Theories Cooperation
Theories Cooperation
Theories Cooperation
Chapter Two
ABSTRACT
Four of the major theoretical orientations to cooperation and competition are
cognitive-development, social-cognitive, behavioural-learning, and social
interdependence theories. Of the four, social interdependence theory has generated the
most research and applications. The basic premise of social interdependence theory is
that the type of interdependence structured in a situation determines how individuals
interact with each other, which, in turn, determines outcomes. The hundreds of studies
that have been conducted indicate that cooperation, compared with competitive and
individualistic efforts, tends to result in higher achievement and productivity, more
positive interpersonal relationships, and greater psychological health. The characteristics
of social interdependency theory include the validating research having high internal and
external validity and the theory being well formulated with clearly defined concepts,
being testable and generating addition research, being dynamic (not static), focusing on
relationship (not intrapersonal) variables, being applicable at multi-levels (e.g.,
intrapersonal, interpersonal), generating both theoretical and demonstration studies, being
cumulative, being profound, being strategic, being powerful, and being the foundation on
which other theories are built. Overall, social interdependence theory is the most useful
theory underlying cooperative learning.
*
* Corresponding Author address
Email: [email protected]
INTRODUCTION
Instructional procedures often have short lives, no matter how effective they seem to be.
To be maintained as an instructional practice over decades and even centuries, the
instructional practice has to be built on a foundation of theory validated by research.
Specifically, the future of cooperative learning depends on its being part of a cycle of theory-
research-practice. There are at least four major theories supporting the use of cooperative
learning (social interdependence, cognitive-developmental, social-cognitive and behavioural
theories). The one most influential in the development and use of cooperative learning is
social interdependence theory. This chapter, therefore, examines the value of social
interdependence theory in developing, validating, and ensuring the future of cooperative
learning. More specifically, this chapter makes the following points.
1. The strength and the promise of cooperative learning lies in the interaction among
theory, research, and practice. There are many theories that never get applied, and
there are many atheoretical practices that are adopted by schools and then
discontinued. In order to be implemented and then institutionalised in schools, an
instructional practice has to derived from a theory validated by research. Cooperative
learning is a clear example of this process. The future of cooperative learning rests
upon it continuing to be built on a foundation of theory.
2. There are three levels of theory: Comprehensive, middle level, and micro level.
While this chapter focuses primarily on middle range theories, micro-theories enrich,
clarify, expand, and enhance the middle range theories.
3. Social interdependence theory has generated the most research and clear operational
procedures that have been applied at all levels of education.
4. There are two types of research studies: Theoretical and atheoretical, demonstration
studies. Both have their place in promoting the use of cooperative learning.
5. Diversity in theorising and in practical procedures is needed to provide a healthy
environment for the continued growth and refinement of cooperative learning. A
wide variety of approaches are needed to provide a creative tension in the evolution
of cooperative learning.
6. This chapter, while mostly focusing on social interdependence theory, is a humble
attempt to summarise the impact of social interdependence theory on the
implementation and institutionalisation of cooperative learning. It is not intended to
imply that social interdependence theory is the only or should be the only theory
underlying cooperative learning.
THEORIES OF COOPERATION
Theory is to practice what the soil is to plants. If the soil is appropriate, the plant will
grow and flourish. If the theory is appropriate, the research and practice will grow and
continuously improve. Without an appropriate theory, research tends to be “mindless
3 Theoretical Approaches to Cooperative Learning
empiricism” and practice tends to be static and stagnant. This chapter focuses on the
theoretical approaches to cooperative learning.
A theory consists of a set of interrelated concepts, definitions, and propositions that
explain or predict events or situations by specifying relations among variables. The word
“theorise” comes from the Greek word “theorein,” which consists of a blend of two words,
“thea” which means to see or observe, and “horan” which means to see a thing attentively or
to contemplate it. Theorising thus consists of observing and studying a phenomenon and then
contemplating and explaining it. Because theories are derived from the current understanding
of a person or group of persons, they are dynamic as they may change according to changes in
the observations and contemplations of the theorists. As situations and environmental
conditions change, theories may change. In addition, theories are limited by the capacity of
humans (or the theorists) to observe accurately and understand correctly the phenomenon
being considered. Since all human brains have inherent limitations, the theories generated by
humans have similar limitations. As human capacity to understand develops, theories also
develop. To an important extent, therefore, all theorising is a reflection of reality perceived by
humans at a certain moment in time and human history.
Without theory the studies on cooperative learning would be isolated data points
unrelated to each other indicating the results of discrete uses of cooperative learning in
specific situations and under specific circumstances. No one study can provide the evidence
that convinces teachers to use cooperative learning, teacher educators to incorporate
cooperative learning into teacher training and inservice training programs, or governments to
mandate the use of cooperative learning in their countries’ schools. To have a widespread and
pervasive impact on education, many studies have to be conducted and organised into a
conceptual network that illuminates what they mean as a whole. The explanatory conceptual
framework that goes beyond the findings of any one study and explains what they all mean
together as a whole, is a theory.
In the social sciences, there are three levels of theorising. First, there are comprehensive
theories that attempt to develop laws of science that hold and apply universally, explaining
the interrelationships among all variables of importance independent of time and space and
historical setting. Early in the 20 th Century there was an attempt to build mega-theories that
explained all or much of human behaviour, such as Lewin’s (1935) field theory, Tolman’s
(1932) purposeful behaviour theory, Hall’s (1935) drive reduction theory, and many others.
Second, there are middle range theories that focus on specific phenomena, yet can
subsume a number of concepts and micro-theories. They are limited in scope and do not
attempt to explain all of human psychology. A classic example is Durkheim's (1897) theory
of suicide, in which he proposed a relationship between the breakdown of social bonds found
in religions (Protestantism vs. Catholicism) as the reason for higher rates of suicide in specific
areas. For cooperative learning, middle-range theories include the four theories discussed in
this chapter (cognitive development theory, social cognitive theory, the group contingencies
aspect of operant conditioning, and social interdependence theory). Social interdependence
theory in particular is a middle-range theory. It deals with a broad range of phenomena (as
reflected in the number of dependent variables studied), serves as a foundation for other
theories, and subsumes many of the micro-theories into a unifying framework (see Johnson,
2003; Johnson & Johnson, 2005a, 2009a). Positive interdependence, for example, may
subsume group reinforcement, entitativity (the perception that a group is a unified and
coherent whole in which the members are bonded together, see Campbell, 1958), role theory,
4 David W. Johnson and Roger T. Johnson
divisions of labour, individual accountability (i.e., responsibility forces), and many other
phenomena.
Third, there are micro-level theories that explain one specific phenomenon. Since the
1950s there has been a trend in social psychology for researchers to formulate micro theories
to explain a single phenomenon, usually explaining causation with individual variables.
Dissonance theory (Festinger, 1957), attribution theory (Heider, 1958), efficacy theory
(Bandura, 1977), reactance theory (Brehm, 1966), and are examples. In terms of cooperative
learning, micro theories include the effects of explaining answers to others, giving and
receiving academic support, heterogeneous group membership, and accountable talk.
Source: Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. (1989). Cooperation and competition: Theory and research.
Edina, MN: Interaction Book Company. Reprinted with permission.
The theories discussed in this chapter are middle-range theories. Generally, the
comprehensive theories are too far removed from cooperative learning and micro theories are
not broad enough to encompass cooperative learning. There are at least four middle range
theoretical perspectives that have guided research on and practice of cooperative learning—
social interdependence, cognitive-developmental, social-cognitive and behavioural theories
(see Figure 1).
Cognitive-Developmental Theory
attain common goals while coordinating one’s own feelings and perspective with a
consciousness of others’ feelings and perspective. From Piaget and related theories comes the
premise that when individuals co-operate on the environment, socio-cognitive conflict occurs
that creates cognitive disequilibrium, which in turn stimulates perspective-taking ability and
cognitive development. Cooperation in the Piagetian tradition is aimed at increasing a
person's intellectual development by forcing him or her to reach consensus with others who
hold opposing points of view about the answer to the problem. A number of researchers have
conducted studies on cooperation from the Piagetian point of view (e.g., Hayek, Toma,
Oberle, & Butera, 2014).
Lev Semenovich Vygotsky (1978) and related theorists claim that human individual
mental functions and accomplishments are socially constructed in interpersonal relationships.
Knowledge is social phenomenon, constructed from cooperative efforts to learn, understand,
and solve problems. Individual mental functioning is the internalised and transformed version
of the accomplishments of a group. A central concept is the zone of proximal development,
which is the zone between what a person can do on his or her own and what the person can
achieve while working in cooperation with older individuals or more capable peers.
According to Vygotsky, unless persons work cooperatively, they will not grow intellectually
and the time persons work alone should therefore be minimised.
A central aspect of cooperation according to cognitive-developmental theory is conflict
among ideas or controversy. Constructive controversy theory (Johnson & Johnson, 1979,
2007, 2009a) posits that being confronted with opposing points of view creates
disequilibrium, uncertainty, or conceptual conflict, which creates a search for more
information and a reconceptualisation of the issue, which results in a more refined and
thoughtful conclusion. The key steps of constructive controversy are organising what is
known into a position, advocating that position to someone who is advocating an opposing
position, attempting to refute the opposing position while rebutting the attacks on one's own
position, reversing perspectives so that the issue may be seen from multiple points of view
simultaneously, and creating a synthesis to which all sides can agree.
Social cognitive theory (Bandura, 2000; Lave & Wenger, 1991) views cooperation as the
shared belief of group members in their collective power to produce desired results. In
collective agency individuals work cooperatively to secure what they cannot accomplish on
their own. Key behaviours are modeling, coaching, and providing conceptual frameworks that
result in understanding of what is being learned (i.e., scaffolding) (Lave & Wenger). Ideally,
the learner will cognitively rehearse and restructure information as well as explaining the
material being learned to a collaborator. Doing so facilitates retaining the material in memory
and incorporating it into existing cognitive structures (Wittrock, 1990). Finally, social-
cognitive theory places cooperation at the center of a community of practice, a group of
people who share a craft or a profession.
An extension of social cognitive theory is situated cognition theory (Suchman, 1987),
which assumes both the physical and social environment have a powerful influence on
cognitive activity. The more important of the two is the social environment, which consists of
both the immediate group and the social communities to which group members belong.
6 David W. Johnson and Roger T. Johnson
Through the social environment, that is, cooperative groups, members develop shared
cognitions that are the property of the group, not of any one individual member. Cooperation,
in other words, involves building and maintaining a shared conception of the problem being
solved. Through dialogue group members learn to understand each other and create shared
cognitions. It is the joint seeking to construct the solution to a problem that creates the
understanding.
Behavioural-Learning Theory
Definitions
The initial formulation of social interdependence theory was published in 1949 (Deutsch,
1949). Its roots, however, lie in the work of two earlier prominent psychologists. In the early
1900s, Kurt Koffka, one of the founders of Gestalt Psychology proposed that groups were
dynamic wholes in which the interdependence among members could vary. In the 1920s and
1930s, Koffka’s proposal was extended by Kurt Lewin, who stated that: (a) the essence of a
group is the interdependence among members (created by common goals) that resulted in the
7 Theoretical Approaches to Cooperative Learning
group being a “dynamic whole” so that a change in the state of any member or subgroup
changes the state of all other members or subgroups; and (b) an intrinsic state of tension in
group members motivates movement toward the accomplishment of the desired common
goals. He proposed that goal interdependence results in the group being a “dynamic whole”
so that a change in the state of any member or subgroup changes the state of any other
member or subgroup. One of Lewin’s graduate students, Morton Deutsch (1949) extended
Lewin’s notions to the relationship among the goals of two or more individuals. In doing so,
he developed a theory of cooperation and competition. Deutsch (1949) defined positive
interdependence (i.e., cooperation) as existing when a situation is structured so that
individuals’ goal achievements are positively correlated; individuals perceive that they can
reach their goals if, and only if, the others in the group also reach their goals. Thus,
individuals seek outcomes that are beneficial to all those with whom they are cooperatively
linked.
He defined negative interdependence (i.e., competition) as existing when a situation is
structured so that individuals’ goal achievements are negatively correlated; each individual
perceives that when one person achieves his or her goal, all others with whom he or she is
competitively linked fail to achieve their goals. Thus, individuals seek an outcome that is
personally beneficial but detrimental to all others in the situation. Later, Deutsch (1962)
added individualistic efforts, which exist when a situation is structured so there is no
correlation among participants' goal attainments. Each individual perceives that he or she can
reach his or her goal regardless of whether other individuals attain or do not attain their goals
(Deutsch, 1962). Thus, individuals seek an outcome that is personally beneficial without
concern for the outcomes of others.
Taken together, these concepts form the basis of social interdependence theory. Social
interdependence exists when individuals share common goals and each individual’s outcomes
are affected by the actions of the others (Johnson & Johnson 1989). It is differentiated from
social dependence (i.e., the outcomes of one person are affected by the actions of a second
person but not vice versa) and social independence (i.e., individuals' outcomes are unaffected
by each other’s actions).
Interaction Patterns
Watson and Johnson (1972) proposed a broad theory that they called “Structure-Process-
Outcome Theory”. That is, the way in which the situation is structured determines the process
of interaction among participants, which in turn determines the outcomes. The structure of the
situation includes the roles adopted by each participant and the norms regulating behaviour in
the situation. But most important of all, it includes the way in which the goals of participants
are interdependent. This became the basic premise of social interdependence theory, that is,
the way in which interdependence is structured determines how individuals interact, which in
turn, determines outcomes (Deutsch, 1949, 1962; Johnson, 1970, 2003; Johnson & Johnson,
1989, 2005, 2009b; Watson & Johnson, 1972).
Positive interdependence tends to result in promotive interaction; negative
interdependence tends to result in oppositional interaction, and no interdependence results in
an absence of interaction. Promotive interaction occurs when members help and assist each
other, exchange resources, give and receive feedback, challenge each other's reasoning, and
encourage increased effort. Group members focus on maximising the success of their
groupmates as well as their own. Oppositional interaction occurs as individuals discourage
8 David W. Johnson and Roger T. Johnson
and obstruct each other’s efforts to achieve. Individuals focus both on increasing their own
success and on preventing anyone else from being more successful than they are. No
interaction exists when individuals work independently without any interaction or
interchange with each other. Individuals only focus on increasing their own success and
ignore as irrelevant the efforts of others. The relationship between the type of social
interdependence and the interaction pattern it elicits is assumed to be bidirectional. Each may
cause the other.
Psychological States
Deutsch (1949, 1962) emphasised three psychological processes inherent in cooperation
and competition. Depending on whether individuals promote or obstruct each other’s goal
accomplishments, there is substitutability (i.e., the actions of one person substitute for the
actions of another), cathexis (i.e., the investment of psychological energy in objects and
events outside of oneself), and inducibility (i.e., openness to influence). Essentially, in
cooperative situations the actions of participants substitute for each other, participants
positively cathect to each other’s effective actions, and there is high inducibility among
participants.
In competitive situations the actions of participants do not substitute for each other,
participants negatively cathect to each other’s effective actions, and inducibility is low. When
there is no interaction, there is no substitutability, cathexis, or inducibility.
Interaction Patterns
1. Giving and receiving help and assistance (both task-related and personal).
2. Exchanging resources and information.
3. Giving and receiving feedback on taskwork and teamwork behaviours.
4. Challenging each other’s reasoning.
5. Advocating increased efforts to achieve. Encouraging others to achieve increases
their and one's own commitment to do so.
6. Mutually influencing each other. Group members actively seek to influence and be
influenced by each other. If a member has a better way to complete the task,
groupmates usually quickly adopt it.
7. Acting in trusting and trustworthy ways.
8. Engaging in the interpersonal and small group skills needed for effective teamwork.
9. Processing how effectively group members are working together and how the
group’s effectiveness can be continuously improved.
Outcomes
The type of social interdependence structured determines how students interact, which in
turn determines the outcomes of the situation. While the research has focused on numerous
outcomes, which may be subsumed within the broad and interrelated categories of effort to
achieve, quality of relationships, and psychological health (Johnson & Johnson, 1989, 2005a,
2009a) (see Figure 2). Figure 2 shows the relationships among the outcomes.
Effort to Achieve
Overall, cooperation tends to promote higher achievement than competitive or
individualistic efforts (effect sizes = 0.67 and 0.64 respectively) (Johnson & Johnson, 1989,
2005a, 2009a). An important aspect of school life is engagement in learning. One indication
of engagement in learning is time on task. Cooperators spent considerably more time on task
10 David W. Johnson and Roger T. Johnson
than did competitors (effect size = 0.76) or students working individualistically (effect size =
1.17). In addition, students working cooperatively tended to be more involved in activities
and tasks, attach greater importance to success, and engage in more on-task behaviour and
less apathetic, off-task, disruptive behaviours. Finally, cooperative experiences, compared
with competitive and individualistic ones, have been found to promote more positive attitudes
toward the task and the experience of working on the task (effect sizes = 0.57 and 0.42
respectively).
Source: Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. (1989). Cooperation and competition: Theory and research.
Edina, MN: Interaction Book Company. Reprinted with permission.
Quality of Relationships
Cooperation generally promotes greater interpersonal attraction among individuals than
does competitive or individualistic efforts (effect sizes = 0.67 and 0.60 respectively) (Johnson
& Johnson, 1989, 2005a, 2009a). Cooperative experiences tend to promote greater social
support than does competitive (effect size = 0.62) or individualistic (effect size = 0.70)
efforts. Stronger effects are found for peer support than for superior (teacher) support. The
high-quality studies tend to have even more powerful effects.
11 Theoretical Approaches to Cooperative Learning
Psychological Health
Several studies have directly measured the relationship between social interdependence
and psychological health (see Johnson & Johnson, 1989, 1999, 2005a, 2009a). The samples
studied included university students, older adults, suburban high-school seniors, juvenile and
adult prisoners, step-couples, Olympic hockey players, and Chinese business executives. The
results indicate that cooperative attitudes are highly correlated with a wide variety of indices
of psychological health, competitiveness was in some cases positively and in some cases
negatively related to indices of psychological health, and individualistic attitudes were
negatively related to a wide variety of indices of psychological health. One important aspect
of psychological health is self-esteem. The studies that have been conducted at the college
level found that cooperation promoted higher self-esteem than did competitive (effect size =
0.47) or individualistic (effect size = 0.29) efforts. Members of cooperative groups also
become more socially skilled than do students working competitively or individualistically.
Finally, there is evidence that cooperation promotes more frequent use of higher level
cognitive and moral reasoning strategies than do competitive (effect size = 0.93) or
individualistic (effect size = 0.97) efforts. Cooperation also tends to promote more accurate
perspective taking than do competitive (effect size = 0.61) or individualistic (effect size =
0.44) efforts. Thus, the more cooperative learning experiences in which students are involved,
the more mature their cognitive and moral decision making and the more they will tend to
take other people’s perspectives into account when making decisions.
Other Outcomes
A meta-analysis was conducted to determine the impact of quality of interpersonal
relationships on achievement. Over 8 decades of research on over 17,000 early adolescents
from 11 countries found that higher achievement and more positive peer relationships were
associated with cooperative rather than competitive or individualistic goal structures (Roseth,
Johnson, & Johnson, 2008). More specifically, 33% of the variation in achievement was
accounted for by positive peer relationships (40% when the low quality studies were
removed). Another meta-analysis was conducted to determine the impact of motivation on
achievement. Johnson, Johnson, Roseth, and Shin (2014) found that the results of 629
independent studies representing 26 different countries indicated that compared to
competitive and individualistic goal structures, the relative effects of cooperative goal
structures on motivation and achievement were positively correlated, accounting for 14%
(24% when low quality studies were removed) of the variance in achievement (and vice
versa). A concern in schools is bullying. Choi, Johnson, and Johnson (2011) examined the
relationship between (a) cooperative, competitive, and individualistic predispositions and
experiences, and (b) harm-intended aggression (i.e., bullying) and prosocial behaviour.
Cooperativeness predicted prosocial behaviours and the absence of harm-intended aggression,
while competitiveness predicted harm-intended aggression. In other words, engaging in
competition and being competitive tends to increase bullying and engaging in cooperative
experiences and being cooperative tends to decrease bullying and increase prosocial
behaviour.
12 David W. Johnson and Roger T. Johnson
It is not enough for a theory to be conceptualised. It must produce valid research, be well
formulated, and ideally be profound, strategic, and powerful. The characteristics of social
interdependence theory separate it from most middle level social science theories (see Figure
3).
Internal Validity
The research validating social interdependence theory has considerable internal validity.
Many of the over 1,200 research studies that have been conducted on social interdependence
have high internal validity, being carefully conducted by skilled investigators under highly
controlled, laboratory and field conditions (Johnson & Johnson, 1989, 2005a, 2009a). When
rated on the variables of random assignment to conditions, clarity of control conditions,
control of the experimenter effect, control of the curriculum effect (same materials used in all
conditions), and verification of the successful implementation of the independent variable, 51
percent of the studies met these criteria.
External Validity
the dependent variables have been used. Participants in the studies varied from ages three to
post-college adults and have varied in economic class, age, sex, nationality, and cultural
background. The studies were conducted with different durations, lasting from one session to
100 sessions or more. Research on social interdependence has been conducted in numerous
cultures in North America (with Caucasian, Black-American, Native-American, and Hispanic
populations) and countries from North, Central, and South America, Europe, the Middle East,
Asia, the Pacific Rim, and Africa. In all of these studies, the results are quite similar. The
diversity of these studies gives social interdependence theory wide generalisability and
considerable external validity.
Types of Studies
One of the strengths of the research on social interdependence is that it consists of both
theoretical and demonstration, atheoretical studies. Of the 1,200 studies that contained enough
data to compute an effect size, 31 percent have been laboratory studies 65 percent have been
field evaluations. Theory driven research begins with some theoretical point, and then inspires
research to confirm, disconfirm, or to develop further the initial theory. Theory is induced,
modified, and refined from research results. The theory-based studies tend to be carefully
controlled laboratory or field experiments with high internal validity. Fifty-one percent of the
studies randomly assigned participants to conditions, clearly defined control conditions,
controlled for an experimenter effect, controlled for a curriculum effect (same materials used
in all conditions), carefully operationalised the independent variable, ensured that the
measures of the dependent variables were both reliable and valid, and verified the successful
implementation of the independent variable. Theoretical research is the heart of verifying the
effectiveness of cooperative learning and ensuring that cooperative learning will still be used
in the future. There are shortcomings, however, with theoretical studies. One of the
shortcomings is lack of external validity. Many of the studies were conducted in social
psychology laboratories using college students as participants. Such studies tend to lack
credibility with many practitioners as, although they validate theory and clarify the power of
cooperative efforts on the dependent variables of interest, they do not demonstrate that
cooperative learning could work in the “real world”.
Atheoretical, demonstration studies on cooperative learning strive to demonstrate that a
specific type of cooperative learning “works” better than other methods of instruction without
testing any theoretical premise. Such studies tend to produce facts without any theoretical
framework, and are often described as “mindless empiricism” (Swedberg, 2014). In mindless
empiricism a person produces facts with no or little reference to theory. The person begins by
collecting data and then summarises the results without linking them to a theory, or a person
tests hypotheses not directly related to any theoretical tradition. Sometimes there are post-hoc
theoretical explanations that are presented as “window dressing” to enhance the importance of
the findings. Atheoretical studies provide independent findings that are not cumulative and do
not add to the “edifice” of knowledge about cooperative, competitive, and individualistic
efforts.
The demonstration literature is aimed at demonstrating that cooperative efforts are
effective in real life settings. Most of these studies were conducted to evaluate a program’s
success (i.e., external validity) without considering the potential theoretical relevance of the
14 David W. Johnson and Roger T. Johnson
results. Demonstration studies may be grouped into four categories (i.e., summative
evaluations, comparative summative evaluations, formative evaluations, and survey studies).
Summative evaluations investigate the central question of whether a particular
cooperative, competitive, or individualistic program produces beneficial results. They are the
largest category of demonstration studies. In education, the comparison is typically between a
cooperative learning method and “traditional” classroom learning. The Johns Hopkins
research on specific cooperative learning programs (Teams-Games-Tournaments [DeVries &
Edwards, 1974], Student Team Achievement Divisions [Slavin, 1980], Team-Assisted
Individualization [Slavin, Leavey, & Madden, 1986], Cooperative Integrated Reading and
Composition Program [Stevens, Madden, Slavin, & Farnish, 1987]) are examples. They
tended to focus on achievement on lower-level learning tasks in actual classes for several
months. The reviews of these studies (Slavin, 1983, 1991; Johnson & Johnson, 2002a) are
organised around a particular method, not a theoretical proposition. While these evaluation
studies are of interest, the information value of their conclusions tends to be limited to the
specific program evaluated.
Comparative summative evaluations involve the comparative question of which of two or
more cooperative learning methods produces the most beneficial effects when compared on
the same criterion measures. In education, for example, the jigsaw method might be compared
with Team Assisted-Individualization. There is an inherent problem with such studies, as it is
difficult if not impossible to tell if both methods have been implemented at the same strength.
The results can be inadvertently be biased through carefully implementing one method at full
strength and loosely implementing the other method at partial strength.
Formative evaluations involve how a cooperative, competitive, or individualistic
program could be improved. Formative evaluations are aimed at improving ongoing
implementations. The critical incident method seems well suited to the diagnosis of training
deficiencies or unintended consequences, as does a combination of surveys with follow-up
interviews of a representative subsample of respondents.
Survey studies (Johnson & Johnson, 1991) investigate the impact of cooperative,
competitive, and individualistic efforts and attitudes on such variables as perceived social
support, self-esteem, and attitudes toward the task. Survey studies have also compared the
responses of individuals in high-use settings (where the type of social interdependence was
frequently used) with the responses of individuals in low-use settings on a number of climate
variables (e.g., Johnson, Johnson, & Anderson, 1983; Johnson & Johnson, 1983; Johnson,
Johnson, Buckman, & Richards, 1986). These studies provide data about the long-term
impact of cooperative efforts on a variety of attitudinal and learning climate outcomes.
Demonstration studies have both weaknesses and strengths. First, like all case studies,
demonstration studies simply indicate that a certain method worked at that time in those
circumstances. In a different classroom different results might be obtained. Second,
demonstration studies are always in danger of being biased because the researcher is typically
evaluating programs they have developed themselves and have a professional and sometimes
a financial stake in their success. Reviews of demonstration studies suffer the same limitation
as reviews are often conducted by the researchers who invented the programs. The third
problem with demonstration studies is that they may evaluate a cluster of cooperative,
competitive, and individualistic elements. The “cooperative method” evaluated may be only
one element of a broader educational package and, therefore, cooperative learning was
confounded with other variables. The original jigsaw procedure (Aronson, Blaney, Stephen,
15 Theoretical Approaches to Cooperative Learning
Like General MacArthur, most psychological theories never die but slowly fade away (as
people lose interest) without either: (a) being built into the larger edifice of well-tested human
17 Theoretical Approaches to Cooperative Learning
knowledge, or (b) being destroyed by recalcitrant facts and abandoned, perhaps regrettably, as
a “nice try” (Johnson, 2003). The strength of a theory is reflected in the extent to which it
builds on a cumulative past. A theory is supposed to build on past theories so that it corrects,
extends, and refines the older theories. This is how knowledge advances. Most psychological
theories are “soft” in the sense that they lack the cumulative character of scientific
knowledge. They tend to be neither corroborated or refuted. Social interdependence theory is
an exception.
Social interdependence theory is clearly cumulative (Johnson, 2003). The historical roots
of social interdependence theory can be traced to a shift from mechanistic to field theories in
physics (Deutsch, 1968). This shift especially influenced the emerging school of Gestalt
Psychology at the University of Berlin in the early 1900s. As the “field” became the unit of
analysis in physics, so did the “whole” or “gestalt” became the focus of the study of
perception and behaviour for Gestalt psychologists. Gestalt psychologists posited that humans
are primarily concerned with developing organised and meaningful views of their world by
perceiving events as integrated wholes rather than a summation of parts or properties. One of
the founders of the Gestalt School of Psychology, Kurt Koffka (1935), proposed that similar
to psychological fields, groups were dynamic wholes in which the interdependence among
members’ goals could vary. Building on the principles of Gestalt Psychology, Kurt Lewin
(1935) proposed that the essence of a group is the interdependence among members which
results in the group being a “dynamic whole” so that a change in the state of any member or
subgroup changes the state of any other member or subgroup. Group members are made
interdependent through common goals. As members perceive their common goals, a state of
tension arises that motivates movement toward the accomplishment of the goals. Morton
Deutsch took Lewin’s notions and extended them into social interdependence theory.
Social interdependence theory focuses on dynamic (not static) states (Johnson & Johnson,
1989, 2005a, 2009a). When individuals engage in cooperative or competitive efforts, each
action either increases or decreases the level of cooperation or competition present in the
situation. The level of cooperation occurring in a group is a quasi-stationary equilibrium
(Lewin, 1935), a balance between the forces for increased cooperation and the forces for less
cooperation. With each action a member takes, the balance shifts, and the level of cooperation
changes. Supporting a member’s efforts tends to move the equilibrium point towards
increased cooperation, while refusing to help a struggling group member tends to move the
equilibrium point towards decreased cooperation.
understand the laws that govern the behaviour of a single individual. They assume that the
causes of an individual’s behaviour are inside the individual, consisting of personality traits,
attitudes, values, skills, aptitudes, brain chemistry, and genes. In contrast, social
interdependence theory focuses on relationship variables that reside among parties (Johnson
& Johnson, 2005a, 2009a). It assumes that the causes of an individual’s behaviour are in the
interaction among individuals, changing constantly according to the way one’s actions affect
the actions of others. Cooperation and competition are relationship variables, involving
interaction between at least two parties.
Profound Theory
Strategic Theory
been used as a guide to provide procedures to solve social problems such as desegregation,
inclusion of handicapped students in the regular classroom, the prevention of drug abuse, and
the raising of low self-esteem of at-risk pupils. There are so many applications of social
interdependence theory that they cannot be all listed here.
Another indicator of strategicness is the number and variety of the dependent variables
studied in the research. Social interdependence is a generic human phenomenon that effects
many different outcomes simultaneously. The dependent variables are often classified into
three major areas (effort to achieve, interpersonal relations, psychological health), so many
different dependent variables have been studied that social interdependence theory is one of
the most strategic theories in the social sciences. Related to effort to achieve, for example,
there have been studies on individual achievement and long-term retention, group and
organisational productivity, higher-level reasoning, generation of new ideas and solutions,
moral reasoning, transfer of training and learning, job satisfaction, learning strategies,
achievement motivation, intrinsic and extrinsic motivation, continuing motivation, time on
task, and many other variables. Related to interpersonal relations, numerous measures of
interpersonal attraction have been used, along with measures of group cohesion, esprit-de-
corps, interpersonal affection and love, liking among diverse individuals (ethnicity, culture,
handicapping conditions, and so forth), attitudes toward diversity, and prejudice. In addition,
there is considerable research on academic support and personal social support. Related to
psychological health, measures have been used for psychological adjustment; cognitive,
social, psychological development; social competencies; self-esteem; personal causation and
locus of control, attributions concerning success and failure, self-efficacy; shared identity;
internalisation of values; and coping with stress. There are few if any theories in social
psychology that is more strategic than social interdependence theory.
Powerful Theory
A theory is powerful when it has considerable internal and external validity and
generalises across a wide range of individual differences, situational variables, cultures, and
historical periods. A weak theory may hold only for males in a very specific set of
circumstances. A strong theory holds no matter what persons are like (gender, economic
class, ethnicity, culture, age), what circumstances prevail, what historical decade the research
was conducted in, which country and culture the research was conducted in, and how long the
research sessions lasted and how many sessions were included in the research (Johnson &
Johnson, 1989, 2005a, 2009a). The more variations in places, people, and procedures the
research can withstand and still yield the same findings, the more powerful the theory. The
research on social interdependence theory indicates that it is a very powerful theory.
Multi-Level Theory
Mediating Variables
elements of effective cooperation were identified as the result of the efforts to understand the
internal dynamics of cooperation so it could be more effectively implemented. The five basic
elements are (Johnson & Johnson, 1974, 1978, 1989; Johnson et al., 2013):
1. Positive interdependence.
2. Individual accountability.
3. Promotive interaction.
4. Social skills.
5. Group processing.
and maintaining effective working relationships, thereby identifying and solving problems in
working together. This is an important addition to the nature of promotive interaction.
Deutsch (1962, 1973, 1985) has posited that trust, conflict, and distributive justice are
mediators of the effectiveness of cooperation. The greater the trust, the more effective
cooperative efforts tend to be. Effective cooperative requires that conflicts of interest are
resolved constructively. The effectiveness of cooperation is increased when benefits are
distributed based on equality and need and when the distribution of benefits according to
equity (i.e., high performers receive more than low performers) is avoided.
While competing theories have been proposed by such social scientists as Piaget (1950)
and Skinner (1968), and in such diverse fields as economics, sociology, anthropology,
ethology, evolution, and psychology, social interdependence theory has been the foundation
for numerous other theories. Those theories deal with such phenomena as trust, conflict,
integrative negotiations, social justice, positive power, and values. Trust is based on the belief
that the other person will behave in a cooperative, not a competitive manner (Deutsch, 1962).
Conflict resolution is based on restoring cooperation among disputants and reducing
competitive behaviour (Deutsch, 1973). Integrative negotiations are grounded in promoting a
cooperative problem-solving (as opposed to a competitive “win-lose”) process of coming to
an agreement (Johnson & F. Johnson, 2013; Johnson & R. Johnson, 2003). Distributive
(social) justice may be based on equality (cooperative rationale), equity (competitive
rationale), or a need basis (altruistic rationale) (Deutsch, 1985). Newly conceptualised views
of positive power are based on the view that power is positive when it enhances the
cooperative aspects of the situation and reduces the competitive aspects of the situation
(Coleman & Tjosvold, 2000; Johnson & F. Johnson, 2013). Cooperation is the basis for social
identity theory and procedural justice (Tyler & Blader, 2000). Recent discussions of
inculcating values detail how cooperation tends to promote prosocial values while
competition tends to promote more self-centered values (Johnson & Johnson, 1996, 2000).
Without social interdependence theory these theories would not be as well formulated or as
meaningful. The greater the validation for social interdependence theory, furthermore, the
more the validation for these and other related theories.
One of the unique aspects of social interdependence theory is that operational procedures
have been developed for each type of social interdependence and the procedures have been
implemented in a wide variety of applied settings. Within education the operational
definitions of cooperative learning derived from social interdependence theory have been
implemented at the preschool, elementary, secondary, university, and adult education levels.
This is rather unique among the wide variety of cooperative learning procedures. From social
interdependence theory, three types of cooperative learning have been operationalised: formal
23 Theoretical Approaches to Cooperative Learning
cooperative learning, informal cooperative learning, and cooperative base groups (Johnson et
al., 2013).
Formal cooperative learning consists of students working together, for one class period
to several weeks, to achieve shared learning goals and complete jointly specific tasks and
assignments (such as problem solving, completing a curriculum unit, writing a report,
conducting an experiment, or having a dialogue about assigned text material) (Johnson et al.,
2013). Any course requirement or assignment may be structured to be cooperative. In formal
cooperative learning teachers:
Informal cooperative learning consists of having students work together to achieve a joint
learning goal in temporary, ad-hoc groups that last from a few minutes to one class period
(Johnson et al., 2013). Students engage in quick dialogues or activities in temporary, ad-hoc
groups in response to a limited number of questions about what is being learned. The brief
dialogues or activities may be used to focus student attention on the material to be learned, set
a mood conducive to learning, help set expectations as to what will be covered in a class
session, ensure that students cognitively process the material being taught, and provide
closure to an instructional session. Informal cooperative learning groups are often organised
so that students engage in three-to-five minute focused discussions before and after a lecture
and two-to-three minute turn-to-your-partner discussions interspersed every ten to fifteen
minutes throughout a lecture. Cooperative base groups are long-term, heterogeneous
cooperative learning groups with stable membership whose primary responsibilities are to
provide support, encouragement, and assistance to make academic progress and develop
cognitively and socially in healthy ways as well as holding each other accountable for striving
to learn (Johnson, Johnson, & Holubec, 2013). Typically, cooperative base groups (a) are
heterogeneous in membership, (b) meet regularly (for example, daily or biweekly), and (c)
last for the duration of the semester, year, or until all members are graduated. Students are
assigned to base groups of three to four members, meet at the beginning and end of each class
session (or week) to complete academic tasks such as checking each members’ homework,
24 David W. Johnson and Roger T. Johnson
routine tasks such as taking attendance, and personal support tasks such as listening
sympathetically to personal problems or providing guidance for writing a paper. These three
types of cooperative learning may be used together. A typical class session may begin with a
base group meeting, which is followed by a short lecture in which informal cooperative
learning is used. The lecture is followed by a formal cooperative learning lesson. Near the end
of the class session another short lecture may be delivered with the use of informal
cooperative learning. The class ends with a base group meeting. These three types of
cooperative learning may be used at all levels of education, and even form the basis for
organising the cooperative school (Johnson & Johnson, 1994a). At the school level, faculty
and staff can meet weekly in teaching teams and/or study groups, engage in school-based
decision making, and structure faculty meetings and school events cooperatively.
Competitive and Individualistic Efforts
Social interdependence theory has been expanded in the past few decades to include the
conditions under which competition may be constructive (Johnson & Johnson, 1974, 1978,
1989, 1999; Stanne et al., 1999; Tjosvold, Johnson, Johnson, & Sun, 2003). Competition
tends to be more constructive when winning is relatively unimportant, all participants have a
reasonable chance to win, and there are clear, specific, and fair rules, procedures, and criteria
for winning. Individualistic efforts may be most appropriate when cooperation is too costly,
the goal is perceived to be important, participants expect to be successful, the task is unitary
and nondivisible, directions for completing the task are simple and clear, there is adequate
space and resources for each student, and what is accomplished will be used subsequently in a
cooperative effort.
While this chapter has focused primarily on social interdependence theory, the authors do
not mean to imply that it is the only theory on which cooperative learning is based or the only
theory that will guide the future evolution of cooperative learning. It has been the most
influential on cooperative learning so far, but this does not mean that it will always be so.
Diversity in theorising (and in practical procedures) is needed to provide a healthy
environment for the continued growth and refinement of cooperative learning. The more
heterogeneous the theorising and the procedures used to implement cooperative learning, the
more promising is cooperative learning’s future. The heterogeneity produces conflict, and
from such intellectual conflict comes creativity and innovation (Johnson & Johnson, 2007).
CONCLUSION
The long-term survival of cooperative learning as an instructional method depends on its
being based on a clearly formulated theory that has been validated by research.
Demonstration studies that show a certain procedure works are not enough to ensure the
continued use of cooperative learning. An example is that although there were a number of
studies that demonstrated that in the early 1970s Teams-Games-Tournaments procedure
produced higher achievement than did traditional classroom learning, very few schools base
their instructional program on Teams-Games-Tournaments today. The same may be said
about Dewey’s Project Method and Frances Parker’s version of cooperative learning.
Hundreds of instructional procedures (if not thousands) have been developed over the past 60
years, nontheoretical demonstration studies were conducted showing that they were effective,
but the procedures are gone, adopted and then dropped despite their effectiveness.
Instructional procedures often have short lives, no matter how effective they are. To be
maintained as an instructional practice over decades and even centuries, requires that they are
built on a foundation of theory validated by theoretical research.
What separates cooperative learning from most other instructional methods is that it is
clearly part of a cycle of theory, research, and practice. While there are at least four middle
range theories that provide a basis for cooperative learning (cognitive developmental, social
cognitive, behavioural, and social interdependence), it is social interdependence theory that
has provided: (a) the major impetus for research on cooperative, competitive, and
individualistic efforts; (b) the clearest operationalisations of cooperative learning; and (c) the
most widespread implementation of cooperative learning at all levels of education (preschool,
elementary, secondary, university, adult education). The basic premise of social
interdependence theory is that the type of interdependence structured in a situation determines
how individuals interact with each other, which, in turn, determines outcomes. Well over
twelve-hundred research studies have been conducted to validate or disconfirm social
interdependence theory. There is sufficient research, with both high internal and external
validity, to test the theory. The hundreds of studies that have been conducted indicate that
cooperation, compared with competitive and individualistic efforts, tends to result in higher
achievement and productivity, more positive interpersonal relationships, and greater
psychological health. The amount, quality, and generalisability of the research provide strong
26 David W. Johnson and Roger T. Johnson
confirmation of the basic propositions of the theory and the effectiveness of cooperative
relative to competitive and individualistic efforts.
The characteristics of social interdependency theory that elevate it above most other
theories include the validating research having high internal and external validity. Social
interdependence theory has generated both theoretical and demonstration studies, the results
of which are cumulative. Social interdependence theory is well formulated with clearly
defined concepts, testable, is dynamic (not static), focuses on relationship (not intrapersonal)
variables, is profound, is strategic, is powerful, and is the foundation on which other theories
are built. Social interdependence theory is applicable at multi-levels (intrapersonal,
interpersonal, and so forth). Finally, social interdependence theory has been applied in many
diverse areas (most notably in education and business) and the applications have had
profound effects on the theory and related research, revealing inadequacies in the theory and
pointing to further research that needs to be conducted.
Overall, social interdependence theory is the most influential theory underlying
cooperative learning. This does not mean that the other theories related to cooperative
learning are inadequate or faulty. This chapter is not meant to imply that social
interdependence theory is the only or should be the only theory underlying cooperative
learning. Each theory has their supporters, research has been conducted to validate them, and
cooperative learning procedures have been operationalised from most of them. A creative
tension among the theories is probably ideal for moving the theories forward and refining
cooperative learning.
REFERENCES
Aronson, E., Blaney, N., Stephan, C., Sikes, J., & Snapp, M. (1978). The jigsaw classroom.
Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change,
Psychological Review, 84(2), 191-215.
Bandura, A. (2000). Exercise of human agency through collective efficacy. Current
Directions in Psychological Science, 9(3), 75-78.
Brehm, J. W. (1966). A theory of psychological reactance. New York: Academic Press.
Campbell, D. (1958). Common fate, similarity, and other indices of status of aggregates of
persons as social entities. Behavioral Science, 3, 14-25.
Choi, J., Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. (2011). Relationship among cooperative learning
experiences, social interdependence, children’s aggression, victimization, and prosocial
behaviors. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 41(4), 976-1003.
Coleman, P., & Tjosvold, D. (2000, July). Positive power: Mapping the dimensions of
constructive power relations. Paper presented at the Social Interdependence Theory
Conference, Silver Wind Farm, Minnesota.
Deutsch, M. (1949). A theory of cooperation and competition. Human Relations, 2, 129-52.
Deutsch, M. (1962). Cooperation and trust: Some theoretical notes. In Jones M (ed.),
Nebraska symposium on motivation (pp. 275-319). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska
Press.
27 Theoretical Approaches to Cooperative Learning
Deutsch, M. (1968). Field theory in social psychology. In G. Lindzey, & E. Aronson (Eds.),
The handbook of social psychology, Vol. 1 (2nd ed., pp. 412-487). Reading, MA:
Addison Wesley.
Deutsch, M. (1973). The resolution of conflict. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Deutsch, M. (1983). The prevention of World War III: A psychological perspective. Political
Psychology, 4(1), 3-31.
Deutsch, M. (1985). Distributive justice: A social psychological perspective. New Haven,
CT: Yale University Press.
DeVries, D., & Edwards, K. (1974). Student teams and learning games: Their effects on
cross-race and cross-sex interaction. Journal of Educational Psychology, 66, 741-749.
Durkheim, E. (1897/2013). Suicide: A study in sociology. New York: Beta Nu Publishing.
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University
Press.
Hall, C. L. (1935). The conflicting psychologies of learning: A way out. Psychological
Review, 42, 491-516.
Hayek, A., Toma, C., Oberle, D., & Butera, F. (in press). Grading hampers cooperative
information sharing in group problem solving. Social Psychology 46.
Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Homans, G. C. (1961). Social behavior: Its elementary forms. Oxford, England: Harcourt,
Brace.
Johnson, D. W. (1970). Social psychology of education. New York: Holt, Rinehart, &
Winston.
Johnson, D. W. (1983a). Resolving marital conflicts constructively. Edina, MN: Interaction
Book Company.
Johnson, D. W. (2003). Social interdependence: The interrelationships among theory,
research, and practice. American Psychologist, 58(11), 931-945.
Johnson, D. W. (2014). Reaching out: Interpersonal effectiveness and self-actualization (11th
ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, F. P. (2013). Joining together: Group theory and group skills
(11th ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T. (1974). Instructional goal structure: Cooperative,
competitive, or individualistic. Review of Educational Research, 44, 213-240.
Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T. (1978). Cooperative, competitive, and individualistic
learning. Journal of Research and Development in Education, 12, 3-15.
Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. (1979). Conflict in the classroom: Constructive controversy
and learning. Review of Educational Research, 49, 51-61.
Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. (1983). Social interdependence and perceived academic and
personal support in the classroom. Journal of Social Psychology, 120, 77-82.
Johnson, D. W, & Johnson, R. (1989). Cooperation and competition: Theory and research.
Edina, MN: Interaction Book Company.
Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. (1991). Cooperative learning and classroom and school
climate. In B. Fraser, & H. Walberg (Eds.), Educational environments: Evaluation,
antecedents and consequences (pp. 55-74). New York: Pergamon Press.
Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. (1994a). Leading the cooperative school (2nd ed.). Edina,
MN: Interaction Book Company.
28 David W. Johnson and Roger T. Johnson
Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. (1994b). Cooperative learning and American values. The
Cooperative Link, 9(3), 3-4.
Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. (1996). Cooperative learning and traditional American values:
An appreciation. NASSP Bulletin, 80(579), 63-65.
Johnson, D. W, & Johnson R. (1999). Learning together and alone: Cooperative,
competitive, and individualistic learning. Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.
Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. (2000). Cooperative learning, values, and culturally plural
classrooms. In M. Leicester, C. Modgill, & S. Modgill (Eds.), Values, the classroom, and
cultural diversity (pp. 15-28). London: Cassell PLC.
Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. (2002a). Cooperative learning methods: A meta-analysis.
Journal of Research in Education, 12(1), 5-14.
Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. (2002b). Multicultural education and human relations. Edina,
MN: Interaction Book Company.
Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. (2003). Field testing integrative negotiations. Peace and
Conflict: Journal of Peace Psychology, 9(1), 39-68.
Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. (2005a). New developments in social interdependence theory.
Psychology Monographs, 131(4), 285-358.
Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. (2005b). Teaching students to be peacemakers (4th ed.).
Edina, MN: Interaction Book Company.
Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. (2005). Essential components of peace education. Theory Into
Practice, 44(4), 280-292.
Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. (2007). Creative controversy: Intellectual challenge in the
classroom (4th ed.). Edina, MN: Interaction Book Company.
Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T. (2009a). An educational psychology success story: Social
interdependence theory and cooperative learning. Educational Researcher, 38(5), 365-
379.
Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T. (2009b). Engergizing learning: The instructional power of
conflict. Educational Researcher, 38(1), 37-52.
Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T. (2010a). Teaching students how to live in a democracy. In
F. Salili, & R. Hoosain (Eds.), Democracy and multicultural education (pp. 201-234).
Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.
Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T. (2010b). The impact of social interdependence on value
education and student wellbeing. In T. Lovat, R. Toomey, & N. Clement (Eds.),
International research handbook of values education and student wellbeing (pp. 825-
848). New York: Springer Press.
Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T. (2014). Constructive controversy as a means of teaching
citizens how to engage in political discourse. Policy Futures in Education, 12(3), 417-
430.
Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T. (in press). Constructive controversy: Teaching students
how to think creatively. In A. Vollmer, M. Dick, & T. Wehner (Eds.), Innovation as a
social process: Constructive controversy – a method for conflict management. Berlin:
Gabler/Eiesbaden/Germany Publishers.
Johnson, D. W., Johnson, R., & Anderson, D. (1983). Social interdependence and classroom
climate. Journal of Psychology, 114, 135-142.
29 Theoretical Approaches to Cooperative Learning
Johnson, D. W., Johnson, R., Buckman, L., & Richards, P. (1986). The effect of prolonged
implementation of cooperative learning on social support within the classroom. Journal
of Psychology, 119, 405-411.
Johnson, D. W., Johnson, R., & Holubec, E. (2013). Cooperation in the classroom (9th ed.).
Edina, MN: Interaction Book Company.
Johnson, D. W., Johnson, R., & Maruyama, G. (1983). Interdependence and interpersonal
attraction among heterogeneous and homogeneous individuals: A theoretical formulation
and a meta-analysis of the research. Review of Educational Research, 53(1), 5-54.
Johnson, D. W., Johnson, R. T., Roseth, C. J., & Shin, T. S. (2014). The relationship between
motivation and achievement in interdependent situations. Journal of Applied Social
Psychology, 44, 622-633.
Johnson, D. W., Maruyama, G., Johnson, R., Nelson, D., & Skon, L. (1981). Effects of
cooperative, competitive, and individualistic goal structures on achievement: A meta-
analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 89, 47-62.
Johnson, D. W., & Matross, R. (1977). Interpersonal influence in psychotherapy: A social
psychological view. In A. Gurman, & A. Razin (Eds.), Effective psychotherapy: A
handbook of research (pp. 395-432). New York: Pergamon Press.
Kessel, K. (2000). Mediation. In M. Deutsch, & P. Coleman (Eds.), The handbook of conflict
resolution: Theory and practice (pp. 522-545). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Koffka, K. (1935). Principles of gestalt psychology. New York: Harcourt, Brace.
Lave, J., & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated learning: Legitimate peripheral participation.
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Lewin, K. (1935). A dynamic theory of personality. New York: McGraw Hill.
Maller, J. B. (1929). Cooperation and competition: An experimental study in motivation. New
York: Teachers College (Columbia University) Press.
May, M., & Doob, L. (1937). Co-operation and competition. Social Science Research Council
Bulletin, 125. New York: Social Science Research Council.
Mayer, A. (1903). Uber einzel-und gesamtleistung des scholkindes [On individual and
collective achievement of the school child]. Archiv fur die Gesamte Psychologie [Archive
of General Psychology], 1, 276-416.
Mead, M. (1936/1961). Cooperation and competition among primitive peoples. Boston, MA:
Beacon.
Merton, R. K. (1957). Social theory and social structure. New York: Free Press.
Popper, K. (1935). Logik der forschung. Zur erkenntnistheorie der modernen
naturwissenschaft. New York: Springer. English translation by K. Popper, J. Freed, & L.
Freed (1959). The logic of scientific discovery. London: Hutchinson.
Qin, Z., Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. (1995). Cooperative versus competitive efforts and
problem solving: A meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 65(2), 129-143.
Piaget, J. (1950). The psychology of intelligence. New York: Harcourt.
Reichenbach, H. (1951). The rise of scientific philosophy. Los Angeles, CA: University of
California Press.
Roseth, C. J., Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T. (2008). The relationship between
interpersonal relationships and achievement within cooperative, competitive, and
individualistic conditions: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 134(2), 223-246.
Skinner, B. (1968). The technology of teaching. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
30 David W. Johnson and Roger T. Johnson
SCH