
of distress on a number of frontal/dysexecutive behaviors. Mood
was assessed with the Beck Depression Inventory–II (BDI-II).13
Cognitive measures. Because they are robust clinical indicators of the disease process,14 motor speed was assessed using the
Speeded Tapping Test15 and verbal memory using the Delayed
Trial of the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test–Revised.16 Measures
of executive functioning included a computerized tower task to
assess planning and reasoning (similar to that used by Saint-Cyr
et al.17) and the Interference task from the Stroop Interference
Test.18
Imaging measures. MRI scans were obtained at the same visit
as the clinical measures and acquired and analyzed using a standard
protocol described in previous PREDICT-HD studies.19–24
Data analysis. Two main methods of analysis were used: the x2
test of association for contingency tables, and analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) for comparisons between the groups. A x2 test
of association was used to examine awareness group (groups A–D)
responses to UHDRS item 79 (see above). For a comparison of
symptoms reported by participant group (groups B and D), a x2
test was used. Analysis groups were compared on clinical, selfreport, cognitive, and imaging measures by ANCOVA models,
adjusting for age, sex, and education. Pairwise group comparisons
were made with follow-up t tests.
RESULTS Table 2 presents demographic variables for
the groups. The groups did not differ for female preponderance or education levels. Age difference was not
statistically significant between the mutation carrier
groups (groups A–D). Baseline progression was indexed by the CAG-Age Product or CAP score,25 which
is computed as CAP 5 (Age at entry) 3 (CAG 2
33.66). CAP scores can be converted to a scaled CAP
score (CAPS) based on a 5-year probability of diagnosis
from study entry.25 While group D’s CAP score was
September 24, 2013
ª"NFSJDBO"DBEFNZPG/FVSPMPHZ6OBVUIPSJ[FESFQSPEVDUJPOPGUIJTBSUJDMFJTQSPIJCJUFE
Table 2
Demographics
Group
A (no Sym, no Dx)
B (Sym, no Dx)
C (no Sym, Dx)
D (Sym, Dx)
E (control)
No.
427
85
18
20
163
Sex, % female
62.30
65.88
72.22
70.00
65.64
Age, y, mean (SD)
42.87 (10.01)
44.79 (9.43)
44.83 (11.28)
43.99 (8.05)
46.69 (10.98)
Education, y, mean (SD)
14.48 (2.73)
14.69 (2.78)
13.89 (2.81)
13.75 (2.00)
14.73 (2.61)
337.98 (75.51)
349.66 (78.94)
387.99 (66.66)
460.30 (114.50)
NA
a
CAP, mean (SD)
Abbreviations: CAP 5 CAG-Age Product; Dx 5 diagnosis; NA 5 not applicable; Sym 5 symptoms.
a
CAP 5 (Age at entry) 3 (CAG 2 33.66).
significantly higher than that in other groups, the CAP
score for the other diagnosed group (group C) was
significantly higher than for groups A and B, indicating
that these undiagnosed groups were further from estimated diagnosis.
Table 3 shows the frequency and percentage of responses to UHDRS item 79 by awareness groups.
There was a statistically significant group effect, x2
(6) 5 155.52, p , 0.0001, with a greater frequency
of group D (symptoms, diagnosis) participants reporting decline than participants from other groups (63.2%
vs 43.4% for group B, 11.8% for group C, and 3.6%
for group A). The comparison between groups B and D
was not statistically significant, x2 (1) 5 1.03, p 5
0.3098, indicating no evidence of a difference of symptom perception among the groups.
Table 4 shows the frequency and percentage of
HDHX item 3 for the subcomparison of the symptomatic groups B and D. More than half of the participants in each group reported a decline of motor
functioning relative to their previous visit.
Table 5 presents results for clinical, self-report,
cognitive, and imaging variables. Analysis group
means and SDs are displayed along with ANCOVA
model results. Pairwise group comparisons are shown
in the last column.
The ANCOVA results column indicates a statistically significant group effect for all variables. The group
Table 3
comparison column shows the details of the inequalities.
For every outcome, the control group had the best performance (e.g., smallest mean total motor score). Group
C or D (i.e., manifest HD) or both had the worst performance or most negative mean score on most of the
outcomes. However, groups reporting HD symptoms,
group B (no diagnosis) along with D (diagnosis), had
the most negative mean score on the BDI-II and the
FrSBe. For most variables, group A (no symptoms, no
diagnosis) had the second-best performance/positive
mean score after the control group.
Comparison of groups C and D. Table 5 shows the
group means and differences on the clinical, questionnaire, cognitive, and imaging measures between the 2
groups with a diagnosis, one group of patients with
impaired awareness of motor symptoms (group C)
and another without (group D). These 2 groups did
not differ on their total motor score on the UHDRS or
any cognitive measures, although they performed worse
on these measures than the 2 other patient groups and
the control group. Group C had significantly (albeit
slightly) better functional capacity, less depressive
symptoms, performed faster on a motor speed task,
and had greater striatal and white matter volumes.
Companions in group C reported significantly lower
levels of dysexecutive behaviors on the FrSBe compared
with group D; there was no statistically significant
Participant self-ratings of HD progression (UHDRS item 79)
Group
HD progression
A (no Sym, no Dx)
B (Sym, no Dx)
C (no Sym, Dx)
D (Sym, Dx)
Total
Worsened
15 (3.6)
36 (43.4)
2 (11.8)
12 (63.2)
65
About the same
347 (82.2)
39 (47.0)
14 (82.4)
4 (21.1)
404
Improved
60 (14.2)
8 (9.6)
1 (5.9)
3 (15.8)
72
Total
422
83
17
19
541
Abbreviations: Dx 5 diagnosis; HD 5 Huntington disease; Sym 5 symptoms; UHDRS 5 Unified Huntington’s Disease Rating
Scale.
Data are n (%) by awareness group (p , 0.0001).
Neurology 81
September 24, 2013
1143
ª"NFSJDBO"DBEFNZPG/FVSPMPHZ6OBVUIPSJ[FESFQSPEVDUJPOPGUIJTBSUJDMFJTQSPIJCJUFE
Table 4
Frequency (%) of symptoms reported (HDHX item 3) by group (B and D)
Group
Symptom type
B (Sym, no Dx)
D (Sym, Dx)
Total
Motor only
57 (67)
11 (55)
68
Mixed motor
28 (33)
9 (45)
37
Total
85
20
105
Abbreviations: Dx 5 diagnosis; Sym 5 symptoms; HDHX 5 Participant Huntington Disease
History.
difference in FrSBe participant ratings between
these groups.
Comparison of groups A and B. Table 5 also shows the
group means and differences on the clinical, questionnaire, cognitive, and imaging measures between the 2
groups that did not have a diagnosis, one reporting
symptoms (group B) and one without symptoms (group
A). Group B participants returned a significantly higher
motor score on the UHDRS, reported less functional
capacity, had a higher level of depressive symptoms
Table 5
(similar to group D), and were slower on the speeded
tapping task. Participants and companions of group B
reported greater dysexecutive behaviors compared with
group A. These 2 prodromal groups did not differ on
any of the cognitive or imaging measures. However,
compared with the control group, both performed significantly worse on Verbal Memory and Stroop Interference and had smaller magnetic resonance volumes.
Post hoc correlation analyses. Examination of the results in
table 5 suggested a relationship between dysexecutive
behaviors (FrSBe) and depressive symptoms (BDI-II).
To explore this possibility, correlations between these 2
measures for each of the groups were analyzed (table e1 on the Neurology® Web site at www.neurology.org). In
all groups, participants with increasing BDI-II scores also
had significantly higher FrSBe scores. Similar significant
results in the same direction were observed for
companion ratings in groups A, B, C, and the controls.
Companions had no significant correlation in group D.
DISCUSSION The main findings in this study were
that half of the patients with newly diagnosed motor
Mean (SD) for analysis groups
Group
Measure
A (no Sym,
no Dx)
B (Sym,
no Dx)
C (no
Sym, Dx)
D (Sym, Dx)
Control
ANCOVA result Group comparisona
UHDRS motor score
4.70 (5.2)
9.02 (6.6)
20.89 (6.9)
23.20 (11.1)
2.66 (2.8)
F4,705 5
117.46;
p , 0.0001
C, D . B . A . Cont
UHDRS TFC
12.85 (0.7)
12.27 (1.2)
12.00 (1.9)
11.20 (2.0)
12.98 (0.2)
F4,705 5 34.57;
p , 0.0001
D , B, C , A, Cont
FrSBe total-participantb
55.6 (16.4)
66.7 (23.7)
59.3 (24.2)
68.8 (25.6)
54.3 (14.8)
F4,691 5 10.47;
p , 0.0001
B, D . A, Cont
FrSBe total-companionb
53.1 (17.9)
62.1 (23.0)
60.7 (22.0)
73.7 (29.4)
47.1 (13.0)
F4,619 5 15.19;
p , 0.0001
D . B . A . Cont; D . C .
Cont
BDI-II
5.71 (7.0)
12.86 (11.7)
7.39 (11.4)
14.55 (12.3)
4.40 (5.8)
F4,703 5 23.51;
p , 0.0001
B, D . A, C, Cont
Speeded Tapping–nondominant
index
253.1 (48.8)
274.9 (63.5)
308.9 (70.1)
380.3 (122.7) 232.9 (28.9)
F4,684 5 45.70;
p , 0.0001
D . C . B . A . Cont
HVLT delayed recall
9.80 (2.1)
9.81 (1.8)
8.13 (2.3)
7.40 (3.4)
10.61 (1.7)
F4,628 5 14.39;
p , 0.0001
C, D , A, B , Cont
Tower task
25.37 (6.5)
24.82 (6.0)
30.05 (8.6)
29.61 (7.3)
24.84 (5.5)
F4,603 5 3.94;
p 5 0.0036
C, D . A, B, Cont
Stroop Interference
47.00 (10.6)
44.72 (9.8)
36.18 (8.7)
32.25 (9.1)
48.43 (9.4)
F4,695 5 18.01;
p , 0.0001
C, D , A, B , Cont
Striatum/ICV ratio
0.010 (0.002)
0.010 (0.001) 0.009 (0.001) 0.006 (0.001) 0.011 (0.001) F4,348 5 31.62;
p , 0.0001
D , A, B, C , Cont
White matter/ICV ratio
0.28 (0.03)
0.28 (0.03)
D , A , Cont; B , Cont; D
,C
0.29 (0.03)
0.27 (0.03)
0.30 (0.03)
F4,348 5 8.30;
p , 0.0001
Abbreviations: ANCOVA 5 analysis of covariance; BDI 5 Beck Depression Inventory–II; Cont 5 control; Dx 5 diagnosis; FrSBe 5 Frontal Systems Behavior
Scale; HVLT 5 Hopkins Verbal Learning Test; ICV 5 intracranial volume; Sym 5 symptoms; TFC 5 Total Functional Capacity; UHDRS 5 Unified Huntington’s Disease Rating Scale.
The less than (,) and greater than (.) symbols indicate a statistically significant group difference in the stated direction. A group that is not statistically
different from any of the others is not listed. For example, on the FrSBe participant measure, groups B and D are not statistically different from each other
but they are higher than group A and control, while group C is omitted because it is not statistically different from any other group.
a
Results of ANCOVA adjusting for age, sex, and education.
b
The FrSBe total score is calculated by summing the products of the frequency and distress scores for items 1–18.
1144
Neurology 81
September 24, 2013
ª"NFSJDBO"DBEFNZPG/FVSPMPHZ6OBVUIPSJ[FESFQSPEVDUJPOPGUIJTBSUJDMFJTQSPIJCJUFE
HD had unawareness and were without motor symptoms. These unaware patients were less likely to be
depressed or to report progression. However, paradoxically aware patients had more striatal atrophy.
The unaware group (group C) was similar on a range
of measures—memory and executive functioning and
motor score—to the group who reported symptoms
and had a motor diagnosis (group D). Group B reported motor symptoms but had no motor diagnosis.
An unexpected finding was that groups C and
D differed significantly on FrSBe companion scores
(D . C; see table 5). This questionnaire may not
be an accurate measure of frontal dysfunction associated with unawareness and may reflect a degree of
depressive symptomatology. A relationship between
depressive symptoms and reporting of dysexecutive
behaviors on the FrSBe is shown in the supplementary material. The reliability of these subjective
reports of frontal dysfunction is also questionable in
the group with unawareness. In other studies, impairment on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test correlated
better with lack of awareness.8
Group C had significantly lower scores on the BDIII than group D. The other symptomatic group, group
B, also had a higher depression rating. Depression is
unlikely to account for the motor symptoms in groups
B and D. Depression may be reactive and related to
awareness of onset of motor dysfunction. Greater insight
and awareness would be expected to be associated with
more depression.26 The finding of less depression in
group C suggests lack of insight in this asymptomatic
group. In a recent review of suicide in dementia, preserved insight was a putative risk factor.27
Participants in the PREDICT-HD Study are not
informed of the HD diagnosis/phenoconversion by
the research team, but some with more symptoms
may have been given a formal diagnosis of HD onset
by their treating HD specialist. Because group D had
slightly more progression of HD, more symptoms
would be expected, but group B although undiagnosed was symptomatic as well. In group B, there
were more motor signs recorded on the UHDRS
compared with group A. Group A participants (without a diagnosis or symptoms) had very subtle motor
signs, were younger, and had a lower CAP score,
being furthest from predicted onset. Symptoms
reported in group B, although undiagnosed and with
a lower motor rating than groups C and D, may represent hypervigilance but some may have had activities that required fine motor skills that are more
sensitive to changes. Considering the range of overlap
between the motor scores for groups B (9.02 6 6.6)
and C (20.89 6 6.9), earlier diagnosis could have
been given to some in group B but the DCL rating
of 4 is a conservative assessment that there can be no
doubt about the diagnosis.
Group C differed in other ways from the groups
with symptoms (groups B and D). Group C was more
likely to report no change or improvement since the
visit the year before, again consistent with unawareness with 88% feeling the same or better and only
12% reporting decline. The participants in the symptomatic groups D and B were much more likely to
report that they were the same or worse. There are
other possible reasons for unawareness. With a very
gradual change over many years, onset might not be
noticed. Better knowledge of the disease including
seeing an affected relative could influence symptom
report. Some motor signs may not be associated with
symptoms, i.e., eye-movement impairment. However, the UHDRS mean score in these groups is such
that some signs would be expected to produce symptoms. If these individuals whose symptom report is at
variance with findings on examination are included in
a clinical trial, efficacy measures, based on self-report
for an intervention, could be inaccurate. For participants classified as still prodromal, those with motor
symptoms of HD (group B) had similar cognitive
functioning on most measures to the asymptomatic,
undiagnosed younger group A participants. Group
B participants had significantly higher ratings of
depression and more frontal/executive dysfunction
compared with those who were “symptom-free”
(group A). Interestingly, group B had similar ratings
on some measures to the groups with an HD diagnosis (C and D) despite fewer signs of disease. Earlier
care or intervention could be offered to a group of
individuals identified with apparently heightened
awareness in the prodromal stages.
There is clearly a group unaware of manifestations
(group C). In addition, there is a group whose participants were more aware of motor symptoms (group
B) even before a definitive diagnosis. Looking at the
groups identified in this analysis, it appears that there
is the expected progression of motor signs in the prodrome leading up to HD diagnosis but not a parallel
progression of symptoms.
There were no significant differences in striatal
volumes between the groups other than group D,
who had a significant reduction of striatal volumes.
In previous studies, patient self-reported ratings of
“frontal” behaviors have not correlated with measures
of striatal atrophy.10 Striatal and white matter volumes may be irrelevant in the study of unawareness
but correlate better with motor manifestations and
other measures of cognitive function. Imaging studies
that target the likely area of pathology in unawareness
have not been reported in HD. In other neurodegenerations, including Alzheimer disease, using a range of
imaging modalities, the predominant changes were in
the right dorsolateral prefrontal region.4,28–31 Orbitofrontal-limbic pathology is suggested as the likely trigger
Neurology 81
September 24, 2013
1145
ª"NFSJDBO"DBEFNZPG/FVSPMPHZ6OBVUIPSJ[FESFQSPEVDUJPOPGUIJTBSUJDMFJTQSPIJCJUFE
for anosognosia/anosodiaphoria in HD.7 Neuroimaging
in extrastriatal areas may be more informative.
This study addressed only the motor phenoconverters. There is increased recognition of cognitive/behavioral
phenotypes.32–36 A more comprehensive study would
include these other HD manifestations.
There is approximately 20% uptake of predictive testing.37,38 The PREDICT-HD participants are a select
group of volunteers. Because pretest and posttest counseling programs are a requirement of predictive genetic
testing, greater awareness of HD symptoms and signs
would be expected in these tested participants. Unawareness in this group and unreliable symptom report could
be more marked in the general population of those at
risk. Unawareness has major implications for better
defining the disease process, time of presentation for
diagnosis and assistance, measures of progression, the
impact of impaired function in daily activities including
driving and in the workplace, as well as perception of
possible discrimination and caregiver burden. Treatment, when available or for symptomatic disease features, could be delayed if the person fails to notice the
changes taking place and to present for care.
Methods of measurement and documentation of
unawareness in this earliest disease stage need further
study. Possible measures in established HD and in
frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer disease are reported that include an anosognosia rating scale39 as well
as several neuropsychological measures of impairment
in the anosognosia group, the Wisconsin Card Sorting
Test and tests of visual-spatial ability on the Wechsler
Adult Intelligence Scale–revised. Patients with HD
studied using the Dysexecutive Questionnaire40 rated
their carer accurately but underrated their own executive dysfunction. Patients with HD self-reported higher
ratings on a competency rating scale than their collateral’s rating and disease severity correlated with measures of executive function.8 In particular, the
collateral’s rating of the patient’s behavioral and functional competency correlated with the patient’s number of Wisconsin Card Sorting Test perseverative
responses and scores on the Dementia Rating Scale
Initiation and Perseveration subscales and the Symbol Digit Modalities Test.
Although the numbers in this study are small,
these results suggest that unawareness of HD motor
changes occurs in a significant number of participants
converting from the prodromal to the early stages
of HD and that self-report of symptoms can be unreliable. Another group may overreport symptoms, or
rater scores for diagnosis including use of the DCL
rating are too conservative or not sufficiently accurate
and objective to reflect subtle but definite HD signs.
These findings in this unique group of participants
in the PREDICT-HD Study evolving to very early
HD have implications for other neurodegenerations.
1146
Neurology 81
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
Elizabeth McCusker: concept and design, data acquisition, analysis and
interpretation, and critical revisions. David Gunn and Eric Epping: data
acquisition, analysis and interpretation, and critical revisions. Clement
Loy and Kylie Radford: concept and design, data acquisition, analysis
and interpretation, and critical revisions. Jane Griffith: data acquisition
and critical revisions. James Mills: analysis and interpretation. Jeffery
Long: analysis and interpretation and critical revisions. Jane Paulsen:
funding, analysis and interpretation, and critical revisions.
ACKNOWLEDGMENT
The authors thank the PREDICT-HD sites, the study participants, the National
Research Roster for Huntington Disease Patients and Families, the Huntington’s
Disease Society of America, and the Huntington Study Group.
STUDY FUNDING
Supported by the NIH, National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke
(NS040068), CHDI Foundation, Inc. (A3917), Cognitive and Functional
Brain Changes in Preclinical Huntington’s Disease (5R01NS054893), 4D
Shape Analysis for Modeling Spatiotemporal Change Trajectories in Huntington’s (1U01NS082086), Functional Connectivity in Premanifest Huntington’s
Disease (1U01NS082083), and Basal Ganglia Shape Analysis and Circuitry in
Huntington’s Disease (1U01NS082085). This publication was supported by
the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences, and the NIH,
through grant 2 UL1 TR000442-06. The content is solely the responsibility
of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIH.
DISCLOSURE
E. McCusker and D. Gunn report no disclosures. E. Epping served on
the advisory board for Lundbeck, Inc. in 2012 and receives funding from
NARSAD and Nellie Ball Trust. C. Loy receives funding from the NIH,
Australian Huntington’s Disease Association, Brain Foundation,
NHMRC, CHDI, Mason Foundation, University of Sydney, China
Study Centre, Prana Biotechnology, and Wesmead Charitable Trust Capital Equipment Grant. K. Radford, J. Griffith, J. Mills, and J. Long report
no disclosures. J. Paulsen receives funding through the NIH for the
PREDICT-HD Study. Go to Neurology.org for full disclosures.
Received December 28, 2012. Accepted in final form June 24, 2013.
REFERENCES
1. McGlynn SM, Schacter DL. The neuropsychology of
insight: impaired awareness of deficits in a psychiatric context. Psychiatr Ann 1997;27:806–811.
2. Flashman LA. Disorders of awareness in neuropsychiatric
syndromes: an update. Curr Psychiatry Rep 2002;4:
346–353.
3. Antoine C, Antoine P, Guermonprez P, Frigard B. Awareness of deficits and anosognosia in Alzheimer’s disease
[in French]. Encephale 2004;30:570–577.
4. Mendez MF, Shapira JS. Loss of insight and functional neuroimaging in frontotemporal dementia. J Neuropsychiatry
Clin Neurosci 2005;17:413–416.
5. Sitek EJ, Slawek J, Wieczorek D. Self-awareness of deficits
in Huntington’s and Parkinson’s disease. Psychiatr Pol
2008;42:393–403.
6. Snowden JS, Craufurd D, Griffiths HL, Neary D. Awareness of involuntary movements in Huntington disease.
Arch Neurol 1998;55:801–805.
7. Sitek EJ, Soltan W, Wieczorek D, et al. Self-awareness of
motor dysfunction in patients with Huntington’s disease
in comparison to Parkinson’s disease and cervical dystonia.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2011;17:788–795.
8. Hoth KF, Paulsen JS, Moser DJ, Tranel D, Clark LA,
Bechara A. Patients with Huntington’s disease have
impaired awareness of cognitive, emotional, and functional
abilities. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2007;29:365–376.
September 24, 2013
ª"NFSJDBO"DBEFNZPG/FVSPMPHZ6OBVUIPSJ[FESFQSPEVDUJPOPGUIJTBSUJDMFJTQSPIJCJUFE
9.
10.
11.
12.
13.
14.
15.
16.
17.
18.
19.
20.
21.
22.
23.
24.
25.
Paulsen JS, Langbehn DR, Stout JC, et al. Detection of Huntington’s disease decades before diagnosis: the PREDICT-HD
Study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2008;79:874–880.
Duff K, Paulsen JS, Beglinger LJ, et al. “Frontal” behaviors
before the diagnosis of Huntington’s disease and their relationship to markers of disease progression: evidence of
early lack of awareness. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci
2010;22:196–207.
Siesling S, van Vugt JP, Zwinderman KA, Kieburtz K,
Roos RA. Unified Huntington’s Disease Rating Scale: a
follow up. Mov Disord 1998;13:915–919.
Grace J, Malloy PF. Frontal Systems Behavior Scale (FrSBE):
Professional Manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 2001.
Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. Manual for the Beck
Depression Inventory-II. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation; 1996.
Stout JC, Paulsen JS, Queller S, et al. Neurocognitive signs
in prodromal Huntington disease. Neuropsychology 2011;
25:1–14.
Reitan RM. Manual for Administration and Scoring of the
Halstead-Reitan of Neuropsychological Test Battery for
Adults and Children. Tucson: Neuropsychology Press; 1979.
Brandt J. The Hopkins Verbal Learning Test: development of a new memory test with six equivalent forms.
Clin Neuropsychol 1991;25:125–142.
Saint-Cyr JA, Taylor AE, Lang AE. Procedural learning and
neostriatal dysfunction in man. Brain 1988;111(pt 4):941–959.
Unified Huntington’s Disease Rating Scale: reliability and
consistency. Huntington Study Group. Mov Disord 1996;
11:136–142.
Pierson R, Johnson H, Harris G, et al. Fully automated
analysis using BRAINS: AutoWorkup. Neuroimage 2011;
54:328–336.
Magnotta VA, Harris G, Andreasen NC, O’Leary DS,
Yuh WT, Heckel D. Structural MR image processing
using the BRAINS2 toolbox. Comput Med Imaging
Graph 2002;26:251–264.
Powell S, Magnotta VA, Johnson H, Jammalamadaka VK,
Pierson R, Andreasen NC. Registration and machine learningbased automated segmentation of subcortical and cerebellar
brain structures. Neuroimage 2008;39:238–247.
Magnotta VA, Heckel D, Andreasen NC, et al. Measurement
of brain structures with artificial neural networks: two- and
three-dimensional applications. Radiology 1999;211:781–790.
Paulsen JS, Nopoulos PC, Aylward E, et al. Striatal and
white matter predictors of estimated diagnosis for Huntington disease. Brain Res Bull 2010;82:201–207.
Aylward EH, Nopoulos PC, Ross CA, et al. Longitudinal
change in regional brain volumes in prodromal Huntington
disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2011;82:405–410.
Zhang Y, Long JD, Mills JA, Warner JH, Lu W, Paulsen JS.
Indexing disease progression at study entry with individuals
26.
27.
28.
29.
30.
31.
32.
33.
34.
35.
36.
37.
38.
39.
40.
at-risk for Huntington disease. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 2011;156B:751–763.
Paulsen JS, Nehl C, Hoth KF, et al. Depression and stages
of Huntington’s disease. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci
2005;17:496–502.
Haw C, Harwood D, Hawton K. Dementia and suicidal
behavior: a review of the literature. Int Psychogeriatr 2009;
21:440–453.
Hanyu H, Sato T, Akai T, et al. Neuroanatomical correlates
of unawareness of memory deficits in early Alzheimer’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2008;25:347–353.
Reed BR, Jagust WJ, Coulter L. Anosognosia in Alzheimer’s disease: relationships to depression, cognitive
function, and cerebral perfusion. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol
1993;15:231–244.
Derouesne C, Thibault S, Lagha-Pierucci S, BaudouinMadec V, Ancri D, Lacomblez L. Decreased awareness of
cognitive deficits in patients with mild dementia of the Alzheimer type. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 1999;14:1019–1030.
Starkstein SE, Jorge R, Mizrahi R, Robinson RG. A diagnostic formulation for anosognosia in Alzheimer’s disease.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2006;77:719–725.
Craufurd D, Thompson JC, Snowden JS. Behavioral
changes in Huntington disease. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 2001;14:219–226.
Kingma EM, van Duijn E, Timman R, van der Mast RC,
Roos RA. Behavioural problems in Huntington’s disease
using the Problem Behaviours Assessment. Gen Hosp Psychiatry 2008;30:155–161.
Rickards H, De Souza J, van Walsem M, et al. Factor
analysis of behavioural symptoms in Huntington’s disease.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2011;82:411–412.
Thompson JC, Snowden JS, Craufurd D, Neary D.
Behavior in Huntington’s disease: dissociating cognitionbased and mood-based changes. J Neuropsychiatry Clin
Neurosci 2002;14:37–43.
Marder K, Zhao H, Myers RH, et al; Huntington Study
Group. Rate of functional decline in Huntington’s disease.
Neurology 2000;54:452–458.
Harper PS, Lim C, Craufurd D. Ten years of presymptomatic testing for Huntington’s disease: the experience of
the UK Huntington’s Disease Prediction Consortium.
J Med Genet 2000;37:567–571.
Creighton S, Almqvist EW, MacGregor D, et al. Predictive, pre-natal and diagnostic genetic testing for Huntington’s disease: the experience in Canada from 1987 to
2000. Clin Genet 2003;63:462–475.
Deckel AW, Morrison D. Evidence of a neurologically
based “denial of illness” in patients with Huntington’s
disease. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 1996;11:295–302.
Ho AK, Robbins AOG, Barker RA. Huntington’s disease
patients have selective problems with insight. Mov Disord
2006;21:385–389.
Neurology 81
September 24, 2013
1147
ª"NFSJDBO"DBEFNZPG/FVSPMPHZ6OBVUIPSJ[FESFQSPEVDUJPOPGUIJTBSUJDMFJTQSPIJCJUFE