Larry M. Hyman
2 Morphological tonal assignments in
conflict: Who wins?
1 Introduction
The goal of this paper is to address the following questions: (i) What is the inventory of morphological “contributors” to verb tone paradigms? (ii) What happens
if the different contributors conflict? (iii) What does this say about how (tonal)
morphology works in general? In §2 I present examples showing that tonal morphology can do anything that non-tonal morphology can do. This is followed by
an examination of Haya verb stem tonology in §3. In §4 I then present cases that
show that the reverse is not true: tonal morphology can do things that non-tonal
morphology cannot do. In these cases which involve tonal action across words,
the result is that tonal morphology often obscures the compartmentalization of
phonology, morphology and syntax. The question of why tone should have such
unique properties is addressed in the conclusion in §5.
2 Tonal morphology can do whatever non-tonal
morphology can do
As discussed in Hyman (2011: 203), it is sometimes claimed that tone cannot
mark certain things. For example, a proposed universal made in the presidential address at the Linguistic Society of America a few years ago was that “No
language uses tone to mark case”. That this is not true is observed in the following examples from Maasai [Nilotic; Kenya, Tanzania](Tucker & Ole Mpaayei 1955:
177–184; cf. Bennett 1974; Plank 1995: 59–62; Payne 2008):
(1)
Nominative
èlʊ̀kʊ̀nyá
èncʊ̀màtá
Class II: èndérònì
ènkólòpà
Class III: òlmérégèsh
òlósówùàn
Class I:
Accusative
èlʊ́kʊ́nyá
èncʊ́mátá
èndèrónì
ènkòlópà
òlmérègèsh
òlósòwùàn
nom vs. acc tone patterns
‘head’
Ln-H vs. L-Hn
‘horse’
‘rat’
H on σ2 vs. σ3
‘centipede’
‘ram’
H on σ2 & σ3 vs. on σ2 only
‘buffalo’
16
Larry M. Hyman
Class IV: òmótònyî
òsínkìrrî
òmótònyî
òsínkìrrî
‘bird’
‘fish’
identical tones–no change
As seen in the examples and summarized to the right, the first three declension
classes show only a tonal difference between their nominative vs. accusative
forms. Many other languages could be cited to show that it is not only case that
can be exclusively marked by tone. As Hyman & Leben (2000: 588) put it, “tonal
morphology... exhibits essentially the same range of morphological properties as
in all of segmental morphology”. As common linguistic sense tells us, if tone can
be a morpheme, it can do everything that a morpheme can do.
Concerning verb tone paradigms, the focus of this paper, we therefore expect
that anything that can be marked by a segmental affix or process can also be
marked by tone. This includes inflectional marking of subject, object, transitivity,
tense, aspect, mood, negation, clause type etc. as well as derivational marking of
causative, applicative, reciprocal, passive and other verb forms, as well as processes that derive one word class from another. As an example, verbs are detransitivized in Kalabari [Ijoid; Nigeria] by assigning a /LH/ melody (Harry & Hyman
2012):
(2)
Transitive
a. kán
H
‘tear, demolish’
Intransitive
kàán
LH
kɔ̀n
b. ányá
ɗìmà
sá↓kí
c. kíkímà
‘judge’
‘spread’
‘change’
‘begin’
‘hide, cover’
kɔ̀ɔ́n
ànyá
ɗìmá
sàkí
kìkìmá
L
H-H
L-L
H-↓H
H-H-L
pákɪ̀rɪ́
H-L-H ‘answer’
↓
gbóló má H-H-↓H ‘join, mix up’
pàkɪ̀rɪ́
gbòlòmá
‘tear, be,
demolished’
LH
‘be judged’
L-H
‘be spread’
L-H
‘change’
L-H
‘begin’
L-L-H ‘be hidden,
covered’
L-L-H ‘be answered’
L-L-H ‘be joined,
mixed up’
The “tone is like everything else” idea would lead us to expect the same degree
of uniformity of tonal exponence as in a segmental paradigm. The same tone
or tonal melody should be as consistent a spell-out of a given morphosyntactic
feature. However, this may not always appear to be the case. Tone has a greater
independence (“autosegmentality”) and ability to wander (see §4). Any study of
a tone system with a reasonably complex tonal morphology must find a way to
describe the alternations that take place within the verb system. Verb tone para-
Morphological tonal assignments in conflict: Who wins?
17
digms are often presented in prose, as a table, or more rarely of individual tone
assignment rules. I reproduce an example of one such table in (3) vs. a set of rules
in (4).
(3) Tone on verb stems in Mambay [Adamawa; Cameroon] (Anonby 2011: 374)
Tonal verb
Example verb
perfective prf plu-prf fut opt impf
class
1 intr /CVX/ yáá’ ‘move away’
H
°H[L
H
H H (lex)
intr /CV/ gé
‘get lost’
H
HL*
H
H H (lex)
tr
ɓéé ‘bite’
H
H
H
H H
L
2 intr
sų̀ų̀ ‘lie down’
L
°H[L
H
H H (lex)
3 intr
vè
‘go’
L
L
L
H L (lex)
4 intr
hèè ‘climb’
L
L
L
L
L (lex)
tr
gìì
‘answer’
L
L*
L*
L
L
L
5 intr/tr
yàà ‘finish’
L
LHL* LHL* LH LH LH
6 intr/tr
ʔòògí ‘drag feet’
LH
LHL* LHL* LH LH LH
°H[L = floating H tone on the left boundary of a L stem; * = replacive melody, i.e.,
dominates the entire verb word.
(4) Final stem tone assignment rules in Haya [Bantu; Tanzania] (Hyman &
Byarushengo 1984: 76)
a. Rules assigning H to the final vowel, independent of tone of radical:
(i) ° → H / + V̄ ] [+neg: pst-2]
(ii) ° → H / + V̄ ] [+obj/non-subj rel: fut-2, prs, pst-1, pst-2]
(iii) ° → H / + V̄ ] [–rel, –neg:fut-2, prs, pst-1, pst-2]
(iv) ° → H / + V̄ ] [imperative (-Subj marker/-Obj marker)]
b. Rules assigning H to the final vowel only if the radical is ° (toneless):
(i) ° → H / + V̄ ] [fut-1]
(ii) ° → H / + V̄ ] [+neg: prs.hab]
(iii) ° → H / + V̄ ] [–neg, +rel: pst-3, pst.hab]
(iv) ° → H / + V̄ ] [subjunctive]
(v) ° → H / + V̄ ] [imperative (+Subj marker /+Obj marker)]
c. Miscellaneous
° → H / C V̄]...] [+neg: pst-2]
18
Larry M. Hyman
Such differences raise the issue of how tones should be assigned within a verb paradigm: (i) by global patterning of partially or fully arbitrary “tonal verb classes”,
as proposed for Mambay; (ii) by reference to the morphosyntactic features of individual cells, as proposed for Haya; (iii) by some other way? To a large extent it may
depend on the situation in the individual language. (I return to Haya in §3.)
What is particularly striking in such systems is that the tone assignments
can conflict in a number of ways: (i) by domain (e.g. root vs. stem vs. word); (ii)
by function (e.g. lexical vs. derivational vs. inflections); (iii) by morphosyntactic
feature (e.g. tense vs. aspect vs. negation). Such potentials hold whether the base
is mono- or polysyllabic, contrasting only two tone heights or several. In the latter
case consider for example the eight tone patterns on monosyllables in Iau [Indonesian; Papuan], which are lexical on nouns vs. morphological on verbs (↑H =
super high).
(5) Tone
Nouns
Verbs
H
bé
‘father-in-law’ bá
‘came’
M
H↑H
bē
‘fire’
bé↑´ ‘snake’
bā
‘has come’
bá↑´ ‘might come’
LM
HL
be᷅
bê
‘path’
‘thorn’
ba᷅
bâ
HM
be᷇
‘flower’
ba᷇
ML
be᷆
‘small eel’
ba᷆
HLM
bé ᷅
‘tree fern’
bá ᷅
(Bateman 1990:
35–36)
totality of action
punctual
resultative durative
totality of action
incompletive
resultative punctual
telic punctual
‘came to get’
‘came to end
point’
‘still not at
telic incompletive
endpoint’
‘come (process)’ totality of action
durative
‘sticking,
telic durative
attached to’
Although the inflectional categories on Iau verbs in (5) lend themselves to a featural, paradigmatic display, the portmanteau tone patterns do not appear to be
segmentable. From the summary table in (6) the only generalizations that can be
extracted are that telic and incompletive both begin H and resultative ends mid:
Morphological tonal assignments in conflict: Who wins?
(6)
Telic
Punctual
Durative
Incompletive
Totality of action
19
Resultative
HL
H
LM
HLM
ML
M
HM
H↑H
In other cases segmenting the tones by morpheme is straightforward, as in
Modo [Central Sudanic; Sudan] (Nougayrol 2006):
(7)
1sg, 2sg, 2pl
/H-/
3sg. 1pl, 3pl
/L-/
/ata, H/ ‘be bitter’
/uba, L/ ‘sing’
átá
H-H
‘you are bitter’
úbà
H-L
‘you sing’
àtá
L-H
‘it is bitter’
ùbà
L-L
‘s/he sings’
As seen, verb roots can be H or L which, when conjugated, can acquire a H- vs.
L- prefix. While the output consists neatly of the four logical combinations in
succession of two tones x two tones (inflection + root), a consistent exponent can
be “subtonal”, consisting of less than a full tone. For example, the four combinations of person + tense features also produce a four-way distinction in Gban
[Mande; Ivory Coast] (Zheltov 2005: 24):
(8)
Present
1st pers.
2nd pers.
rd
3 pers.
Past
(1 = lowest tone, 4 = highest tone)
sg
pl
sg
pl
ɪ᷉2
u2
ɪ᷉4
u4
ɛɛ2
aa2
ɛɛ4
aa4
ɛ1
ɔ1
ɛ3
ɔ3
[–raised]
[+upper]
[–upper]
[+raised]
As seen, tone differs consistently between 1st/2nd vs. 3rd person, the latter being
one step lower. In addition, all tones are two steps higher in the past than they are
in the present. In (8) I have arbitrarily represented the person features as [±upper]
and the tense features as [±raised], although they could have been reversed.
In addition to the above concatenativity, tone (and other prosodic features)
show the familiar sensitivity to internal morphological structure. Thus consider
20
Larry M. Hyman
the “tonal layers” [strata] which Andersen (1992–4: 61) reports for Dinka [Western
Nilotic; Sudan], which is monosyllabic, but polymorphemic:
(9) wé̤ec ‘kick it hither!’ [kick.centripetal.2sg]
voice
length
inflectional layer
(2sg)
–
–
derivational layer (CP)
[+breathy]
+1
root layer
(‘kick’)
–
1
tone
H
L
HL
In derivational terms, the root /wêc/ ‘kick’ acquires breathiness, vowel length,
and L tone to become intermediate wè̤ec when undergoing the centripetal
(‘hither’) derivation. The L is then replaced by H to realize the 2nd person singular subject of the imperative. As Andersen puts it, “The morphological layers are
simultaneous but ‘vertically’ ordered, with the root as the ‘deepest’ layer, optionally followed by the derivational layer, followed by an inflectional layer.” This
results in the above “cyclic” effects. (For more on cyclicity in tonal phonology and
morphology, see Pulleyblank 1985, 1986.)
In a quite different kind of system, polysyllabic Chichewa [Bantu; Malawi],
verb stems can be toneless or can have a single H on either their final or penultimate syllable (Kanerva 1989, Mtenje 1987, among others). With some dialect differences, H tones are assigned as follows (Hyman & Mtenje 1999: 98–99):
(10) a. Final H tone is assigned by
(i) verb roots with a lexical /H/
(ii) certain derivational suffixes such as /-its-/ ‘intensive’, /-ik-/ ‘stative’,
/-uk-/ ‘reversive intr.’ and (in Nkhotakota dialect) passive /-idw-/
(iii) the subjunctive final vowel /-é/ (= the only case of a TAM conditioning
final H)
(iv) non-reflexive object prefixes (in Nkhotakota dialect).
b. Penultimate H tone is assigned by
(i) some affirmative tenses
(ii) most negative tenses
(iii) the reflexive prefix /-dzí-/
(iv) object prefixes in Ntcheu dialect (vs. final H in Nkhotakota)
c. In the absence of one of the above conditioning factors the stem will be
toneless.
Morphological tonal assignments in conflict: Who wins?
21
As seen, final H is mostly lexical and derivational (with the exceptions of subjunctive -é and object prefixes in Nkhotakota dialect), while penultimate H is
inflectional, marking TAM, negation, the relative prefix, and object prefixes in
Ntcheu dialect.
In cases where the more than one morpheme contributes the same tonal
assignment, only one H is realized. Thus, although the following example from
Nkhotakota dialect (Sam Mchombo, pers. comm.) has four sponsors of final H
tone, only one final H actually materializes (Hyman & Mtenje 1999: 101):
(11) [ ti- [ [ [ [ pez- ] -etsets- ] -edw- ] -e ] ]
H
H
H
H
‘let’s be found a lot’
→ ti-pez-etsets-edw-é ...
1pl-find-intens-pass-sbjv
In general, in cases of conflict, penultimate H overrides final H – but with one
crucial exception: subjunctive final -é overrides reflexive/object prefix penultimate, presumably because of scope considerations. While it may seem that all Hs
may be assigned by rule, Hyman & Mtenje point to evidence that at least some Hs
need to be underlyingly linked to their sponsor – but still follow instructions as
to where to go (to the final or penultimate syllable). In (12a), the object prefix /H/
shifts to the penult (Ntecheu dialect). (The remote past tense marker is /-naa-/
to which the H of the subject prefix /tí-/ spreads. The phrase-penultimate vowel
lengthens by general rule.)
(12) a. /tí-naa-mú-fotokozer-a/ → tí-naa-fotokozéer-a
‘we explained to him’
|
|
|
|
Hi
Hj
Hi
Hj
b. /ti-ná-mú-fotokozer-a/ → ti-ná-mú-fotokozeer-a ‘we explained to him’
| |
|
H H→Ø
H
However, whenever an object prefix is preceded by a H tone morpheme, there
is no penultimate H tone. This is seen in (12b), where the general past prefix is
/-ná-/. As shown, the /H/ of the /-mú-/ is deleted by what is commonly known as
Meeussen’s Rule in Bantu: the second of two Hs in succession is deleted. Since
it would be odd to have a rule that said “object prefixes assign a H to the penult
unless they are preceded by a H”, I follow the earlier account with underlying
sequences of Hs as in (12b).
Finally, note that there no rules in Chichewa which shift a pre-existing final
H to the penult: all penultimate assignment rules insert a /H/ at the same time.
For this reason one cannot assume that the penultimate pattern simply applies
22
Larry M. Hyman
an instruction to mark off the last syllable as extrametrical. Hyman & Mtenje’s
(1999: 102) proposal is that final H is assigned at the stem domain, while penultimate H is assigned within a larger “macro-stem” domain. The “later” macro-stem
domain thus overrides the earlier stem domain. The major exception is subjunctive -é, which is stem-level despite its greater scope.
Other cases in the literature show that the “uppermost” morphological structure wins. Thus, Inkelas (2011: 75) provides the following word tree structure from
Hausa [Chadic; Nigeria]:
(13)
nèn-nè:mó: ‘seekrepeatedly!’
nén-né:mó:
i.e. IMPERATIVE >>
VENTIVE
>> base
né:mó:
CVCPLURACTIONAL
nè:má: (LH)
‘seek’
-ó: (H)
-Ø (LH)
VENTIVE
IMPERATIVE
As seen, there is an override system of imperative >> ventive >> base not unlike
the Dinka example: inflectional tone overrides derivational tone which in turn
overrides base tone.
In addition to “layers” (cycles, strata, domains) based on derivational vs.
inflectional morphology, paradigmatic conflicts may require a hierarchical
ranking of the tonal spell-outs by inflectional features (tense, aspect, mood,
negation). A case of this arises in Leggbó [Cross River; Nigeria] (Hyman, Narrog,
Paster & Udoh 2002: 407). In the following table, the first indicated tone goes on
the root and the second on a suffix (if present). (MCA = main clause affirmative;
SRA = subject relative affirmative clause; ORA = object relative affirmative clause;
NEG = negative (all clause types); “irrealis” = future/conditional.)
(14) a.
Root tone:
MCA/ORA
/L/ /M/
SRA
/L/ /M/
NEG
/L/ /M/
Perf/Prog H-M M-M L-M M-M H-M M-M
Habitual
L-L
M-L
L-L
M-L H-M M-M
Irrealis
L-L
M-L
L-L
M-L
L-L
(Irrealis assigns L-L /
M-L M-L)
Morphological tonal assignments in conflict: Who wins?
b.
Root tone:
MCA/ORA
/L/ /M/
SRA
/L/ /M/
23
NEG
/L/ /M/
Perf/Prog H-M M-M L-M M-M H-M M-M (other than irrealis,
negatives assign H-M /
Habitual L-L M-L L-L M-L H-M M-M
M-M)
Irrealis
L-L M-L
c.
Root tone:
MCA/ORA
/L/ /M/
SRA
/L/ /M/
Perf/Prog H-M M-M L-M M-M
Habitual
Irrealis
d.
Root tone:
L-L
NEG
/L/ /M/
M-L
L-L
L-L
M-L
H-M M-M (other than irrealis
and negatives,
habituals assign L-L /
M-L)
SRA
/L/ /M/
NEG
/L/ /M/
MCA/ORA
/L/ /M/
M-L
Perf/Prog H-M M-M L-M M-M H-M M-M (unshaded leftover
cells require specific
Habitual
L-L M-L
tone assignments)
Irrealis
L-L M-L
Although Leggbó noun roots lexically contrast H, M and L tone there is only a
binary contrast on verb roots: M tone roots vs. roots which alternate between H
and L. In (14) I have represented the contrast as one between /L/ and /M/. In
looking over such tabular arrays, the strategy is to start with tone assignments
that affect an entire row or column. We see first see that no column has the same
tonal pattern throughout. However, in (14a) there is one row that is consistent:
the irrealis assigns a L tone suffix deriving L-L and M-L patterns independent
of clause type or negation. Once we take this predictable assignment out of the
equation we see in (14b) that the next generalization is that negation assigns H-M
to L roots and M-M to M roots. In (14c) we can now see that the habitual aspect
assigns L-L/M-L to cells unclaimed by the irrealis or negation. This leaves a few
leftover cells in (14d) where the lexical L vs. M root tones are realized with a M
suffix. By following this procedure Hyman et al (2002) were able to establish the
following ordered hierarchy, where earlier assignments block later tonal assignments:
(15) Irrealis
L-L/M-L
>>
Negative
H-M/M-M
>>
Habitual
L-L/M-L
>>
Other
24
Larry M. Hyman
Hyman & Olawsky (2004: 107) follow the same procedure in analyzing verb
tones in Dagbani [Gur; Ghana], shown in the tables in (16). (Incipient = ‘about to’;
the (H) or (LH) in parentheses were not elicited, but is extrapolated.)
(16) a.
MCA
MCN
RCA
RCN
Prf Imprf Prf Imprf Prf Imprf Prf Imprf
Prs
LH
LH
lex
LH
lex
LH
lex
LH
Rec.Pst
LH
LH
lex
LH
lex
LH
lex
LH
Gen.Pst
LH
LH
lex
LH
lex
LH
lex
LH
Incipient
lex
LH
lex
LH
lex
LH
lex
(LH)
Future
H
H
H
H
(H)
(H)
H
H
b.
MCA
MCN
RCA
RCN
Prf Imprf Prf Imprf Prf Imprf Prf Imprf
Prs
LH
LH
lex
LH
lex
LH
lex
LH
Rec.Pst
LH
LH
lex
LH
lex
LH
lex
LH
Gen.Pst
LH
LH
lex
LH
lex
LH
lex
LH
Incipient
lex
LH
lex
LH
lex
LH
lex
LH
Future
c.
H
MCA
MCN
RCA
RCN
Prf Imprf Prf Imprf Prf Imprf Prf Imprf
Prs
LH
Rec.Pst
LH
Gen.Pst
LH
lex
lex
lex
Incipient
lex
lex
lex
lex
Future
lex
LH
lex
lex
lex
LH
H
lex
LH
lex
LH
25
Morphological tonal assignments in conflict: Who wins?
d.
MCA
MCN
RCA
RCN
Prf Imprf Prf Imprf Prf Imprf Prf Imprf
Prs
lex
Rec.Pst
LH
Gen.Pst
Incipient
lex
LH
lex
lex
lex
LH
lex
LH
lex
lex
lex
lex
lex
lex
lex
Future
LH
H
In (16a) we observe that the future assigns the same H tone to all verbs in all contexts. Once the future is removed, in (16b) we see that the imperfective assigns
LH tone to all remaining verbs. In (16c) we assign LH tone to main clause affirmative perfectives – with the exception of the incipient. At this point, in (16d), the
remaining perfectives (non-MCA and incipient) receive their lexical H or L tone as
a kind of default. (It is interesting to note that the underlying verb root tones are
distinguishable only in these contexts.) We can in fact combine (16b,c) to arrive
at the following ranking:
(17) Future
H
>> {MCA [-Incip], [Imperfective]}
LH
>> Lexical (default)
H vs. L
It can be noted that in both Leggbó and Dagbani, future tense ~ irrealis mood
are ranked higher than aspect, as per Bybee’s (1985) Relevance Hierarchy. We
should think of such disjunctive tone patterns as vying for the same “slot” exactly
as Anderson (1986) discussed some time ago for Georgian prefixes. With this in
mind we now return to a more complicated case from Haya, which was briefly
displayed in (4).
3 Haya verb stem tonology
Recall the Mambay and Haya cases in (3) and (4), neither of which recognized
hierarchies as in Leggbó and Dagbani. In Mambay, Anonby (2011) sets up a table
of “tone classes” to summarize the paradigmatic tones across different TAMs,
while Hyman & Byarushengo (1984) present a number of morphological rules
assigning tones by specific combinations of inflectional features. At the time I
considered Haya to be relatively unruly, defying generalization. I now return after
three decades to see if we can do better. In what follows I am concerned only with
26
Larry M. Hyman
the assignment of H tones within the verb stem (root + suffix(es)), not with the
prefixal domain.
As seen in (18), Haya verb stems may have one of four tone patterns with at
most one H tone, predictable from whether the root is underlying /H/ or toneless
and whether there is a suffixal H or not:
(18)
root tone + suffix tone output H
a.
/H/
/H/
on final vowel
suffix H is assigned to FV; root
H is deleted
b.
/Ø/
/H/
on second mora suffix H is realized on second
mora of stem
c.
/H/
/Ø/
on first mora
root realizes its /H/ on its first
mora
d.
/Ø/
/Ø/
none
no stem H
As summarized to the right, a single H may be realized on the final vowel, the
second mora, or the first mora, depending on the input tones. In the one case
where the root is toneless and there is no suffix H, the verb stem will be toneless.
Examples are given in (19), where the tones are indicated as they appear before
the application of postlexical tone rules (Hyman & Byarushengo 1984: 60):
(19)
root + sfx output underlying
output of lexical phonology
H
a. /H/ /H/ on FV (ba-) /kom-il-e/
→ (ba-) kom-il-é ‘they tied up’
H
H
H (Pst-2)
b. /Ø/ /H/ on μ2 (ba-) /jun-il-e/
→ (ba-) jun-íl-e
‘they helped’
H
H
(Pst-2)
c. /H/ /Ø/ on μ1 (ba-aa-) /kom-il-e/ → (b-áa-) kóm-il-e ‘they have
H
H
H
tied up’ (Prf)
d. /Ø/ /Ø/
Ø
(ba-aa-) /jun-il-e/
(b-áa-) jun-il-e ‘they have
H
helped’ (Prf)
The main clause affirmative forms in (19a,b) are in the yesterday past tense (Past2)
which requires a suffixal H, while those in (19c,d) are in the perfect (Perf), which
does not assign a H. In fact, such suffix tones are assigned by the morphology
in one of three ways: (i) a suffixal H is assigned to the FV, e.g. the Past2 tense in
(19a,b); (ii) no suffixal H is assigned, e.g. the Perfect tense-aspect in (19c,d); (iii)
Morphological tonal assignments in conflict: Who wins?
27
a suffixal H is assigned to the FV only if the root is toneless. This is the case in the
past habitual (PH) forms in (20).
(20)
root + sfx output underlying
output of lexical phonology
H
a. /H/ /-Ø/ on μ1 (ba-a-) /kom-ag-a/ → (ba-a-) kóm-ag-a ‘they used to
H
H
tie up’ (PH)
‘they used to
b. /Ø/ /-H/ on μ2 (ba-a-) /jun-ag-a/ → (ba-) jun-ág-a
Ø
H
H
help’ (PH)
In (20a) there is no suffixal H tone, since the root /-kóm-/ ‘tie up’ has H tone. In
(20b), however, there is a suffix H, since /-jun-/ ‘help’ is toneless. This H is realized on the second mora of the verb stem, as expected. Hyman and Byarushengo
refer to this as the “polar H” suffix, since it is present if the root is Ø, but absent
if the root is H. One might propose that the H suffix is assigned to all verb forms
in such tenses, but that it is subsequently deleted after a H root. The same Meeussen’s Rule applying in (12b) in Chichewa is independently required in Haya to
delete the second of two Hs are on adjacent moras, so one might first shift the
suffixal H to the second mora and then delete it after a H root initial mora. Since
the two types of H suffix would still have to be distinguished (those which would
shift even after a H root vs. those which don’t), I will instead assume that there
are two different H assignment rules, one of which is sensitive to whether the root
has a H tone or not.
With this established we now can consider which combinations of inflectional features and clause types require which finals. In the following table, H
indicates that a suffix H is assigned when the root is either Ø or /H/, while (H)
indicates the polar suffix which is assigned only when the root is Ø. Those cells
which do not take either suffixal H are indicated by Ø:
(21)
Pres Hab
Past1
Past2
Past3
Past Hab
Future1
MCA
SRA
ORA
MCN
SRN
ORN
Focus
H
Ø
H
(H)
(H)
(H)
–––+++
Ø
Ø
H
Ø
Ø
Ø
–––+++
H
Ø
H
H
H
H
–––+++
Ø
(H)
(H)
Ø
Ø
Ø
+––+++
(H)
(H)
(H)
Ø
Ø
Ø
–––+++
(H)
(H)
(H)
(H)
(H)
(H)
–––+++
28
Larry M. Hyman
(21)
MCA
SRA
ORA
MCN
SRN
ORN
Focus
Future2
H
Ø
H
Ø
Ø
Ø
–––+++
Progressive
H
Ø
Ø
Ø
Ø
Ø
+++++
Perfect
Ø
Ø
Ø
Ø
Ø
Ø
+++++
Inceptive
Ø
Ø
Ø
Ø
Ø
Ø
+++++
Persistive
Ø
Ø
Ø
Ø
Ø
Ø
+++++
Subjunctive
Imperative
(H)
(H)
++
H
(= subjunctive)
++
(MCA = main clause affirmative; SRA = subject relative affirmative; ORA =
object relative affirmative; MCN = main clause negative; SRN = subject relative negative; ORN = object relative negative.)
As seen, Haya distinguishes three degrees of past tense, and two degrees of future
tense. The inceptive is translated as ‘have done X before’, while the persistive
translates as ‘still do X’. (I address the last Focus column below.) As in the Leggbó
and Dagbani cases, I have begun by shading in the rows that have the same tone
assignment throughout. As seen, the Future-1 assigns a H suffix tone throughout,
despite the fact that the segmental morphology may differ between main vs. relative and affirmative vs. negative clauses. In the following examples it is observed
that Future-1 takes the marker /-la-V-/ in the affirmative, but only the empty mora
/-V-/ in the negative (-V- is realized by lengthening of the preceding vowel, i.e. [a]
in these examples):
(22) a. AFF (ba-la-a-) kom-a vs. (ba-la-a-) /jun-a/
H
H
b. NEG (ti-ba-a-) kom-e vs. (ti-ba-a-) /jun-e/
H
H
‘they will tie up/help’
(Fut-1)
‘they will not tie up/help
(Fut-1)
Note that the FVs are also different in the affirmative vs. negative, and yet the
polar suffixal H remains constant. In addition, a number of TAMs fail to have a
suffix H, which I have also shaded. One other important generalization is that
although the segmental morphology can vary, any given tense has the same negative tones, whether in the MCN, SRN, or ORN. I shall therefore conflate the last
three columns as NEG in what follows.
In (23) I reorganize the material in the earlier table by their affirmative tone
patterns to reveal that there are in fact three TAM “tone classes”, with affirmative
Morphological tonal assignments in conflict: Who wins?
29
TAM residues marked by an asterisk (I will deal with each of these separately
below):
(23)
MCA SRA ORA NEG Focus
comments:
1a Past2
H
Ø
H
1b Present Habitual
H
Ø
H
1c Future2
H
Ø
H
1d Past1
Ø*
Ø
H
2a Future1
(H)
(H)
(H)
2b Past Habitual
(H)
(H)
(H)
Ø
–––+ all have same segmental TAM marking
2c Past3
Ø*
(H)
(H)
Ø
+––+ MCA has different
segmental marking
3a Perf, Incep,
Persist
Ø
Ø
Ø
Ø
++++ all Ø; Prf, Incep
change marking
3b Progressive
H*
Ø
Ø
Ø
++++ MCA has different
segmental marking
H
–––+ none of these change
their TAM segmental
(H) –––+
marking in neg (or
Ø –––+ affirm)
Ø
±––+ MCA has different
segmental marking
(H) –––+ (H) throughout, -laprefix only in AFF
#H =
4
Ø
4
1
=9
# (H) =
2
3
3
2
= 10
#Ø =
5
8
4
8
= 25
(The most common = Ø, hence Hyman &
Byarushengo propose rules assigning Hs)
Since there are three possible suffix tone assignments (H, (H), Ø) and three affirmative clause types (MCA, SRC, ORA), there are in principle 3 × 3 = nine possible
tone assignments. Instead, there are three general affirmative patterns, with the
asterisked exceptions to be explained below. (There are nine patterns when we
include negatives, not 27.) Class 1 consists of TAMs which have H-Ø-H tonal suffix
assignments in the three affirmative columns. Class 2 consists of TAMs which
have polar (H) tonal suffix assignments throughout. Class 3 consists of those
TAMs which do not receive either type of suffixal H. In other words, we come close
to being able to equate class 1 with -H, class 2 with -(H), and class 3 with -Ø. (The
class 1 SRA forms present an obvious obstacle.) For reference, I have included
30
Larry M. Hyman
comments on the segmental morphology in the last column. Full paradigms of
examples are available in Hyman & Byarushengo (1984: 93–101).
I have yet to discuss the focus column, where [+F] refers to TAMs which resist
H tone deletion when non-final in their clause, while those which reduce their H
tones are marked [–F]. For example in (24a) the present habitual affirmative final
H suffix is deleted when a constituent follows in the same clause:
(24) a. Prs Hab Affirmative is [-F] : /ba-jun-á/ → ba-jun-a káto
‘they help Kato’
b. All negatives are [+F]
: /ti-ba-jun-á/ → ti-ba-jun-á káto
‘they don’t help Kato’
In the corresponding negative in (24b), however, which is built by prefixing ti- to
the affirmative, the same final suffix H does not delete. As indicated, all negatives
are [+F], whereas affirmative TAMs can be [+F] or [–F], as discussed by Hyman &
Watters (1984: 259–262), who argue that the [+F] TAMs are “intrinsically focused”.
The reason for going into this is that there is a further generalization: all tensed
affirmative [+F] are Ø except the MCA progressive, which receives a H suffix. Progressive forms are illlustrated in (25).
(25) a. MCA marked by ni- : /ni-ba-jun-á/
→ ni-ba-jun-á káto
‘they are helping Kato’
b. MCN Prog with -li : /ti-bá-lí-ku-jun-a/ → ti-bá-li-ku-jun-a káto
neg-they-cop-inf-help-fv
‘they aren’t helping Kato’
c. SRA Prog with -li : /á-ba-lí-ku-jun-a/ → a-bá-li-ku-jun-a káto
‘they who are helping Kato’
The reason why the MCA progressive is exceptionally in class 1 is that it is built by
adding the focus marker ni- to the present habitual (cf. ni káto ‘it’s Kato’). While
the progressive is intrinsically [+F] and therefore should have a -Ø final, its H
suffixal tone is a carry-over from the present habitual. The two other asterisked
exceptions can also be accounted for historically (see below).
What the above analysis suggests is that the tone patterns can be assigned by
classes of TAMs, at least as far as the affirmative forms are concerned. The following questions thus naturally arise:
– First, do the affirmative groupings represent natural classes of TAM features?
It does not appear so. Class 1 includes the present habitual, the two more
recent past tenses (Past1, Past2), and the general future tense (Future2). Class
2 is also incoherent: while past habitual and Past3 go together as distant pasts
Morphological tonal assignments in conflict: Who wins?
–
–
31
(they partially share segmental morphology as well), the near future (Future1)
doesn’t. Finally, however, class 3 has been claimed to be coherent: all are
[+F]. (I am limiting my attention to the indicative TAMs, since the imperative
and subjunctive, both [+F], take H and (H), respectively.)
The second question concerns whether one can predict one column from
another. The quick answer is: not across the board. In class 1 the MCA and ORA
are identical, both receiving suffixal H (vs. SRA Ø). Class 2 affirmatives all take
suffixal H, with the MCA Past3 being exception (see below). We have already
seen that all class 3 TAMs are Ø except for the MCA progressive explained above.
A third question is what the significance is of the near future (F1) always
receiving a polar (H) suffixal tone? (There is undoubtedly an historical explanation involving the source of the empty -V- mora alluded to above.)
What remains to be explained is why the MCA Past1 and Past3 are exceptional.
The Past1 should be H, but is exceptionally Ø. The reason for this is seen in (26).
(26) a. y-áa-kóm-a
‘he tied up’
[+F] (= “disjoint” form)
b. y-a-kom-a káto ‘he tied up Kato’ [–F] (= “conjoint” form)
As seen, when the verb is phrase-final, the tense marker is -áa-. When it is followed by any word within the same clause, as in (26b), not only do the H tones of
the tense marker and verb root reduce, but the tense marker is now -a-, i.e. short.
This is the only tense that does this. The prefixal differences in (26a–b) are in
fact a relic of an older system which Meeussen (1959) termed “disjoint” vs. “conjoint” verb forms: -áa- was the focused (or disjoint) marker of this tense and -athe unfocused (or conjoint) marker, as they are in the recent past in Kirundi. My
hypothesis therefore is that phrase-final Past1 ends Ø rather than H because the
form with -áa- is in fact [+F]. We of course can’t tell this for certain, because the
[±F] distinction has an effect only when something follows the verb. The second
part of the hypothesis is that the non-final form with -a- is not only [–F], as we
know from (26b), but also takes a suffixal H in line with other class 1 TAMs, which
however automatically deletes. In this way Past1 falls into place.
There is also an historical explanation for why Past3 is [+F] only in the MCA.
First, as seen in (27), it is only in the MCA that it is marked with the prefix -ka- and
the FV -a, as in (27a).
(27) a. /bá-ka-jun-a/ ‘they helped’ → bá-ka-jun-a káto ‘they helped Kato’ [+F]
b. /a-ba-a-jun-íl-e/ ‘they who helped’ →
a-ba-a-jun-il-e káto
‘they who helped Kato’ [–F]
32
Larry M. Hyman
In the SRA in (27b) the markers are -a- and the final -il-e, which also occur in
Past3 negative forms. What’s significant is that the prefixal and suffixal segmental allomorphy corresponds with [±F]. This is because the -ka- prefix has a rather
complex history (see Botne 1999 and Nurse 2008 for discussion). In Haya, -ka- has
an interesting complementary distribution in three different contexts: In MCA, it
marks distant past (Past3), as in (27a). In negative clauses it marks the perfect and
incipient, contrasting with the affirmative as in (28a,b).
(28) a. MCA perfect : b-áa-jun-il-e
‘they have helped’
[+F]
b. MCN perfect : ti-bá-ka-jun-il-e ‘they haven’t helped’ [+F]
Its third function is to mark past consecutive clauses following either a negative
or relative clause (‘he didn’t come and see me’, ‘the man who came and saw me’).
To account for the exceptional Ø and [+F] of the MCA Past3 form all we need to say
is that these were features of its originally function (I would guess perfect, since
the perfect tends to be inherently focused in the sense of Hyman & Watters 1984).
As seen, although Haya tonal assignments appear to be somewhat chaotic
on first approach, there are some major regularities, which are obscured in a few
cases by recent changes in the TAM system. What then can we conclude from
this admittedly limited look at three languages? If Leggbó, Dagbani, and Haya are
representative – and I can add that other Bantu languages are often like Haya –
then I would suggest two generalizations: (i) there is a tendency for negatives to
be tonally identical across clause types (MCN, SRN, ORN); (ii) there is a tendency
for a future or irrealis to be tonally identical across clause types and negation.
However, I would not be surprised to find completely contradictory systems, as
TAM systems are relatively unstable and may change in one, but not other clause
types, as we saw in the case of Haya past3.
4 Tonal morphology can do more than non-tonal
morphology can do
In §1 I suggested that tonal morphology often obscures the compartmentalization
of phonology, morphology and syntax. In this final section I would like to demonstrate this, and at the same time show that grammatical tone can do things that
non-tone cannot.
The first example comes from Kikuria [Bantu; Tanzania, Kenya], which
assigns a H tone to one of the first four moras of the verb stem, depending on
Morphological tonal assignments in conflict: Who wins?
33
the tense (Marlo & Mwita 2009: 2). As seen in (29), once this H is assigned to the
underlined mora, the H spreads to the penult:
(29) a.
b.
c.
d.
μ1
μ2
μ3
μ4
n-to-on-to-on-to-reto-ra-
[ hóótóótér-a
[ hoótóótér-a
[ hootóótér-a
[ hootoótér-a
‘we have reassured’
‘we have been reassuring’
‘we will reassure’
‘we are about to reassure’
Past
Past progressive
Future
Inceptive
The interesting question which arises is: What happens if the verb stem is too
short, i.e. doesn’t have enough moras for the intended H tone assignment, e.g. to
the fourth mora? Marlo & Mwita (2009) demonstrate the results as in (30).
(30) a.
b.
c.
d.
μ4
μ4
μ4
μ4
to-rato-rato-rato-ra-
[ karaaŋg-á
[ sukur-ǎ
[ βun-a°
[ ry-a°
‘we are about to fry’
‘we are about to rub’
‘we are about to break’
‘we are about to eat’
H tone assignment:
[ sukur-a μ́
[ βun-a μ μ́
[ ry-a μ μ μ́
In (30a) the verb stem has four moras and the H therefore is assigned to the FV.
In (30b), where the stem is one mora short, a rising tone is obtained. When the
stem is either two or three moras short as in (30c,d), there is a level L° tone, as
if the H tone is floating after the verb, keeping the L tone from downgliding, as
a prepausal L would normally do. What is extremely interesting is that when the
verb is non-final, the mora count continues onto the next word. Marlo & Mwita
show this with the toneless noun object eɣetɔɔkɛ ‘banana’:
(31) a.
b.
c.
d.
μ4
μ4
μ4
μ4
to-rato-rato-rato-ra-
[ karaaŋg-á
[ sukur-a
[ βun-a
[ ry-a
éɣétɔ́ɔ́kɛ
éɣétɔ́ɔ́kɛ
eɣétɔ́ɔ́kɛ
eɣetɔ́ɔ́kɛ
‘we are about to fry a banana’
‘we are about to rub a banana’
‘we are about to break a banana’
‘we are about to eat a banana’
Again, the H is assigned to the FV in (31), since the verb stem has four moras. The
H continues to spread to the penult of the noun object. In (31b) it is assigned to
the first mora of the noun and then again spreads to the penult. In (31c) the H is
assigned to the second mora of the noun, and then spreads. Finally, in (31d), the H
is assigned to the third mora of the noun and spreads just one mora to the penult.
Such an array of tone assignments is already quite remarkable (particularly to the
fourth mora). However, what is really unusual is that this suffixal H is expected to
be stem- or perhaps word-level morphology (although the prefixes are irrelevant),
as in other Bantu languages, but is calculated at the phrase level! There seems to
be a violation of a basic principle, a violation of what we might think of as canoni-
34
Larry M. Hyman
cal morphology (Corbett 2007): Morphs should stay on their own word! Instead,
we have something that seems like co-phonologies operating at the phrase level.
(For co-phonologies, see Inkelas 2011 and references cited therein.)
A second such violation occurs in the rather restricted tone system of Chimwiini [Bantu; Somalia], which has the following properties (Kisseberth 2009): (i)
Tone is only grammatical. There are no tonal contrasts on lexical morphemes, e.g.
noun stems or verb roots. (ii) Privative H tone is limited to the last two syllables:
final H vs. penultimate H. As an example consider the paradigm in (32).
(32)
1st pers.
} final H:
2nd pers.
3rd pers. penult H:
singular
n- ji:lé ‘I ate’
ji:lé ‘you (sg) ate’
≠ {
jí:le ‘s/he ate’
plural
chi- chi-ji:lé ‘we ate’
ni- ni-ji:lé ‘you pl. ate’
wa- wa-jí:le ‘they ate’
As seen, first and second person subjects condition final H tone in the past tense,
while third person subjects condition penultimate H tone. As also seen in the
above table, the only difference between second and third person singular is
tonal. It is clear that tone has a morphological function in the above examples.
But the plot thickens when an object is added, as in (33).
(33) a. jile: n̪amá
b. jile: n̪áma
‘you (sg) ate meat’
‘s/he ate meat’
jile ma-tu:ndá
jile ma-tú:nda
‘you (sg) ate fruit’
‘s/he ate fruit’
We now see that the final vs. penultimate distinction is realized on the noun
object. The tonal morphology is thus phrasal. Kisseberth (2009) also shows that
phrasal domains can be nested, depending on information structure, with each
right edge receiving the appropriate final or penultimate H:
(34) a. Ø-wa-t̪ind̪il̪il̪e w-a:ná ] n̪amá ] ka: chi-sú ]
‘you (sg)cut for the children meat with a knife’
b. Ø-wa-t̪ind̪il̪il̪ew-á:na ] n̪áma ] ka: chí-su ]
‘s/he cut for the children meat with a knife’
What we have here is a case of tone being able to have long distance effects: If
the H had been a lexical property, say, of the verb root, as it can be in Digo (Kisseberth 1984) or Giryama (Volk 2011), we would treat this as pure phonology. Given
however that the tonal distinctions are exclusively grammatical, conditioned by
specific grammatical morphemes or constructions, we have to address the question of what exactly this is. If tone is a phrasal clitic, it’s quite unlike English pos-
Morphological tonal assignments in conflict: Who wins?
35
sessive ’s, which serves an appropriate grammatical function at the end of a noun
phrase. Should the above tonal distinctions be identified with:
(35) a. morphology? = a property of [1st/2nd pers.] vs. [3rd pers.] subject prefixes
b. phonology? = a property of the phonological phrase – H is semidemarcative)
c. syntax?
= a property of the syntactic configurations which define
the P-phrases
c. intonation? = not likely – who ever heard of a 1st/2nd vs. 3rd person
intonation?
An anonymous reviewer suggests that the 1st/2nd vs. 3rd person subject tone difference is not “different” from what can be expressed segmentally: “In my opinion,
the Chimwiini example on which the demonstration is based calls for a syntactic
analysis, and can be satisfactorily analyzed as illustrating an uncommon type
of agreement, with the additional complication that the agreement markers are
not segments, but morphotonemes.” The problem is that Kisseberth & Abashekh
(2011) report several other grammatical morphemes and constructions that also
assign a final H tone. As Kisseberth & Abasheikh crucially point out, “The default
accent is penult. Final accent occurs only in certain morphological or syntactic
structures” (p.1990). One of these is the common conjunction na ‘and’ which
Kisseberth shows to assign final H tones phrasally, just like first and second
person subjects. Thus, in (36) the conjunction na assigns a final H on the noun
muu-nthu ‘man’ (Kisseberth & Abasheikh 2011: 1995):
(36) mw-éendo na sifa
z-a muu-nthú
behavior and characteristics of man
‘the behavior and characteristics of a man’
It is hardly likely that na ‘and’ would condition “an uncommon agreement” on
the last word of its phonological phrase.
As seen, both the Kikuria and Chimwiini cases do a good job of obscuring the
boundaries between morphemes, words and phrases, and ultimately phonology,
morphology, and syntax. Both are effective examples of how tone can function
as the glue holding a grammar together. This brings us to the last question: What
else can do this other than tone? What can be sponsored by a morpheme in one
word but travel at considerable distance to be realized on another? There are more
such cases, in fact (see Harry & Hyman 2012 for a preliminary survey). Speakers of
languages seem to be better equipped to package and exploit melodies and other
syntagmatic properties of pitch at both the word and phrase level than any other
36
Larry M. Hyman
phonological property. It seems superfluous at this time, but I can’t help repeating: Tone is different! (Hyman 2011)
Acknowledgments
This paper was originally presented at the workshop “Tons et Paradigmes Flexionnels: Modélisation et Parcimonie/Disentangling the Inflectional Role of
Tone”, Maison de la Recherche, Université de Paris 3, June 17–18, 2013. I am grateful to the organizers and participants at the workshop for their helpful comments
as well as to the editors and two anonymous reviewers. I am especially grateful
to Charles Kisseberth for his collegiality in sharing his enormous knowledge and
insights into Chimwiini phonology.
References
Andersen, Torben. 1992–1994. Morphological stratification in Dinka: on the alternations of
voice quality, vowel length, and tone in the morphology of transitive verbal roots in a
monosyllabic language. Studies in African Linguistics 23: 1–63.
Anderson, Stephen R. 1986. Disjunctive ordering in inflecitonal morphology. Natural Language
and Linguistic Theory 4: 1–31.
Anonby, Erik J. 2011. A grammar of Mambay: An Adamawa language of Chad and Cameroon.
Köln: Rüdiger Köppe Verlag.
Bateman, Janet. 1990. Iau segmental and tonal phonology. Miscellaneous Studies of Indonesian
and Other Languages in Indonesia 10: 29–42.
Bennett, Patrick R. 1974. Tone and the Nilotic case system. Bulletin of the School of African and
Oriental Studies 37: 19–28.
Botne, Robert. 1999. Future and distal -ka-’s: Proto-Bantu or nascent form(s)? In Jean-Marie
Hombert & Larry M. Hyman (eds), Bantu historical linguistics: Theoretical and empircal
perspectives, 473–515. Stanford: C.S.L.I.
Broadwell, George Aaron. 2000. Macuiltianguis Zapotec tone paradigms. Ms. SUNY Albany.
Bybee, Joan L. 1985. Morphology. A study of the relation between meaning and form.
Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Corbett, Greville G. 2007. Canonical typology, suppletion, and possible words. Language 83:
8–42.
Harry, Otelemate and Larry M. Hyman. 2012. Construction Tonology: The Case of Kalabari. UC
Berkeley presentation February 27. Revised and expanded version submitted.
Hyman, Larry M. 2011. Tone: Is it different? In The Handbook of Phonological Theory (2nd
edition) 197–239. Blackwell.
Hyman, Larry M. 2012. Issues in the phonology-morphology interfacein African languages. To
appear in the proceedings of the 43rd Annual Conference on African Languages, Tulane
University, March 15–17, 2012. Cascadilla Proceedings Project.
Morphological tonal assignments in conflict: Who wins?
37
Hyman, Larry M. and Ernest Rugwa Byarushengo. 1984. A model of Haya tonology. In G. N.
Clements & John Goldsmith (eds), Autosegmental studies in Bantu tone, 53–103.
Dordrecht: Foris Publications.
Hyman, Larry M. and William R. Leben. 2000. Suprasegmental processes. In Handbook on
Inflection and Word Formation, 587–594. Berlin: De Gruyter.
Hyman, Larry M. and Al Mtenje. 1999. Prosodic morphology and tone: the case of Chichewa.
In Harry van der Hulst, René Kager & Wim Zonneveld, eds., The prosody-morphology
interface, 90–133. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Hyman, Larry M., Heiko Narrog, Mary Paster, and Imelda Udoh. 2002. Leggbó verb inflection:
A semantic and phonological particle analysis. Proceedings of the 28th Annual Berkeley
Linguistic Society Meeting, 399–410.
Hyman, Larry M. and Knut Olawsky. 2004. Dagbani verb tonology. In Trends in African
Linguistics 4: 97–108. Africa World Press, Inc.
Hyman, Larry M. and John R. Watters. 1984. Auxiliary focus. Studies in African Linguistics 15:
233–273.
Inkelas, Sharon. 2011. The interaction between morphology and phonology. In Handbook of
Phonological Theory, 2nd edition, 68–102. Blackwell.
Kanerva, Jonni. 1989. Focus and phrasing in Chichewa phonology. Doctoral dissertation,
Stanford University.
Kisseberth, Charles W. 1984. Digo tonology. In G. N. Clements & John Goldsmith (eds), Autosegmental studies in Bantu tone, 105–182. Dordrecht: Foris Publications.
Kisseberth, Charles W. 2009. The theory of prosodic phrasing: the Chimwiini evidence. ACAL
40, Urbana-Champaign, April 9–11, 2009.
Kisseberth, Charles W. and Mohammad Imam Abasheikh. 2011. Chimwiini phonological
phrasing revisited. Lingua 121: 1987–2013.
Marlo, Michael & Chacha Mwita. 2009. Problems in Kuria H tone assignment. Ms. Indiana
University & Kenyatta University.
Meeussen, A. E. 1959. Essai de grammaire rundi. Tervuren: Musée Royal de l’Afrique Centrale.
Mtenje, Al. 1987. Tone shift principles in the Chichewa verb: a case for a tone lexicon. Lingua
72: 169–209.
Nougayrol, Pierre. 2006. Tones and verb classes in Bongo. In Al-Amin Abu-Manga, Leoma
Gilley, & Anne Storch (eds), Insights into Nilo-Saharan Language, History and Culture,
335–345. Köln: Rüdiger KöppeVerlag.
Nurse, Derek. 2008. Tense and aspect in Bantu. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Payne, Doris. 2008. Maasai Sentences (Syntax). MS at http://darkwing.uoregon.edu/~maasai/
Maa%20Language/masyntax.htm
Plank, Frans. 1995. (Re-)introducing Suffixaufnahme. In Frans Plank (ed.), Double case:
agreement by Suffixaufnahme, 3–110. Oxford University Press.
Pulleyblank, Douglas. 1985. A Lexical Treatment of Tone in Tiv. in D.L. Goyvaerts (ed.) African
Linguistics, 421–476. Benjamins.
Pulleyblank, Douglas. 1986. Tone in lexical phonology. Dordrecht: Kluwer.
Tucker, Archibald Norman and John Tompo Ole Mpaayei. 1955. A Maasai Grammar with
Vocabulary. London: Longmans, Green and Company.
Volk, Erez. 2011. Mijikenda tonology. Doctoral dissertation, Tel Aviv University.
Zheltov, Alexander. 2005. Le système des marqueurs de personnes en gban. Mandenkan 41:
23–28.
38
Larry M. Hyman
Appendix
Since the Paris 2013 workshop “Tons et paradigmes flexionnels : modélisation
et parcimonie/Disentangling the inflectional role of tone” was organized by Jean
Léo Léonard and Enrique Palancar, specialists of Mexican tone systems, I decided
to include a few words on an interesting tonal paradigm from Macuiltianguis
Zapotec for which Broadwell (2000) presents evidence for the following (partial)
structure:
VERB
ASPECT
completive Lhabitual Ø- [M]
potential H-
1sg. H
(→ stressed syllable)
ROOT
(base tones)
In the following table, two possible input systems are considered: Broadwell’s /H,
M, L, Ø/ vs. /H, M, Ø/. The stressed syllable is underlined.
The rules that appear to be needed are as follows:
1. 1sg. H is assigned to the stressed syllable. If the latter is lexically H, the rule is
blocked.
2. Aspectual H- or L- is assigned to first syllable, overriding /Ø/. Potential Hforms HL contour with lexical L.
3. Phonology: HL-Ø → HL-L (H-L if first vowel is short).
4. Shaded = not predicted by my rules:
(i) /Ø-H/ 3sg. potential should be H-H (H→ L after H?)
(ii) /LH/ 1sg. potential should be HL if 1sg. is spelled out first
(iii) 3rd pers. /L/ potential should be HL. Avoidance of *HLH is general.
Base Tone
(Broadwell)
(without /M/)
Ø-M
Ø-Ø
1st pers
3rd pers
H
Ø
H-M
L-M
H-M
M-M
H-M
H-M
H1:1sg
H1:1sg
H1:1sg
H1:Pot
Ø-M
Ø-Ø
1st pers
3rd pers
H
Ø
L-H
L-M
M-H
M-M
H-M
H-M
H2:1sg
H2:1sg
H1:1sg
H1:Pot
Ø-H
Ø-H
1st pers
3rd pers
H
Ø
H-H
L-H
H-H
M-H
H-H
H-H
H1:1sg
H1:1sg
H1:1sg
H1:Pot
Ø-H
Ø-H
1st pers
3rd pers
H
Ø
L-H
L-H
M-H
M-H
H-H
H-L
Ø-L
Ø-L
1st pers
3rd pers
H
Ø
H-L
L-L
H-L
M-L
H-L
H-L
H1:1sg
H1:1sg
H1:1sg
H1:Pot
L-M
L-Ø
1st pers
3rd pers
H
Ø
HL-L
L-M
HL-L
L-M
HL-L
HL-L
H1:1sg
H1:1sg
H1:1sg
H1:Pot
L-H
L-H
1st pers
3rd pers
H
Ø
HL-L
L-H
HL-L
L-H
HL-L
HL-L
H1:1sg
H1:1sg
H1:1sg
H1:Pot
L-H
L-H
1st pers
3rd pers
H
Ø
L-H
L-H
L-H
L-H
HL-H
HL-H
LH
LH
1st pers
3rd pers
H
Ø
HL
LH
HL
LH
LH
LH
H1:1sg
H1:1sg
L
L
1st pers
3rd pers
H
Ø
HL
L
HL
L
HL
L
H1:1sg
H1:1sg
H-M
H-Ø
1st pers
3rd pers
H
Ø
H-M
H-M
H-M
H-M
H-M
H-M
HL + H
HL + H
1st pers
3rd pers
H
Ø
HL-H
HL- H
HL- H
HL- H
LH- H
LH- H
H1:Pot
H1:Pot
H1:Pot
H1:Pot
H1:1sg
H1:1sg
H1:Pot
39
Completive L- Habitual Ø- Potential H- assignment of grammatical Hs:
Morphological tonal assignments in conflict: Who wins?
(underline = stress)