Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 52

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,

Memory, and Cognition


Manuscript version of

Conceptual Anchoring Dissociates Implicit and Explicit Category Learning


J. David Smith, Brooke N. Jackson, Markie N. Adamczyk, Barbara A. Church

Funded by:
• National Institutes of Health, Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human
Development

© 2021, American Psychological Association. This manuscript is not the copy of record and may not exactly
replicate the final, authoritative version of the article. Please do not copy or cite without authors’ permission.
The final version of record is available via its DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000856

This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 1

Conceptual Anchoring Dissociates Implicit and Explicit Category Learning

J. David Smith1

Brooke N. Jackson1

Markie N. Adamczyk2

Barbara A. Church1,2,a

Authors Note
1
Language Research Center, Georgia State University
2
Department of Psychology, Georgia State University
a
To whom correspondence may be addressed
The preparation of this publication was supported by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver
National Institute of Child Health & Human Development of the National Institutes of Health
under Award Number R01HD093690. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and
does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. The authors
declare no financial interest and no conflicts of interest. The data, analyses, and questionnaires
can be viewed using the following link to open science framework (Church, 2020).
https://osf.io/ystr9/?view_only=211b3682a92146d481f0d95e11067137
Correspondence can be addressed to:
Barbara Church
Language Research Center
Georgia State University
3401 Panthersville Rd
Decatur, GA 30034
[email protected]

Word Count: 9860

October 6, 2020
DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 2

Abstract

Categorization researchers have long debated the possibility of multiple category-learning

systems. The need persists for paradigms that dissociate explicit-declarative category-learning

processes (featuring verbalizeable category rules) from implicit-procedural processes (featuring

stimulus-response associations lying beneath declarative cognition). The authors contribute a

new paradigm, using perfectly matched exclusive-or (XOR) category tasks differing only in the

availability or absence of easily verbalizeable conceptual content. This manipulation transformed

learning. The conceptual task alone was learned suddenly, by insightful rule discovery,

producing explicit-declarative XOR knowledge. The perceptual task was learned more gradually,

consistent with associative-learning processes, producing impoverished declarative knowledge.

We also tested participants under regimens of immediate and deferred reinforcement. The

conceptual task alone was learned through processes that survive the loss of trial-by-trial

reinforcement. All results support the idea that humans have perceptual-associative processes for

implicit learning, but also an overlain conceptual system that under the right circumstances

constitutes a parallel explicit-declarative category-learning system.

Keywords: category learning, implicit cognition, explicit cognition, associative learning,

category rules, procedural learning


DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 3

Conceptual Anchoring Dissociates Implicit and Explicit Category Learning

Introduction

Categorization is an influential area in cognitive science because it is a crucial ability (e.g.,

Ashby & Maddox, 2011; Knowlton & Squire, 1993; Murphy, 2002; Nosofsky, 1986; Smith et

al., 2008; Smith et al., 2016). Humans could have multiple systems or processes to manage the

diverse demands of category tasks involving family-resemblance, rule-based, conjunctive,

disjunctive, random, or ad hoc categories, just as they have multiple memory systems or

processes. Indeed, memory and categorization systems might be meaningfully interdependent

(e.g., explicit categorization and declarative memory; implicit categorization and procedural

memory).

However, a theoretical countercurrent holds that assuming multiple categorization systems

is unparsimonious and unjustified (e.g., Le Pelley et al., 2019). It suggests that a single system

can predict the relevant categorization phenomena (e.g., Newell et al., 2010; Nosofsky et al.,

2005) if one assumes that tasks vary in difficulty-complexity and that this variation produces

empirical phenomena that are mistaken for the operation of separate systems. It is a lasting idea

in cognitive science that singleness is elegant and scientifically preferable.

Thus, a sharp theoretical debate has persisted, especially focused on the possibility of

dissociating explicit-declarative categorization processes (featuring verbal category rules) from

implicit-procedural processes (featuring associations strengthened by processes akin to operant

conditioning). Accordingly, the need persists for additional dissociative paradigms to help

resolve this debate. Here, we contribute a new paradigm dissociating implicit and explicit

categorization systems as described now.


DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 4

Implicit-Procedural and Explicit-Declarative Categorization

Implicit category learning. Our approach draws from the neuroscience of categorization

(Ashby et al., 1998; Maddox & Ashby, 2004; Seger & Miller, 2010) that distinguishes different

neural systems of learning. A hypothesized implicit system is energized by one of the brain’s

primary reinforcement mechanisms. It likely grounds skill and habit learning (Mishkin et al.,

1984) and learning during instrumental conditioning as well as some forms of discrimination

learning and perceptual categorization (Ashby & Ennis, 2006; Barnes et al., 2005; Knowlton et

al., 1996; O'Doherty et al., 2004; Seger & Cincotta, 2005). It is allied to various forms of

associative learning, though it does not encompass all forms of associative learning (e.g.,

Pavlovian conditioning). This form of implicit learning occurs gradually and associatively,

relying on trial repetition and immediate reinforcement (Maddox et al., 2003; Maddox & Ing,

2005). Participants learning implicitly may not be aware of their category knowledge or able to

verbalize it (Ashby et al., 1998; Ashby & Ell, 2001). This implicit system is linked to particular

parts of the basal ganglia. For example, extrastriate visual cortex projects to the tail of the

caudate nucleus that then projects on to premotor cortex (Alexander et al., 1986). The caudate

nucleus is well situated to associate percepts to actions, perhaps its primary role (Rolls, 1994;

Wickens, 1993).

Thinking beyond neuroscience, readers will see that this implicit process has a long history

within the literature on categorization. It was central to Shepard et al.’s (1961) founding

exploration of the six logical classification tasks. They asked whether a unitary, associative

mechanism could account for the tasks’ relative difficulties. It was central to Lee Brooks’ (e.g.,

1978) seminal research exploring nonanalytic classification, by which participants associate

category labels to specific remembered stimuli. It was the basis of exemplar theory and exemplar
DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 5

models (e.g., Nosofsky, 1986; Nosofsky et al., 1994). It dominated the comparative literature on

categorization (because nonverbal animals may have only the associative process—Smith et al.,

2004). This implicit process creates behavioral equivalence classes, allowing organisms to

behave equivalently toward perceptually similar things. One may think of these equivalence

classes as “categories” or not, and readers may differ on this point. Nonetheless, the learning of

these classes has been central to the broader categorization literature, and therefore this

associative process plays a central role in the multiple-systems debate.

Explicit category learning. An explicit system of learning could be supported by

declarative memory. Explicit learning occurs through hypothesis testing reliant on working

memory (Fuster, 1989; Goldman-Rakic, 1987) and executive attention (Posner & Petersen,

1990). These cognitive utilities are known to support hypothesis testing and rule formation (e.g.,

Brown & Marsden, 1988; Robinson et al., 1980). This explicit system should learn quickly,

perhaps suddenly through insightful discovery. Participants would construe the task for

themselves and develop their own rule to guide performance. Their explicit category knowledge

would be held in working consciousness and should generally be verbalizeable. This explicit

neural system is likely grounded in the prefrontal cortex, the anterior cingulate gyrus, the head of

the caudate nucleus, and the hippocampus (Ashby et al., 1998; Ashby & Ell, 2001).

Various dissociations have been demonstrated between implicit and explicit category

learning (Ashby & Valentin, 2017). For example, Smith et al. (2014) used a regimen of deferred

reinforcement that is one aspect of the present method. Participants completed a block of trials

with no feedback. Then they received their positive outcomes grouped together and following

that their negative outcomes grouped together. Reinforcement was displaced in time from trial
DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 6

performance. Knowing which trials were completed correctly was difficult and therefore

associatively crediting reinforcement to particular stimulus-response pairs was impossible.

Given this feedback regimen, participants learned matched category structures thought to

elicit implicit-procedural and explicit-declarative learning. Smith et al. (2014) observed a

striking dissociation. Implicit learning was devastated under deferred reinforcement, because of

the difficulty in assigning reinforcement credit already described. But explicit learning remained

intact, because participants held in active mind the rule applied during the block, evaluated the

rule’s success at block’s end, and kept or replaced it. Smith et al. (2018) found converging

dissociative results using a different reinforcement regimen that is also incorporated here.

Empirical Goals

Though these dissociations support the dissociative framework described, that framework is

not universally accepted. Therefore, we pursued three empirical goals to develop a distinctive

dissociative paradigm that could advance the debate.

One empirical goal was to move this area beyond its dependence on the rule-based (RB) and

information-integration (II) tasks that are often used to distinguish explicit and implicit learning

(e.g., Ashby & Valentin, 2017). The RB-II dissociative framework is illuminating. We use it

(e.g., Smith et al., 2012). However, it causes interpretative problems. One problem is that the

nonidentical RB and II tasks are rotations of one another in perceptual space, so that the RB and

II category solutions can be construed to have different dimensionalities. This raises questions

about the difficulty-complexity of the tasks and can seem to give comfort to a single-system

description. So, we sought a pair of implicit/explicit tasks that were identically structured in

perceptual space and equated for stimulus dimensionality and other stimulus-to-stimulus
DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 7

relationships. We thought this might give us equivalent complexity and allow a stronger

dissociative interpretation.

A second major empirical goal was to combine in our study several of the components that

have created dissociative demonstrations in this literature. We hoped in this way to create within

one paradigm a particularly strong and meaningful implicit-explicit dissociation. To this end, we

studied the backward learning curves that can catch humans having explicit and instantaneous

categorization insights. We studied category verbalization to distinguish explicit from implicit

learning within a categorization task. We studied alternative reinforcement regimens that were

theoretically predicted to differentially affect explicit and implicit learning.

A third major empirical goal was to produce our dissociation in a new way, by varying the

kind and level of category knowledge that the implicit and explicit tasks foster. It has been a tacit

assumption within the perceptual categorization literature, and within the RB-II area of study,

that explicit category solutions take the form of conceptual rules that are often verbalizeable.

This raised the possibility that we might produce a dissociation by varying the cognitive

affordances of our tasks, so that one task fostered abstract-conceptual cognition more than the

other. Then, by a range of converging measures, we could catch the explicit mind in the act of

seizing these conceptual affordances. In this respect, the present approach represented

constructive outreach from research on perceptual categorization to research on conceptual

categorization and higher-level concepts (e.g., Medin & Wattenmaker, 1987; Murphy & Medin,

1985; Wattenmaker et al., 1986). It seems to us that there are possibilities for constructive

cooperation and cross talk across the perceptual and conceptual areas of categorization research,

though these have largely remained separate in the past. One crucial binding principle might be,
DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 8

for example, that explicit categorization is after all a privileged locus for conceptual and theory-

based categorization.

Empirical Approach

We built matched pairs of XOR category tasks. XOR tasks have had a distinguished career

in psychology (e.g., Feldman, 2000; Nosofsky et al., 1994; Shepard et al., 1961; Smith et al.,

2004). For example, Shepard et al. (1961) discovered that the XOR learning trajectory and error

pattern disconfirmed the unitary associative-learning theory they were exploring. Rather,

participants seemed to be using rules and dimensional hypotheses, which Shepard et al. thought

were likely carried by the explicit symbolic vehicle of language. It is remarkable that the

literature on implicit and explicit category learning is still trying—after 60 years—to possibly

find its way back to Shepard et al. That the systems debate remains one of the central debates in

categorization after six decades underscores the theoretical importance of resolving it.

Our task pairs presented a crucial contrast. Though the tasks were logically identical, with

the tasks’ stimuli placed and spaced apart identically in the same two-dimensional space, one

task was configured so that it might make conceptual content discoverable as the task unfolded

trial by trial. This task presented an abstract-conceptual affordance that could support correct

classification. The other task simply had its stimulus values shifted globally (a simple coordinate

translation through stimulus space), so that it did not present such an affordance that we (or, as it

turned out, participants) could discern. In this latter task, we thought that perceptual appearance

and associative learning might dominate. We will refer to the conceptual and perceptual tasks,

respectively, to communicate this distinction.

Participants completed tasks under the contrasting reinforcement regimens of deferred and

immediate reinforcement (e.g., Smith et al., 2014, 2018). Deferred reinforcement appears to
DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 9

suppress an important kind of reinforcement-based associative learning, while leaving relatively

intact participants’ processes of hypothesis testing and explicit rule learning. Thus, this contrast

seemed apt to dissociate in an additional way implicit and explicit category-learning processes.

We predicted that 1) the conceptual and perceptual tasks would be learned rapidly and

slowly, respectively; 2) the tasks would be learned suddenly and gradually, respectively; 3) only

the conceptual task would be robustly learnable under deferred reinforcement; 4) only the

conceptual task would elicit clear verbalizations of the category principle; and 5) only the

perceptual task (in our view, requiring associative learning) would become essentially

unlearnable under deferred reinforcement (in our view, disabling the necessary type of

associative learning).

We believe that no single-system description can make these predictions. A single-system

description cannot predict both sudden insight learning and slow gradual associative learning

(e.g., Smith & Ell, 2015). It cannot explain why the two kinds of category knowledge—

perceptual and conceptual—would obey different processing principles. There are not perceptual

and conceptual levels in a single system. It cannot explain why deferred reinforcement disrupts

one kind of learning but not the other if attentional difficulty is held constant. From the single-

system perspective, there must not be two forms of learning. Finally, the single-system viewpoint

cannot explain why one form of category knowledge would be conscious and verbalizeable and

one not. We acknowledge that specifying the single-system’s predictions is fraught, because its

proponents have not clearly defined the single system they endorse. For example, Le Pelley et al.

(2019, p. 1408) explicitly acknowledged that they could not provide “rigorous definitions of the

interrelated constructs of cognitive complexity, memory demands, and task difficulty” on which

the single-system idea depends heavily.


DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 10

Nonetheless, confirming our five converging positive predictions would strongly suggest

that humans have perceptual-associative processes for implicit learning, but also an overlain

conceptual system that under the right circumstances constitutes a parallel explicit-declarative

system for category learning. In fact, because the explicit and implicit processes considered in

this article stand so diametrically opposed across many dimensions of human cognition, they

could turn out to represent the clearest possible dissociation between two learning systems.

Experiment 1

Method

Participants. One hundred and eighty-two Georgia State undergraduates1—with normal or

corrected vision—participated for partial course credit. Sessions lasted for 52 minutes or 480

trials. Participants were assigned randomly to a task and reinforcement condition using their

sequential participant number. Because of the need to supply different verbal instructions,

immediate reinforcement participants (receiving immediate feedback after every trial) and

deferred reinforcement participants (receiving deferred feedback only after an ensuing trial had

been completed) were tested separately in groups of up to four at a time. Participants’ data were

excluded if they completed fewer than 480 trials or did not complete a final questionnaire2. The

final data set included 40, 31, 39, and 28 participants divided, respectively, among these

conditions: conceptual task/immediate reinforcement (CI+), conceptual task/deferred

reinforcement (CD), perceptual task/immediate reinforcement (PI), and perceptual task/deferred

reinforcement (PD). Twelve participants were excluded for not enough trials (0, 2, 2, and 8,

1
Power analyses using effect size estimates based on Smith et al., 2018 suggested that .95 power
could be achieved for all sub-analyses with a sample of 148. A sample size of only 40 would
achieve that power level for the crucial three-way interaction.
2
In follow-up analyses, all GLM analyses were repeated including participants dropped for no
questionnaire to confirm the added power would not change the basic pattern of findings.
DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 11

respectively, in the four conditions). Thirty-two participants were excluded for not completing a

final questionnaire (4, 15, 3, and 10 participants, respectively). These exclusions were principled

and necessary—they let us equate learning experience, analyze completed protocols, and study

performance levels in relation to participants’ introspections and verbalized rules.

Stimuli. The stimuli were red rectangles, varying in width and height, presented on a black

background in the computer screen’s top center (Figure 1). There were four stimuli in each task,

two contrasting but memorizeable stimuli in each category. Category A stimuli occupied the

lower-left and upper-right quadrants of the stimulus space. Category B stimuli occupied the

upper-left and lower-right quadrants. These placements honored the tasks’ XOR structure.

Figure 1

Stimuli used in Experiment 1.

A. B.

Note: Boxes in the right upper and left lower quadrants were category A. The left upper and right lower
stimuli were category B. A.) Stimuli presented in the conceptual task. B.) Stimuli in the perceptual task.

The widths and heights of stimuli varied between tasks. In the conceptual task, the Category

A stimuli were 65 (width)-44 height and 130-88 in screen pixels. These dimensions produced

squares on our running screens, given that screen pixels are taller than they are wide. This

abstract property of Category A stimuli provided a conceptual entry into this task beyond just

memorizing four shapes and their correct category responses. The B stimuli were 65-88 and 130-

44 in pixels—respectively, nondescript standing-up and lying-down rectangles.


DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 12

In the perceptual task, we simply added 100 pixels to the widths of all four stimuli. The A

stimuli were 165-44 and 230-88 in pixels. The B stimuli were 165-88 and 230-44. Now all

stimuli were lying-down rectangles, highly discriminable and memorizeable as shapes but

presumably not conceptually codeable in some additional way that could support correct

categorization. Of course, participants might have discovered some conceptual construal of the

task that we could not discern. For this reason, the experiment’s results will offer their own

comment on this presumption. Even absent such a conceptual cue, the four stimuli were mutually

discriminable as shown in Figure 1. They varied by a factor of 3 in area and in shape as well. The

stimuli were also easily perceptible, brightly colored, and large.

Categorization trials. On each trial, the rectangle that was to be categorized appeared at

the screen’s far right. Leftward were the large letters “A” and “B”, with a participant-controlled

cursor between them. Participants pressed keys (S, L, labelled A, B, corresponding spatially to

the two screen icons). Top and Bottom trials were displayed at the screen’s top and bottom, for

reasons to be explained.

Reinforcement regimens. Our crucial manipulation was to disrupt the normal cycle of

immediate reinforcement following response. In the immediate reinforcement conditions, this

cycle was sustained. Participants saw a stimulus, categorized it, and received immediate

reinforcement. After correct responses, they saw Correct +1 Points Total Points N+1. After

incorrect responses, they saw Incorrect -1 Points Total Points N-1. In the latter case they

received a brief penalty timeout.

In the deferred reinforcement conditions, feedback was deferred as follows. Following a

Bottom trial, participants received deferred feedback regarding the previous Top trial. For

example, they might see (given a correct response) presented at the top of the screen in the
DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 13

position for Top trial feedback, Last Trial Correct +1 Points Total Points N+1. Or, following

a Top trial, they might see (given an incorrect response) presented at the bottom of the screen in

the position for Bottom trial feedback, Last Trial Incorrect -1 Points Total Points N-1. In the

latter case they received a brief penalty timeout. The next trial then followed.

The deferred feedback was positioned spatially to show to which trial the feedback

pertained—this was the purpose of the alternating Top and Bottom trials (see Figure 2 for an

example). This reinforcement did not concern a presently available stimulus, or the most recent

stimulus presentation, or the most recent behavioral response. Thus, associative learning was

disrupted representationally, temporally, and behaviorally.

Figure 2

Trial and feedback structure in the deferred condition in Experiment 1

Instructions: immediate reinforcement condition. Participants were told they would

categorize boxes as Category A or B, that A and B boxes would occur equally often, and that

they would have to guess at first but could learn to respond correctly. They were told that even

though the boxes alternated top and bottom on the screen, this had nothing to do with their

Category A or B status. They knew they would gain or lose points for correct and incorrect
DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 14

responses, respectively, and that they would receive a timeout for errors. They were told that

errors would cost them points, and time to earn points, and possibly make their session longer.

They were told they would receive immediate feedback after each trial.

Instructions: deferred reinforcement condition. The instructions were similar, excepting

the description of reinforcement. Participants were told that after Trial 2 they would receive

feedback from Trial 1, and so forth, with feedback always lagging one trial behind. They were

told that even though the boxes alternated top and bottom on the screen, this had nothing to do

with their Category A or B status, but was done to help them keep track of whether the feedback

they received applied to a top or bottom trial.

Debriefing phase. An additional aspect of our method was to collect participants’ explicit

reports of their task construals, especially in the conceptual tasks that allowed a conceptual

construal. Therefore, we gave them a debriefing questionnaire containing these items:

Why did you use Response A in this task? That is, what WAS a Type A Stimulus?

Why did you use Response B in this task? That is, what WAS a Type B Stimulus?

These let us analyze participants’ declarative understanding of the categories they had learned.

Analytic methods: questionnaires. The questionnaire items were independently blind

coded by two research assistants in our laboratory. They were instructed to give a questionnaire a

1 or 2 if the participant indicated that they simply used either the height or the length of the box

to categorize. They were to give a questionnaire a 3 if the participant said “A’s were squares and

B’s rectangles” (the correct verbalization of the conceptual task), or a 4 if they said the

equivalent of “A’s are shorter and narrower or taller and wider; B’s are taller and narrower or

shorter and wider (the correct verbalization of the perceptual task). In essence, the latter

verbalization simply described the four stimuli in an item-specific manner, because in this case
DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 15

there is no constructive conceptual reframing. Codes 5 and 6 were used for explanations like

those for codes 3 and 4 but flipping which were A’s and B’s. Raters used code 7 for anything

else. A third rater provided back-up ratings that were used only to resolve the rare disagreements

between the two students, who were blind to each other’s ratings and also blind to the level of

performance achieved by the participant who completed the questionnaire. Interrater reliability

between the first two raters was 85.5% agreement, Cohen’s = .777. This approach to coding

participant reports—using a third rater to break ties—was somewhat conservative regarding

testing our hypotheses, because it ensured that we retain for analysis all of the difficult and

uncertain reports that caused our original raters difficulty. Finally, a 4th blind rater coded

approximately 25% of the data, and showed a concordance of 91.43% agreement, Cohen’s =

.863, with the final coding.

Results

Accuracy analyses. Figure 3 presents proportions of correct responses for each condition

by 20-trial block. Table 1 presents the means and standard deviations of accuracy overall and in

the last 100 trials. The data from Figure 3 were entered into a three-way general linear model

(GLM) with task (conceptual, perceptual) and reinforcement (immediate, deferred) as between-

participant factors and trial block as a within-participant factor. The significant main effects of

task F (1, 134) = 46.265, p < .001, p2 = .257, and reinforcement, F (1, 134) = 36.109, p < .001,

p2 = .328, showed that participants performed better in the conceptual task and the immediate

condition. The significant main effect of trial block F (23, 3082) = 22.770, p < .001, p2 = .145,

confirmed that learning occurred. The significant interactions between task and reinforcement, F

(1, 134) = 3.930, p = .049, p2 = .028, block and reinforcement, F (23, 3082) = 4.064, p < .001,

p2 = .033, and the three-way, F (23, 3082) = 3.930, p < .001, p2 =.029, suggested that
DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 16

reinforcement differentially affected performance across tasks. The insignificant interaction

between task and block, F < 2, suggested that the learning trajectory was similar in the two tasks.

Parallel GLM analyses were also conducted adding in the participants dropped for missing

questionnaires, as requested by an interested reviewer. These found the same pattern of results

except that the task by block interaction became significant, F (23, 3818) = 2.874, p < .001, p2 =

.017 with larger N’s in each group (CI = 46, CD = 44, PI = 42, PD = 38).

Figure 3

Proportion correct across 20-trial blocks in the four conditions

Note: CI = Conceptual Immediate, CD = Conceptual Deferred, PI = Perceptual Immediate, PD =


Perceptual Deferred.
DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 17

Table 1

Means and standard deviations (SD) of proportion correct across all trials and in the last 100 trials in
each condition.

Condition N All Mean All SD Last 100 Mean Last 100 SD


Conceptual 40 .954 .090 .979 .079
Immediate
Conceptual 31 .795 .209 .838 .235
Deferred
Perceptual 39 .828 .163 .907 .157
Immediate
Perceptual 28 .566 .127 .530 .158
Deferred

To clarify the three-way interaction, we conducted three two-factor GLMs with task and

reinforcement as between-participant factors. One used the difference score between the first and

last block as the dependent variable to encompass the whole learning trajectory. The other two

used the first and last blocks separately as the dependent measure to explore the early or late

focus of the trajectory differences. When examining the learning scores, there was a significant

main effect of reinforcement, F (1, 134) = 18.200, p < .001, p2 = .120, reflecting more learning

with immediate reinforcement. The crucial significant interaction, F (1, 134) = 14.416, p < .001,

p2 = .097, reflected sharply reduced learning in the PD condition (all other Fs < 1). Planned

comparisons found that immediate and deferred learning levels were not statistically

distinguishable in the conceptual task, t (69) = .306, p = .760, d = .071, but were significantly

different in the perceptual task, t (65) = 6.385, p < .001, d = 1.662. The very high performance

(.907 correct) shown by participants in the last 100 trials of the PI condition confirms the full

discriminability of the stimuli in that task and their consequent individual memorizeability.

Separate analyses of the first and last blocks found significant main effects of task, F (1,

134) = 30.528, p < .001, p2 = .186, F (1, 134) = 28.577, p < .001, p2 = .176 (first, last block

respectively) and reinforcement, F (1, 134) = 4.713, p = .032, p2 = .034, F (1, 134) = 63.145, p
DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 18

< .001, p2 = .320, indicating that conceptual tasks and immediate reinforcement produced faster

and more total final learning. (Indeed, this speed advantage was expressed even in the elevated

correct proportions for participants in Block 1, some of whom are already discovering the

conceptual rule.) There was a significant interaction between task and reinforcement only in the

last block, F < 2, F (1, 134) = 13.443, p < .001, p2 = .091, reflecting bigger differences in final

learning between the reinforcement conditions in the perceptual condition, but less differential

effect in initial learning. Planned comparisons of immediate versus deferred reinforcement in

each task (Conceptual and Perceptual, respectively) during the first block, t (69) = 2.027, p =

.047 d = .476, t (65) = .914, p = .364, d = .229 and the last block, t (69) = 3.162, p = .002, d =

.723, t (65) = 7.862, p < .001, d = 1.907, supported this interpretation.

Backward learning curves. Following Smith et al. (2014) and Smith and Ell (2015), we

examined the suddenness of learning in the different conditions. We defined learners as those

who at any point completed three consecutive 20-trial blocks with 0.95 accuracy. In the four

conditions (CI, CD, PI, PD), there were 39, 23, 31, and 3 learners. Chi square analyses found that

the proportion of learners in the four conditions was significantly different from expected

learning levels if there were no relationship between condition and learning, 2 (3, N = 138) =

61.443, p < .001; w = .667. Parallel Chi Square analyses including the participants dropped for

missing questionnaires found the same significant result with a greater number of learners (CI =

43, CD = 34, PI = 34, PD = 4).

Next, we studied the trajectory by which participants arrived at criterion. To do so, we

created backward learning curves (BLCs). That is, we aligned the trial blocks at which learners

reached criterion, and we examined performance levels backward and forward from that point.

Readers should note the following idiosyncrasy of BLCs. Participants who reached criterion very
DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 19

quickly in the task will have fewer pre-criterion data points but more post-criterion data points.

For late learners, the pattern is reversed. Thus, different participants and different numbers of

participants are captured by different data points in an BLC graph. The alignment of all

participants’ performance at the start of the criterion run lets one see the consensual pathway

toward criterion taken by learners. Without alignment, the arrival at criterion would fall in many

different blocks for many different participants, and any consensual pathway would be muddied

away. Figure 3 presented the data in this way—this is the idiosyncrasy of Forward Learning

Curves.

Illustrating the BLC analysis, Figure 4 shows BLCs for each condition. The solid black

circles show the performance of 39 learners in the CI condition. Performance transformed at the

point of the criterion run. In the 5 blocks before that point, participants averaged .550 correct—

essentially chance performance. In the 5 blocks after criterion, they averaged .992 correct (a

sudden improvement of .442 or suddenly 9 more correct responses per 20-trial block) and

sustained that level going forward. We believe that this pattern is consistent with, and only

consistent with, the sudden discovery of a conceptual category rule (e.g., Smith & Ell, 2015). No

operant associative-learning mechanism produces a learning curve of this character. This pattern

of instantaneous, insightful rule discovery diagnoses uniquely the explicit category-learning

processes that for some reason have remained so controversial within the categorization

literature. Shortly we will add verbalizations—declarative category knowledge—into the mix.


DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 20

Figure 4

Backwards learning curves for learners in Experiment’s four conditions

A. B.

Note: A. CI and CD conditions. B. PI and PD conditions. We aligned the trial blocks at which participants
reached criterion (Block 0), to show the path by which they solved their task. C: Conceptual P: Perceptual
I: Immediate Reinforcement D: Deferred Reinforcement.

From this conclusion follows predictions for the CD condition. First, because testing

hypotheses is more complicated under deferred reinforcement, we expected fewer criterial

learners. There were 23 learners, not 39 as before. Nonetheless, we expected to see the same

saltatory leap of rule discovery among the successful learners. And we observed just that (.552

correct, 5 blocks before criterion, .986 correct, 5 blocks after criterion, a transition of .434—

Figure 4A, grey triangles).

In contrast, in the PI condition, we expected rule discovery to occur less strongly because

the conceptual content was less discoverable given the nature of the rectangle stimuli. However,

under immediate reinforcement, participants could still learn associatively, gradually building up

response strengths binding correct responses to the four specific stimuli. Or, they could use

exemplar memorization to connect the 4 discriminable stimuli to correct responses. It is good to


DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 21

remember that implicit learners may well not be learning the formal XOR concept, or even a

differentiation between two coherent categories, but rather learning associative connections from

specific stimuli to correct response choices. In fact, we did have 31 learners in this condition. But

learning obeyed a different dynamic (black circles in Figure 4B). Now there was a more gradual

improvement in performance approaching the criterion run, as befits the gradual strengthening of

correct stimulus-response mappings (.726 correct, 5 blocks before criterion, .976 correct, 5

blocks after criterion, a transition of .251). More precisely, performance increased from 0.777 in

the last block before criterion to 0.953 at the criterion block itself. This was a performance

change of .176, a minimal transition (i.e., less than 4 more correct responses per block) compared

to the conceptual conditions. We also confirmed these differences statistically through

comparisons of the corrected3 criterion scores (Block 0 minus Block -1) for learners in each task.

We found that when compared to the perceptual immediate condition both the conceptual

immediate, t(68) = 5.201 , p < .001, d =1.242, and the conceptual deferred conditions, t(52) =

4.772, p < .001, d = 1.318, showed significantly greater jumps in learning.

Moreover, some of the small transition in the PI condition is artifactual. The criterial blocks

must contain high performance levels. They cannot contain any block of 16/20 (80%) or lower,

for then criterion is unreachable. Such a block would be relegated to the phase before criterion,

creating an artifactual pre-post separation. Similarly, the block just before criterion essentially

cannot contain any block of 17/20 (85%) of higher, or this block would just serve to define

criterion earlier. The artifactual separation is accentuated. Smith and Ell (2015) discussed this

selective sampling aspect of BLCs in detail.

3
Criterion scores were calculated by subtracting the proportion correct in block -1 from block 0.
If block -1 did not exist, we used .5 (chance performance) to stand in for the missing block -1.
DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 22

Simulations. We used formal modeling to examine our results from this perspective. We

placed simulated participants into a 100-block “task”, with performance governed by a pre-

criterion and post-criterion underlying competence that we could let vary systematically from 0.5

to 1.0. In our taskless simulation, correct performance was simply determined by the throw of a

100-sided dice weighted by the operative competence (e.g., 80 successes in 100 for an 0.80

competence). Given this framework, we could isolate the simulants that 1) could reach the

defined criterion used in the actual experiment; 2) could produce the performance we observed

before criterion; and 3) could produce the performance we observed after criterion. In this way,

we could ask what underlying competence levels our real participants likely had at different

points in the task.

Figure 5A shows the result of our simulation for the CD condition we have already

discussed. This graph illustrates all the levels of competence before and after criterion that can

produce the data pattern we observed (i.e., .552 and .986 before and after criterion). Competence

before criterion must be extremely low (maximum 0.69). Competence after criterion must be

extremely high (minimum 0.90), respectively. There is a minimum gap of 0.21 between these.

This inherent gap represents the pure leap of insight / competence that we are considering in this

article. The formal simulation confirms that this gap must exist, just as did the extensive

simulations in Smith and Ell (2015).


DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 23

Figure 5

A formal model isolated all the values of underlying pre- and post-criterion competence (black dots) that
can reproduce the observed levels of pre- and post-criterion performance in Experiment 1

Note: A. CD condition. B. PI condition. For example (A), these participants had a maximum underlying
competence of .69 before criterion and a minimum .90 competence after reaching criterion. There was an
instantaneous increase of at least .21 in this transition.

Figure 5B shows in the same way the result of our simulation for the PI condition. This

graph illustrates all the levels of competence before and after criterion that can produce the data

pattern we observed (i.e., .726 and .976 before and after criterion). Competence after criterion

can sink down into the mid 0.80’s and still produce what we observed in that phase. But

competence before criterion in the mid 0.80’s can also predict what we observed in that phase.

Thus, this simulation is consistent with the possibility that in the PI condition there was not any

intrinsic leap at all. Only in the conceptual conditions must one conclude that there is a sudden

leap in competence. Smith and Ell (2015) showed that gradualistic associative models cannot

accommodate these sudden performance improvements.

Finally, we point out that the results from the PD condition also fit our predictions. Our

theoretical perspective suggested that the perceptual task would deny participants the explicit

learning process that sometimes survives deferred reinforcement. And the deferred reinforcement
DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 24

would deny participants the complementary implicit-associative process. Learning should have

collapsed, and it did. There were 3 criterial learners, about 10% of the sample. In the aggregate

there was almost no learning at all.

Verbalizations. Fifty-nine of 71 participants (83%) were coded to have correctly described

the conceptual condition. In the CI condition, with 40 total participants, with 39 strong learners,

38 participants (all of them learners) stated the task’s conceptual grounding. There was

essentially a perfect concordance between successful learning and the task’s conceptual

construal. In the CD condition, with 31 total participants, with 23 strong learners, 21 participants

(all of them learners) reported the task’s conceptual grounding. All non-learners failed to report

this construal. There was a nearly perfect concordance between learning and a conceptual

declaration again. In both conceptual conditions, participants’ explicit-declarative category

knowledge accounted extremely sensitively for their performance and for the suddenness of their

rule discovery. Thus, the verbalizations were also consistent with the theoretical interpretation

that explicit-declarative learning characterized the conceptual conditions. These participants

discovered the alternative conceptual reframing afforded by the task.

On the other hand, only 12 of the 67 participants in the perceptual tasks (18%) were coded

to have correctly described the perceptual condition. This percentage is more than four times

smaller than we observed in the conceptual conditions. Moreover, of those 12, five showed their

knowledge by drawing (not verbalizing) their knowledge. The others simply described more or

less fully all four specific stimuli that appeared within the task. This confirms our sense as we

prepared the experiment that the perceptual task would not provide to participants an alternative

abstract-conceptual route to reframing the task, so that they would need to fall back on

memorizing the four stimuli. These participants apparently did not discover an alternative
DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 25

conceptual reframing of their task. In the immediate reinforcement condition, with 39 total

participants and 31 strong learners, 10 were coded as correctly describing the task (i.e., correctly

describing the four stimuli presented under the correct category label). Of those, eight were

learners (2 nonlearners) and four drew rather than verbalized their description. In the deferred

reinforcement condition, with 28 total participants and 3 strong learners, 2 of those strong

learners were coded as correctly describing the XOR rule. One drew the stimuli, and one

verbalized their correct description. Learning in the perceptual task was thus more visual and

more about specific-item associations to the four stimuli. The availability of these specific-item

associations confirms in another way the full individual discriminability of the stimuli in the

perceptual task.

Experiment 2

Experiment 2 explored these phenomena in a procedure different in three respects. We used

a different stimulus domain, involving multiple circle stimuli to be judged relationally, instead of

one rectangular stimulus. We adopted a different reinforcement regimen, one involving deferred

reinforcement delivered only after the completion of each six-trial block. Third, the available

conceptual construal was now about the abstract conceptual relation (Same or Different) between

two stimuli, rather than a shape label given one stimulus. In many other respects, the methods

were like those in Experiment 1 and so we only note the points of contrast.

Method

Participants. One hundred and eighty Georgia State undergraduates4—with normal or

corrected vision—participated for partial course credit. Sessions lasted for 52 minutes or 300

4
Power analyses using effect size estimates based on Smith et al., 2014 suggested that .95 power
could be achieved for all sub-analyses of interest with a sample of 152. A sample of 52 would
achieve that power for the crucial three-way interaction.
DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 26

trials. Participants’ data were excluded if they completed fewer than 300 trials or did not

complete a final questionnaire. Participants were assigned randomly to a task and reinforcement

condition using their sequential participant number. The final data set after principled exclusions

included 171 GSU participants divided among the four conditions as follows: CI condition (46),

CD condition (43), PI condition (42), and PD condition (40). Five participants were excluded for

not enough trials (0, 2, 1, and 2, respectively, in the mentioned conditions). Four participants

were excluded for not completing a final questionnaire (3 in the CD condition and 1 in the PI

condition).

Stimuli. On each trial, participants saw a pair of circle stimuli, in red (left) and green

(right) that always just touched at the 3:00 o’clock tangent point. The four XOR stimuli were

placed in perceptual space as already described. In the conceptual task, the A stimuli had left and

right radii of 25-25 and 50-50. Category A stimuli instantiated the abstract relation of SAME.

Category B stimuli had left and right radii of 25 50 and 50 25. They instantiated the abstract

relation of DIFFERENT (Figure 6A). In the perceptual task, we simply added 45 to the radius of

all left circles. Once again, this was a simple coordinate translation through perceptual space.

Now all trials presented differently sized circles, so all trials were DIFFERENT (Figure 6B). The

translation through perceptual space only had the effect of changing the task’s conceptual

affordances.
DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 27

Figure 6

Stimuli used in Experiment 2

A. B.

Note: Circles in the right upper and left lower quadrants were category A. The left upper and right lower
stimuli were category B. A.) Stimuli presented in the conceptual task. B.) Stimuli in the perceptual task.

Here, too, the individual stimuli (circle pairs) were obviously discriminable from one

another and individually memorizeable for that reason. They varied strongly in overall size and

in the circles’ differential areas. The performances achieved by learners after criterion will

strongly confirm this claim of discriminability.

Categorization trials. In Experiment 2, there was no need for alternating trials Top and

Bottom to help participants understand that feedback applied to a former trial. Therefore, all

trials were presented at screen center. The collection of responses from participants was exactly

as already described. Trials continued for 15 20-trial blocks.

Reinforcement regimens. In the CI and PI conditions, immediate reinforcement was

delivered as already described. Now, though, we chose a different approach to defer

reinforcement and disrupt the time-locked stimulus-response-reinforcement cycle. Participants

saw and responded to six stimuli in succession without receiving any feedback. Then they

received summary feedback as follows. In successive screen messages, they were told about all

of their correct responses in the block as their Total Points tally was also incremented. Then, they

were told about all of their incorrect responses in the block as their Total Points tally was also

decremented (Figure 7). Each error message was accompanied by a brief penalty timeout. Under
DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 28

these conditions, stimuli and responses were separated in time and by intervening trials.

Outcomes were scrambled away from the original order of presentation. There was no way to

know which precise trials had been answered correctly or not. There was no way to strengthen

the associative bonds between particular stimuli and responses. We expected operant associative-

learning mechanisms to be disabled. However, explicit-conceptual learning processes might not

be disabled. Participants could hold their working hypothesis in memory and wait for the

summary feedback to come, judging then appropriately whether that hypothesis or rule was

working out for them.

Figure 7

Trial and feedback structure in the deferred condition in Experiment 2

Instructions: immediate conditions. These instructions were as already described,

except that participants were told they would categorize pairs of circles as Category A or B.
DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 29

Instructions: deferred conditions. These instructions were the same, except that their

feedback instructions suited their deferred reinforcement regimen. They were told they would be

given summary feedback every six trials.

Analytic methods: questionnaires. At the end of the experiment participants filled out the

questionnaire described in Experiment 1. The questionnaire items were independently blind

coded by two research assistants. They were instructed to give a questionnaire a 1 if the

participant said “A’s were the same and B’s different” (the correct verbalization of the

conceptual task), or a 2 if they said the equivalent of “A’s are smallest with medium large or

medium small with largest, B’s were smallest with largest or the two mediums (the correct

verbalization of the perceptual task). Again, this was simply a description of the task’s four

stimuli. Codes 3 and 4 were used for explanations like those for codes 1 and 2 but with only one

type of stimuli correctly described. Code 5 was used if the person said they memorized the A’s

and B’s and code 6 for anything else. A third rater provided backup ratings that were used only

to resolve the rare disagreements between the two students, who were blind to each other’s

ratings and also blind to the level of performance achieved by the participant who completed the

questionnaire. Interrater reliability between the first two raters was 87.7% agreement, Cohen’s

= .801. This approach to coding reports—using a third rater to break ties—was somewhat

conservative regarding testing our hypotheses for the reason already described. Finally, a 4th

blind rater coded approximately 25% of the data, and showed a concordance of 90.70%

agreement, Cohen’s = .845, with the final outcome of the coding process.

Results

Figure 8 presents performance for the four conditions by 20-trial block. Table 2 presents the

means and standard deviations of overall accuracy and terminal performance (last 100 trials).
DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 30

The former data were entered into a three-way GLM with task (conceptual, perceptual) and

reinforcement (immediate, deferred) as between-participant factors and block as a within-

participant factor. The significant main effects of task F (1, 167) = 92.483, p < .001, p2 = .356,

and reinforcement, F (1, 167) = 131.320, p < .001, p2 = .440, showed that participants

performed better in the conceptual task and with immediate reinforcement. The significant main

effect of trial block, F (14, 2338) = 20.280, p < .001, p2 = .108, confirmed that learning

occurred. The interaction between task and reinforcement, F (1, 167) = 3.459, p = .065, p2 =

.020, was not significant, but the significant interactions between block and reinforcement, F (14,

2338) = 3.330, p < .001, p2 = .020, and the three-way, F (23, 3082) = 4.232, p < .001, p2 =.025,

suggested that reinforcement condition differentially affected learning across tasks. The

insignificant interaction between task and block, F < 2, suggested that the learning trajectory was

similar in the two tasks.

Figure 8

Proportion correct across 20 trial blocks in the four conditions

Note: CI = Conceptual Immediate, CD = Conceptual Deferred, PI = Perceptual Immediate, PD =


Perceptual Deferred.
DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 31

Table 2

Mean and standard deviations (SD) of proportion correct across all trials and in the last 100 trials in
each condition

Condition N All Mean All SD Last 100 Mean Last 100 SD


Conceptual Immediate 46 .955 .048 .976 .032
Conceptual Deferred 43 .683 .199 .737 .228
Perceptual Immediate 42 .721 .158 .787 .186
Perceptual Deferred 40 .525 .066 .543 .105

Again, to clarify the three-way interaction, we conducted three two-factor GLMs with task

and reinforcement as between-participant factors. One used the difference score between the first

and last block as the dependent variable to encompass the whole learning trajectory. The other

two used the first and last blocks as the dependent measure to explore the early or late locus of

these differences. Examining learning scores, there was a significant main effect of

reinforcement, F (1, 167) = 7.498, p < .001, p2 = .043, reflecting greater learning with

immediate reinforcement. The crucial significant interaction, F (1, 167) = 8.202, p < .001, p2 =

.047, reflected sharply reduced learning in the PD condition (all other Fs < 1). Planned

comparisons found that learning in the immediate and deferred reinforcement conditions were

not statistically distinguishable in the conceptual task, t (87) = .085, p = .933, d = .018, but were

in the perceptual task, t (80) = 4.247, p < .001, d = .941. Separate analyses of the first and last

blocks found significant main effects of task, F (1, 167) = 44.147, p < .001, p2 = .209, F (1,

167) =41.445 , p < .001, p2 = .199 (first, last block respectively) and reinforcement, F (1, 167) =

35.049, p < .001, p2 = .173, F (1, 167) = 71.603, p < .001, p2 = .300, indicating that conceptual

tasks and immediate reinforcement produce both faster and more final learning overall. There

was a significant interaction between task and reinforcement only in the first block, F (1, 167) =

13.054, p < .001, p2 = .073, F < 2, reflecting bigger differences in initial learning between the

reinforcement conditions in the conceptual condition, but similar differences between types of
DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 32

reinforcement in final learning. Planned comparisons of immediate versus deferred

reinforcement in each task (Conceptual and Perceptual respectively) during the first, t (87) =

5.968, p < .001 d = 1.263, t (80) = 1.997, p = .049, d = .442 and the last blocks, t (87) = 6.062, p

< .001, d = 1.265, t (80) = 5.902, p < .001, d = 1.308, supported this interpretation.

Using the same learning criterion as in Experiment 1, we found that in the four conditions

there were 45, 18, 16, and 0 learners. Chi square analyses found that the proportion of learners in

the four conditions is significantly different from expected learning levels if there was no

relationship between condition and learning, 2 (3 N = 171) = 85.111, p < .001; w = .705.

These learners then let us study the path by which participants reached criterion (Figure 9).

The performance of 45 learners in the CI condition transformed again at Block 0, with a .383

increase in performance (.598 correct, 5 blocks before criterion to .980, 5 blocks after criterion,

Figure 9A—black circles). Rule discovery, not gradually strengthening associative connections,

is consistent with this suddenness.


DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 33

Figure 9

Backwards learning curves for learners in Experiment 2’s three conditions that produced learners
reaching criterion

A. B.

Note: A CI and CD conditions. B. PI condition. Trial blocks are aligned at the point participants reached
criterion (Block 0). C: Conceptual P: Perceptual I: Immediate Reinforcement D: Deferred Reinforcement.

As in Experiment 1, deferred reinforcement made hypothesis testing more difficult. There

were 18 learners in the CD condition, not 45. It is expectable for two reasons that deferred

reinforcement had a greater effect on learning in E2 (41.86% learners) compared to E1 (77.42%

learners). First, the working memory demands required to hold on to choices while awaiting

deferred feedback probably interferes more with the discovery of abstract relational concepts like

same/different than with the discovery of basic perceptual concepts like square/rectangle. This

would be consistent with recent research showing that concurrent working memory load

interferes with participants’ transition from perceptually based matching to conceptual

same/different matching in a relational match to sample task (Smith et al., 2019). Second, in E2,

hypothesis testing had to operate over a deferral of 6 trials in a block, not just one lagged trial as

in E1.
DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 34

Despite these effects, we expected to see the same saltatory leap of rule discovery among

the successful learners. And we observed just that (a transition of .467: .519 correct in the five

blocks before criterion, .986 correct in the five blocks after criterion, Figure 9A, grey triangles).

The transition at criterion was especially strong here, possibly because deferred reinforcement

undercuts associative-learning processes, leaving the participant with only conceptual discovery.

We expected less learning in the PI condition because, even though the stimuli were placed

identically in stimulus/perceptual space, the placement did not provide an intuitive conceptual

route to performance. But we expected associative-perceptual learning to still occur, by

developing stimulus-response linkages or by exemplar memorization (but likely not by learning

the formal XOR task structure or two coherent, differentiable categories). In fact, we did have 16

learners in this condition. Their learning clearly obeyed a different dynamic (Figure 9B—black

circles). There was a markedly gradual improvement in performance, as befits some gradual

learning process. At criterion, there was a performance change of .125 between the pre-criterion

block, .816, and criterion, .941 (.786 correct in the five blocks before criterion, .958 correct in

the 5 blocks after criterion, a transition of .172). This change was very small and perhaps the

result of the sampling constraints already described in Experiment 1. Again, this pattern was

consistent with gradual, associative learning. The difference in the suddenness of learning can be

seen clearly in the very different backwards curves producing similar overall performance in the

CD and PI conditions (compare Figures 9A and 9B). Also, statistical comparisons of the criterion

scores (Block 0 minus Block -1)5 for learners found that when compared to the PI condition both

5
Criterion scores were calculated by subtracting the proportion correct in block -1 from block 0.
If block -1 did not exist, we used .5 (chance performance) to stand in for the missing block -1.
DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 35

the CI condition, t(61) = 3.740 , p < .001, d =1.254, and the CD condition, t(32) = 6.292, p <

.001, d = 1.3, showed significantly greater jumps in learning.

There were no learners in the PD condition. The deferred reinforcement disabled associative

learning processes. The translation of the task through stimulus space undermined the task’s

conceptual availability. Learning should have been doubly hamstrung in this condition, and it

was.

We examined participants’ verbalizations as before. In the conceptual conditions, 61 of 89

participants reported the task’s conceptual grounding (68%). In the CI condition, with 46 total

participants, with 45 strong learners, 42 participants (all learners) reported the task’s conceptual

grounding. In the CD condition, with 43 total participants, with 18 learners, 19 participants (18

learners and one participant whose last block was perfect but who still did not meet criterion)

reported that grounding. Here, too, there was a strong concordance between learning and a

conceptual declaration. In both conditions, participants’ declarative category knowledge

accounted for their performance and for the suddenness of their learning. On the other hand, only

6 of the 82 participants in the perceptual tasks (7%, a percentage nine times less) were coded to

have correctly described the perceptual condition—that is, they correctly described the four

stimuli presented in the task. Of those 6, 2 showed their knowledge through drawing, not

verbalizing. All came from the group of 16 learners in the immediate reinforcement condition.

Overall, just as in Experiment 1, the results are only consistent with the explanation that explicit-

declarative learning uniquely characterized and transformed the conceptual conditions.


DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 36

General Discussion

The Difficulty Hypothesis

Through a 20-year theoretical debate, the single-system idea has depended on an amorphous

construct of task difficulty. It proposes that seeming multiple system results can be explained by

assuming a unitary learning system, close to the associative-memory system described here, with

some tasks just harder than others. Unfortunately, the difficulty hypothesis in categorization is

untenable. The descriptor “difficult” has no psychological meaning unless one understands the

processes brought to a task. And single system proponents have not defined the construct of

difficulty in any principled processing manner that transcends case by case convenience (Ashby

et al., 2020).

The present results represent additional failures of the unitary hypothesis. First, single

system models cannot accommodate the performance leaps that participants showed in our

conceptual tasks (Smith & Ell, 2015). Participants adopted instead insightful rule discovery.

Second, single-system accounts cannot explain the qualitative shift of task knowledge from the

tacit and behavioral to the conscious and aware. A second learning process resident in working

consciousness can explain this shift. In fact, Smith et al. (2019) showed they could disrupt this

learning process using working-memory interference in a relational task like that used in

Experiment 2. Third, a single system cannot explain the transition from concrete-perceptual to

abstract-conceptual information. It has no second register of abstract nodes overlaying perceptual

nodes. Fourth, single-system accounts cannot explain the task-selective use of declarative

language shown here. Fifth, single system accounts cannot explain why conceptual learning

selectively survives deferred reinforcement. Deferred reinforcement is a powerful block to many

kinds of associative learning (stimulus response, stimulus stimulus, stimulus reinforcement, etc,).
DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 37

Clearly, participants have swapped in some qualitatively different learning process that can

reflect on feedback dislocated in time and rearranged out of trial by trial order.

To force these qualitative results to fit a single-system model, one would have to collapse

together the implicit and the explicit, the conscious and the automatic, the reflexive and the

reflective, the verbal and the behavioral, probably the striatal and the pre-frontal, and the

reinforcement dependent and independent. By doing so, you would crush together the principal

diametrical oppositions in human cognition into one vague mass, slowing theory development

and empirical progress. Therefore, we believe it is time for the field to adopt a disciplined

multiple systems understanding of humans’ performance in discrimination, classification, and

categorization tasks.

Human Knowledge and Unitary Description

The present findings reach beyond perceptual categorization toward the literature on

category naturalness and structural constraints on category learning (e.g., Medin &

Schwanenflugel, 1981; Medin & Wattenmaker, 1987; Wattenmaker et al., 1986). This research

showed that humans are flexible category learners, sometimes succeeding in learning poorly

structured categories, XOR categories, even random categories. This research, like the present

research, showed that humans can use their knowledge (e.g., of causality, illness, and so forth) in

support of category learning. We credit this research, in no way claiming it is our distinctive

insight that humans bring conceptual knowledge to the task of categorization.

However, the focus in that area was category naturalness and the search for structural

constraints on category learnability. Research interpretations focused there. Medin and his

colleagues found that interactive-cue models let one account for many learning flexibilities. They

introduced the theoretical narrative that this kind of processing system might serve much of
DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 38

categorization. There might be no need for envisioning different categorization systems or brain

loci. All categories might be equally learnable through the process of configural cue encoding or

exemplar memorization. This theoretical narrative naturally suggested that categorization is

unitary. By this path, exemplar theory and exemplar models emerged and dominated the

literature for 25 years.

It is ironic that the research on humans’ learning flexibility was redirected to support the

unitary description. The present research helps repair this narrative. Our research shows, by

several converging cognitive dissociations, that humans recruit the knowledge structures that

transform XOR tasks by recruiting higher levels of explicit-declarative categorization. Seen in

that light, the beautiful theory- and knowledge-based work of Medin and others supports the

existence of multiple systems in categorization.

The Content of Explicit Categorization

Our research offers clues about the building blocks of explicit-conceptual categories. First,

explicit categorization is especially linked to the learning of category rules, particularly

verbalizeable category rules. Second, it is linked to category content that transcends the level of

concrete perceptual appearance. Third, it is linked to the use of higher-level relational cues, like

Same/Different. Illustrating this point, the relation of Sameness can apply equally no matter

whether color, shape, size, or any other perceptual features are shared. A perceptual associative

system can gain no traction on an appearance-transcending relation of this kind. Fourth, it is

more of an executive function, a conclusion strengthened by the demonstration that a concurrent

working-memory load interferes with relational learning (Smith et al., 2019). It seems to us that

all of these—the abstractive, the verbal, the rule-based, the relational, the executive—could be
DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 39

components of a system that builds explicitly more conceptual, theory-based concepts. Indeed,

explicit cognition could well be the preferred place for conceptual, theory-based categorization.

We are not alone in raising these possibilities. Developmental, cognitive, and comparative

psychologists have all considered explicit cognition, relational concepts, and language as closely

allied (e.g., Christic & Gentner, 2014; Grafman & Litvan, 1999; Halford et al., 2010; Hummel &

Holyoke, 2003). The possible bridges between the perceptual and conceptual level of cognitive

functioning have also been carefully analyzed (Penn et al., 2008). But, regarding categorization,

the realization of a multiple systems perspective serves an especially important role, for it knits

together two branches of the categorization literature (perceptual and theory-based

categorization) that have remained mostly separate.

Why Multiple Category Systems?

Explicit and implicit learning systems might play complementary roles in cognition. For

example, humans’ operant associative learning has strengths and weaknesses. It produces stable,

adaptive behavior shaped by contingencies. It is permastored and lasting. It is attainable by many

species, because it need not depend on selective attention, conceptual abstraction, or

consciousness. However, operant associative learning is dependent on immediate reinforcement

and extensive trial repetition. It learns slowly and incrementally. It is inertial—the unlearning of

even destructive habits can be extraordinarily difficult. Learning cannot occur off-line or with

displacement in time or space from the task’s trials. New approaches cannot be chosen, or old

approaches dropped, in a crisis. This learning steers like the Queen Mary.

Explicit learning is a perfect complement to implicit learning because it turns on a dime. It

does not depend on immediate reinforcement or event repetition. Learning can occur off-line and

with displacement. Learning and unlearning can occur suddenly at need. Explicit learning is
DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 40

superior when the organism lacks conditioned responses and trained habits. It is crucial in crises,

when the organism has to answer the question: So, what do I do now? Thus, we believe that

implicit and explicit learning may both confer behavioral fitness in their own ways, to the point

that other vertebrate lines could have evolved their own version of multiple learning systems.

Comparative studies

Accordingly, one considers an evolutionary perspective toward multiple systems. How

have these systems emerged during evolution, and in which vertebrate lines? This intriguing

evolutionary story shows once more the theoretical power of the multiple-systems description.

In the most sophisticated relational task—the Relational Matching to Sample (RMTS) task

(e.g., Smith et al., 2013), humans and some apes perform successfully. It is meaningful that the

successful apes are those who have received proto-language training (e.g. Premack, 1976;

Thompson et al., 1997). Thus “language” enters the evolutionary story. Monkeys have failed in

RMTS tasks (e.g., Smith et al., 2013) or shown glimmers of success given dogged training (e.g.,

Fagot & Thompson, 2011). It is meaningful that in Maugard et al. (2013), baboons performing

RMTS tasks were disrupted by working-memory interference as humans were in Smith et al.

(2019). Thus, executive processing enters the evolutionary story. In less complicated relational

tasks, with content like in Experiment 2, monkeys are robustly successful (e.g., Shields et al.,

1997), but not pigeons. Pigeons mainly fail in the RMTS task and in true pairwise

Same/Different paradigms (Young et al., 1997), though sometimes they can find low-level cues

like visual calmness/busyness with which to buttress their performance.

The same evolutionary story is told by the rule-based category tasks of the RB-II literature

(see Castro et al., 2020; Qadri et al., 2019; Smith, Ashby, et al., 2011; Smith et al., 2012). The
DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 41

same evolutionary story is told by the extensive comparative literature on cognitive self-

awareness or metacognition (see Zakrzewski et al., 2017, for review).

This comparative research is illuminated by the multiple-systems description. The species

progression—pigeons, monkeys, apes, humans—is explained if one acknowledges two learning

systems: a basic, operant associative-learning process shared across the species (learning

theorists have accepted this basic process for 100 years), and an explicit-conceptual system that

is also emerging, especially in the primate line (judging by the limits on current research). This is

why Church et al. (2020; also Smith & Church, 2021) proposed that the multiple-systems idea

may be the most powerful explanatory tool today in comparative psychology, for it explains

dozens of findings about humans and animals in discrimination and classification tasks.

In fact, it is an interesting question whether macaques could appreciate the conceptual

affordances of the XOR tasks adopted here. Macaques generally find XOR category structures

problematic because they must transcend perceptual appearance to put dissimilar objects together

in the same category (e.g., Smith, Coutinho, et al., 2011). However, their performance might be

facilitated if one gave them higher-level affordances as we did humans here. Of course,

macaques would have to appreciate these affordances in a languageless way, granting a test of

the necessity of language for this purpose.

Here one sees once more that the hypothesis of multiple learning systems is richly productive

of new directions for theoretical growth and empirical investigation. Thus, for many reasons,

including the new dissociative findings between explicit and implicit learning demonstrated here,

we hope this article helps foster the field’s consensus that multiple systems of category learning

probably underlie performance in humans and possibly other species, too. For we believe this
DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 42

dissociative framework has great potential to accelerate theoretical and empirical progress in our

area, just as it transformed theory and research in memory—also after a long and sharp debate.
DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 43

References

Alexander, G. E., DeLong, M. R., & Strick, P. L. (1986). Parallel organization of functionally

segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 9,

357–381. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ne.09.030186.002041

Ashby, F. G., Alfonso-Reese, L. A., Turken, A. U., & Waldron, E. M. (1998). A

neuropsychological theory of multiple systems in category learning. Psychological Review,

105, 442-481. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.105.3.442

Ashby, F. G., & Ell, S. W. (2001). The neurobiology of human category learning. Trends in

Cognitive Science, 5, 204-210. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01624-7

Ashby, F. G., & Ennis, J. M. (2006). The role of the basal ganglia in category learning. In B. H.

Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory.

(Vol. 46, pp. 1–36). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(06)46001-1

Ashby, F. G., & Maddox, W. T. (2011). Human category learning 2.0. Annals of the New York

Academy of Sciences, 1224, 147–161. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05874.x

Ashby, F. G., Smith, J. D. & Rosedahl, L. A. (2020). Dissociations between rule-based and

information-integration categorization are not caused by differences in task

difficulty. Memory & Cognition, 48, 541-552. https://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13421-019-

00988-4

Ashby, F. G., & Valentin, V. V. (2017). Multiple systems of perceptual category learning:

Theory and cognitive tests. In H. Cohen and C. Lefebvre (Eds.), Handbook of

categorization in cognitive science (2nd ed., pp. 157-188). Elsevier.

https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-08-101107-2.00007-5
DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 44

Barnes, T. D., Kubota, Y., Hu, D., Jin, D. Z., & Graybiel, A. M. (2005). Activity of striatal

neurons reflects dynamic encoding and recoding of procedural memories. Nature, 437,

1158–1161. https://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nature04053

Brooks, L. R. (1978). Nonanalytic classification and memory for instances. In E. Rosch & B.

Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization (pp. 169–211). Erlbaum.

Brown, R. G., & Marsden, C. D. (1988). Internal versus external cures and the control of

attention in Parkinson’s disease. Brain, 111, 323–345.

https://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/111.2.323

Castro, L., Savic, O., Navarro, V., Sloutsky, V. M., & Wasserman, E.A. (2020). Selective and

distributed attention in human and pigeon category learning. Cognition, 204, Epub

https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104350

Christie, S., & Gentner, D. (2014). Language helps children succeed on a classic analogy task.

Cognitive Science, 38, 383-397. https://dx.doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12099

Church, B. A. (2020, October 6). DATA Conceptual anchoring dissociates implicit and explicit

category learning. Retrieved from osf.io/ystr9. https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/YSTR9

Church, B. A., Jackson, B. N., & Smith, J. D. (2020, in press). Dissociable learning processes: A

comparative perspective. In Krause, M. Hollis, K. L. & Papini, M. R. (Eds.), Evolution of

learning and memory mechanisms. Cambridge University Press.

Fagot, J., & Thompson, R. K. R. (2011). Generalized relational matching by guinea baboons

(Papio papio) in two-by-two-item analogy problems. Psychological Science, 22, 1304-

1309. https://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797611422916

Feldman, J. (2000). Minimization of Boolean complexity in human concept learning. Nature,

407, 630–633. https://dx.doi.org/10.1038/35036586


DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 45

Fuster, J. M. (Ed.). (1989). The prefrontal cortex, (2nd ed.). Lippincott-Raven.

Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1987). Circuitry of the prefrontal cortex and the regulation of behavior by

representational knowledge. In F. Plum & V. Mountcastle (Eds.), Handbook of physiology

(pp. 373-417). American Physiological Society. https://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cphy.cp010509

Grafman, J, & Litvan, I. (1999). Importance of deficits in executive functions. Lancet, 354,

1921-1923. https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(99)90438-5

Halford, G. S., Wilson, W. H., and Phillips, S. (2010). Relational knowledge: The foundation of

higher cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 497-505.

https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.08.005

Hummel J. E., & Holyoak K. J. (2003). A symbolic-connectionist theory of relational inference

and generalization. Psychological Review, 110, 220-264. https://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-

295x.110.2.220

Knowlton, B. J., & Squire, L. R. (1993). The learning of categories: Parallel brain systems for

item memory and category knowledge. Science, 262, 1747-1749.

https://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.8259522

Knowlton, B. J., Mangels, J. A., & Squire, L. R. (1996). A neostriatal habit learning system in

humans. Science, 273, 1399–1402. https://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.273.5280.1399

Le Pelley, M. E., Newell, B. R., & Nosofsky, R. M. (2019). Deferred feedback does not

dissociate implicit and explicit category learning systems: Commentary on Smith et al.

(2014). Psychological Science, 30, 1403-1409.

https://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797619841264
DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 46

Maddox, W. T., & Ashby, F. G. (2004). Dissociating explicit and procedural-learning based

systems of perceptual category learning. Behavioural Processes, 66, 309–332.

https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2004.03.011

Maddox, W. T., Ashby, F. G., & Bohil, C. J. (2003). Delayed feedback effects on rule-based and

information-integration category learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,

Memory, and Cognition, 29, 650-662. https://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.29.4.650

Maddox, W. T., & Ing, A. D. (2005). Delayed feedback disrupts the procedural-learning system

but not the hypothesis testing system in perceptual category learning. Journal of

Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 100-107.

https://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.31.1.100

Maugard, A., Marzouki, Y., & Fagot, J. (2013). Contribution of working memory processes to

relational matching-to-sample performance in baboons (Papio papio). Journal of

Comparative Psychology, 127, 370-379. https://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0032336

Medin, D. L., & Schwanenflugel, P. J. (1981). Linear separability in classification learning.

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 7, 355–368.

https://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.7.5.355

Medin, D. L., & Wattenmaker, W. D. (1987). Category cohesiveness, theories and cognitive

archeology. In U. Neisser (Ed.), Concepts and conceptual development: Ecological and

intellectual factors in categorization (pp. 25-62). Cambridge University Press.

Mishkin, M., Malamut, B., & Bachevalier, J. (1984). Memories and habits: Two neural systems.

In G. Lynch, J. L. McGaugh, & N. M. Weinberger (Eds.), Neurobiology of human learning

and memory (pp. 65–88). The Guilford Press.

Murphy, G. L. (2002). The big book of concepts. MIT Press.


DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 47

Murphy, G. L., & Medin, D. L. (1985). The role of theories in conceptual coherence.

Psychological Review, 92, 289–316. https://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.92.3.289

Newell, B. R., Dunn, J. C., Kalish, M. (2010). The dimensionality of perceptual category

learning: A state-trace analysis. Memory & Cognition, 38, 563-81.

https://dx.doi.org/10.3758/MC.38.5.563

Nosofsky, R. M. (1986). Attention, similarity, and the identification-categorization relationship.

Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115, 39-57.

https://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0096-3445.115.1.39

Nosofsky, R. M., Gluck, M. A., Palmeri, T. J., McKinley, S. C., & Glauthier, P. (1994).

Comparing models of rule-based classification learning: A replication and extension of

Shepard, Hovland, and Jenkins (1961). Memory and Cognition, 22, 352-369.

https://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03200862

Nosofsky, R. M., Stanton, R. D., & Zaki, S. R. (2005). Procedural interference in perceptual

classification: Implicit learning or cognitive complexity? Memory & Cognition, 33, 1256-

1271. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/bf03193227

O’Doherty, J., Dayan, P., Schultz, J., Deichmann, R., Friston, K., & Dolan, R. J. (2004).

Dissociable roles of ventral and dorsal striatum in instrumental conditioning. Science,

304, 452–454. https://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1094285

Penn, D. C., Holyoak, K. J., & Povinelli, D. J. (2008). Darwin’s mistake: Explaining the

discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds. Behavioral Brain Sciences, 31, 109-

178. https://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x08003543
DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 48

Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The Attention systems in the human brain. Annual

Review of Neuroscience, 13, 25–42.

https://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ne.13.030190.000325

Premack, D. (1976). Intelligence in ape and man. Lawrence Erlbaum.

Qadri, M. A. J., Ashby, F. G., Smith, J. D., & Cook, R. G. (2019). Testing analogical rule

transfer in pigeons (Columba livia) and humans (Homo sapiens). Cognition, 183, 256-268.

https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.11.011

Robinson, A. L., Heaton, R. K., Lehman, R. A., & Stilson, D. W. (1980). The utility of the

Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in detecting and localizing frontal lobe lesions. Journal of

Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 48, 605–614. https://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-

006X.48.5.605

Rolls, E. T. (1994). Neurophysiology and cognitive functions of the striatum. Revue

Neurologique, 150, 648–660.

Seger, C. A., & Cincotta, C. M. (2005). The roles of the caudate nucleus in human classification

learning. Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 2941–2951.

https://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3401-04.2005

Seger, C A., & Miller, E. K. (2010). Category learning in the brain. Annual Review of

Neuroscience. 33, 203-219. https://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.051508.135546

Shepard, R. N., Hovland, C. I., & Jenkins, H. M. (1961). Learning and memorization of

classifications. Psychological Monograph, 75, 1-42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0093825

Shields, W. E., Smith, J. D., & Washburn, D. A. (1997). Uncertain responses by humans and

rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) in a psychophysical same-different task. Journal of


DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 49

Experimental Psychology: General, 126, 147-164. https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0010-

0277(96)00726-3

Smith, J. D., Ashby, F. G., Berg, M. E., Murphy, M. S., Spiering, B., Cook, R. G., & Grace, R.

C. (2011). Pigeons’ categorization may be exclusively nonanalytic. Psychonomic Bulletin &

Review, 18, 414-421. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13423-010-0047-8

Smith, J. D., Berg, M. E., Cook, R. G., Boomer, J., Crossley, M. J., Murphy, M. S., Spiering, B.,

Beran, M. J., Church, B. A., Ashby, F. G., & Grace. R. C. (2012). Implicit and explicit

categorization: A tale of four species. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36, 2355-

2369. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.09.003

Smith, J. D., Boomer, J., Zakrzewski, A. C., Roeder, J., Church, B. A., & Ashby, F. G. (2014).

Deferred feedback sharply dissociates implicit and explicit category learning. Psychological

Science, 25, 447-457. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797613509112

Smith, J. D. & Church, B. A. (2021). A dissociative framework for understanding same-different

conceptualization. Current Opinion in Behavioral Science, 37, 13-18.

https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2020.06.004

Smith, J. D. Coutinho, M. V. C., & Couchman, J. J., (2011). The learning of exclusive-or

categories by monkeys (Macaca mulatta) and humans (Homo sapiens). Journal of

Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 37, 20-29.

https://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0019497

Smith, J. D., & Ell, S. (2015). One giant leap for categorizers: One small step for categorization

theory. PLOS One, 23. https://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0137334


DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 50

Smith, J. D., Flemming, T. M., Boomer, J., Beran, M. J., & Church B. A. (2013). Fading

perceptual resemblance: A path for rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) to conceptual

matching? Cognition, 129, 1598-1614. https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.08.001

Smith, J. D., Jamani, S., Boomer, J., & Church, B. A. (2018). One-back reinforcement

dissociates implicit-procedural and explicit-declarative category learning. Memory &

Cognition, 46, 261–273. https://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13421-017-0762-8

Smith, J. D., Jackson, B. N., & Church, B. A. (2019). Breaking the perceptual-conceptual barrier:

Relational matching and explicit cognition. Memory & Cognition, 47, 544-560.

https://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13421-018-0890-9

Smith, J. D., Minda, J. P., & Washburn, D. A. (2004). Category learning in rhesus monkeys: A

study of the Shepard, Hovland, and Jenkins tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology:

General, 133, 398-414. https://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.133.3.398

Smith, J. D., Redford, J. S., & Haas, S. M. (2008). Prototype abstraction by monkeys (Macaca

mulatta). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137, 390–401.

https://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.137.2.390

Smith, J. D., Zakrzewski, A. C., Johnson, J. M., Valleau, J. C., & Church, B. A. (2016).

Categorization: The view from animal cognition. Behavioral Sciences, 6, 12.

http://dx.doi.org/10.3390/bs6020012

Thompson, R. K. R., Oden, D. L., & Boysen, S. T. (1997). Language naïve chimpanzees (Pan

troglodytes) judge relations between relations in a conceptual matching-to-sample task.

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 23, 31–43.

https://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.23.1.31
DISSOCIATING IMPLICIT AND EXPLICIT CATEGORY LEARNING 51

Wattenmaker, W. D., Dewey, G. I., Murphy, T. D., & Medin, D. L. (1986). Linear separability

and concept learning: Context, relational properties and concept naturalness. Cognitive

Psychology, 18, 158-194. https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(86)90011-3

Wickens, J. (1993). A theory of the striatum. Pergamon Press.

Young M. E., Wasserman E. A., & Garner, K. L. (1997). Effects of number of items on the

pigeon's discrimination of same from different visual displays. Journal of Experimental

Psychology: Behavior Processes, 23, 491-501. https://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0097-

7403.23.4.491

Zakrzewski, A. C., Johnson, J. M., & Smith, J. D. (2017). The comparative psychology of

metacognition. In J. Call, G. M. Burghardt, I. M. Pepperberg, C. T. Snowdon, & T. Zentall

(Eds.), APA Handbook of Comparative Psychology: Perception, Learning, and Cognition

(pp. 703-721). American Psychological Association.

You might also like