YanSchuetzeEglington 2020 Preprint

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 40

A Review of the Interleaving Effect: Theories and Lessons for Future Research

Veronica X. Yan

Department of Educational Psychology

The University of Texas at Austin

Brendan A. Schuetze

Department of Educational Psychology

The University of Texas at Austin

Luke G. Eglington

Institute for Intelligent Systems

University of Memphis

Author note: Portions of this research were presented at the 60th annual meeting of the

Psychonomic Society in Montreal, Canada. We thank Matthan Moy and Kimberly Nguyen for

their assistance in coding.

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Veronica X. Yan, Department of

Educational Psychology, The University of Texas at Austin, 1912 Speedway STE 504, Austin

TX 78712. Email: [email protected]


TWO-STAGE FRAMEWORK OF SEQUENCING EFFECTS 1

Abstract

Numerous studies have shown that an interleaved study sequence of examples (e.g.,

ABCBACACB) from different categories, relative to a blocked sequence (e.g., AAABBBCCC),

often yields superior category learning. Some explanations for sequencing effects centers on

attentional processes, others focus on memory processes, and the two are often pitted against

each other. We propose a new integrative two-stage framework for sequencing effects in

category-learning and support this framework using a meta-analytic approach. We show, using a

meta-analytic approach, that a combination of memory and attentional predictors explains

significantly more variance in sequencing effects than attentional factors alone. This approach

also allowed us to examine the nature of the existing evidence, which revealed inferential

limitations due to how researchers typically design experiments. We provide suggestions for

future research on sequencing effects in category learning that would both test the two-stage

framework and increase the impact of future research.

Keywords: interleaving, spacing, sequencing effects, category learning, theoretical framework


TWO-STAGE FRAMEWORK OF SEQUENCING EFFECTS 2

A Review of the Interleaving Effect: Theories and Lessons for Future Research

In our everyday lives, the need to make rapid decisions is supported by our fundamental

capacity to categorize the world around us (e.g., objects, people, experiences, actions). While

there are many different approaches researchers have taken to examine what processes are

involved in category learning, an emerging body of literature has focused on how the study and

practice of multiple categories should be sequenced. This research has revealed a strikingly

counterintuitive finding: instead of focusing on learning one category at a time (referred to as

‘blocking’), it can be better to mix up and switch between different categories (referred to as

‘interleaving’). In the present paper, we conduct both a critical review and a quantitative review

of the interleaving effect in category learning. First, we review the existing theories, finding that

they largely fall into one of two categories: those relating to attention and those relating to

memory. Next, we propose a new two-stage framework (attention, then memory) that integrates

these existing theories. Using a meta-analytic approach, we examine the extent to which the

existing literature is able to test this two-stage framework. We show that including both

attentional and memory factors explained variance in the interleaving effect better than

attentional factors alone. However, as we also discovered, large gaps and confounded

experimental designs in the category literature preclude strong tests of interactions that would be

predicted from our new integrative two-stage model. We conclude with suggestions for future

directions in the study of applied category learning.

The Interleaving Effect

We often think that to be a diligent, successful learner, we must focus, single-mindedly,

on one thing at a time. In education, this assumption is manifest in different ways. For example,
TWO-STAGE FRAMEWORK OF SEQUENCING EFFECTS 3

the concepts in our textbooks are blocked chapter by chapter, homework or practice worksheets

often focus on one problem type at a time, and accelerated learning programs where students

take just one short, intensive course at a time are gaining in popularity (e.g., intensive coding

“bootcamps” like HackReactor, or Colorado College’s “Block Plan” in which students only take

one class at a time, five days per week for three and a half weeks). One of the more

counterintuitive findings in the cognitive psychology literature, however, has been that

interleaving study and practice of different concepts can benefit long-term learning1. The

interleaving effect refers to the finding in the cognitive psychology literature that alternating

between different concepts can benefit learning more than focusing on one concept at a time.

One important initial step is to establish whether a particular effect exists and to

understand whether the effect is generalizable beyond the initially studied context. A recent

meta-analysis of the interleaving effect (Brunmair & Richter, 2019) found that there was a

significant overall effect of interleaving (Hedges g = .42). They also found, however, significant

heterogeneity of the effect size. Separating the existing literature by stimuli sets (paintings,

naturalistic photos, artificial images, math-related tasks, expository texts, words, and tastes), the

effect appeared reliable except for in the case of expository texts and words. As an instructional

practice, interleaving promises huge potential for transforming learning. However, it is important

to understand underlying mechanisms and boundary conditions before scaling to practice.

Therefore, while documenting generalizability of an effect is important, an arguably more

important goal is to be able to understand the processes that give rise to an effect and how they

1
While recently popular in the educationally-relevant cognitive literature as it relates to concept
and category learning, the idea that intermixing training confers benefits is not new. See
contextual interference from early verbal learning research (Battig, 1966, 1978) and from motor
skills training research (Lee, 1992; Shea & Morgan, 1979) and catastrophic interference from
connectionist network research (Maclelland, McNaughton & O’Reilly, 1995; McCloskey &
Cohen, 1989).
TWO-STAGE FRAMEWORK OF SEQUENCING EFFECTS 4

relate to the heterogeneity of an effect. Understanding the underlying mechanisms not only allow

researchers to better predict the boundaries of generalizability, but they can also reveal more

fundamental processes of human learning.

An Emphasis on Underlying Processes

While there are a surprising number of effects that generalize across both verbal and

motor skills learning tasks, the extant literature has been careful to separate discussion of motor

skills and verbal tasks (e.g., Soderstrom and Bjork, 2015; Seger & Miller, 2010). Less care,

however, has been taken with separating out the processes that underlie educationally-relevant

cognitive (i.e., non-motor) tasks. Different educationally-relevant tasks, however, also involve

different types of learning processes. For example, differentiating cancerous cells from non-

cancerous cells requires the ability to recognize and categorize different types of cells. A failure

to correctly identify cells might be linked to a problem of discrimination and categorization. In

contrast, being able to apply the appropriate mathematics formula to a particular mathematics

problem requires that learners are not only able to recognize and categorize the type of problem

that is presented, but also to be able to retrieve the correct procedure or formula for that type of

problem, and to be able to correctly implement the procedural steps in the appropriate order. A

failure to correctly solve a mathematics problem could be due to a problem of discrimination and

categorization, but it could also be a problem with retrieval or implementation.

Brunmair and Richter (2019) chose not to include motor learning studies, correctly, in

our view, as the evidence for dissociations between inductive conceptual learning and procedural

learning is strong enough to warrant separate analyses. Nevertheless, Brunmair and Richter also

included several tasks outside of the focus of our present review. In particular, their inclusion of

tasks involving taste memory is not mirrored in the inclusion standards of the present meta-
TWO-STAGE FRAMEWORK OF SEQUENCING EFFECTS 5

analysis, as taste memory most likely evolved to meet a set of specialized evolutionary

constraints — for example maximizing enjoyment and nutrition, while maintaining aversion to

toxins — distinct from the functions served and brain regions instantiated by other memorial

processes (Lin, Arthurs, & Reilly, 2017; Núñez-Jaramill et al., 2010; Palmerino, Rusiniak, &

Garcia, 1980). They also grouped stimuli by surface features rather than by underlying processes.

For example, under mathematical tasks, they combined both studies in which category

discrimination is emphasized (e.g., being able to recognize when different statistical tests are

appropriate, Sana et al., 2017) with studies which also require procedural skills such as problem

solving (Rohrer et al., 2014). Finally, they included some studies that were about memory rather

than about category learning (e.g., Hausman & Kornell, 2014), or in which blocked and

interleaved schedules were not matched (e.g. studies that used adaptive algorithms, Rau et al.,

2010).

Attention-based Accounts of the Interleaving Effect

In Brunmair and Richter (2019), the authors examined potential moderators of attention-

based accounts of the interleaving effect: namely, the discrimination hypothesis (that interleaving

enhances attention to features that discriminate between different categories, Kang & Pashler,

2012; Kornell & Bjork, 2008) and its extension, the sequential attention hypothesis (that

interleaving draws attention to between-category differences and that blocking draws attention to

within-category similarities; Carvalho & Goldstone, 2015, 2017). The discrimination hypothesis

argues that the benefit of interleaving examples from different categories is that the juxtaposition

of these different categories highlights the differences between them. Interleaving enhances

discriminative contrasts, blocking does not. The sequential attention theory (Carvalho &

Goldstone, 2017) extends the discrimination hypothesis, positing that the sequence in which
TWO-STAGE FRAMEWORK OF SEQUENCING EFFECTS 6

examples are studied influences the features on which a learner focuses their attention: Under

interleaved sequences, where examples from different categories are juxtaposed, learners'

attention is drawn to the features that help discriminate between the different categories (as in

discriminative contrast theory). Under blocked sequences, where different examples from the

same category are juxtaposed, learners' attention is drawn to the common features that define the

category.

These attention-based theories hold implications for the types of moderators that would

be important. Namely the moderators that have been identified include category similarity,

temporal spacing, temporal juxtaposition and active (vs. passive) learning. Category similarity is

thought to matter because the more similar the to-be-learned categories are to each other, the

greater the interleaving benefit should be; in cases where the need for between-category

discrimination is low or trivial, a blocking benefit might be obtained. Indeed this pattern of

results is demonstrated in Carvalho and Goldstone (2014), who showed that high-similarity

categories were better learned when interleaved while low-similarity categories (where within-

category commonalities were also more difficult to notice) were better learned when blocked.

Temporal spacing (immediately sequential presentations versus spaced presentations) and

temporal juxtaposition (simultaneous versus sequential presentations) are thought to matter

because these manipulations either make it easier or more difficult for learners to attend to the

important features (Birnbaum et al., 2013; Carvalho & Goldstone, 2014; Kang & Pashler, 2012;

Sana et al., 2017; Zulkiply & Burt, 2013): simultaneous presentations are more likely facilitate

compare and contrast processes than sequential presentations, which in turn is more likely to

facilitate these processes than spaced presentations. Finally, another corollary of the attentional

bias hypothesis is that sequence effects should be larger under conditions of active learning than
TWO-STAGE FRAMEWORK OF SEQUENCING EFFECTS 7

under conditions of passive learning, as under active learning, participants may be more likely to

actively search for the key distinguishing features that discriminate between categories. On the

other hand, during a passive learning task, participants might try to create a “positive

characterization of each category” (Carvalho & Goldstone, 2015, p. 282), and hence benefit from

blocked sequencing.

Memory-based Accounts of the Interleaving Effect

Importantly however, there are also memory-based accounts of the interleaving effect

that are missing from Brunmair and Richter’s analyses. In fact, the last decade of interest in the

interleaving effect arose as an extension of the spacing effect. The spacing effect (Carpenter,

2017; Cepeda et al., 2006) is one of the most robust effects in the cognitive psychology

literature, and is the finding that distributing repetitions over time leads to better learning than

massing those repetitions. Kornell and Bjork (2008) initially designed their experiments to be an

illustration of a situation in which spacing learning out over time should not be beneficial for

learning, hypothesizing that when category exemplars were spaced, it would make it difficult to

identify commonalities and abstract the categorical information. Instead—to their surprise—the

results revealed that spacing (or, what the literature now refers to as interleaving) category

exemplars enhanced category induction.

Spacing is inherent to interleaved schedules: under interleaved schedules, examples from

a given category are inherently distributed from each other. Spacing theories of interleaving

extend the well-known benefits of spacing repetitions for long-term memory to the learning of

categories, and predict that there should be an optimal level of spacing or difficulty (Cepeda et

al., 2008). The study-phase retrieval hypothesis (Appleton-Knapp, Bjork, & Wickens, 2005;

Birnbaum, Kornell, Bjork, & Bjork, 2013; Thios & D’Agostino, 1976), for example, assumes
TWO-STAGE FRAMEWORK OF SEQUENCING EFFECTS 8

that prior presentations are retrieved and elaborated on at the time of subsequent presentations

(see also ‘reminding theory’ of spacing, Benjamin & Ross, 2011). In other words, spacing out

examples from a given category promotes retrieval or reminding of prior examples, which in turn

strengthens learning; in contrast, blocking presentation of examples from the same category does

not afford the forgetting that engages subsequent retrieval processes. Similarly, the retrieval

effort hypothesis (Pyc & Rawson, 2009) posits that the more effortful (but still successful)

retrieval promoted by distributing examples leads to deeper processing. In massed or blocked

sequences, the lack of spacing between presentations means that there is no need for retrieval or

elaboration, and hence, no memorial benefit. When intervals are too long between examples

from the same category, retrieval processes may fail, resulting in no additional learning benefit

(Appleton-Knapp, Bjork, & Wickens, 2005). What constitutes “too long,” however, can be

difficult to pinpoint. A related theory, the forgetting-as-abstraction hypothesis (Vlach, 2014),

extends the study-phase retrieval theory for category learning: intervals between presentations of

examples from the same category allow the learners to forget the category-irrelevant details that

are specific to individual examples and to consolidate only the category-relevant details that are

present across category examples (Vlach, 2014; Vlach & Kalish, 2014).

These memory-based theories also hold implications for the types of moderators that

would be important. Namely, that the effect may be moderated by the number of categories or

the number of study examples and retention interval between end of study and final test. Spacing

accounts predict that increasing the spacing between examples from a given category should

improve learning. Birnbaum et al. (2013) tested this theory directly, by varying the average

number of examples that intervened between two examples from the same category, while

holding discrimination variability constant. They found that large spacing (15 intervening
TWO-STAGE FRAMEWORK OF SEQUENCING EFFECTS 9

examples) led to better learning than did small spacing (three intervening examples). Most

studies have not manipulated spacing in this way, but rather tend to interleave all categories

together, which means that the number of categories can be used as a proxy for the spacing

between stimuli from the same category. Memory-based theories would also imply that other

important moderators include the number of presentations (i.e., examples or repetitions — the

more repetitions, the larger the interleaving effect; Pavlik & Anderson, 2005; Shaughnessy,

Zimmerman & Underwood, 1972; Underwood, 1970) and the retention interval (spacing effects

should become stronger with at a delayed test compared to an immediate test; Cepeda et al,

2008; Rawson & Kintsch, 2005).

A Two-stage Framework of Sequencing Effects

Although discrimination (i.e. attention-based) and spacing (i.e., memory-based)

hypotheses have often been pitted against each other in the literature (e.g., Foster et al., 2019;

Kang & Pashler, 2012), the likely reality is that they are not mutually exclusive. We argue that

the two types of processes are not competing, but rather sequential. That is, drawing from

classical models of memory (e.g., multi-store model, Atkinson & Shiffrin, 1968), it is likely that

sequencing effects influence category learning in two stages: First, an attention-based stage of

category discovery in which attention must be drawn to the relevant features. In this stage,

sequencing effects are likely determined by category structure and similarity. Second, a memory-

based stage in which learners need to memorize the cluster of features and the association

between features and category labels: sometimes interleaving may be more beneficial; other

times blocking may be more beneficial. In this stage, sequencing effects are likely to follow the

same patterns as spaced learning effects in memory. Here, interleaving should be more

beneficial. Finally, attention and memory processes are likely cyclical throughout the process of
TWO-STAGE FRAMEWORK OF SEQUENCING EFFECTS 10

learning: The features that we attend to are more likely to be encoded into our memories, and our

memories of past experiences likely shape how we attend to future stimuli (Kim & Rehder,

2011).

Figure 1 is a schematic that illustrates some of the ways in which blocking or interleaving

benefits may arise from this two-stage framework. What this schematic also illustrates is why the

two-stage framework would make different predictions and situates different findings in the

literature. In particular, the schematic illustrates the problem with pitting attention-based theories

against memory-based theories. For example, the effect of inserting fillers in between to-be-

studied category examples is often cited as evidence in favor of the attention-based theories

(discriminative contrast or sequential attention theory) and as evidence against the memory-

based theories. These studies show that if spacing disrupts juxtaposition and hence, contrastive

processes, between examples from different categories (e.g., inserting trivia question fillers

between study examples), then the interleaving benefit is eliminated (Birnbaum et al., 2013;

Sana, Yan, & Kim, 2017). Situated within the two-stage framework however, these results

support the role of attention-based processes but do not constitute evidence against memory-

based theories. Rather, if learning is gated at the attentional stage, then we would not expect

spacing benefits to emerge at the subsequent memory stage either. In fact, in a different study,

Birnbaum et al. (2013) found that when learning is not gated at the attentional stage (i.e., fillers

were not inserted to disrupt discriminative contrasts between category examples), larger lags

between examples of a given category leads to greater learning. This benefit cannot be attributed

to an attentional process as in this study, as the number of discriminative contrasts afforded to

the learner were the same between the short-lag condition and the long-lag condition; rather the

benefit is attributed to memory processes.


TWO-STAGE FRAMEWORK OF SEQUENCING EFFECTS 11

Conversely, if the to-be-learned categories are each distinct enough and directing

learners’ attention to the shared and discriminating features is unnecessary, then we might expect

to find only benefits spaced (interleaved) practice on memory. Foster and colleagues (2019), for

example, asked participants to practice four types of mathematics problems in an interleaved or

blocked sequence. They chose a “critical” problem type and then varied whether the other three

problem types were similar or dissimilar to the critical problem type. They found that the

interleaving benefit was the same regardless of category set and argued that this supported the

distributed-practice hypothesis over the discriminative contrast hypothesis. We interpret the

results differently in our two-stage framework: For the similar set in which the categories were

required discrmination, we propose that interleaving benefited learning at the attentional stage

(as well as potentially the memory stage); for the dissimilar set in which categories were distinct,

we propose that the attentional stage was unnecessary and hence, we see only benefits of spacing

at the memory stage. That is, the same outcome—better performance following interleaved

practice—could be attained via different pathways.

The implication of this two-stage framework is that researchers should pay more attention

to the specific processes that might be required of any given type of learning content. For the

purposes of conducting a meta-analysis of existing sequencing research, it also points to coding

for memory-related experimental features and possible interactions. With a more complex model

guiding predictions of sequencing effects, this raises another challenge for conducting a meta-

analysis: It raises the need for a greater sampling of the experimental space. The goals of this

present paper are therefore two-fold: (a) to examine both attention-based and memory-based

moderators of the interleaving effect and (b) to identify the nature of existing studies and to the

gaps that are currently missing in the literature. We begin by conducting a quantitative review.
TWO-STAGE FRAMEWORK OF SEQUENCING EFFECTS 12

Method

As an exploratory step to understanding the simultaneous contributions of attentional and

memory processes, we conducted a meta-analysis of the interleaving effect. We defined our

parameters differently to those of Brunmair and Richter (2019) to focus more closely on the

underlying processes and limiting our analysis only to studies that tested category learning. Most

importantly, while Brunmair and Richter (2019) coded only moderators that tested attention-

based accounts, we also coded moderators that test memory-based accounts (see Tables S1 and

S2 in Supplemental Online Materials for a summary of the moderators, the number of effect

sizes, the sample size and the average effect sizes for each level of the moderators).

To keep the focus on our theoretical and methodological arguments about future

directions for research, below, we support these arguments through the reporting of key meta-

analytic findings, particularly in the form of reporting overall effects and multiple regression

model comparison. Our method was pre-registered (https://osf.io/gsvu6/) and the full details of

our meta-analysis method and the detailed results are reported in the Supplemental Online

Materials. In other words, the main contribution of this review is not the specific outcome of the

systematic quantitative review per se, but rather how the gaps can inform future research.

Results

Overall Interleaving Effect Size

A random effects model computed with the metafor R package (Viechtbauer, 2010; R

Core Team, 2018) found that the overall effect size of the interleaving effect size, nesting effects

(k = 205) within publications (k = 61), was significantly larger than zero, g = 0.44, 95% CI =

[.34, .53], p < .001. This effect size is very similar to that found in Brunmair and Richter (2019),
TWO-STAGE FRAMEWORK OF SEQUENCING EFFECTS 13

g = 0.42. It is important to remember that the overall effect size is computed with all studies,

including those that would be predicted to find a benefit of blocking. Unsurprisingly, we also

found a significant heterogeneity of effect sizes, Q(204) = 1213.03, p < .001. From our

univariate random effects model, we calculated I2 = 86.70, indicating 86.70 percent of the

variance in outcomes was due heterogeneity (Viechtbauer, 2019). The forest plot is presented in

Figure S1 in the Supplemental Online Materials, and the funnel plots are presented in Figure S2.

We found no evidence of publication bias (see Supplemental Online Materials for full details).

Multiple Regression Model Comparison

There were substantial correlations between the different moderators that we coded in

for our meta-analysis. The Cramer’s V correlations are reported in the correlation matrix depicted

in Figure 2. Due to concerns with the confounded nature of moderators assessed in this meta-

analysis, it would be inappropriate to rely on single moderator regression models, as these could

be indicative of significant, yet spurious, relationships (Lipsey, 2003)2.

We built four nested mixed-effects regression models predicting the benefit of

interleaving relative to blocking using the R (R Core Team, 2018) package metafor (Viechtbauer,

2010). These models were: 1. Experimental (atheoretical); 2 Attention-based Factors; 3.

Combined Memory-based and Attention-based Factors; and 4. A model incorporating the

number of categories by similarity interaction term. Random effects were composed of

experiments nested within paper. We report the relative fits of these models using likelihood

tests, AIC, and BIC, when appropriate. These multiple regression analyses are informed by the

2
Although separate tests of individual moderators may be significant, significance does not
necessarily mean that the moderators, themselves, explain unique variance. Given the correlated
nature of many of the moderators of interest, any single moderator model could actually reflect
the contributions of multiple, correlated moderators.
TWO-STAGE FRAMEWORK OF SEQUENCING EFFECTS 14

predominant theories of the effects of interleaving, however, they were not pre-registered, as we

had not anticipated the high amount of confounding seen in the present dataset, which precluded

single-moderator analysis. The results of each model and their fit indices are presented in Table

1.

Attention and Memory Factors: Model Comparison

Comparing the fit between Model 1 and Model 2 allows us to examine the contribution of

the attention-based factors. Consistent with the findings from Brunmair and Richter (2019),

incorporating attention-based factors (Model 2) led to a significantly better fit than incorporating

only experimental factors (Model 1), χ2 = 32.95, df = 4, p < .001. Comparing the fit between

Model 2 and Model 3 allows us to examine whether adding memory-related factors adds to

variance explained, over and above the attentional factors. Overall, the best fitting model of the

present meta-analytic data was Model 3, representing the combined contributions of memory-

and attention-implicated moderators. Likelihood tests found that Model 3 fit better than Model 2,

χ2 = 21.00, df = 2, p < .001. BIC and AIC also favored Model 3.

Memory-Attention Interaction

We pre-registered that we would explore two interactions. One was the interaction between the

number of categories and supervised/unsupervised learning — however, there were not enough

studies in which unsupervised learning was used. The other interaction we pre-registered was an

interaction between the number of categories and similarity.

In our meta-analysis, we treat the number of categories as a proxy for spacing between

examples from the same category. The two-stage framework would predict that benefits of

increased number of categories (a process that benefits memory) would be most apparent for
TWO-STAGE FRAMEWORK OF SEQUENCING EFFECTS 15

stimuli sets that are dissimilar, as those would not benefit from attention processes. We included

this one interaction term in addition to the predictors of Model 3. We found that this interaction

term was significant (b = 0.03, SE = .01, p = .017), indicating that the slope representing the

benefit of the number of categories for the interleaving effect was steeper for dissimilar items3.

This interaction is depicted in Figure 3. In other words, in support of dual processes of memory

and attention, when the number of categories being studied increases, we see that the interleaving

effect grows at a faster rate when the items are dissimilar. We did not explore any other potential

interactions, however, due to limitations of the existing studies, which we describe in further

detail in the next section.

Discussion

Limitations of the Extant Empirical Literature

Our quantitative review of the existing literature provided evidence that sequencing has a

significant and multifaceted effect on category learning. On average, interleaving is beneficial,

but that average is highly misleading. This basic finding is aligned with the findings of Brunmair

and Richter (2019). What we additionally found—different from that of prior reviews—is that a

model in which memory factors are added on top of attentional factors better accounts for the

variance than attentional factors alone. In the midst of the present quantitative review,

particularly as the result of analyzing our meta-analytic dataset, we realized that certain troubling

patterns in the predominant manners of conducting interleaving experiments resulted in a far

3
This model with the interaction term, however, was not significantly different from Model 3, χ 2
= 5.63, df = 2, p = .059.
TWO-STAGE FRAMEWORK OF SEQUENCING EFFECTS 16

lower level of generalizability of our findings than we had initially anticipated. We detail three

core problems. First, a large proportion of the empirical literature uses the same or very similar

experimental paradigms. Second, there is a serious problem of confounded moderators (Lipsey,

2003). Third, we find evidence of restricted ranges of moderators. That is, apart from the strong

correlations between moderators of interest, many moderators have not been experimentally

examined across their entire possible range of values. Each of these issues limits our ability to

disambiguate the true effects of these moderators with implications for furthering theoretical

understanding and practical generalization. In the following sections, we detail each of these

issues.

Proliferation of a Single Paradigm

One particular paradigm represented a large proportion of the effect sizes. Specifically,

36 out of 100 of the published effect sizes and 89 out of 209 of the total (published and

unpublished) effect sizes used paintings stimuli and passive study. Moreover, when painting

stimuli were used, the number of categories was usually 12 (k = 50). In the interest of full

disclosure, many of these were associated with the first author of this present paper (k = 37). In

other words, experimental design seems to be biased in the distribution of underlying

experimental variable values. Similar to how many participants are sampled from a convenient

source (college psychology courses), there is a tendency towards convenience sampling of

experimental designs. We do not believe that this tendency is unique to a particular researcher,

nor to the interleaving literature; rather this is likely a typical feature of most empirical

psychology research. Indeed, it is arguably a rigorous way of proceeding with research: When an

effect is first described using a particular paradigm, it is important to replicate it as closely as

possible (Pashler & Harris, 2012). Retaining the same paradigm is also then important for testing
TWO-STAGE FRAMEWORK OF SEQUENCING EFFECTS 17

underlying processes: researchers can systematically change one experimental feature at a time,

testing whether that change yields differences in the experimental outcome. If research were not

paradigmatic in this way, it would be difficult to isolate why an outcome has been amplified or

attenuated. Yet, such a paradigmatic focus becomes problematic for generalization and situating

effects in a broader understanding of learning processes. Moreover, as we found in our review,

researchers rarely explore the experimental design space in a systematic fashion.

Confounded Moderators Leave Gaps in the Evidence Base

One of the consequences of having a bias in experimental design is that potentially

important moderators may become confounded within the literature space. Although the two-

stage framework makes predictions about interactions, we were limited in our ability to explore

these in our quantitative review as a result of confounded moderators and non-systematic

manipulation of important variables (e.g., number of categories, active or passive participation).

Confounded moderators limit the conclusions that can be drawn broadly concerning the

relative importance of likely explanatory factors. Figure 2 presents a Cramer’s V pairwise

correlation matrix between our moderators. Cramer’s V is a non-parametric measure of

association based upon the chi-squared statistic, which measures the association between

nominal variables and always results in a value between 0 and 1 (there are no negative Cramer’s

V statistics; hence it is non-directional). Two advantages of using Cramer's V measure is that it is

better at dealing with categorical variables (as most of our moderators are) and does not assume

linear relationships. Generally, V values between 0.1 and 0.3 are considered small effect sizes,

between 0.3 and 0.5 considered medium, and greater than 0.5 considered large (Osteen & Bright,

2010; Kotrlik, Williams, & Jabor, 2011). As this matrix shows, the associations between these

variables and others are extremely high, precluding clear interpretation in several cases. Of the
TWO-STAGE FRAMEWORK OF SEQUENCING EFFECTS 18

66 correlations reported in Figure 2, 42% were of medium size (Vs between 0.3 and 0.5) and

15% were large (Vs greater than or equal to 0.5). This high correlation between moderators is a

known problem in meta-analysis (e.g., Lipsey, 2003), and we implemented one of the proposed

solutions (multivariate analysis). However, the broader problem is that the data available to go

into the meta-analysis is consistently confounded.

In Figure 4, we provide four examples that further illustrate the issue with confounded

moderators. Each panel in Figure 4 displays a combination of three moderators, with each point

in the plot representing an effect size within our meta-analysis dataset. These four panels are not

a randomly selected set of moderators; rather, we selected combinations of moderators that are

relevant to the two-stage framework. Panel A plots a key moderator within attention-based

hypotheses (similarity of the categories) against a key moderator within the memory-based

hypotheses (number of categories), by stimuli type (visual or cognitive)—this represents the

interaction that we tested in our meta-analysis. Although we did find that this interaction was

significant, what is immediately apparent from Panel A is that (a) very few studies have used

more than 8 or 12 categories; and (b) experiments using cognitive stimuli are particularly ill-

represented across the range of number of categories—rather, most of the studies using cognitive

stimuli present participants with only between 2-4 categories.

The gaps in the evidence base are more apparent in the other three panels: Panel B

reveals that no interleaving studies using cognitive stimuli has presented examples

simultaneously, even though there is a large body of research on category learning on

simultaneous versus sequential study (e.g., Birnbaum et al., 2013; Carvalho & Goldstone, 2014;

Kang & Pashler, 2012). Panels C and D of Figure 4 both point to the insufficiency of existing

studies to test certain predicted interactions of the two-stage framework: the interaction between
TWO-STAGE FRAMEWORK OF SEQUENCING EFFECTS 19

category similarity and presence of interfiller trials, and the interaction between category

similarity and presentation simultaneity. If to-be-learned categories are similar and difficult to

discriminate, then learners’ attention to the critical distinguishing features will be hindered if an

intertrial filler is inserted. Hence, we would expect an intertrial filler to attenuate the interleaving

effect when categories are similar. On the other hand, if the to-be-learned categories are

dissimilar, then we would expect intertrial fillers to amplify the interleaving effect via memory

processes. Similarly, when categories are similar, category learning will benefit primarily from

attentional processes. Interleaving under both simultaneous and sequential presentation allows

learners to attend to the features that distinguish categories, and hence an interleaving benefit

should emerge in both presentation modes. When categories are dissimilar, the need for

attentional processes is low and hence, category learning will benefit primarily from memory

processes. Interleaving under simultaneous presentation is much less likely to engage retrieval or

memory processes and hence, an interleaving benefit should emerge only under sequential

presentation. As Panels C and D show, however, there were almost no studies in which

experimenters tested the effect of sequencing on the learning of dissimilar categories with inter-

trial fillers and there was not a single study in which categories were dissimilar and

simultaneously presented.

Issues of Restricted Range

During the course of this research, it became apparent that the extant range of values for

important moderators is restricted. We will focus on the number of categories as an example

moderator. However, this issue of restricted range is also relevant to other moderators, including

those that are typically treated categorically (e.g., similarity) that should perhaps be placed

within a continuous range of similarity. Moderator values were also frequently set to one of a
TWO-STAGE FRAMEWORK OF SEQUENCING EFFECTS 20

few values (e.g., 6 or 12 for the number of categories). Restricting the range of values can be

convenient for executing standard experimental designs (e.g., making the number of categories

divisible by 4 to accommodate a 2x2 design), but also carry consequences. Restricting the range

of values for a moderator impedes theoretical development and practical application. In essence,

restricting the range of potentially important moderators restricts the range of potentially

important conclusions as well. This is not to say that moderators must be manipulated in all

experiments, but rather that if very few researchers (or none) manipulate a moderator, the

possibility of generalization is significantly curtailed. For instance, regarding meta-analysis and

theory-building, failing to explore the effects of greater or fewer categories across or within

experiments makes it challenging to know when and how attentional mechanisms that may

underlie interleaving effects actually operate. Participants may compare across exemplars, but

are these benefits influenced by the variety and number of recent examples from which to

compare? To what extent and under what conditions does a participants’ own individual abilities

(e.g., operation span, Kane & Engle, 2003) become relevant? These are important questions that

need very specific answers in order to broadly benefit education. In short, restricting the range of

values contributes to impeding theoretical development that would help educators and

developers of educational technology understand if, and, the extent to which students should

interleave.

The Two-Stage Framework Roadmap for Future Research

The two-stage framework is naturally cyclical; attention and memory interact

dynamically over time (Turke-Brown, 2007). This framework is in progress, and future research

is needed to fill in blank spots in the relations between attention and memory to elucidate how

best to schedule practice for students to learn categories. Our meta-analysis confirmed some
TWO-STAGE FRAMEWORK OF SEQUENCING EFFECTS 21

findings, but also revealed shortcomings in experimental design. Specifically, attention and

memory are frequently studied separately (attention more so than memory), but should be

studied together in order to give a more complete picture of category learning. Below we

describe several possibilities for future research that could account for the issues described above

(proliferation of a single paradigm, confounded moderators, restricted range) and understand

interleaving as inherently involving both attention and memory factors.

Broadening the Stimulus Space

One of the issues we identified was a proliferation of a single experimental paradigm—a

substantial amount of interleaving research on category learning has used a small number of

stimuli, especially paintings, birds, and butterflies. This is likely attributable to convenience and

a desire to relate findings to prior work (e.g., that also used paintings). However, if the goal is to

generate prescriptions for how to best teach categories (even visual categories), constraining

research to a small number of stimuli is likely to introduce idiosyncratic stimuli-specific biases in

experimental findings. Such constraints also may limit the ability of researchers to properly

manipulate important variables (e.g., similarity, number of categories). Even if for example the

similarity among paintings was quantified, and a large number of painters (and paintings) were

included, generalization would be difficult. In short, a broad swath of stimuli need to be utilized

to evaluate whether broad claims can be made about how to implement interleaving.

Parametric Manipulations of Critical Variables

In addition to broadening the stimulus space, it may also be important to systematically

manipulate levels of the existing variables. Under parametric manipulation, variables of interest

are systematically tested across multiple levels of intensity (Brand et al., 2019), in opposition to
TWO-STAGE FRAMEWORK OF SEQUENCING EFFECTS 22

the two-armed A/B experimental design traditionally employed in cognitive psychology. In

interleaving studies, this might look like implementing experimental designs where the rate of

interleaving is manipulated across a range of values, or likewise, the number of categories.

Critically, this will allow testing of potentially important interactions between variables.

Parametric manipulation has been used to great effect in neighboring research areas of cognitive

psychological research, most notably in Cepeda et al.’s (2008) investigation of the spacing effect

in which the researchers systematically varied multiple levels of spacing intervals with multiple

levels of retention interval (yielding 26 different conditions).

We recommend conducting experiments with parametric manipulations, because this

approach would allow for greater exploration of the experiment-space that may be relevant for

understanding the effect of interleaving. The modal interleaving study in our review used

paintings as stimuli, had six categories per condition, and treated interleaving as a binary factor

(interleaved or blocked). However, extant theories aiming to explain interleaving imply that all

of these attributes (stimuli, number of categories being learned, rate of interleaving) may

interact. For example, if attention across exemplars facilitates comparison and possibly category

learning when exemplars are interleaved, the number of categories will naturally influence the

efficacy of comparing exemplars. Relatedly, working memory capacity may be differentially

relevant depending on the similarity and number of categories. Thus, the similarity, number of

categories, and rate of interleaving need to be considered simultaneously. Successful

implementation may entail collaborative multi-site projects with large samples sizes (e.g., Klein

et al., 2014) and alternative experimental designs.


TWO-STAGE FRAMEWORK OF SEQUENCING EFFECTS 23

Alternative Experimental Designs

The non-systemic traversal of the interleaving experiment-space4 is likely to be sub-

optimal for determining when and the extent to which interleaving should be applied. Given

existing experimental preferences (e.g., choosing a particular rate of interleaving and number of

categories and comparing to a blocked condition), a complete understanding of interleaving—

even with the use of parametric experimental manipulations—could require gigantic sample

sizes and decades (or longer) of careful research. However, if the goal is to understand the

functional relationship between, for example, the number of categories and (rate of) interleaving,

there may be more efficient methods for experimental design. For example, Lindsay, Mozer,

Huggins, and Pashler (2013) sought to optimize the balance between the rate of interleaving and

amount of fading (e.g., starting with easy examples and increasing difficulty). Their aim was to

devise a method to avoid running large A/B style tests comparing different values of each

variable against each other, and instead assume there was a functional relationship that could be

traversed to find an optimum. For example, instead of manipulating just two levels of each

variable (with a large number of participants at each level), multiple levels of each variable are

chosen (with a smaller number of participants at each level) to allow researchers to model

learning. Such a method might preclude pairwise comparison of points on that functional line

depending on statistical power, but overall would give stronger evidence about the relationship

among the variables of interest. Other recent research has shown how adaptive experimental

design can be more efficient when using multi-armed bandit algorithms (Rafferty, Ying, &

Williams, 2019). Adopting alternative approaches may help avoid some of the pitfalls that have

4
An example coordinate in this experiment-space could be high similarity among categories, ten to-be-learned-
categories, and an intermediate rate of interleaving (e.g., alternate categories 50% of the time), etc.
TWO-STAGE FRAMEWORK OF SEQUENCING EFFECTS 24

become common in many areas of social science (Watts, 2017) and get to the heart of answering

the question “how and when should interleaving be applied?”.

Testing Memory Factors. While our first three recommendations are those that apply

more broadly to the field, our review has also indicated several ways forward for testing theory

specific to the interleaving effect. Our quantitative analysis indicated that more categories

predicted a larger effect of interleaving. This finding supports a spacing effect (more categories

being interleaved leads to more spacing between repetitions of the same category), and by

extension, the contribution of memory processes. But under what circumstances would more

categories leading to a larger interleaving effect be explained by attentional theories? Typically,

such theories concern comparisons a participant could make between the current exemplar they

are viewing and the immediately preceding exemplar (within or between categories depending

on rate of interleaving). In fact, there is evidence that disrupting comparison with the

immediately preceding exemplar (by inserting fillers) can eliminate the benefit of interleaving in

some cases (Birnbaum, 2013). However, if attentional explanations are mostly concerned with

attention towards immediately preceding examples, it is not clear how a purely attentional

explanation of interleaving could explain why increasing the number of categories (e.g., from 6

to 12) increases the benefit of interleaving. Of course, a participant may be reminded of prior

(nonadjacent) exemplars, and compare them to the present exemplar under consideration. That

process would involve recall, and thus spacing would become an additional relevant factor.

However, to our knowledge, there are no experiments directly testing how spacing and

attentional mechanisms may together contribute to interleaving effects. For instance, an

experiment could insert inter-trial fillers while manipulating the number of categories and

retention interval. Most interleaving experiments seem to have been tailored for studying
TWO-STAGE FRAMEWORK OF SEQUENCING EFFECTS 25

attentional rather than memory effects (e.g., 90% of the experiments in our meta-analysis have a

retention interval of less than 24 hours).

Hybrid and Adaptive Schedules. A particularly relevant example connecting attention

and memory is how expertise influences information-processing. Expertise is known to be highly

relevant for both how attention is devoted and what is retained. This is clear in educational

research—expertise can change what instructional format is most beneficial for learning

(Cronbach & Snow, 1977; Kalyuga, Ayres, Chandler & Sweller, 2003). Experts generally

process information relevant to their domain differently, such as preferentially directing attention

to more relevant features (Kim & Rehder, 2011) and chunking information more effectively

(Chase & Simon, 1973). This interaction between expertise (prior learning), attentional

allocation, and subsequent learning is likely to be highly relevant for determining optimal

interleaving schedules. As knowledge is acquired, participants' attention will be directed

differently and thus scheduling that most prior research has shown to be effective for naive

learners may become inefficient. For example, blocking may be initially preferable due to

dissimilar category structure, but once the participant learns relevant category features enabling

across category comparison, interleaving may be introduced to future improve learning via

spacing and across-category comparisons. Future educational research should explore the

efficacy of this approach by manipulating category similarity as well as altering the schedule

according to student performance. More precise adaptive scheduling could be achieved with a

computational model. However, although there are recent models that account for category

similarity (Carvalho & Goldstone, 2019) and others that track memory as a function of spacing

(Walsh et al, 2018; Pavlik, Eglington, & Harrell-Williams, 2020; Eglington & Pavlik, 2020), to

our knowledge there are no models that explicitly account for both simultaneously.
TWO-STAGE FRAMEWORK OF SEQUENCING EFFECTS 26

Conclusion

For the present review, we initially set out to meta-analyze the interleaving literature. We

found a medium beneficial effect of interleaving, moderated by several important factors. Our

analysis also revealed that multiple processes may contribute to explaining the interleaving

effect. In contrast to prior work, we coded for moderators that are likely to be relevant for both

memory-based accounts (e.g., number of categories) and those more relevant to attention-based

accounts (e.g., similarity, inter-trial fillers). In line with prior research, our analyses indicated

that interleaving enabled attentional mechanisms that promoted learning, as long as the

categories themselves had the proper similarity structure. However, interleaving also naturally

increases spacing, and, to add to existing literature, we found evidence that this spacing also

contributes to the interleaving effect. In other words, we found evidence that both attentional and

spacing accounts are partially supported, which should not be entirely surprising, given the

interplay between attention and memory.

However, coding this literature also revealed significant issues concerning how

interleaving research is carried out, especially the importance of more systematic and

comprehensive manipulation of relevant variables and alternative experimental designs to further

elucidate how to effectively implement interleaved practice. For example, despite the known

relationship between the similarity structure of learning materials and the effect of interleaving

(Carvalho & Goldstone, 2014), the similarity among categories has rarely been quantified (or

manipulated directly), and a significant plurality of research has been with specific sets of

stimuli. Our two-stage framework lays out a roadmap for possible interactions to explore, but

these were interactions that we could not examine in the present review given gaps in the

existing evidence base. The non-systematic nature of experimental design within the interleaving
TWO-STAGE FRAMEWORK OF SEQUENCING EFFECTS 27

literature hence makes it more challenging to broadly interpret overall findings and develop

specific theories and models that can guide future research and pedagogy. On a more positive

note, these issues highlight new areas of research and motivate new experiments that may help

further refine the two-stage framework (depicted in Figure 1), and lead to improved pedagogical

decision-making for how to best learn categories.


TWO-STAGE FRAMEWORK OF SEQUENCING EFFECTS 28

References

Abushanab, B., & Bishara, A. J. (2013). Memory and metacognition for piano melodies: Illusory

advantages of fixed- over random-order practice. Memory & Cognition, 41(6), 928–937.

https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-013-0311-z

Ashby, F. G., Alfonso-reese, L. A., Turken, U., & Waldron, E. M. (1998). A neuropsychological

theory of multiple systems in category learning. Psychological Review, 442–481.

Ashby, F. G., & Maddox, W. T. (2005). Human Category Learning. Annual Review of

Psychology, 56(1), 149–178. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070217

Birnbaum, M. S., Kornell, N., Bjork, E. L., & Bjork, R. A. (2013). Why interleaving enhances

inductive learning: The roles of discrimination and retrieval. Memory & Cognition, 41(3),

392–402. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-012-0272-7

Carvalho, P. F., & Goldstone, R. L. (2014). Putting category learning in order: Category

structure and temporal arrangement affect the benefit of interleaved over blocked study.

Memory & Cognition, 42(3), 481–495. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-013-0371-0

Carvalho, P. F., & Goldstone, R. L. (2015). The benefits of interleaved and blocked study:

Different tasks benefit from different schedules of study. Psychonomic Bulletin &

Review, 22(1), 281–288. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0676-4

Carvalho, P. F., & Goldstone, R. L. (2017). The sequence of study changes what information is

attended to, encoded, and remembered during category learning. Journal of Experimental

Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 43(11), 1699–1719.

https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000406

Carvalho, P. F., & Goldstone, R. (2019, September 13). A computational model of context-

dependent encodings during category learning. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/8psa4


TWO-STAGE FRAMEWORK OF SEQUENCING EFFECTS 29

Cepeda, N. J., Vul, E., Rohrer, D., Wixted, J. T., & Pashler, H. (2008). Spacing Effects in

Learning: A Temporal Ridgeline of Optimal Retention. Psychological Science, 19(11),

1095–1102. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02209.x

Chase, W. G., & Simon, H. A. (1973). Perception in chess. Cognitive psychology, 4(1), 55-81.

Chun, M. M., & Turk-Browne, N. B. (2007). Interactions between attention and memory.

Current opinion in neurobiology, 17(2), 177-184.

Cronbach, L., & Snow, R. (1977). Aptitudes and instructional methods: A handbook for research

on interactions. New York: Irvington.

Dunlosky, J., Rawson, K. A., Marsh, E. J., Nathan, M. J., & Willingham, D. T. (2013).

Improving Students’ Learning With Effective Learning Techniques: Promising

Directions From Cognitive and Educational Psychology. Psychological Science in the

Public Interest, 14(1), 4–58. https://doi.org/10.1177/1529100612453266

Eglington, L. G., & Kang, S. H. K. (2017). Interleaved Presentation Benefits Science Category

Learning. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 6(4), 475–485.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2017.07.005

Eglington, L. G., & Pavlik Jr, P. I. (2020). Optimizing practice scheduling requires quantitative

tracking of individual item performance. npj Science of Learning, 5(1), 1-10.

https://doi.org/10.1038/s41539-020-00074-4

Hall, K. G., Domingues, D. A., & Cavazos, R. (1994). Contextual interference with skilled

baseball players. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 78, 835-841.

Jolly, E., & Chang, L. J. (2019). The Flatland Fallacy: Moving Beyond Low–Dimensional

Thinking. Topics in Cognitive Science, 11(2), 433-454.

Kalyuga, S., Ayres, P., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2003). The expertise reversal effect.
TWO-STAGE FRAMEWORK OF SEQUENCING EFFECTS 30

Educational Psychologist, 38(2), 3–31.

Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2003). Working-memory capacity and the control of attention: the

contributions of goal neglect, response competition, and task set to Stroop interference.

Journal of experimental psychology: General, 132(1), 47-70.

https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.132.1.47

Kang, S. H. K., & Pashler, H. (2012). Learning painting styles: Spacing is advantageous when it

promotes discriminative contrast. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26(1), 97–103.

https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1801

Kim, S., & Rehder, B. (2011). How prior knowledge affects selective attention during category

learning: An eyetracking study. Memory & Cognition, 39(4), 649-665.

Klein, R. A., Ratliff, K. A., Vianello, M., Adams, R. B., Jr., Bahník, Š., Bernstein, M. J., . . .

Nosek, B. A. (2014). Investigating variation in replicability: A “many labs” replication

project. Social Psychology, 45(3), 142-152. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000178

Kornell, N., & Bjork, R. A. (2008). Learning Concepts and Categories: Is Spacing the “Enemy of

Induction”? Psychological Science, 19(6), 585–592. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-

9280.2008.02127.x

Kotrlik, J. W., Williams, H. A., & Jabor, M. K. (2011). Reporting and Interpreting Effect Size in

Quantitative Agricultural Education Research. Journal of Agricultural Education, 52(1),

132-142.

Mayfield, K. H., & Chase, P. N. (2002). The effects of cumulative practice on mathematics

problem solving. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35(2), 105–123.

https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2002.35-105

Lindsey, R. V., Mozer, M. C., Huggins, W. J., & Pashler, H. (2013). Optimizing instructional
TWO-STAGE FRAMEWORK OF SEQUENCING EFFECTS 31

policies. In Advances in neural information processing systems (pp. 2778-2786).

McCabe, J. (2011). Metacognitive awareness of learning strategies in undergraduates. Memory &

Cognition, 39(3), 462–476. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-010-0035-2

Noh, S. M., Yan, V. X., Bjork, R. A., & Maddox, W. T. (2016). Optimal sequencing during

category learning: Testing a dual-learning systems perspective. Cognition, 155, 23–29.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.06.007

Ollis, S., Button, C., & Fairweather, M. (2005). The influence of professional expertise and task

complexity upon the potency of the contextual interference effect. Acta Psychologica,

118(3), 229–244. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2004.08.003

Osteen, P., & Bright, C. Effect Sizes and Intervention Research. Society for Social Work and

Research Conference. San Francisco, CA. January 14-17, 2010.

https://archive.hshsl.umaryland.edu/handle/10713/3582

Ostrow, K., Heffernan, N., Heffernan, C., & Peterson, Z. (2015). Blocking Vs. Interleaving:

Examining Single-Session Effects Within Middle School Math Homework. In C. Conati,

N. Heffernan, A. Mitrovic, & M. F. Verdejo (Eds.), Artificial Intelligence in Education

(pp. 338–347). Springer International Publishing.

Pashler, H., & Harris, C. R. (2012). Is the replicability crisis overblown? Three arguments

examined. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(6), 531-536.

Pavlik Jr, P. I., Eglington, L. G., & Harrell-Williams, L. M. (2020, May 2). Generalized

Knowledge Tracing: A Constrained Framework for Learner Modeling. arXiv preprint

arXiv:2005.00869.

Rafferty, A., Ying, H., & Williams, J. (2019). Statistical Consequences of using Multi-armed

Bandits to Conduct Adaptive Educational Experiments. JEDM | Journal of Educational


TWO-STAGE FRAMEWORK OF SEQUENCING EFFECTS 32

Data Mining, 11(1), 47-79. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.3554749

Rau, M. A., Aleven, V., & Rummel, N. (2013). Interleaved practice in multi-dimensional

learning tasks: Which dimension should we interleave? Learning and Instruction, 23, 98–

114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2012.07.003

R Core Team (2018). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation

for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. https://www.R-project.org/.

Rohrer, D., Dedrick, R. F., Hartwig, M. K., & Cheung, C.-N. (2019). A randomized controlled

trial of interleaved mathematics practice. Journal of Educational Psychology.

https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000367

Rohrer, D., & Taylor, K. (2007). The shuffling of mathematics problems improves learning.

Instructional Science, 35(6), 481–498. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-007-9015-8

Rohatgi, A. (2018). WebPlotDigitizer - Web Based Plot Digitizer. Austin, Texas, USA.

Retrieved from https: //automeris.io/WebPlotDigitizer

Sana, F., Yan, V. X., & Kim, J. A. (2017). Study sequence matters for the inductive learning of

cognitive concepts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 109(1), 84–98.

https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000119

Sana, F., Yan, V. X., Kim, J. A., Bjork, E. L., & Bjork, R. A. (2018). Does Working Memory

Capacity Moderate the Interleaving Benefit? Journal of Applied Research in Memory and

Cognition, 7(3), 361–369. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2018.05.005

Tauber, S. K., Dunlosky, J., Rawson, K. A., Wahlheim, C. N., & Jacoby, L. L. (2013). Self-

regulated learning of a natural category: Do people interleave or block exemplars during

study? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(2), 356–363. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-

012-0319-6
TWO-STAGE FRAMEWORK OF SEQUENCING EFFECTS 33

Vlach, H. A. (2014). The Spacing Effect in Children’s Generalization of Knowledge: Allowing

Children Time to Forget Promotes Their Ability to Learn. Child Development

Perspectives, 8(3), 163–168. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12079

Watts, D. J. (2017). Should social science be more solution-oriented?. Nature Human Behaviour,

1(1), 0015.

Wolfe, J. M. (2019). Visual Attention: The Multiple Ways in which History Shapes Selection.

Current Biology, 29(5), R155-R156.

Yan, V. X., Bjork, E. L., & Bjork, R. A. (2016). On the difficulty of mending metacognitive

illusions: A priori theories, fluency effects, and misattributions of the interleaving benefit.

Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(7), 918–933.

https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000177

Yan, V. X., Soderstrom, N. C., Seneviratna, G. S., Bjork, E. L., & Bjork, R. A. (2017). How

should exemplars be sequenced in inductive learning? Empirical evidence versus

learners’ opinions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 23(4), 403–416.

https://doi.org/10.1037/xap0000139

Viechtbauer, W. (2010). Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. Journal

of Statistical Software, 36(3), 1-48. URL: http://www.jstatsoft.org/v36/i03/

Viechtbauer, W. (2019). I^2 for Multilevel and Multivariate Models. The Metafor package:

A meta-analysis package for R. http://www.metafor-

project.org/doku.php/tips:i2_multilevel_multivariate

Zulkiply, N., & Burt, J. S. (2013). The exemplar interleaving effect in inductive learning:

Moderation by the difficulty of category discriminations. Memory & Cognition,

41(1), 16–27. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-012-0238-9


TWO-STAGE FRAMEWORK OF SEQUENCING EFFECTS 34

Zulkiply, N., McLean, J., Burt, J. S., & Bath, D. (2012). Spacing and induction: Application to

exemplars presented as auditory and visual text. Learning and Instruction, 22(3), 215–

221. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2011.11.002
TWO-STAGE FRAMEWORK OF SEQUENCING EFFECTS 35

Table 1
Comparison of Multiple Regression Results

1. Expt. 2. Attentional 3. Attentional and


0. Null Model Factors Factors Memory Factors
Predictor (Value = 1)

Intercept 0.45 (.05) *** 0.81 (0.16) *** 0.83 (0.17) *** 0.26 (.13) *
Schedule Manipulation (Within) -0.12 (0.09) -0.11 (0.09) -0.14 (.08)
Publication Status (Published) -0.10 (.09) -0.09 (0.10) -0.07 (.09)
Stimuli Type (Cognitive) -0.45 (0.12) *** -0.49 (0.12) *** -0.25 (.12) *
Population Pool (MTurk) -0.26 (0.09) ** -0.28 (0.10) ** -0.18 (.09) *
Sequential or Simultaneous
Presentation (Simultaneous) 0.07 (0.09) 0.09 (.09)
Intertrial Filler (Yes) -0.26 (0.12) * -0.26 (0.11) *
Similarity (Dissimilar) -0.32 (.06) *** -0.33 (0.06) ***
Study Type (Active) 0.07 (.08) 0.10 (0.08)
Retention Interval (>= 1 Day) 0.01 (0.10)
Number of Categories 0.04 (0.01) ***

Model Fit Statistics


Negative Log Likelihood 193.40 183.80 168.47 157.63
AIC 392.80 381.61 358.93 341.25
BIC 402.68 404.66 395.16 384.06

Note. Model fit statistics, such as negative log likelihood, AIC, and BIC, can be used to compare the relative fits of each model, with

lower scores indicating better fits.

* p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001.


TWO-STAGE FRAMEWORK OF SEQUENCING EFFECTS 36

Figure 1

Schematic of the Two-stage Framework of Sequencing Effects


TWO-STAGE FRAMEWORK OF SEQUENCING EFFECTS 37

Figure 2

Cramer’s V Correlation Matrix between Moderators

Note. Cramer’s V is a non-parametric correlation statistic. The columns representing each

moderator are ordered from left-to-right, most-to-least correlated.


TWO-STAGE FRAMEWORK OF SEQUENCING EFFECTS 38

Figure 3

Interaction Between Number of Categories and Similarity on Interleaving Effect Size


TWO-STAGE FRAMEWORK OF SEQUENCING EFFECTS 39

Figure 4

Four Examples Illustrating Uneven Distribution of Focal Moderators Across Experiments

Note. Data points are jittered due to the categorical nature of the moderators.

You might also like