TVP Sociolinguistic Coursebook
TVP Sociolinguistic Coursebook
TVP Sociolinguistic Coursebook
(A Coursebook)
1
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Chapter I Contents
1. Sociolinguistics
2. Society
3. Culture
4. Language
5. The Possible Relationships between
Language and Society
6. The Possible Relationships between
Language and Culture
7. The Possible Relationships between
Language and Thought
8. The Possible Relationships between
Language, Culture, Society, and Thought.
1. SOCIOLINGUISTICS
‘Sociolinguistics is that part of linguistics which is concerned with language as a social and
cultural phenomenon’ [102].
‘Sociolinguistics is the study of language in relation to social factors, that is, social class,
educational level and type of education, age, sex, ethnic origin, etc.’ [82, p. 262].
2
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Examining the way people use language in different social contexts provides a wealth of information
about the way language works, as well as about the social relationships in a community, and the way
people convey and construct aspects of their social identity through their language. Sociolinguistics
has close connections with social sciences, in particular, sociology, anthropology, and education.
Sociolinguistics first appeared in the West in the 1960s and was pioneered by linguists such
as William Labov in the U.S. and Basil Bernstein in the UK. Many scholars considered the year
1964 as the year in which sociolinguistics was officially accepted in an interdisciplinary conference
organized by the sociologists and linguists.
3
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
In any community, the distinguishable varieties or codes which are available for use in
different social contexts form a kind of repertoire of available options [45, p.8]. The members of
each communities have their distinctive linguistic or verbal repertoires. In other words, in every
community there is a range of varieties from which people select according to the social context in
which they are communicating.
In sociolinguistics, variation in language use among speakers or groups of speakers is a
principal concern. Examining the way people use language in different social contexts provides a
wealth of information about the way language works, as well as about the social relationships in a
community, and the way people convey and construct aspects of their social identity through their
language. Sociolinguists are interested in the different types of linguistic variation influenced by and
used to express and reflect social factors.
a-The participants
i-who is speaking, and
ii-who are they speaking to?
b-The setting or social context of the interaction: where are they
speaking?
c-The topic: what is being talked about?
4
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
These social factors prove important in describing and analysing all kinds of interaction.
They are basic components in sociolinguistic explanations of why we didn’t all speak the same way,
and why we don’t all speak in the same way all the time.
The notion of social context or context of situation is often related with that of context of
culture, which is usually described as the general context for language as system [39].
Society and culture act as the social and cultural contexts for appropriate use of language in
communication.
In other words, the sociolinguist’s aim is to move towards a theory which provides a
motivated account of the way language is used in a community, and of the choices people make
when they use language.
Follow-up activity 1.1.True/False: Discuss with your friends and decide whether the
following statements are true or false:
4-Society and culture act as the social and cultural contexts for language use.
5
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
II-Research task:
Basing yourself on the section 1.3.3. and the Guideline to Research Assignment, think of a
research topic to be developed into a research assignment.
2. SOCIETY
The term "society" came from the Latin word societas, which in turn was derived from the
noun socius ("comrade, friend, ally; adjectival form socialis) used to describe a bond or interaction
among parties that are friendly, or at least civil. Without an article, the term can refer to the entirety
of humanity (also: "society in general", "society at large", etc.)
The term society, thus, is used to refer to the people in general, living together in
communities. It can be used to refer to a particular community of people who share the same
customs, laws, languages etc. e.g. a speech / language community.
Human societies are characterized by patterns of relationships (social relations) between
individuals who share a distinctive culture and institutions. Social structure is the network of the
social relations. In a human society, an individual has one or more than one social positions.
6
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
In caste societies, a person’s location in the social strata is ascribed by birth rather than based
on individual accomplishments. Movement between strata, or castes, is prohibited or severely
limited. There are rigid lines between the castes and no form of social mixing is allowed.
Sociologists describe a situation where one’s social position is determined at birth as ascription.
A man is born into a jati (a division of caste) and this is the only way of acquiring
membership. The Hindu doctrine of karma teaches the young Hindu that he is born into a particular
sub-caste because that is where he deserves to be born. Each caste is traditionally associated with a
particular occupation. Caste membership is linked to status: not only houses, but clothes, customs
and manners become symbols of status for those who share a common culture.
Social class (or simply class) is a set of concepts in the social sciences and political theory
centered on models of social stratification in which people are grouped into a set of hierarchical
social categories.
Industrial society gave rise to class-based systems of stratification. In class societies, social
stratification is based on a combination of ascribed and achieved statuses. Strata are largely
established along economic lines but are not as clearly delineated as a caste system. Class societies
allow movement between classes based on individual accomplishments.
In common sense, the term "social class," is usually synonymous with "socio-economic
class," defined as: "people having the same social, economic, or educational status," e.g. "the
working class"; "an emerging professional class."
Today, in Western society, the concepts of social class often assume three general
categories: a very wealthy and powerful upper class that owns and controls the means of
production; a middle class of professional workers, small business owners, and low-level managers;
and a lower class, who rely on low-paying wage jobs for their livelihood and often experience
poverty
7
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
The dimensions of power and solidarity have been fundamental to sociolinguistic theory
since Brown and Gilman [10] introduced the concept in relation to the pronoun system. They
introduced the framework of power and solidarity by reference to the linguistic choices that must be
made in languages that have "polite" and "familiar" forms of the second person pronoun. The
"polite" pronoun is referred to as "V" from the French vous. The "familiar" pronoun is referred to as
"T" from French tu.
8
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
a-Power is a relationship between at least two persons and it is nonreciprocal in the sense that
both cannot have power in the same area of behavior [10].
b-Power is the degree to which one interlocutor is able to control the behavior of the other
[Sterling, 2000, as cited in [10].
There are many bases of power-physical strength, wealth, age, sex, institutionalized role in
the church, the state, the army, or within the family. The relation called older than, parent of,
richer than, stronger than, and noble than are all asymmetrical and attended the same school or
have the same parents, or practice the same profession are a set of relations which are
symmetrical.
Solidarity forms express intimacy and familiarity [Sterling, 2000, as cited in [10]. Solidarity
can be achieved in interactions where interlocutors share some common attribute — for instance,
attendance at the same school, work in the same profession, membership in the same family, etc. The
solidary relationship is symmetrical in that if Speaker A has the same parents (or attended the same
school, etc.) as Speaker B, then B has the same parents as A. It is important to note that not every
shared personal attribute creates solidarity. For example, two people who have the same color eyes
or same shoe size will not automatically have an intimate relationship. But should they share
political membership, religion, birthplace or other common attributes “that make for like-mindedness
or similar behavior dispositions,” the likelihood of a solidary relationship increases.
Power and solidarity are two types of social relationships which influence the language
choice in communication.
Follow-up activity 1.2.True/False: Discuss with your friends and decide whether the
following statements are true or false:
1-A society, or a human society, is a group of people related to each other through persistent
relations, or a large social grouping sharing the same geographical or virtual territory, subject to the
same political authority and dominant cultural expectations.
2-Today, in Western societies, social classes consist of castes based on the Hindu religion.
4-The term social role refers to a pattern of behaviour that is associated with a particular
position in society.
9
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
5-Power and solidarity are the two types of social relationships which affect language use in
communication.
3. CULTURE
“Tại phương Tây, văn hóa – culture…. đều xuất xứ từ chữ Latinh cultus có nghĩa là khai
hoang, trồng trọt, trông nom cây lương thực; nói ngắn gọn là sự vun trồng. Sau đó từ cultus
được mở rộng nghĩa, dùng trong lĩnh vực xã hội chỉ sự vun trồng, giáo dục, đào tạo, và phát
triển mọi khả năng của con người.
Ở Phương Đông, trong tiếng Hán cổ, từ văn hóa bao hàm ý nghĩa văn là vẻ đẹp của nhân
tính, cái đẹp của trí thức, trí tuệ con người có thể đạt được bằng sự tu dưỡng của bản thân
và cách thức cai trị đúng đắn của nhà cầm quyền. Còn chữ hóa trong văn hóa là việc đem
cái văn (cái đẹp, cái tốt, cái đúng) để cảm hóa, giáo dục và hiện thực hóa trong thục tiễn, đời
sống” [116, p.8].
Kroeber and Kluckhohn [1952, as cited in [81, p.89] identified 164 different definitions
ofcultures in 1952. However, the concept of culture is very difficult, perhaps even impossible, to
define because it is a theory and/or abstract name for a very large, complex, multi-dimensional
phenomenon. Hundreds of definitions of culture have been developed under different conditions
under which different scholars have worked. These scholars have all had different views about what
constitutes the concept and meaning of culture. We will now look at different definitions of cultures
in the following parts:
Culture is, in the words of E.B.Tylor,"that complex whole which includes knowledge, belief,
art, morals, law, custom and any other capabilities and habits acquired by man as a member of
society"[Tylor, 1974, as cited in [154].
10
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
More recently, the UNESCO described culture as the “set of distinctive spiritual, material,
intellectual and emotional features of society or a social group that encompasses, in addition to art
and literature, lifestyle, ways of living together, value system, traditions and beliefs” [UNESCO,
2002, as cited in [81, pp. 90-91].
Hofstede, Hofstede and Minkov define culture as the ‘collective programming of the mind
that distinguishes the members of one group or category of people from others” [41, p.4].
According to Hofstede, Hofstede and Minkov [41, pp.4-6], every person carries within him
or herself patterns of thinking, feeling, and potential acting that were learned throughout the person’s
lifetime. Much of it was acquired in early childhood, because at that time a person is most
susceptible to learning and assimilating.
Using the analogy of the way computers are programmed, such patterns of thinking, feeling,
and acting are mental programs, or software of the mind. This does not mean, of course, that people
are programmed the way computers are. A person’s behavior is only partially predetermined by his
or her mental programs: he or she has a basic ability to deviate from them and to react in ways that
are new, creative, destructive, or unexpected. The software of the mind only indicates what reactions
are likely and understandable, given one’s past.
The sources of one’s mental programs lie within the social environments in which one grew
up and collected one’s life experiences. The programming starts within the family; it continues
within the neighborhood, at school, in youth groups, at the workplace, and in the living community.
Mental programs vary as much as the social environments in which they were acquired.
A customary term for such mental software is culture. This is culture in the narrow sense.
Culture as mental software, however, corresponds to a much broader use of the word that is common
among sociologists and, especially, anthropologists.
Culture is always a collective phenomenon, but it can be connected to different collectives.
Within each collective there is a variety of individuals. Most commonly the term culture is used for
tribes or ethnic groups (in anthropology), for nations (in political science, sociology and
management), and for organizations (in sociology and management). A relatively unexplored field is
the culture of occupations (for instance, of engineers versus accountants, or of academics from
different disciplines). The term can also be applied to the genders, to generations, or to social
classes. However, changing the level of aggregation studied changes the nature of the concept of
‘culture’. Societal, national and gender cultures, which children acquire from their earliest youth
onwards, are much deeper rooted in the human mind than occupational cultures acquired at school,
or than organizational cultures acquired on the job. The latter are exchangeable when people take a
new job. Societal cultures reside in (often unconscious) values, in the sense of broad tendencies to
prefer certain states of affairs over others. Organizational cultures reside rather in (visible and
conscious) practices: the way people perceive what goes on in their organizational environment.
Oxford Advanced Learner’s Dictionary (7th Edition) [47, p.357] defines the term culture as
11
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
a-way of life: (U) the customs and beliefs, art, way of life and social organization of a
particular country or group / (C) Country, group, etc. with its own beliefs.
b-Art/ music / literature: (U) art, music, literature, etc. thought of as a group.
c-Beliefs / Attitudes (C, U) the beliefs and attitudes about something that people in a
particular group or organization share.
“Văn hóa là sự tổng hợp của mọi phương thức sinh hoạt cùng biểu hiện của nó mà loài
người đã sản sinh ra nhằm thích ứng những nhu cầu đời sống, và đòi hỏi của sự sinh tồn”
[Hồ Chí Minh, as cited in [116, p.9].
“Văn hóa là một hệ thống hữu cơ các giá trị vật chất và tinh thần do con người sáng tạo và
tích lũy qua quá trình hoạt động thực tiễn, trong sự tương tác giữa con người với môi trường
tự nhiên và xã hội của mình” [117, p.10].
When used as a count noun, "a culture" is the set of customs, traditions and values of a
society or community, such as an ethnic group or nation [154].
“Culture is like an iceberg: the deeper layers (e.g. traditions, beliefs, values) are hidden
from our view; we only see and hear the uppermost layers of cultural artifacts (e.g. fashion,
trends, pop music) and of verbal and nonverbal symbols. However, to understand a culture
with any dephth, we have to match its underlying values accurately with its respective norms,
meanings and symbols. It is the underlying set of beliefs and values that drives people’s
thinking, reacting, and behaving” [99, p.10].
12
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
harmony in society. Without these rules and regulations, society would be in chaos. Culture
simplifies everyday life decisions. Culture provides the means for satisfying physiological,
psychological, and social needs.
Culture also makes it possible for human society to communicate using verbal and non-
verbal codes of communication. Culture explains how a distinct group understands received
information. Culture determines a particular communication, negotiation, persuasion, and discussion
style. Culture shapes perception, and develops attitudes, feelings, images, and stereotypes.
Culture structures the governmental bodies. It influences the social, political, economic,
financial, educational, kinship, religious, health, and recreational systems of a society. Culture
influences family, social, and work relationships.
Culture binds people together; it determines the identity of the group of people. Culture
identifies the uniqueness of the social group, its values, beliefs, and thoughts. Members of the same
culture share similar thoughts and experiences. Shared experiences, values, and norms give the
members of a society a sense of their common identity. Culture helps to define who they are.
and Organizations: Software of the Mind Hofstede added a sixth dimension, indulgence versus
self-restraint, as a result of co-author Michael Minkov’s analysis of data from the World Values
Survey.
The six dimensions are labelled:
14
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
with it. It focuses on aspects not covered by the other five dimensions, but known from
literature on “happiness research”. Indulgence stands for a society that allows relatively free
gratification of basic and natural human desires related to enjoying life and having fun. Restraint
stands for a society that controls gratification of needs and regulates it by means of strict social
norms [42].
Each country has been positioned relative to other countries through a score on each
dimension. The dimensions are statistically distinct and do occur in all possible combinations,
although some combinations are more frequent than others.
Below are the features of low-context cultures (LCC) and high-context cultures (HCC):
There are the low-context cultures (LCC) and high-context cultures (HCC) depending
upon the level of information included in a communication message (Hall, 1976). In the low-context
cultures (LCC) most of the information and meaning is contained in verbal messages, very little in
the contextual message. Messages have clear meanings and line logic; explicit direct verbal
communication and clear intentions are emphasized. Western cultures (Germany, Sweden,
Switzerland, Australia, the United States, France) belong to LCC, where there is a need for explicit
instructions, signs, and procedures that explain how to behave. In the high-context cultures (HCC)
very little information is coded in the verbal message; most information is coded in the non-verbal
and contextual message. All events can be understood only in context; meanings vary depending
upon circumstances, and categories can change. Emphasis is on spiral logic, implicit imprecise and
indirect communication. Intentions are not clearly specified and discretion in expressing opinions is
exercised (China, Japan, Korea, Taiwan, Vietnam, Latin America, Mexico). The HCC value face
saving, honor shame and obligations, avoid confrontations, and use smoothing strategies to manage
conflicts in interpersonal relations. Members of HCC are also more careful in initial interactions and
make assumptions based upon a stranger’s cultural background. They ask more indirect questions
(Gudykunst, 1983); direct questions are considered to be rude (Elashmawi, 1991). Those who belong
to HCC (the Japanese, Chinese, Southeast Asians, Indonesians, Micronesians, and Indians) expect
others to sense the rules of behavior.
15
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Values are not only held by individuals, they are also the domain of the collective. Anderson
and Taylor make this point clear by stating, “values guide the behavior of people in society and
shape the social norms in a given culture” [as cited in [84, p.171]. In short, values underline the
16
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
qualities and actions that people consider necessary and vital to sustain their culture. They establish
the standards for maintaining a culture.
In general, there are two types of values: instrumental and terminal [81, p.124].
Instrumental values are preferable modes or means of behavior (to be honest, obedient…). These
values may be moral and socially accepted or may be concerned with personal competency or self-
actualization. Terminal values are goals (freedom, world peace). They may be personal and social.
Instrumental values are means to terminal values. Rokeach [1973, as cited in [81] found that the
three most important terminal values are a world of peace, family security, and freedom, whereas
the three most important instrumental values are honesty, ambition and responsibility.
A value system is the system of criteria by which one can evaluate one’s own and other’s
behavior and apply sactions to it. It is a system of socially accepted guidelines that show the cultural
norms of a society and specify the ways in which people should behave, a system of standards that
permit individuals to make decisions.
Reisinger [81, pp.127-160] presented the following cultural value dimensions in the book
International Tourism: Cultures and Behavior: a-Parson’s pattern variables, b-Kluckholn and
Strodtbeck’s value orientation, c-Stewart’s cultural patterns, d-Hall’s cultural differentiation, e-
Hofstede’s dimensions of cultural variability, f-Bond’s Confucian cultural patterns, g-Argyle’s
cultural differentiation, h-Schein’s, Trompenaars’ and Maznevski’s cultural differentiation, i-
Schneider and Barsoux’s cultural assumptions, j-Inglehart’s cultural dimensions, and k-Minkov’s
World Value Survey.
Cultural identity refers to one’s sense of belonging to a particular culture. It is (the feeling)
of identity of a group, culture, or individual as far as this individual is influenced by his or her
belonging to the culture [81, p.110]. It is formed in a process that results from membership in a
particular culture, and it involves learning about and accepting the traditions, heritage, language,
religion, ancestry, aesthetics, thinking patterns, and social structures of a culture. That is, people
internalized the beliefs, values, norms, and social practices of their culture and identify with that
culture as part of their self-concept.
17
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Follow-up activity 1.3.True/False: Discuss with your friends and decide whether the
following statements are true or false:
1-The term culture originally meant the cultivation of the soil. Later, it developed the
meaning cultivation of the soul or mind.
2- As a count noun, the term culture means the set of customs, traditions and values of a
societies.
3-Culture does not make it possible for human society to communicate using verbal and non-
verbal codes of communication. Culture does not determines a particular communication,
negotiation, persuation, and discussion style.
4. LANGUAGE
1-What is the main difference between the two terms Language and A language?
4.1. Language
The term Language as used in dictionaries to refer to the complex of universally human
potentiality for vocal communication / gift of speech / human method of communication/ part of
human genetic endowment.
The phrase a language usually means anyone of several thousand systems of oral
communication used by different human societies. A language is culture-bound and culture specific.
18
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
4.2.4. Vernacular
Vernacular is a term used to refer to the least self-conscious style of speech used by people in
relaxed conversation with friends and family. It is the most natural language for the speakers in the
group.
A vernacular is also a variety of language which has not been standardised and is not the
official language in the country.
For most people their mother tongue is the language they know best, but this too is not
always the case, especially when the mother tongue is not the language used in formal education.
The term mother tongue is not a technical one and carries with it a variety of emotional
connotations.
19
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
variety can be used as a social and cultural marker: national identity, group identity and personal
identity.
At the level of individuals and groups interacting with one another, within communicative
function, language serves expressive function (conveying feelings or emotions), directive function
(requesting or demanding), referential function (true or false propositional content), poetic
function (aesthetic), phatic function (empathy and solidarity), and metalinguistic (reference to
language itself) [86, pp.12-13].
The conclusion from the definitions is that a speech community comprises people who are in
habitual contact with each other by means of speech which involves either a shared language or
language varieties commonly used in the area.
Follow-up activity 1.4.True/False: Discuss with your friends and decide whether the
following statements are true or false:
4-A national language is one that unites a nation and separates it from surrounding nations.
20
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Language also functions as the marker of social identity and cultural identity.
5.2.1. Social structure may either influence or determine linguistic structure and / or behavior.
a-Physical environment reflected in its language, e.g. words for snow in Eskimo language (50
words), words for rice in the Vietnamese language, words for macaroni in Italian (500 words); the
Arabs are said to have 6,000 words for camels and camel equipment.
b-Social environment reflected in language, e.g. language varieties based on social factors;
5.2.2. Linguistic structure and / or behaviour may either influence or determine social structure
Examples are Whorfian hypothesis, the claims of Bernstain. The ‘Sapir-Whorf hypothysis’
combines two principles. The first is known as linguistic determinism: it states that language
determines the way we think. The second follows from this, and is known as linguistic relativity: it
states that the distinctions encoded in one language are not found in any other languages. Basil
Bernstein was a British sociologist and linguist, known for his work in the sociology of education.
Basil Bernstein made a significant contribution to the study of communication with his
sociolinguistic theory of language codes. Within the broader category of language codes are
elaborated and restricted codes. Littlejohn [2002, as cited in [144] suggests that Bernstein’s theory
shows how the language people use in everyday conversation both reflects and shapes the
assumptions of a certain social group. Furthermore, relationships established within the social group
affect the way that group uses language, and the type of speech that is used. In his theory, Bernstein
asserts a direct relationship between societal class and language. According to Bernstein in Class,
21
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Codes and Control (1971), “Forms of spoken language in the process of their learning initiate,
generalize and reinforce special types of relationship with the environment and thus create for the
individual particular forms of significance” [Bernstein,1971, as cited in [144].
5.2.4. A fourth possibility is to assume that there is no relationship at all between linguistic
structure and social structure and each is independent of the other.
In addition, society provides the non-linguistic or social context (the broader social situation
in which a linguistic item is used) for language use in communication. The social context is the
whole situation in which an utterance is made (i.e. who is speaking to whom about whom, whether
formally or informally, why, when and where).
Follow-up activity 1.5.True/False: Discuss with your friends and decide whether the
following statements are true or false:
3-Linguistic structure and/or behaviour may either influence or determine social structure.
4-In the Whorfian hypothesis, language provides a screen or filter to reality. It determines
how speakers perceive or organize the world around them.
5-The third possible relationship between language and society is that the relationship is bi-
directional. Language and society may influence each other.
1-What are the possible relationships between culture and language? How does a culture
influence the style of communication in a culture?
22
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
a-The connection between culture and language has been noted as far back as the classical
period and probably long before. Language and culture are interrelated [143].
b-The origin of language and the origin of culture are often thought to stem from the same
evolutionary process in early man. Language and culture both emerged as means of using symbols to
communicate, to construct social identity and to maintain coherence within a social group. Language
is the most important means of human communication. A language is a system of communication
within a speech community. A culture is also a communication system. It uses non-verbal and
verbal cues for communication [143].
c-Language is the direct expression of a people’s national character and the most essential
carrier of their common culture. The German romanticists of the 19th century such as Johann
Gottfried Herder and Wilhelm von Humboldt, often saw language not just as one cultural trait
among many but rather as the direct expression of a people's national character, and as such as
culture in a kind of condensed form. Herder for example suggests, "Denn jedes Volk ist Volk; es hat
seine National Bildung wie seine Sprache" (Since every people is a People, it has its own national
culture expressed through its own language). Franz Boas, founder of American anthropology, like
his German forerunners, maintained that the shared language of a community is the most essential
carrier of their common culture. Boas was the first anthropologist who considered it unimaginable to
study the culture of a foreign people without also becoming acquainted with their language. For
Boas, the fact that the intellectual culture of a people was largely constructed, shared and maintained
through the use of language, meant that understanding the language of a cultural group was the key
to understanding its culture. Language is the deepest manifestation of culture. Language reflects
cultural values. It expresses cultural realities, embodies cultural realities, through verbal and non-
verbal aspects and symbolizes cultural realities [143].
d-However, a language is also a part of the larger culture of the community that speak them.
A community's ways of speaking or signing are a part of the community's culture, just as other
shared practices are [143].
e-Language has the function of social identity. Humans use language as a way of signalling
identity with one cultural group and difference from others. Language is a marker of identity:
national, social, cultural and personal identity. A community's ways of speaking or signing are a part
of the community's culture, just as other shared practices are. Language use is a way of establishing
and displaying group identity. Ways of speaking function not only to facilitate communication, but
also to identify the social position of the speaker. The difference between languages does not consist
only in differences in pronunciation, vocabulary or grammar, but also in different "cultures of
speaking" [143].
f-A language is culture-bound and culture-specific. That a language is culture-bound means
that the language is tied to or restricted by its culture. When we communicate in a society, we must
act according to the socio-cultural rules of that society and culture. Culture helps determine the
appropriate communicative behavior within a variety of socio-cultural contexts by prescribing
23
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
certain rules. When communicating with members of one’s culture, people rely on deeply
internalized cultural rules that prescribe the appropriate behavior for specific communication
situation. These rules facilitate the ability to communicate effectively with one another. Each culture
has its particular rules or conventions that largely determine language usage in restrictive speech
events in daily communication. Culture also determines a particular communication, negotiation,
persuation and discussion style. That language is culture-specific means that there are particular
linguistic features (words, expressions, communication styles…) unique to that culture. Culture-
specificity means something (a thing, thought, idea,….) unique to a particular culture. For example,
in Vietnam, we have culture-specific words such as áo dài, bánh chưng, cuốc lủi, gạo tám, gạo tẻ,
gạo nếp…Vietnamese people use such expressions as chào ông (bà, anh chị…) ạ in greeting and
follow the principle of” Xưng khiêm, hô tôn’ in addressing. Vietnamese communication style tends
to be harmonious, formal, polite and indirect.
g-Culture makes it possible for human society to communicate using verbal and non-verbal
codes of communication. Culture explains how a distinct group understands received information.
Culture determines a particular communication, negotiation, persuasion, and discussion style.
Culture shapes perception, and develops attitudes, feelings, images, and stereotypes. All the levels in
communication are affected by cultural dimensions: verbal, non-verbal and etiquettes [81].
h-Culture provides the context within which words and, more generally, grammatical
systems are interpreted. The context for an instance of language (text) is an instance of culture
(situation). And the context for the system that lies behind each text (language) is the system which
lies behind each situation – namely, the culture [39, p.7].
i-There is the coordination between culture, language and communication. Culture affects
communication, and communication affects culture. Communication is culture and culture is
communication. It is through language and communication that culture is passed down, created and
modified from one generation to the next [81].
The connection between culture and language has been noted as far back as the classical
period and probably long before. The ancient Greeks, for example, distinguished between civilized
peoples and bárbaros "those who babble", i.e. those who speak unintelligible languages. The fact that
different groups speak different, unintelligible languages is often considered more tangible evidence
for cultural differences than other less obvious cultural traits.
The German romanticists of the 19th century such as Johann Gottfried Herder and Wilhelm
von Humboldt, often saw language not just as one cultural trait among many but rather as the direct
expression of a people's national character, and as such as culture in a kind of condensed form.
Herder for example suggests, "Denn jedes Volk ist Volk; es hat seine National Bildung wie seine
Sprache" (Since every people is a People, it has its own national culture expressed through its own
language).
24
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Franz Boas, founder of American anthropology, like his German forerunners, maintained that
the shared language of a community is the most essential carrier of their common culture. Boas was
the first anthropologist who considered it unimaginable to study the culture of a foreign people
without also becoming acquainted with their language. For Boas, the fact that the intellectual culture
of a people was largely constructed, shared and maintained through the use of language, meant that
understanding the language of a cultural group was the key to understanding its culture. At the same
time, though, Boas and his students were aware that culture and language are not directly dependent
on one another. That is, groups with widely different cultures may share a common language, and
speakers of completely unrelated languages may share the same fundamental cultural traits.
Numerous other scholars have suggested that the form of language determines specific cultural traits.
This is similar to the notion of Linguistic determinism, which states that the form of language
determines individual thought. While Boas himself rejected a causal link between language and
culture, some of his intellectual heirs entertained the idea that habitual patterns of speaking and
thinking in a particular language may influence the culture of the linguistic group. Such belief is
related to the theory of Linguistic relativity. Boas, like most modern anthropologists, however, was
more inclined to relate the interconnectedness between language and culture to the fact that, as B.L.
Whorf put it, "they have grown up together".
Indeed, the origin of language, understood as the human capacity of complex symbolic
communication, and the origin of complex culture is often thought to stem from the same
evolutionary process in early man. Evolutionary anthropologist Robin I. Dunbar has proposed that
language evolved as early humans began to live in large communities which required the use of
complex communication to maintain social coherence. Language and culture then both emerged as a
means of using symbols to construct social identity and maintain coherence within a social group too
large to rely exclusively on pre-human ways of building community such as for example grooming.
Since language and culture are both in essence symbolic systems, twentieth century cultural theorists
have applied the methods of analyzing language developed in the science of linguistics to also
analyze culture. Particularly the structural theory of Ferdinand de Saussure which describes
symbolic systems as consisting of signs (a pairing of a particular form with a particular meaning) has
come to be applied widely in the study of culture. But also post-structuralist theories that nonetheless
still rely on the parallel between language and culture as systems of symbolic communication, have
been applied in the field of semiotics. The parallel between language and culture can then be
understood as analog to the parallel between a linguistic sign, consisting for example of the sound
[kau] and the meaning "cow", and a cultural sign, consisting for example of the cultural form of
"wearing a crown" and the cultural meaning of "being king". In this way it can be argued that
culture is itself a kind of language. Another parallel between cultural and linguistic systems is that
they are both systems of practice that is they are a set of special ways of doing things that is
constructed and perpetuated through social interactions. Children, for example, acquire language in
the same way as they acquire the basic cultural norms of the society they grow up in – through
interaction with older members of their cultural group.
However, languages, now understood as the particular set of speech norms of a particular
community, are also a part of the larger culture of the community that speak them. Humans use
language as a way of signalling identity with one cultural group and difference from others. Even
among speakers of one language several different ways of using the language exist, and each is used
25
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
to signal affiliation with particular subgroups within a larger culture. In linguistics such different
ways of using the same language are called "varieties". For example, the English language is spoken
differently in the USA, the UK and Australia, and even within English-speaking countries there are
hundreds of dialects of English that each signals a belonging to a particular region and/or subculture.
For example, in the UK the cockney dialect signals its speakers' belonging to the group of lower
class workers of east London. Differences between varieties of the same language often consist in
different pronunciations and vocabulary, but also sometimes of different grammatical systems and
very often in using different styles (e.g. cockney Rhyming slang or Lawyers' jargon). Linguists and
anthropologists, particularly sociolinguists, ethnolinguists and linguistic anthropologists have
specialized in studying how ways of speaking vary between speech communities.
A community's ways of speaking or signing are a part of the community's culture, just as
other shared practices are. Language use is a way of establishing and displaying group identity.
Ways of speaking function not only to facilitate communication, but also to identify the social
position of the speaker. Linguists call different ways of speaking language varieties, a term that
encompasses geographically or socio-culturally defined dialects as well as the jargons or styles of
subcultures. Linguistic anthropologists and sociologists of language define communicative style as
the ways that language is used and understood within a particular culture..
The difference between languages does not consist only in differences in pronunciation,
vocabulary or grammar, but also in different "cultures of speaking". Some cultures for example have
elaborate systems of "social deixis", systems of signaling social distance through linguistic means.
In English, social deixis is shown mostly though distinguishing between addressing some people by
first name and others by surname, but also in titles such as "Mrs.", "boy", "Doctor" or "Your
Honor", but in other languages such systems may be highly complex and codified in the entire
grammar and vocabulary of the language. In several languages of east Asia, for example Thai,
Burmese and Javanese, different words are used according to whether a speaker is addressing
someone of higher or lower rank than oneself in a ranking system with animals and children ranking
the lowest and gods and members of royalty as the highest. Other languages may use different forms
of address when speaking to speakers of the opposite gender or in-law relatives and many languages
have special ways of speaking to infants and children. Among other groups, the culture of speaking
may entail not speaking to particular people, for example many indigenous cultures of Australia
have a taboo against talking to one's in-law relatives, and in some cultures speech is not addressed
directly to children. Some languages also require different ways of speaking for different social
classes of speakers, and often such a system is based on gender differences, as in Japanese and
Koasati.
Follow-up activity 1.6.True/False: Discuss with your friends and decide whether the
following statements are true or false:
1-A language is the most direct expression of a people’s national character. A language is a
marker of a people’s national identity.
26
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
A variety of different authors, theories and fields purport influences between language and
thought [170].
Many point out the seemingly common-sense realization that upon introspection we seem to
think in the language we speak. A number of writers and theorists have extrapolated upon this idea.
The Sapir–Whorf hypothesis in linguistics states that the grammatical structure of a mother
language influences the way adherents to it perceive the world. The hypothesis has been largely
abandoned by linguists as it has found at best very limited experimental support, at least in its strong
form.
According to Cognitive therapy, founded by Aaron T. Beck, our emotions and behavior are
caused by our internal dialogue.
Neuro-linguistic programming, founded by Richard Bandler and John Grinder, claims that
language "patterns" and other things can affect thought and behavior. It takes ideas from General
Semantics and hypnosis, especially that of the famous therapist Milton Erickson. Many do not
consider it a credible study, and it has no empirical scientific support.
Various other schools of persuasion directly suggest using language in certain ways to
change the minds of others, including oratory, advertising, debate, sales, and rhetoric.
The most obvious influence of language and culture on thought is that of vocabulary. In
addition to words, sentence structures and the persuasiveness of an emotional speech or well-written
novel also influence people’s thought in terms of their cognitive and affective organizations.
The influence of language and thought and behavior can perhaps best be seen in the world of
advertising. The culture beliefs, attitudes, overt and covert aspirations, pragmatic designs and
fantasies, actions and reactions are studied by advertisers around the world to find the basis for the
concepts and language that will inspire the people of any given locale to buy a product of one
manufacturer rather than that of another.
27
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
According to Crystal [20, pp.14-15], it seems evident that there is the closest of relationships
between language and thought: everyday experience suggests that much of our thinking is facilitated
by language. But is there identity between the two? Is it possible to think without language? Or does
our language dictate the ways in which we are able to think? Such matters have exercised
generations of philosophers, psychologists, and linguists, who have uncovered layers of complexity
in these apparently a straightforward question. How close is this relationship between language and
thought? It is usual to see this question in terms of two extremes. First, there is the hypothesis that
language and thought are totally separate entities, with one being dependent on the other. At the
opposite extreme, there is the hypothesis that language and thought are identical- that it is not
possible to engage in any rational thinking without using language.
Within the first position, there are three possibilities: a-language might be dependent upon
thought, b-thought might be dependent on language, and c-language and thought are interdependent.
The traditional view, which is widely held at a popular level, adopts the first of these: people have
thoughts, and then they put these thoughts into words. It is summarized in such metaphorical views
of language as the dress or tool of thought. The second possibility has also been widely held: the
way people use language dictates the lines along which they can think. This view can be represented
in the language acquisition field, in the argument that the child’s earliest encounters with language
are the main influence on the way concepts are learned. The most influential expression of this
position is found in the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis. A third possibility, which is also held these days, is
that language and thought are interdependent – but this is not to say that they are identical. To see
language and thought are interdependent is to recognize that language is a regular part of the process
of thinking, at the same time recognizing that we have to think in order to understand language.
Our contention is that language and thought are not identical. Language and thought differ in
that:
a-Language is material whereas thought is spiritual
b-Units of thought do not coincide with the units of language. Units of thought are concepts,
propositions and interferences whereas units of language are phonemes, words, sentences…
Follow-up activity 1.7.True/False: Discuss with your friends and decide whether the
following statements are true or false:
1-The Sapir–Whorf hypothesis states that social structure influences linguistic structure.
2-Neurolinguistic programming claims that language patterns and other things can affect
thought and behaviour.
3-Various other schools of persuasion directly suggest that using language in certains ways
can change the minds of others.
4-David Crystal’s first hypothesis is that language and thought are totally separate entities,
with one being dependent on the others.
28
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
The extent to which language, thought, culture and society are interrelated and influence one
another have been matters of controversy. Stated simplistically, the current concensus is that they
are interrelated aspects of a whole: the human being. How are language, culture, thought and
society interrelated?
The origin of language and the origin of culture is often thought to stem from the same
evolutionary process in early man. Language and culture are both in essense symbolic systems.
They both emerged as means of using symbols to communicate and to construct socio-cultural
identity. Language is, most of all, an important means of human communication in the society.
Humans use language for the sake of communication and as a way of signalling identity with one
cultural group and difference from others. Culture is also a communication system. It uses verbal
and non-verbal cues that distinguish one speech community from another. Culture makes it possible
for human society to communicate using verbal and non-verbal language. Culture helps determine
the appropriate communicative behaviour within a variety of social and physical contexts by
prescribing certain rules. When communicating with members of your own culture, you and other
people rely on deeply internalised cultural rules that prescribed the normative behaviour for specific
communication situation. These rules facilitate your ability to communicate effectively with one
another. Each culture has its particular rules or conventions that largely dertermine language usage
in restrictive speech events in daily communication. Culture also determines a particular
communication, negotiation, persuation and discussion style. Communication is culture and culture
is communication. It is through communication that culture is passed down, created and modified
from one generation to the next. Culture is also an integral part of the interaction between language
and thought. Another important point in the relationship between language, culture and society is
that language use varies according to the socio-cultural contexts in which language is used. The
socio-cultural contexts are very important in appropriate language choice in communication. In
conclusion, language, culture, society and thought are interrelated aspects of human beings living in
a society.
Whether one begins or ends with language, thought, society or culture, the other aspects are
woven in; the circular patterns holds, with each influencing and being influenced by each of the
others. They are not all the same thing, but one can not survive without the other.
29
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Follow-up-activity 1.8. True / False: Decide whether the following are true or false:
1-Language is the most important means of communication in the society.
2-Language is culture-specific and culture-bound.
3-A language is never a marker of identity.
4-Language and culture provide the socio-cultural contexts for appropriate language use.
5- Language, culture, society and thought are interrelated aspects of human beings living in a
society.
CHAPTER I REVIEW
I-Discuss with your friends and answer the following questions:
1-What is sociolinguistics?
2-What is the main difference between sociolinguistics and the sociology of language?
3-What are the main social factors which have an influence on language use?
8-What is culture?
9-Which of the definitions of culture presented above do you like best and why?
10-What are the purposes of a culture? What are the aspects of a culture?
13-What are the functions of language at societal level? Group and individual level?
30
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
16-How does the social structure influence linguistic structure and/or behaviour?
18-Which of the relationships between language and culture are important to you?
22-Which of the three possibilities within David Crystal’s first hypothesis do you think is the
most accepted concerning the relationship between language and thought?
23-What are the possible relationships between language, culture and society?
II-True/False: Discuss with your friends and decide whether the following statements
are true or false:
4-Society and culture act as the social and cultural contexts for language use.
5-The term society is used to refer to the people in general living together in communities.
8-The term social role refers to a pattern of behaviour that is associated with a particular
position in society.
9-Power and solidarity are the two types of social relationships which affect language use in
communication.
13- Hall (1976) differentiated cultures on the basis of different communication styles: low-
context cultures and high-context cultures.
14-Culture makes it possible for human society to communicate using verbal and non-verbal
codes of communication. Culture determines a particular communication, negotiation, persuation,
and discussion style.
18-Linguistic structure and or behaviour may either influence or determine social structure.
19-In the Whorfian hypothesis, language provides a screen or filter to reality. It determines
how speakers perceive or organize the world around them.
20-The third possible relationship between language and society is that the relationship is bi-
directional. Language and society may influence each other.
21-A language is the most direct expression of a people’s national character. A language is a
marker of a people’s national identity.
26-The Sapir–Whorf hypothesis states that social structure influences linguistic structure.
27-Various other schools of persuasion directly suggest that using language in certains ways
can change the minds of others.
28-David Crystal’s first hypothesis is that language and thought are totally separate entities,
with one being dependent on the others.
30-Language, culture, society and thought are interrelated aspects of human beings living in
a society.
32
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Chapter II Contents
1. Language Varieties
2. Dialect
3. Standard Language
4. International Language
5. Idiolect
6. Territorial and Regional Dialects of the
English Language and the Vietnamese
Language
7. Sociolect
8. Code Choice
1. LANGUAGE VARIETIES
Pre-lecture activity 2.1. Discuss:
1-Are there differences in pronunciation, morphology, vocabulary and syntax between
British English and American English? Give examples.
2-Do males and females speak differently?
In other words, in every community there is a range of varieties from which people select
according to the social contexts in which they are communicating. Language variation is
influenced by social factors of which the most important are participants, setting, topic and
function.
According to Troudale [100, pp.53-55] there are three waves of variationist sociolinguistics.
The first wave (associated with variationist studies in the 1960s and 1970s, such as Labov, 1966
and Trudgill, 1974) was concerned with surveys of urban populations, where informants were
selected using a method of (quasi-) random sampling. Many such surveys were concerned with
the relationship between variation and ongoing change in the community, particularly with the
social embedding and transmission of language change. The second wave of studies, exemplified
by Rickford (1986), took a broadly ethnographic approach: investigators spent a great deal of time
in the community under observation, becoming part of that community in some cases, and trying
to understand the social practices of the individual involved. Such studies began to consider the
social meaning of linguistic variation, and the projection of identity which could be shown to
correlate with particular linguistic practices. The studies also aimed to show connections
between local and supra-local linguistic patterns. The issue of identity has become even more
prominent in the third wave of social dialectology, where variation (particularly stylistic variation)
34
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
According to Holmes [45, p.6], sociolinguists use the term variety (or sometimes
code) to refer to any set of linguistic forms which patterns according to social factors. Variety is a
sociolinguistic term referring to language in context. A variety is a set of linguistic forms used
under specific social circumstances, i.e. with a distinctive social distribution. Variety is, therefore,
a broad term which includes different accents, different linguistic styles, different dialects and even
different languages which contrast with each other for social reasons. It has proved a very useful
sociolinguistic term because it is linguistically neutral and covers all the different realisations of
the abstract concept “language’ in different social contexts.
Hudson [49, p.24] states that “a variety of language is a set of linguistic items with similar
social distribution”. O’Grady, Dobrovolky and Aronoff [74, p.126] employ the term
language variety to refer to “any distinguishable form of speech used by a speaker or a group of
speakers”. According to Wardhaugh [107, p.23] a language variety is “ a specific set of
‘linguistic items’ or ‘human speech patterns’ (presumably, sounds, words, grammatical features,
etc.) which we can uniquely associate with some external factor (presumably, a geographical area
or a social group)”.
For our purpose, a language variety is a set of specific forms of a language used by a
speaker or a group of speakers in a certain social context. Varieties can be languages, dialects,
styles or registers.
Saville-Troike [86, pp.62-86] indicates that there are the following language varieties:
a-Varieties associated with setting
b-Varieties associated with activity domain
c-Varieties associated with region
d-Varieties associated with ethnicity
e-Varieties associated with social class, status, and role
f-Varieties associated with role-relationships
g-Varieties associated with sex
h-Varieties associated with age
i-Varieties associated with personality states and “abnormal speech”
j-Native and non-native varieties.
35
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
A COMMON CORE or nucleus is present in all the varieties so that, however esoteric a
variety may be, it has running through it a set of grammatical and other characteristics that are
present in all the others. It is this fact that justifies the application of the name English to all the
varieties.
“A linguistic variable is a linguistic item which has various forms (variants)” [82, p.304].
The different forms (variants) of a linguistic variable are related to differences in linguistic and
socio-cultural factors. There are linguistic variables in the phonology, morphology, lexicon and
syntax of a language.
c-Lexicon
i-Child, offspring and kid: synonyms in English on different levels of English
usage. Offspring is formal, child neutral and kid in formal.
ii-Autumn-fall: territorial synonyms. Autumn is used in the UK whereas fall is used in
the US.
d-Syntax
i-I have not been to Canberra.
I haven’t been to Canberra.
36
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
English: How do you do?/ Good morning/ Good afternoon / Good evening
/ Good day
Vietnamese: Chào anh, chào chị / Đi đâu đấy? / Ăn cơm chưa?
As seen above, each variable has a set of variants. When we study variation, it is
important to define as precisely as possible what the objects of the investigation (the variables)
are. The general or abstract feature that you are investigating is what is called the variable. The
actual realisation of the variable in speech is known as the variant. We often see co-variation
between linguistic variables and other variables associated with different forms (variants of the
variable) may be related to differences in the socio- economic background, education, age,
gender…. of the speakers.
The important fact to remember is that a linguistic variable is an item in the structure of a
language, an item that has alternate realizations (variants), as one speaker realizes it one way and
another a different way or the same speaker realizes it differently on different occasions.
For a clearer example of the notion of linguistic variable, let us examine the /h/ variable
with two variants [h] and [Ø] in British English.
According to Chambers & Trudgill [1980, as cited in Crystal [20, p.32], in British English,
the accent which carries most prestige pronounces /h/ at the beginning of words such as head. But
in most other accents of England and Wales, it is common to omit /h/ in this position. This means
that in British English the /h/ variable with two variants [h] and [Ø]. Regions do not pronounce or
omit /h/with total consistency, however, as can be seen from the results of two studies of this
variable carried out in Norwich and Bradford.
The research topic was /h/-dropping in Bradford and Norwick. The research questions are:
1-What are the situations of /h/ dropping in Bradford and Norwick? and 2-What are the social
factors that lead to /h/dropping in Bradford and Norwick?
The informants were grouped into five social classes, based on such factors as their
occupation, income, and education. The proportion of /h/-dropping was calculated with the
following results:
37
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Table 2.1. The proportion of /h/-dropping [Chambers & Trudgill, 1980, as cited in [20, p.32].
Class Bradford Norwich
The correlation is clear. In both areas, there is more /h/-dropping as one moves down the
social scale. The social factor that led to /h/ dropping was social class differences. Moreover, the
proportion is always greater in Bradford, suggesting that the phenomenon has been longer
established in that area.
Follow-up activity 2.1.True/False: Discuss with your friends and decide whether
the following statements are true or false:
4-A linguistic variable is a linguistic item which has only one zero variant.
2.1. Language
A language has been defined as a system of communication. A language can be thought of
as a collection of dialects that are usually linguistically similar, used by different social groups who
choose to say that they are speakers of one language which functions to unite and represent them to
other groups [45, p.138].
38
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
2.2. Dialect
The term dialect (from the ancient Greek word Διάλεκτος diálektos, "discourse", from διά
diá, "through" + λέγω legō, "I speak") has been used by linguists to refer to a variety of a language
that is a characteristic of a particular group of the language's speakers. The term is applied most
often to regional speech patterns, but a dialect may also be defined by other factors, such as social
class. A dialect that is associated with a particular social class can be termed a sociolect; a regional
dialect may be termed a regiolect or topolect [123].
In Oxford Advanced Learner’s Dictionary [47], the term dialect is defined as follows: ”The
form of a language that is spoken in one area with grammar, words or pronunciations that
may be different from other forms of the same language” [47, p.402].
“A dialect is a variety of language, spoken in one part of a particular region (regional
dialect) or by people belonging to a particular social class (social dialect or sociolect),
which is different in some words, grammar and / or pronunciation from other forms of
the same language. A dialect is often associated with a particular accent” [82, p.151].
Dialects are linguistic varieties which differ in pronunciation, vocabulary and grammar
from each other and from Standard English (which is itself a dialect). A dialect is distinguished by
its vocabulary, grammar, and pronunciation (phonology, including prosody). Consider the
following examples [45, pp.132-133]:
A British visitor to New Zealand decided that while he was in Auckland he would look up
an old friend from his war days. He found the address, walked up the path and knocked on the
door.
‘Gidday,’said the young man who opened the door. ‘What can I do for you?’
‘I’ve called to see me old mate Don Stone,’ said the visitor.
‘Oh he’s dead now mate,’ said the young man.
The visitor was about to express condolences when he was thumped on the back by Don
Stone himself. The young man had said, ‘Here’s dad now mate’, as his father came in the gate.
There are many such stories of mistakes based on regional accent differences. To British
ears, a New Zealander’s dad sounds like an English person’s dead, bad sounds like bed and six
sounds like sucks. Americans and Australians, as well as New Zealanders, tell of British visitors
who were given pens instead of pins and pans instead of pens. On the other hand, an American’s
god sounds like an English person’s guard, and an American’s ladder is pronounced identically
with latter.
There are vocabulary differences in the varieties spoken in different regions too.
Australians talk of sole parents, for example, while people in England call them single parents,
and New Zealanders call them solo parents. South Africans use the term robot for British traffic-
light. British wellies (Wellington boots) are New Zealand gummies (gumboots), while the word
togs refers to very different types of clothes in different places. In New Zealand, togs are what you
39
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Similarly, the pronunciation of bath with the same vowel as in sat distinguishes a
speaker from the north of England from a southerner.
Pronunciation and vocabulary differences are probably the differences people are most
aware of between different dialects of English, but there are grammatical differences, too. Consider
the following examples [45, pp.132-133]:
The dialects of a single language may be defined as mutually intelligible forms of that
language which differ in systematic ways from each other [29, p.251].
40
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Haugen [40] has pointed out that language and dialect are ambiguous terms.
Ordinary people use these terms quite freely in speech; for them a dialect is almost
certainly no more than a local non-prestigious variety of a real language. In contrast, scholars
often experience considerable difficulty in deciding whether one term should be used rather than
the other in certain situations.
Haugen points out that the confusion goes back to the Ancient Greeks. The Greek
language in Ancient Greece was actually a group of distinct local varieties (Ionic, Doric, and Attic)
descended by divergence from a common spoken source with each variety having its own literary
traditions and uses, e.g. Ionic for history, Doric for choral and lyric works, and Attic for tragedy.
Later, Athenian Greek, the koinē - or ‘common’ language- became the norm for the spoken
language. Haugen points out that the Greek situation has provided the model for all later usages of
the two terms with the resulting ambiguity.
The meanings of the Greek terms which were translated as ‘language’ and ‘dialect’ were in
fact quite different from the meanings these words have in English now. In the English language,
the term dialect is used both for local varieties of English and for the various types of informal,
lower class, or rural speech. In terms of social norm, a dialect is a language that is excluded from
polite society. It is often equivalent to nonstandard or even substandard. Chambers and Trudgill
[12] state that in common usage, a dialect is a substandard, low-status, often rustic form of
language, generally associated with the peasantry, the working class, or other groups lacking in
prestige. Dialect is also a term which is often applied to forms of language, particularly those
spoken in more isolated parts of the world, which have no written form [12, p.3].
The stereotypical ‘dialect’ speaker is an elderly rural person who is all but unintelligible to
modern city dweller. Dialects originally meant language varieties based on geography
(Geographical variety). Dialect is a term referring to a variety of a language spoken in a certain
place; that is, a geographically distinct variety of a language. There are urban and rural dialects.
Popular notions about dialects are:
Gumperz [1971, as cited in [107, p.28-29] pointed out that there is some difficulty in
distinguishing between the two terms language and dialect due to the following reasons:
a-In some cases, two language varieties which are structurally similar and mutually
41
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
intelligible by their speakers are considered two different languages, e.g. local varieties of Dutch and
German on the border, or Thai and Laos languages.
b-In other cases, there are dialects of a language which are structurally different and
mutually unintelligible by their speakers, e.g. China situation (Cantonese and Mandarin)
Perhaps, some of the difficulties we have with trying to define the terms language and
dialect arise from trying to subsume that various different types of systems of communication
under the one label.
One such attempt [Bell, 1976, as cited in [107, pp.31-38] has listed seven criteria that may be
useful in discussing different kinds of languages. These are: a-standardization, b-vitality, c-
historicity, d-autonomy, e-reduction, f-mixture and g-de facto norms.
Steward puts forth for criteria to differentiate between the terms language, dialect and other
varieties of the same language [1968, as cited in [111, p.119]. They are standardization, autonomy,
historicity and vitality.
Table 2.2. Steward ‘s criteria to differentiate between language and dialect
[1968, as cited in [111, p.119].
Standardisation Autonomy Historicity Vitality
Standard + + + +
language
Dialects - - + +
the language, but also the language functions defined by the socio-political factors: the social
values of language and language varieties. A language has prestige which a dialect lacks. A
language can be regarded as a collection of mutually intelligible dialects (or varieties). A dialect
is a subordinate variety of a language. We can regard a language as a collection of mutually
intelligible dialects.
Languages are not purely linguistic entities. They serve social functions. In order to
define a language, it is important to look to its social and political functions, as well as its linguistic
features. So a language can be thought of as a collection of dialects that are usually linguistically
similar, used by different social groups who choose to say that they are speakers of one language
which functions to unite and represent them to other groups [45, p.138].
2.4. Isogloss
Isogloss is a line shown on a map and which represents the boundary between two
linguistic features, for example the isogloss which separates the use of [ʊ] (in the north of England)
from [ʌ] (in the south of England) in the strut lexical set or the isogloss separating the presence or
absence of non-prevocalic /r/: the absence is typical of large regions of the south, but parts of the
south and south-west still retain /r/ in this position.
In each country people may speak a language with several dialects. Northern American
English, Midland American English and Southern American English are different regional dialects
of American English. Regional dialects are the dialects which are used in different regions of the
same country.
2.5.2. Sociolects
Social groups or classes with factors such as occupation, place of residence, education,
income, racial or ethnic origin, caste, religion, gender may use a variety of language in
communication. This variety is called a sociolect.
43
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Follow-up activity 2.2.True/False: Discuss with your friends and decide whether the
following statements are true or false:
1-The dialects of a single language may be defined as mutually intelligible forms of that
language which differ in systematic ways from each other.
3-The preferred view of dialects is that everyone speaks a dialect. No dialect is superior to
the others.
4-In the English language the term dialect is used for both local variaties of English and for
the various types of informal, lower class, or rural speech.
”The standard language (also standard dialect, standard language, standard) is the variety
of a language which has the highest status in a community or nation, and which is usually
based on the speech and writing of educated native speakers of the language” [82, p.271].
‘Standard language is (the type of language variety) believed to be correct and used by
most people’ [47, p.1438].
The creation of a prescriptive standard language, derives from the national (cultural,
political, social) cohesion requiring an agreed, standardized tongue. Generally, standard languages
usually are established upon [189]:
Standard varieties are codified varieties. Codification can be achieved through grammar
45
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
and dictionaries which record, and sometimes prescribe, the standard forms of the language.
Dictionary writers generally take the usage of educated and socially prestigious members of the
community as their criterion.
A standardised variety can also be used to give prestige to speakers, marking off those who
employed it from those who do not. Another term for standard variety is “superposed variety”.
3.2. Standardisation:
Standardisation refers to the process by which a language has been codified in some way.
That process usually involves the development of such things as grammars, spelling, books,
dictionaries, and possibly a literature. Standardisation is sometimes deliberately undertaken quite
rapidly for political reasons. The standardisation process occasionally results in some languages
actually achieving more than one standard variety.
46
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
The first two refer primarily to the form, the last two to the function of language. The first
and the last are concerned with society, the second and third with language.
Table 2.3. The four aspects of language development [40]
Form Function
Society Selection Acceptance
Language Codification Elaboration
Follow-up activity 2.3.True/False: Discuss with your friends and decide whether
the following statements are true or false:
1-The standard language / variety is the variety of a language which has the highest status in
a community or nation, and which is usually based on the speech and writing of educated native
speakers of the language.
4-In the process of codification of the form, a variety is chosen as a norm to be developed.
4. INTERNATIONAL LANGUAGE
According to Wikipedia [167] the term international language can be used to refer to:
The term "auxiliary" implies that it is intended to be an additional language for the
people of the world, rather than to replace their native languages. Often, the phrase is used to
refer to planned or constructed languages proposed specifically to ease worldwide international
47
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
However, it can also refer to the concept of such a language being determined by
international consensus, including even a standardized natural language (e.g. International
English), and has also been connected to the project of constructing a universal language.
In other conceptions, it may be the primary language of all speakers, or the only existing
language. Some mythological or religious traditions state that there was once a single universal
language among all people, or shared by humans and supernatural beings, however, this is not
supported by historical evidence.
The original Lingua Franca was a mixed language composed mostly (80%) of Italian with
a broad vocabulary drawn from Turkish, French, Spanish, Greek and Arabic. It was in use
throughout the eastern Mediterranean as the language of commerce and diplomacy in and around
the Renaissance era. At that time, Italian speakers dominated seaborne commerce in the port cities
of the Ottoman empire.
Franca was the Italian word for Frankish. Its usage in the term lingua franca originated
from its meaning in Arabic, dating from before the Crusades, whereby all Europeans were
called "Franks" or Faranji in Arabic. The term lingua franca is first recorded in English in
1678.
48
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
In this respect, major world languages are dominated by languages of European origin. The
historical reason for this is the period of European colonialism. World languages originating with
historical colonial empires include English, Spanish, Portuguese and French.
Follow-up activity 2.4.True/False: Discuss with your friends and decide whether
the following statements are true or false:
1-The term International language can be used to refer to a universal language, a lingua
franca, or a world language.
2-IAL is a language means for communication between people for different nations who do
not share a common native language.
3-The term universal language may refer to a hypothetical, historical, mythical or
constructed language to be spoken and understood by all living things, beings and objects alike.
4-A lingual franca is a language systematically used to communicate between persons not
sharing a mother tongue.
5-A world language is a language spoken in a certain country.
The language system of an individual as expressed by the way he or she speaks or writes
within the overall system of a particular language.
In its widest sense, someone’s idiolect includes their way of communicating, for example,
their choice of utterances and the way they interpret the utterances made by others. In a narrower
sense, an idiolect may include those features, either in speech or writing, which distinguish one
individual from others, such as voice, quality, pitch and speech rhythm.
49
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
In each country people may speak a language with several dialects. Northern American
English, Midland American English and Southern American English are different regional dialects
of American English. Regional dialects are the dialects which are used in different regions of the
same countries.
For more information on territorial and regional dialects of the English language, please refer
to [172].
The term British English is used by linguists to contrast the form of English used in Britain
with American English, and also with Australian English, South African English, etc. In broad
terms, British English is English as used throughout the United Kingdom, but it is often more
narrowly understood as the English language of England.
Dialects and accents vary amongst the four countries of the United Kingdom, as well as
within the countries themselves. The major divisions are normally classified as English English
(or English as spoken in England), Welsh English (not to be confused with the Welsh language),
Irish English and Scottish English (not to be confused with the Scots language).
6.2.2. A brief history of the development of the English language
English belongs to Anglo-Frisian group within the Western branch of the Germanic
languages, a sub-family of the Indo-European languages. It is related most closely to the Frisian
language, to a lesser extent to Netherlandic (Dutch-Flemish) and the Low German (Platteutsch)
dialects, and more distantly to modern High German.
50
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
English was not the original language of England. In fact, three languages or language
groups preceded English as spoken languages in the British Isles before the Anglo- Saxons began
helping themselves to island real estate. The original language of the British Isles is not known,
but archaeological investigations verify that the British Isles were thoroughly inhabited before the
arrival of the Celts.
Sometime during the millennium before Christ, Celtic peoples began settling the British
Isles. The Celtic languages make up the Celtic branch of Indo-European and are, therefore,
distantly related to English. Of the various Celtic languages once spoken on the island, some
have become extinct, Cornish as recently as 200 years ago, while others have survived and are still
spoken today: Irish in Ireland, Scottish Gaelic in Scotland, and Welsh in Wales.
The following part will look at the development of the English language:
The word "English" is from Old English "Englisc", and that comes from the name of the
Angles. The Angles were named from Engle, their land of origin. England and English (originally
Englaland and Englisc) are named after the Angles.
51
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
French, which becomes the official language in England. Educated English people trilingual
(French, Latin, English). Chaucer. England begins to become recognised as a political entity within
Britain.
e-Early Modern English (c.1450 – 1750)
Includes the Renaissance, the Elizabethan era and Shakespeare. The role of the Church, of
Latin and of French declines and English becomes a language of science and government.
Britain grows commercially and acquires overseas colonies. English taken to the Americas,
Australia, India. Slave trade carries African people speaking different African languages to
Caribbean and America, giving rise to English creoles. English acquires a typographic identity
with the rise of printing. Many attempts to ‘standardise and fix’ the language with dictionaries
and grammars. Union of England and Scotland, 1707.
For further information on the history of the English language, please, refer to [129].
6.2.2.2. The introduction of English into other parts of Great Britain [36, pp.4-10).
English arrived early in Scotland. By the seventh century the northern English kingdom of
Bernicia had extended its territory—and its dialect—into what is now Southern Scotland. This
dialect is the source of Scots, an ancient dialect of English that may be viewed as parallel with
Modern English in their common derivation from Old English. By the middle of the sixteenth
century Scots was becoming influenced by English in word forms and spellings, a process
encouraged by the use of English Bibles in Scotland in the absence of a Scots Bible. When James
VI of Scotland succeeded Queen Elizabeth I in 1603 to become James I of England, combining the
thrones of the two kingdoms, there was a quickening of the pace of adoption of English in Scotland
for writing and by the gentry for speech. The final blow to Scots as the standard dialect of Scotland
was the Act of Union in 1707, when the two kingdoms were formally united. Despite attempts at
reviving Scots, it remains restricted mainly to literary uses and to some rural speech. It has,
however, influenced Scottish English, the standard variety of English in Scotland.
About 80,000 people speak Scottish Gaelic, a Celtic language that is confined to the West
Highlands and the Western Isles of Scotland, but nearly all of them are bilingual in Gaelic
and English.
52
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Wales was England's first colony. It was ruled from England as a principality from the
beginning of the fourteenth century, and was incorporated into England by the Acts of Union of
1535 and 1543, which promoted the use of English for official purposes. The standard variety of
English in Wales is thought to be identical with that in England. There are, however, distinctive
Welsh English accents. According to a 1991 census, over half a million inhabitants of Wales above
the age of 3 (19 per cent) speak Welsh, a Celtic language, most of whom are bilingual in Welsh
and English. As a result of current education policies, the number of Welsh speakers among the
young is now increasing.
English was permanently introduced into Ireland when the Normans invaded the country
during the twelfth century and settled French and English speakers in the eastern coastal region,
though many of their descendants adopted Irish (or Irish Gaelic), the Celtic language of the native
inhabitants. In the sixteenth century the Tudor monarchs began a policy of bringing to Ireland
large numbers of English settlers, and later also Scottish settlers, to displace the Irish from their
land. By 1800 English was the language of half the population. The famines of 1846-8 led to mass
emigration from Ireland, most of those who emigrated being Irish speakers, the poorer part of the
population. During the nineteenth century English was promoted in the Catholic education system
in opposition to the use of Irish by Protestant proselytizing societies. Despite attempts since
independence to revive the use of Irish in the Republic of Ireland, there are few Irish monolinguals
and perhaps only 2 per cent of the population use Irish regularly.
6.2.2.3. The spread of English in other first-language countries [36, pp. 4-10]:
Britain was an Empire for 200 years between the 18th and 20th centuries and English
language continued to change as the British Empire moved across the world - to the USA, Australia,
New Zealand, India, Asia and Africa. They sent people to settle and live in their conquered
places and as settlers interacted with natives, new words were added to the English vocabulary.
Beginning in the early seventeenth century, the English language was transported beyond
the British Isles by traders, soldiers, and settlers. During the next two centuries Britain acquired
territories throughout the world. In some of these territories, British settlers were sufficiently
numerous to dominate the country linguistically as well as in other respects, so that the indigenous
population came to adopt English as their first or second language.
More importantly for the future of English, the numbers of the early settlers were swelled
enormously by waves of immigration and even when the newcomers brought another language
their descendants generally spoke English as their first language. All the major countries outside
the British Isles where English is the dominant language have succeeded in assimilating
linguistically their immigrants from non-English-speaking countries: the United States, Canada,
Australia, and New Zealand.
The first permanent English settlements were established in the New World, beginning with
53
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
the founding of Jamestown in 1607. The colonial period came to an end when the American
colonies rejected British rule in the War of Independence (1776-83). Both before and after their
independence, the Americans acquired territories that were occupied by speakers of other
languages—Amerindian languages, Dutch, French, and Spanish. These have influenced American
English, together with the languages of immigrants in late periods—notably German and Yiddish.
It is estimated that over 27 million United States residents speak a language other than English at
home, about half of whom use Spanish. Every year over half a million new immigrants enter the
United States, most of them from non-English-speaking countries and most of them Spanish
speakers.
Canada became a British possession in 1763, wrested from the French. After the
American War of Independence, large numbers of loyalists settled in Canada, followed during the
next century by waves of immigrants from the United States and the British Isles. Canada has a
large minority of unilingual French speakers (nearly 17 per cent), concentrated in the province of
Quebec, as well as an almost equal percentage of bilingual speakers in French and English,
which are the joint official languages of Canada. Virtually all Canadians speak English or French,
apart from some rural indigenous or immigrant communities.
In 1770 Captain James Cook claimed the eastern coast of Australia for Britain. Soon
afterwards, penal colonies were established to which convicts were transported from Britain. Until
after the Second World War, immigration from Asian countries was restricted and most
immigrants were English-speaking. Many of the Aborigines (the indigenous population before
British colonization), who number fewer than 200,000, speak only English. The first British
settlement in New Zealand was in 1792. New Zealand became part of New South Wales and then
after 1840 a British colony in its own right. Most settlers have been English-speaking. The
indigenous Maori language, spoken by about 300,000, has official status in the courts.
Most of the other countries where English is the majority language are islands with
relatively small populations located in the Atlantic or Caribbean (for example, Bermuda and
Grenada) and were once—or still are—British colonies.
As seen, the English language came to British Isles from northern Europe in the fifth
century. From the fifteenth century, the British began to sail all over the world and became
explorers, colonists and imperialists. They took the English language to North America, Canada
and the Caribbean, to South Africa, to Australia and New Zealand, to South Asia (especially
India), to the British colonies in Africa, to South East Asia and the South Pacific.
54
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
55
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
West Midlands Veree few cahs mayd it oop the long ill
Central Midlands Veri few cahs mayd it oop the long ill
Northeast Midlands Veree few cahs made it up the long hill
East Midlands Veree foo cahs mayd it oop the long ill
Upper Southwest Veree few carrs mayd it up the long ill
Central Southwest Veree few carrs mayd it up the long iooll
Lower Southwest Veree foo cahs mayd it up the long iooll
South Midlands Veree few cars made it up the long hill
East Anglia Veree foo cahs mayd it up the long (h)ill
Home Counties Veree few cahs mayd it up the long iooll
Two major areas in Trudgill classification of modern dialect areas in England are Northern
and Southern.
Map 2.1. and Figure 2.1. Modern English dialecsts [21, p.325].
56
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
The most important differences between Northern and Southern areas in terms of
pronunciation are the vowels /ɑ:/ and /ʌ/. The North of England is somewhat more conservative in
its phonology compared to the South and has not gone through many of the changes found in the
South of England. The two most obvious of these are:
a-the lengthening of low vowels before voiceless fricatives, for example, [pæs] for [pa:s] as
in the word pass.
Table 2.5. North and south pronunciation differences of the vowel /ɑ:/
b-the lowering of Early Modern English /ʊ/ to /ʌ/, for example, [kʊt] for [kʌt] as the the
word cut.
Table 2.6. North and South pronunciation differences of the vowel /ʌ/.
57
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Scottish dialect expressions that are well known to English people are aye for ‘yes’, wee
58
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
for ‘little’, bairns for ‘children’ and ‘I dinna ken’ for ‘I don’t know’. Scottish
pronunciation is noted for its burred r’s and distinctive vowel sounds. Words like rice for tide
are pronounced more like /reis/ or /teid/ than standard /rais/ or /taid/ sound. Educated Scottish
accents have features in common with RP.
In Wales, dialect usage include boyo for ‘man’ and look you for ‘you see’. Well-known
Irish dialect forms include Would you be after wanting for do you want, and the repetition of a
phrase at the end of a sentence, such as at all, at al. Welsh, Irish and some Scottish accents often
have an attractive lilt (=rising and falling intonation pattern).
These dialects and accents are sometimes made fun of.
59
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
have changed somewhat over the past 300 years, but not as rapidly (because of the relative isolation
of the speakers) as elsewhere in the country.
By contrast, many of the Plymouth colonists came from counties in the east of England – in
particular, Lincolnshire, Nottinghamshire, Essex, Kent and London, with some from the Midlands,
and a few from further afield. These eastern accents were rather different – notably, lacking an r after
vowels – and they proved to be the dominant influence in this area. The tendency ‘not to pronounce
the r’ is still a feature of the speech of people from New England.
The later population movements across America largely preserved the dialect distinctions
which arose out of these early patterns of settlement. The New England people moved west into the
region of the Great Lakes; the southerners moved along the Gulf Coast and into Texas; and the
midlanders spread throughout the whole of the vast, mid-western area, across the Mississippi and
ultimately into California. The dialect picture was never a neat one, because of widespread north–
south movements within the country, and the continuing inflow of immigrants from different parts of
the world. There are many mixed dialect areas, and pockets of unexpected dialect forms. But the
main divisions of north, midland, and south are still found throughout America today.
During the seventeenth century, new shiploads of immigrants brought an increasing variety
of linguistic backgrounds into the country. Pennsylvania, for example, came to be settled mainly by
Quakers whose origins were mostly in the Midlands and the north of England. People speaking very
different kinds of English thus found themselves living alongside each other, as the ‘middle’ Atlantic
areas (New York, in particular) became the focus of settlement. As a result, the sharp divisions
between regional dialects gradually began to blur.
Then, in the eighteenth century, there was a vast wave of immigration from northern Ireland.
The Irish had been migrating to America from around 1600, but the main movements took place
during the 1720s, when around 50,000 Irish and Scots-Irish immigrants arrived. By the time
independence was declared (1776), it is thought that one in seven of the colonial population was
Scots-Irish. Many stayed along the coast, especially in the area of Philadelphia, but most moved
inland through the mountains in search of land. They were seen as frontier people, with an accent
which at the time was described as ‘broad’. The opening up of the south and west was largely due
to the pioneering spirit of this group of settlers.
By the time of the first census, in 1790, the population of the country was around 4 million,
most of whom lived along the Atlantic coast. A century later, after the opening up of the west, the
population numbered over 50 million, spread throughout the continent. The accent which emerged
can now be heard all over the so-called Sunbelt (from Virginia to southern California), and is the
accent most commonly associated with present-day American speech.
It was not only England which influenced the directions that the English language was to take
in America, and later the USA. The Spanish had occupied large parts of the west and south-west.
The French were present in the northern territories, around the St Lawrence River, and throughout
the middle regions (French Louisiana) as far as the Gulf of Mexico. The Dutch were in New York
(originally New Amsterdam) and the surrounding area. Large numbers of Germans began to arrive at
the end of the seven-teenth century, settling mainly in Pennsylvania and its hinterland. In addition,
60
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
there were increasing numbers of Africans entering the south, as a result of the slave trade, and this
dramatically in- creased in the eighteenth century: a population of little more than 2,500 black slaves
in 1700 had become about 100,000 by 1775, far out-numbering the southern whites.
The nineteenth century saw a massive increase in American immigration, as people fled the
results of revolution, poverty, and famine in Europe. Large numbers of Irish came following the
potato famine in Ireland in the 1840s. Germans and Italians came, escaping the consequences of the
failed 1848 revolutions. And, as the century wore on, there were increasing numbers of Central
European Jews, especially fleeing from the pogroms of the 1880s. In the first two decades of the
twentieth century, immigrants were entering the USA at an average of three-quarters of a million a
year. In 1900, the population was just over 75million. This total had doubled by 1950.
Within one or two generations of arrival, most of these immigrant families had come to speak
English, through a natural process of assimilation. Grandparents and grandchildren found themselves
living in very different linguistic worlds. The result was a massive growth in mother-tongue use of
English.
According to the 1990 census, the number of people (over five years of age) who spoke only
English at home had grown to over 198 million – 86 per cent of the population. This figure increased
to 215million in the 2000 census (though representing a fall to 82 per cent of the population). This is
almost four times as many mother-tongue speakers as any other nation.
Some commentators have suggested that the English language was a major factor in
maintaining American unity throughout this period of remarkable cultural diversification – a ‘glue’
which brought people together and a medium which gave them common access to opportunity. At
the same time, some minority groups began to be concerned about the preservation of their cultural
and linguistic heritage, within a society which was becoming increasingly monolingual. The seeds of
a conflict between the need for intelligibility and the need for identity were beginning to grow – a
conflict which, by the later decades of the twentieth century, had fuelled the movement in support of
English as the official language of the USA.
6.3.2. American English [120]
6.3.2.1. American English
American English, also called United States English or U.S. English, is the set of
varieties of the English language native to the United States. English is the most widely spoken
language in the United States and is the common language used by the federal government,
considered the de facto language of the country because of its widespread use.
The use of English in the United States is a result of British colonization of the Americas.
The first wave of English-speaking settlers arrived in North America during the 17th century,
followed by further migrations in the 18th and 19th centuries. Since then, American English
has been influenced by the languages of West Africa, the Native American population, German,
Dutch, Irish, Spanish, and other languages of successive waves of immigrants to the United States.
Any American English accent perceived as free of noticeably local, ethnic, or cultural
61
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
markers is popularly called "General American," described by sociolinguist William Labov as "a
fairly uniform broadcast standard in the mass media," [as cited in [120] but otherwise not a
mainstream standard English of the country, according to historical and present linguistic evidence.
According to Schilling [88, p.38), there are at least three theories for where and when
General American English arose. The first theory is that General American English is New
England English: Yankee English. The second theory is that General American English was born of
the early dialect mix in Philadenphia, the mid-Atlantic, and points westward – the Midland dialect
area. The third theory is that General American English comes from a koine, the one that arose in
the 1800s, in the interior of the United States.
The term "General American" was first disseminated by American English scholar George
Philip Krapp, who, in 1925, described it as "Western" but "not local in character." In 1930,
American linguist John Samuel Kenyon, who largely popularized the term, considered it
equivalent to the speech of "the North," or "Northern American," but, in 1934, "Western and
Midwestern."
62
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
By the 1940s, a common definition for General American was any American English
accent that excluded the regionally distinct sounds of the American South, Eastern New England,
and New York City; by the 1960s, this further came to exclude the regional sounds of the Mid-
Atlantic region and Western Pennsylvania. By the 2000s, American sociolinguist William Labov
concluded that, if any sound system could be recognized as "General American," it would
essentially be a convergence of those features shared among Western American English, Midland
American English, and Standard Canadian English.
Now typically regarded as falling under the General American umbrella are the dialects of
the American West, Western New England, and perhaps much of the American Midland and
Canada. Once in the earlier 20th century but no longer now included are the dialects of the Mid-
Atlantic United States, the Inland North, and Western Pennsylvania.
Anglicist William A. Kretzchmar, Jr. explains in a 2004 article that the term "General
American" arose as a name for a presumed most common or "default" form of American English,
especially to be distinguished from marked regional speech of New England or the South.
"General American" has often been considered to be the relatively unmarked speech of "the
Midwest", a vague designation for anywhere in the vast midsection of the country from Ohio west
to Nebraska, and from the Canadian border as far south as Missouri or Kansas. No historical
justification for this term exists, and neither do present circumstances support its use... [I]t implies
that there is some exemplary state of American English from which other varieties deviate. On the
contrary, [it] can best be characterized as what is left over after speakers suppress the regional and
social features that have risen to salience and become noticeable.
Despite the common perception of there being a mainstream American accent that is free of
any regional features or regional influence, the General American sound system does, in fact, have
traceable regional origins: namely, the Northern speech patterns of the non- coastal Eastern United
States, including interior Pennsylvania, upstate New York, and the adjacent Midwestern region,
prior to the Northern Cities Vowel Shift of the mid-20th century.
The fact that a rural, broadly Midwestern dialect became the basis of what is General
American English is often attributed to the mass migration of Midwestern farmers to California
and the Pacific Northwest from where it spread, since California speech itself became prevalent in
nationally syndicated films and media via the Hollywood film industry.
General American, like the British Received Pronunciation (RP) and the prestige accents
of many other societies, has never been the accent of the entire nation.
A General American pronunciation is most directly descended from a generalized
pronunciation of the early 1900s from the inland Northeastern and Midwestern states, which likely
gained ground nationally by being spoken particularly by many newscasters and radio and
television announcers; this has led the accent to being sometimes referred to as an American
newscaster accent, Network English or Network Standard. General American is sometimes
promoted as preferable to other regional accents and prestigious. In the United States, classes
promising "accent reduction", "accent modification," or "accent neutralization" generally attempt to
63
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
a-Perhaps the most noticeable of these differences is in the vowel sound in such words as
fast, path, grass, dance, can’t, half. At the end of the eighteenth century southern England
began to change from what is called a flat a /ӕ/ to a broad a /a:/ in these words, that is from a
sound like the a /ӕ/ in man /mӕn/ to one like the a /a:/ in father /fa:ðə/. The change affected
words in which the vowel occurred before f, sk, sp, st, ss, th, and n followed by certain consonants.
In parts of New England the same change took place, but in most other parts of the country the old
sound was preserved, and fast, path, etc., are pronounced with the vowel of pan /pӕn/. In some
speakers there is a tendency to employ an intermediate vowel, halfway between the a of pan and
father, but the ‘flat a’/ӕ/ must be regarded as the typical American pronunciation.
b-In terms of phonemic differences, the phoneme /ɒ/does not occur in American English,
and words which have this vowel in British pronunciation will instead have /a:/ or /ɔ:/ in American
English. For instance, got is /gɒt/ in British English, but /ga:t/ in American English, while dog is
64
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
e-In GAm, flapping is common: when either a /t/ or a /d/ occurs between a sonorant
phoneme and an unstressed vowel phoneme, it is realized as an alveolar-flap allophone [ɾ]. This
sounds like a /d/ to RP speakers.
The process of coining new lexical items started as soon as the colonists began borrowing
names for unfamiliar flora, fauna, and topography from the Native American languages. Examples
of such names are opossum, raccoon, squash and moose (from Algonquian). Other Native
American loanwords, such as wigwam or moccasin, describe articles in common use among
Native Americans. The languages of the other colonizing nations also added to the American
vocabulary; for instance, cookie, cruller, stoop, and pit (of a fruit) from Dutch; levee, portage
("carrying of boats or goods") and (probably) gopher from French; barbecue, stevedore, and
rodeo from Spanish.
Among the earliest and most notable regular "English" additions to the American
vocabulary, dating from the early days of colonization through the early 19th century, are terms
describing the features of the North American landscape; for instance, run, branch, fork,
snag, bluff, gulch, neck (of the woods), barrens, bottomland, notch, knob, riffle, rapids,
watergap, cutoff, trail, timberline and divide. Already existing words such as creek, slough,
sleet and (in later use) watershed received new meanings that were unknown in England.
Other noteworthy American toponyms are found among loanwords; for example, prairie,
butte (French); bayou (Choctaw via Louisiana French); coulee (Canadian French, but used also in
Louisiana with a different meaning); canyon, mesa, arroyo (Spanish); vlei, skate, kill (Dutch,
Hudson Valley).
With the new continent developed new forms of dwelling, and hence a large inventory of
words designating real estate concepts (land office, lot, outlands, waterfront, the verbs locate
and relocate, betterment, addition, subdivision), types of property (log cabin, adobe) in the
18th century; frame house, apartment, tenement house, shack, shanty in the 19th century;
project, condominium, townhouse, split-level, mobile home, multi- family in the 20th century),
and parts thereof (driveway, breezeway, backyard, dooryard; clapboard, siding, trim,
baseboard; stoop (from Dutch), family room, den; and, in recent years, HVAC, central air,
walkout basement).
65
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Ever since the American Revolution, a great number of terms connected with the U.S.
political institutions have entered the language; examples are run, gubernatorial, primary
election, carpetbagger (after the Civil War), repeater, lame duck and pork barrel. Some of
these are internationally used (for example, caucus, gerrymander, filibuster, exit poll).
The rise of capitalism, the development of industry and material innovations throughout the
19th and 20th centuries were the source of a massive stock of distinctive new words, phrases and
idioms. Typical examples are the vocabulary of railroading (see further at rail terminology) and
transportation terminology, ranging from names of roads (from dirt roads and back roads to
freeways and parkways) to road infrastructure (parking lot, overpass, rest area), and from
automotive terminology to public transit (for example, in the sentence "riding the subway
downtown"); such American introductions as commuter (from commutation ticket), concourse,
to board (a vehicle), to park, double-park and parallel park (a car), double decker or the noun
terminal have long been used in all dialects of English. Trades of various kinds have endowed
(American) English with household words describing jobs and occupations (bartender,
longshoreman, patrolman, hobo, bouncer, bellhop, roustabout, white collar, blue collar,
employee, boss [from Dutch], intern, busboy, mortician, senior citizen), businesses and
workplaces (department store, supermarket, thrift store, gift shop, drugstore, motel, main
street, gas station, hardware store, savings and loan, hock [also from Dutch]), as well as
general concepts and innovations (automated teller machine, smart card, cash register,
dishwasher, reservation (as at hotels), pay envelope, movie, mileage, shortage, outage, blood
bank).
b-English words that survived in the United States and not Britain
A number of words and meanings that originated in Middle English or Early Modern
English and that always have been in everyday use in the United States dropped out in most
varieties of British English; some of these have cognates in Lowland Scots. Terms such as fall
("autumn"), faucet, diaper, candy, skillet, eyeglasses, and obligate, are often regarded as
Americanisms. Fall for example came to denote the season in 16th century England, a contraction
of Middle English expressions like "fall of the leaf" and "fall of the year". During the 17th
66
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
century, English immigration to the British colonies in North America was at its peak and the new
settlers took the English language with them. While the term fall gradually became obsolete in
Britain, it became the more common term in North America. Gotten (past participle of get) is
often considered to be an Americanism, although there are some areas of Britain, such as
Lancashire and North- eastern England, that still continue to use it and sometimes also use putten
as the past participle for put (which is not done by most speakers of American English).
Many compound nouns have the form verb plus preposition: add-on, stopover, lineup,
shakedown, tryout, spin-off, rundown ("summary"), shootout, holdup, hideout, comeback,
cookout, kickback, makeover, takeover, rollback ("decrease"), rip-off, come-on, shoo-in, fix-
up, tie-in, tie-up ("stoppage"), stand-in.
Noun endings such as -ee (retiree), -ery (bakery), -ster (gangster) and -cian (beautician)
are also particularly productive. Some verbs ending in -ize are of U.S. origin; for example,
fetishize, prioritize, burglarize, accessorize, itemize, editorialize, customize, notarize,
weatherize, winterize, Mirandize; and so are some back- formations (locate, fine-tune, evolute,
curate, donate, emote, upholster, peeve and enthuse).
Differences in grammatical structurs are relatively minor, and normally do not affect
mutual intelligibility; these include: different use of some verbal auxiliaries; formal (rather than
notional) agreement with collective nouns; different preferences for the past forms of a few verbs
(for example, AmE/BrE: learned/learnt, burned/burnt, and in sneak, dive, get); different
prepositions and adverbs in certain contexts (for example, AmE in school, BrE at school); and
whether or not a definite article is used, in very few cases (AmE to the hospital, BrE to hospital).
There are also differences in sentence structures, e.g. British: Did you have…?, American: Have
you got…?
67
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
a-The North (i. Northeastern New England , ii. Southeastern New England , iii. Southwestern
New England , iv. Inland North, v. The Hudson Valley, vi. Metropolitan New York).
The Northern dialect is set off by the use of such vocabulary terms as pail rather than
bucket, angleworm for earthworm, and pit rather than seed in a cherry. Phonologically, it has a
phonemic distinction between the vowels in morning and mourning, /s/ in greasy, and /u:/ in
root. Eastern New England is set off from the rest of the Northern dialect by the loss of postvocalic
/r/ (in such words as barn, four, daughter) and the use of /a:/ for /æ/ in words such as aunt, bath,
and half.
b-The Midland:
i-North Midland (Delaware Valley, Susquehanna Valey, Upper Ohio Valley, Northern
West Virginia)
ii- South Midland (Upper Potomac & Shenandoah, Southern West Virginia & Eastern
Kentucky, Western Carolina & Eastern Tennessee)
The Midland dialect is distinguished by vocabulary items such as skillet for frying pan,
blinds for window shades, and poke for a paper sack. Phonologically, it retains postvocalic /r/
and has /θ/ finally in with. Northern Midland is distinguished by run for a small stream and /a:/ in
frog, hog, and fog, which do not rhyme with dog. Southern Midland has redworm for
earthworm, pack for carry, and /a:/ for /ai/ in words such as write and ride.
c-The South (i. Delmarva, ii. The Virginia Piedmont, iii. Northeatern North Carolina, iv.
Cape Fear & Peede Valleys, v.The South Carolina Low Country)
Southern English is marked by the loss of postvocalic /r/. /z/ in Mrs., and the use of tote
for carry and snap beans for string beans. It shares with Southern Midland the use of you-all
for the second person plural pronoun, /ju:/ in words such as news and due, shucks rather
than husks for the coverings of corn, and might could for might be able to.
and Puritan settlements. Southern dialects began with immigrants for southwestern Great Britain,
including Wales. The third dialect family, the Midlands, comes from a wave of immigrants
originating in northern England, Ireland, and Scotland. It spread west horizontally from the
Philadelphia area.
Map 2.2. Major dialect areas in the USA: Northern, Midland, Southern [22, p.34]
Australian English began to diverge from British English after the founding of the Colony of
New South Wales in 1788 and was recognised as being different from British English by 1820. It
arose from the intermingling of early settlers from a great variety of mutually intelligible dialectal
regions of the British Isles and quickly developed into a distinct variety of English.
69
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
As a distinct dialect, Australian English differs considerably from other varieties of English
in vocabulary, accent, pronunciation, register, grammar and spelling.
The primary way in which Australian English is distinctive from other varieties of English is
through its unique pronunciation. It shares most similarity with other Southern Hemisphere accents,
in particular New Zealand English. Like most dialects of English it is distinguished primarily by its
vowel phonology.
The vowels of Australian English can be divided according to length. The long vowels,
which include monophthongs and diphthongs, mostly correspond to the tense vowels used in
analyses of Received Pronunciation (RP) as well as its centring diphthongs. The short vowels,
consisting only of monophthongs, correspond to the RP lax vowels. There exist pairs of long and
short vowels with overlapping vowel quality giving Australian English phonemic length distinction,
which is unusual amongst the various dialects of English, though not unknown elsewhere, such as in
regional south-eastern dialects of the UK and eastern seaboard dialects in the US. As with General
American and New Zealand English, the weak-vowel merger is complete in Australian English:
unstressed /ɪ/ (sometimes written as /ɨ/ or /ᵻ/) is merged into /ə/ (schwa), unless it is followed by a
velar consonant.
There is little variation with respect to the sets of consonants used in various English dialects.
Australian English is non-rhotic.
6.4.3. Variation
Academic research has shown that the most notable variation within Australian English is
largely sociocultural. This is mostly evident in phonology, which is divided into three sociocultural
varieties: broad, general and cultivated.
70
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
In addition, some Australians speak creole languages derived from Australian English, such
as Australian Kriol, Torres Strait Creole and Norfuk.
The broad, general and cultivated accents form a continuum that reflects minute variations
in the Australian accent. They can reflect the social class, education and urban or rural background
of speakers, though such indicators are not always reliable. According to linguists, the general
Australian variant emerged some time before 1900. Recent generations have seen a comparatively
smaller proportion of the population speaking with the broad variant, along with the near extinction
of the cultivated Australian accent. The growth and dominance of general Australian accents perhaps
reflects its prominence on radio and television during the late 20th century.
The ethnocultural dialects are diverse accents in Australian English that are spoken by the
minority groups, which are of non-English speaking background. A massive immigration from Asia
has made a large increase in diversity and the will for people to show their cultural identity within
the Australian context. These ethnocultural varieties contain features of General Australian English
as adopted by the children of immigrants blended with some non-English language features, such as
the Afro-Asiatic and Asian languages.
Although Australian English is relatively homogeneous, some regional variations are notable.
The dialects of English spoken in South Australia, Western Australia, New South Wales, Victoria,
Tasmania, Queensland and the Torres Strait Islands differ slightly from each other. Differences exist
both in terms of vocabulary and phonology.
71
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Most regional differences come down to word usage. For example, swimming clothes are
known as cossies or swimmers in New South Wales, togs in Queensland, and bathers in Victoria,
Tasmania, Western Australia and South Australia; what is referred to as a stroller in most of
Australia is called a pusher in Victoria and usually a pram in Western Australia and Tasmania.
Preference for synonymous words also differs between states. For example, garbage (i.e., garbage
bin, garbage truck) dominates over rubbish in New South Wales and Queensland, while rubbish is
more popular in Victoria, Tasmania, Western Australia and South Australia. The word footy
generally refers to the most popular football code in the particular state or territory; that is, rugby
league in New South Wales and Queensland, and Australian rules football elsewhere. Beer glasses
are also named differently in different states. Distinctive grammatical patterns exist such as the use
of the interrogative eh (also spelled ay or aye), which is particularly associated with Queensland.
6.4.4. Vocabulary
Australian English has many words and idioms which are unique to the dialect and have been
written on extensively, with the Macquarie Dictionary, widely regarded as the national standard,
incorporating numerous Australian terms.
Australian poetry, such as "The Man from Snowy River", as well as folk songs such as
"Waltzing Matilda", contain many historical Australian words and phrases that are understood by
Australians even though some are not in common usage today.
72
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
During the late 14th and early 15th centuries English seemed to return as a prestige
language [60, pp.2-5; 89, pp.69-70]. The close of the fourteenth century brought the first post-
conquest king of England whose first language was English. At the end of the 14th century and the
beginning of the 15th century, English came to predominate as the official language of record. In
1423, Parliament records were kept virtually all in English. Henry V (r. 1413-1422) established
English as an official language. This meant that English would now be the language of government.
A standard form of English accompanied the rise of the institution known as "Chancery standard'.
Chancery comes from the word chancel, a chapel of the king, where the chaplains of the court
originally spent their time between services, writing the kings’ letters. By the end of the 14th
century, Chancery was the production house for official government documents. By the mid-15th
century, the term came to refer to the national bureaucracy as a whole. Chancery established
special forms of spelling and handwriting that were taught to scribes for the production of official
documents. From about 1430, government documents began to be written in English and, since it
was considered vital that these be understood throughout the country, it was felt that the language
used in these documents needed to be consistent and uniform. Since government documents were
produced by clerks in the Chancery in Westminster, it was these bureaucrats who decided which
variants were chosen. These clerks came from various parts of the country, but were all resident in
London. The variety of English used in their documents appears to have been based on London
English, with some features originating further north, probably from the East or Central Midlands.
As in many cases, the Chancery variant is most similar to that used in present-day standard English.
Since the process of standardisation involves the reduction of variants, it is evident that the
Chancery documents mark the first stage in the process of standardisation: the selection of a
variety. Thus, a new standard form of Middle English, known as Chancery Standard, developed
from the dialects of London and the East Midlands. Chancery English contributed to the
development of writing that was a standard, irrespective of the speech or dialect of the writer.
Spelling was standardized without regard to pronunciation. Chancery standard was the first
standard of writing (the written form of the English language). It contributed significantly to the
development of a standard English. The part of England that contributed most to the formation of
this standard was the East Midland district, and it was the East Midland type of English that became
its basis, particularly the dialect of the metropolis, London. According to Baugh and Cable [4,
pp.179-181], several causes contributed to the attainment of this result.
In the first place, as a Midland dialect the English of this region occupied a middle position
between the extreme divergences of the north and south. It was less conservative than the Southern
dialect, less radical than the Northern. In its sounds and inflections it represents a kind of
compromise, sharing some of the characteristics of both its neighbors. In the second place, the
East Midland district was the largest and most populous of the major dialect areas. A third factor,
more difficult to evaluate, was the presence of the universities, Oxford and Cambridge, in this
region. In the fourteenth century the monasteries were playing a less important role in the
dissemination of learning than they had once played, while the two universities had developed into
73
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
important intellectual centers. So far as Cambridge is concerned any influence that it had would be
exerted in support of the East Midland dialect.
By far the most influential factor in the rise of Standard English was the importance of
London as the capital of England. The history of Standard English is almost a history of London
English. Indeed, it is altogether likely that the language of the city would have become the
prevailing dialect without the help of any of the factors previously discussed. London was, and still
is, the political and commercial center of England. It was the seat of the court, of the highest
judicial tribunals, the focus of the social and intellectual activities of the country. In the
practicalities of commerce the London economy was especially important as “an engine of
communication and exchange which enabled ideas and information to be distributed and business
to be done across an increasingly extensive, complex and varied field” [4].
Thus, according to Holmes [45, pp.79-80] standard English emerged in the form of Chancery
standard in the fifteenth century from a variety of regional English dialects, largely because it was
the variety used by the English Court and the influential merchants of London. The area where the
largest proportion of the English population lived at that time was in a neat triangle containing
London, where the Court was based, and the two universities, Oxford and Cambridge. In addition,
the East Midlands was an important agricultural and business area, and London was the hub of
international trade and exports to Calais. It was also the centre of political, social and intellectual
life in England.
It was the dialect used in this area which was the basis for what we now think of as standard
English. It was prestigious because of its use in Court. It was influential because it was used by the
economically powerful merchant class. In the latter part of the fifteenth century the London standard
had been accepted, at least in writing, in most parts of the country. People who came to London from
the provinces recognised this and often learned it, and this of course made it useful. The more
people who used it, the less effort people had to make to understand regional varieties. It is easy to
see how such a code would rapidly develop formal H functions in the context of administration and
government.
Standard varieties are codified varieties. Codification is usually achieved through grammars
and dictionaries which record, and sometimes prescribe, the standard forms of the language.
Dictionary writers (or lexicographers) have to decide which words to include in the dictionary as
part of the standard variety, which forms to mark as dialectal, and which to omit altogether. They
generally take the usage of educated and socially prestigious members of the community as their
criterion.
The codification process, which is part of the development of every standard variety, begun
in the Chancery, was accelerated in the case of English by the introduction of printing. In 1476,
William Caxton, the first English printer, set up his printing press in Westminster. He used the
speech of the London area and especially, the Chancery standard - the newly emerging standard
dialect - as the basis for his translations. In other words, he used the vocabulary, the grammar and
the pronunciation of this dialect when looking for words, constructions and spellings to translate
74
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
works from French. Selecting forms was not always straightforward. Like other codifiers, he
reported that he consulted the best writers of the upper class for judgements on usage problems. By
using Chancery English, William Caxton established a national literary standard in print based on
the written standard of official documentation. The invention of printing press made books available
to more people. The books became cheaper and more people learned to read. Printing brought
standardization to people.
Chancery Standard contributed significantly to the development of a Standard English, and
the political, commercial and cultural dominance of the "East Midlands triangle" (London-
Oxford-Cambridge) was well established long before the 15th Century, but it was the printing
press that was really responsible for carrying through the standardization process. With the advent
of mass printing, the dialect and spelling of the East Midlands (and, more specifically, that of the
national capital, London, where most publishing houses were located) became the de facto
standard and, over time, spelling and grammar gradually became more and more fixed. A standard
written English emerged (particularly in spelling and grammar), codified and eventually codified
by grammarians in the eighteenth century.
It is difficult to overestimate the influence of the first printers in fixing and spreading the
written form of English. The language they used was the London literary English. With cheap
printed books becoming available to a great number of readers all over England, the London form
of literary English recognised throughout the country.
Thus, from the geographical viewpoint we may say that the country wide standard form of
written English was derived from the speech of London. It is natural that the greatest influence
exerted by the printers was that on the written form of the word. The written form of words
worked out by Caxton was adopted as standard and has in many cases remained unchanged even to
the present day.
Spoken English was standardized later than the written form. Cruttenden [19, pp.74-75] is
of the opinion that standard spoken English imitated the speech of London.
“Although written English gained ground rapidly in the fifteenth century, any
writing which commented on the spoken language did not appear until the sixteenth
century, when one type of regional speech began to be said to have prestige. It was
London and the speech of the monarch's court which was held up as the dialect to be
imitated'[19, pp.74-75].
Throughout the seventeenth and eighteenth centuries the speech of London and its court are
held up as the acme of pronunciation. Despite criticisms, the court continues to be held up by some
as the model for polite speech. Sheridan asserts that “the pronunciation of English, as used by people
of the best taste at court is so perfect that there are few of our words capable of improvement” [as
cited in [19, pp.74-75].
It is concluded that “those sounds…which are the most generally received among
75
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
the learned and polite, as well as the bulk of speakers, are the most legitimate.” Throughout
the centuries, there have been efforts in pronunciation codification. The standard
pronunciation has been Received Pronunciation (RP). The codification of pronunciation
began in the late eighteenth century, when elocutionists such as Thomas Sheridan and John
Walker produced explicit guides to correct pronunciation in the form of pronouncing dictionaries.
In the 17th century the type of speech used in London and in the universities was
unanimously proclaimed the best type of English. The phoneticians and grammarians recommended
it as a model of correct English.
During the 17th century, the gap between the written language and the spoken language
became narrower. With the spread of education more people learned to speak correctly, in the way
prescribed by grammars and textbooks. The use of standard speech as distinguished from local
dialects is insisted upon the grammars and dictionaries of the 18th century.
In the 20th century the growing economic and cultural influence of the United States and its
status as a superpower following the Second World War has, along with worldwide broadcasting in
English by the BBC and other broadcasters, significantly accelerated the spread of the language
across the planet. By the 21st century, English was more widely spoken and written than any
language has ever been. A major feature in the early development of Modern English was the
codification of explicit norms for standard usage, and their dissemination through official media
such as public education and state sponsored publications. In 1755 Samuel Johnson published his A
Dictionary of the English Language which introduced a standard set of spelling conventions and
usage norms. In 1828, Noah Webster published the American Dictionary of the English Language
in an effort to establish a norm for speaking and writing American English that was independent
from the British standard. Within Britain, non-standard or lower class dialect features were
increasingly stigmatised, leading to the quick spread of the prestige varieties among the middle
class [129].
As seen, Standard British English originated from the speech of London. Standard English is
associated with RP (Received Pronunciation). It was also called King’s English, Queen’s English,
BBC English, General British and Modern type of RP (MRP)
Today, Standard English is codified to the extent that the grammar and vocabulary of
English are much the same everywhere in the world. Standard English is so powerful that it exerts a
tremendous pressure on all local varieties.
The development of standard English illustrates the three essential criteria which
characterize a standard [45, pp.79-81]: it was an influential or prestigious variety, it was codified and
stabilised and it served H functions in that it was used for communication at Court, for literature and
for administration. It also illustrates that what we refer to as a standard language is always a
particular dialect which has gained its special position as a result of social, economic and political
influences. A standard dialect has no particular linguistic merits, whether in vocabulary, grammar or
pronunciation. It is simply the dialect of those who are politically powerful and socially prestigious.
Once it begins to serve as a norm or standard for a wider group, however, it is likely to develop the
wider vocabulary needed to express the new functions it is required to serve.
76
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Standard English, according to Crystal [22], has the following five essential features:
a-SE is a variety of English-a distinctive combination of linguistic features with a particular
role to play in a country. There is nothing in the grammar and vocabulary of SE to tell us which part
of a country it comes from.
b-The linguistic features of SE are chiefly matters of grammar, vocabulary, and orthography
(spelling and punctuation). It is important to note that SE is not a matter of pronunciation: SE is
spoken in a wide variety of accents (including any prestige accent a country may have, such as
British RP).
c-SE is the variety of English which carries most prestige within a country. ’Prestige’ is a
social concept, where by some people have high standing in the eyes of others, whether this derives
from social class, material success, political strength, popular acclaim, or educational background.
The English that these people choose to use will, by this very fact, become the standard
within their community. In the words of one US linguist, SE is ‘the English used by the powerful’.
d-The prestige attached to SE is recognized by adult members of the community, and this
motivates them to recommend SE as a desirable educational target. It is the variety which is used as
the norm of communication by the community’s leading institutions, such as its government, law
courts, and media.
It is, therefore, the variety which is likely to be the most widely disseminated among the
public. It will, accordingly, be widely understood - though not to the same extent by everyone, and
with varying comprehension of some of its features.
77
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
78
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
The geographical spread of English is unique among the languages of the world, not only in
our time but throughout history. The movement of English around the world began with the
pioneering voyages to the Americas, Asia and the Antipodes, continued with the 19th century
colonial developments in Africa and South Pacific and took a significant further step when it was
adopted in the 20th century as an official or semi-official language by many newly independent
states. English is now the dominant or official language in over 60 countries and is represented in
every continent and in three major oceans: Atlantic, Indian and Pacific. It is this spread of
representation which makes the application of the term world language a reality [21, p. 106].
According to Greenbaum [36, pp.12-14], English is the majority first language in twenty-
three countries. It is an official language or a joint official language in about fifty other
countries, where it is used in addition to the indigenous first languages for a variety of public and
personal functions. It is also used as a second language, though without official status, in countries
such as Bangladesh and Malaysia. Countries where English is a first or second language are located
in all five continents. The total population of these countries amounts to around 2.5 billion, about
49 per cent of the world's population. Where English is a first or second language, it is used
internally for communication between nationals of the same country. In addition, English is used
extensively as a foreign language for international communication by people who do not ordinarily
employ it when speaking or writing to their compatriots.
The number of first-language speakers of English has been estimated at well over 300
million, of whom over 216 million live in the United States. The United Kingdom has about 53
million, Canada over 17 million, and Australia about 14 million. Countries where English is a
majority first language may have large percentages of bilingual speakers and speakers for
whom English is a second language. For example, Canada has a large minority of unilingual
French speakers (nearly 17 per cent) as well as an almost equal percentage of speakers who are
bilingual in French and English. Most countries with second-language speakers of English are
former British colonies, such as India and Nigeria. English has been retained as an official
language in the majority of these countries after independence because none of the indigenous
languages was accepted by all citizens as the sole national language. As an official second
language, English is used in a variety of public functions: in government, in the law courts, in
broadcasting, in the press, and in education. In many African and Asian countries it serves as the
79
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
The present-day status of international / world status of English is primarily the result of two
factors: the expansion of British colonial power, which peaked towards the end of the 19th century,
and the emergence of the United States as the leading economic power of the 20th century. It is the
latter factor which continued to explain the position of the English language today. The USA
contains nearly four times as many English mother tongue (EMT) speakers as the next most EMT
nation (the UK), and these two countries comprise 70 per cent of all EMT speakers in the world.
Such dominance, with its political and economic underpinnings, gives the American a controlling
interest in the way the language is likely to develop [21, p.106].
Thus, the pre-eminence of English for international communication is, in part, indebted to
the spread of English as a first or second language for internal communication in numerous
countries that were once part of the British Empire. The role of English as an international
language has gathered momentum since the end of the Second World War through the economic
and military global dominance of the United States and the resources it deploys for scientific and
technological progress.
The English taught to foreign learners is generally British or American English in their
standard varieties. Except for pronunciation the differences between the two are relatively minor, as
indeed they are between the standard varieties in any of the countries where English is the majority
first language.
The mass media are ensuring, if anything, the smoothing of differences and are
encouraging reciprocal influences, though the influence of American English is predominant.
Despite some trivial variation in spelling and punctuation, and some more important variation in
vocabulary, the standard first-language varieties of written English are remarkably homogeneous.
The situation in countries where English is primarily a second language is fluid and
80
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
varies. In the past these countries have looked to British or American English for language
norms. But there are indications that in some countries—such as India, Nigeria, and Singapore—
local models of English are being sought that are based on their own educated varieties. This
nativization of English augues well for the continued use of English for internal functions in those
countries.
The present role of English as an international language derives from its geographical
spread and the prestige and practical value it has acquired through the United States in the last
few decades. It cannot be attributed to the intrinsic superiority of English over potential other
candidates. It is possible to point to some features that appear to make English easier to learn than
some other languages. English has few inflections, so foreign learners do not have to memorize
declensions and conjugations. It has natural rather than grammatical gender, so learners do not need
to memorize the gender of each noun and do not have to cope with ensuring gender agreement
between the noun and an accompanying article or adjective. For most Europeans at least, the
Germanic and Romance elements that constitute the bulk of English vocabulary provide welcome
help. On the other hand, the absence of inflections has increased the importance of prepositions
and the burden of memorizing the preposition that goes with a particular verb, noun, or adjective
in a particular meaning: look at and look to, pride in and proud of, afraid of and alarmed at.
English also confronts the learner with a multitude of idiomatic combinations, particularly verbs
with adverbs such as get by, do in, turn up, make out. The frequent absence of correlation
between pronunciation and spelling is a serious obstacle for learning to read and write.
The future of English as a global / international language will depend very largely on the
political, economical, demographic and cultural trends in the world. The beginning of the 21st
81
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
century is a time of global transition. According to some experts, faster economic globalization is
going hand in hand with the growing use of English. More and more people are being encouraged
to use English rather than their own language.
On the other hand, the period of most rapid change can be expected to be an uncomfortable
and at times traumatic experience for many people around the world. Hence, the opposite view,
that the next 20 years or so will be a critical time for the English language and for those who
depend upon it. The patterns of usage and public attitudes to English which develop during this
period will have long-term effects for its future in the world.
The notions of World English and World Englishes are far from similar, although the terms
are often mistakenly used interchangeably. World English refers to the English language as a
lingua franca used in business, trade, diplomacy and other spheres of global activity, while World
Englishes refers to the different varieties of English and English- based creoles developed in
different regions of the world.
The spread of English around the world is often discussed in terms of three distinct
groups of users as presented in the most influential model of the spread of English: Braj Kachru's
model of World Englishes.
82
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
In this model the diffusion of English is captured in terms of three Concentric Circles of
the language: The Inner Circle, the Outer Circle, and the Expanding Circle.
Table 2.10. The three circles of English and their attributes [89, p.3]
Several models of "simplified English" have been suggested for teaching English as a
foreign language.
.
Vietnamese has traditionally been divided into three dialect regions: North, Central, and
South. There are various mutually intelligible regional varieties (or dialects), the main five being:
83
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
For further information on dialects in Vietname, please refer to [111, p.210], [196] and [132]
Follow-up activity 2.6. True/False: Discuss with your friends and decide whether the
following statements are true or false:
1-In broad term, British English is the English used throughout the United Kingdom, but it is
often more narrowly understood as the English of England.
2-The two major regional dialects of England are Northern and Southern dialects.
3-According to Kurath (1949), three main dialects in America are Northern, Midland and
Southern.
4-According to Holmes [45, pp.79-80], standard English emerged in the form of Chancery
standard in the fifteenth century.
5-The term World Englishes refers to the English language as a lingua franca used in
business, trade, diplomacy and other spheres of global activity, while the term World English refers
to different varieties of English.
7.1. Sociolect
7.1.2. Sociolects
One of the principal concerns of sociolinguistics over the past decades has been describing
language in use. The study of sociolinguistic variation is essentially the description of the
differential use of language by different social groups – particularly social classes (class
stratification studies). A number of important concepts and findings have emerged from this
work on social classes and the use of language which now form part of the basic currency of the
discipline. One of the findings is that social stratification gives rise linguistically to social class
dialects or sociolects (They are also called culturolects). Social class differences correlate with
sociolect differences.
Let us look at caste dialects in India and social-class dialects in the West as examples.
The bestknown caste system is that of Hindu society in India. It is based on Hindu
religion, which preaches that people have more than one life and that they are born into a
particular caste in life according to their behavior in their previous life. The system of castes in
85
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
India can be divided into Brahmins (priests) and non-Brahmins. The non-brahmins include
Kshatiyas (warriors), Vaisyas (farmers and merchants), Sundras (servants), and Harijas (the
untouchables).
Castes in India
Brahmins priests
Kshatriyas soldiers
Vaishyas traders
Shudras Servants & labourers
Harijan (untouchable) the worst work, refused by others
There are rigid lines between the castes and no form of social mixing is allowed.
Sociologists describe a situation where one’s social position is determined at birth as
ascription. A man is born into a jati (a division of caste) and this is the only way of acquring
membership. The Hindu doctrine of ‘karma’ teaches the young Hindu that he is born into a
particular sub-caste because that is where he deserves to be born. Linguistic correlates of caste can
be found at all levels of structure. There are clear differences in Indian languages between the
speech of Brahmins and non-Brahmins. There are several clear-cut distinctions between the
phonology, vocabulary, and grammar of Brahmin and non-Brahmin speech.
Brahmin Non-Brahmin
Vocabulary
tungu sheep orangu
alambu wash kaluyu
jalō water taṇṇi
Phonology
krāfu haircut krāppu
jīni sugar cīni
vāreppano banana vāreppolo
Grammar
-du it -ccu
vandudu it came vanduccu
paṇra he does pannuhā
Labov [57] deals with social stratification in his work entitled The Social Stratification of
English in New York City. As the tittle of the work states, SSENYC deals with social
stratification: the fine-scale linguistic layering of people along the “linear social” scale which
in this book is usually termed “socioeconomic class” (SEC). Most sociolinguistic studies of the
last two decades rely on some kind of scalar index like Labov’s for their operational definition of
social class.
Today, the concepts of social class often assume three general categories: a very wealthy
and powerful upper class that owns and controls the means of production; a middle class of
professional workers, small business owners, and low-level managers; and a lower class, who
rely on low-paying wage jobs for their livelihood and often experience poverty.
U non-U
Have a bath Take a bath
Bike, bicycle cycle
luncheon dinner
riding Horse riding
87
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
sick Ill
knave jack
mad mental
Looking-glass mirror
Writing-paper Note-paper
jam preserve
wireless radio
Table-nalkin serviette
Lavatory-paper Toilet-paper
rich wealthy
vegetables greens
pudding weet
telegram wire
England Britain
Scotch Scottish
These vocabulary differences were rather like those which distinguished Brahmin and non-
Brahmin caste speech. The nature of upper-class language has changed over 30 years later, but
the terms U and non-U are still well-known.
We can also find vocabulary differences between social classes in many types of sociolects
such as gender language, slang, prefessional language, ethnic language.
Class A B C D
MMC 0 0 3 28
LMC 0 10 15 42
UWC 5 15 74 87
MWC 23 44 88 95
LWC 29 66 98 100
88
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
The table shows whether the variable (-ng) in such words as walking was pronounced as /ŋ/or
/n/ (0=no use of /n/; 100=100% use of /n/.) The consistency with which speakers increase their use
of /n/ as their language becomes more spontaneous and casual reflected at every social level.
ii-Dropping the /h/ [Chambers & Trudgill, 1980, as cited in [20, p.38]
In British English, the accent which carries most prestige pronounces /h/ at the beginning of
words such as head. But in most other accents of England and Wales, it is common to omit /h/in
this position. Regions do not pronounce or omit /h/with total consistency, however, as can be seen
from the results of two studies of this variable carried out in Norwich and Bradford.
The speakers were grouped into five social classes, based on such factors as their occupation,
income, and education. The proportion of /h/-dropping was calculated with the following results:
Table 2.16. Dropping the /h/[Chambers & Trudgill, as cited in [20, p.38]
Class Bradford Norwich
The correlation is clear. In both areas, there is more /h/-dropping as one moves down the
social scale. Moreover, the proportion is always greater in Bradford, suggesting that the
phenomenon has been longer established in that area.
card, for and form. For our purposes, there are two possible variants of [r].
Either it is present and pronounced [r], or it is absent ([r] or [Ø]). If you listen to a range of
dialects you will find that sometimes people pronounce [r] following a vowel, and sometimes
they don’t [Ø]. In some regions, pronouncing [r] is part of the standard prestige dialect – in
the Boston and New York areas of the eastern USA, for example, in Ireland and in Scotland
(though recent research suggests that this is changing in the speech of young working-class people
in Glasgow and Edinburgh). In other areas, standard dialect speakers do not pronounce [r] after
vowels (or ‘post-vocalically’ as linguists describe it) in words like car and card. In areas where
[r] pronunciation is prestigious, sociolinguists have found patterns like those described above for
[h]- dropping and -in vs -ing ([in] vs [iŋ]) pronunciation. The higher a person’s social group, the
more [r] they pronounce.
90
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
LWC 97%
The table shows that the suspicion is quite justified - there is a clear correlation between
social class and usage of –s.
91
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
New York dialect region who were native speakers of both English and Spanish. Today, it is the
customary dialect of many Hispanic Americans of diverse national heritages, not simply Puerto
Ricans, in the New York metropolitan area and beyond along the northeastern coast of the United
States.
Follow-up activity 2.7.1.T/F: Decide whether the following are true or false:
1-Social stratification gives rise to sociolects.
2-A sociolect is a variety of language belonging to a particular social class.
3-There are no linguistic differences between Brahmin and Non-Brahmin in India.
4-Sociolect researches in many different countries have revealed a certain relationship
between social classes and language patterns.
5-We can find linguistic differences between social classes in many types of sociolects.
94
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
95
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Phonological differences between the speech of men and women have been noted in a
variety of languages [107, p.316]. In the area of morphology and vocabulary, many of the studies
have focused on English.
Lakoff [59] published an influential account of women's language. She listed a set of basic
assumptions about what marks out the language of women. Among these are claims that women:
a-Hedge: using phrases like “sort of”, “kind of”, “it seems like”, and so on.
b-Use (super)polite forms, e.g “Would you mind...”, “I'd appreciate it if...”, “...if you don't
mind”.
c-Use tag questions, e.g. “You're going to dinner, aren't you?”
96
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
d-Speak in italics: intonational emphasis equal to underlining words - so, very, quite.
e-Use empty adjectivesg.g. divine, lovely, adorable, and so on
f-Use hypercorrect grammar and pronunciation: English prestige grammar and clear
enunciation.
g-Use direct quotation (men paraphrase more often).
h-Have a special lexicon: women use more words for things like colours, men for sports.
i-Use question intonation in declarative statements: women make declarative statements into
questions by raising the pitch of their voice at the end of a statement, expressing uncertainty. For
example, “What school do you attend? Eton College?”
j-Use “wh-” imperatives, e.g “Why don't you open the door?”
k-Speak less frequently
l-Overuse qualifiers,e.g. “I think that...”
m-Apologise more, e.g.“I'm sorry, but I think that...”
n-Use modal constructions: (such as can, would, should, ought - “Should we turn up the
heat?”)
o-Avoid coarse language or expletives
p-Use indirect commands and requests, e.g “My, isn't it cold in here?” ( which is really a
request to turn the heat on or close a window)
q-Use more intensifiers, especially so and very, e.g. “I am so glad you came!”
r-Lack a sense of humour: women do not tell jokes well and often don't understand the punch
line of joke.
Lakoff [59] also claims that women use color words like mauve, beige, aquamarine,
lavender, and magenta but most men do not. She also maintains that adjectives such as
adorable, charming, divine, lovely, and sweet are also commonly used by women but only very
rarely by men. Women are also said to have their own vocabulary for emphasizing certain effects
on them, words and expressions such as so good, such fun, exquisite, lovely, divine, precious,
adorable, darling, and fantastic. In her work, Lakoff identified a number of female linguistic
features which were unified by their function of expressing lack of confidence.
In Living Language, Keith and Shuttleworth [55, p.222] record suggestions that:
a-women: talk more than men, talk too much, are more polite, are indecisive/hesitant,
complain and ask more questions, support each other, are more co-operative, whereas
b-men: swear more, don't talk about emotions, talk about sport more, talk about women and
machines in the same way, insult each other frequently, are competitive in conversation, dominate
conversation, speak with more authority, give more commands, interrupt more.
97
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Tannen [95] reveals a very different conversational style by men and women. Women,
Tannen says, use conversation to make connections and establish intimacy and community, what she
refers to as “rapport talk”. Conversation is more cooperative than competitive, although sometimes
competition is masked by apparent cooperation. Men, on the other hand, see conversation as means
of establishing status and power. Information giving, or “report talk” is one way to achieve a higher
status in conversation. In simple terms, men tend to negotiate status through conflict and report talk
whereas women create a sense of community through rapport talk.
Mills [2003, as cited in [169] contests the view that women are more polite than men.
Lakoff [1975, as cited in [169] identified three forms of politeness: formal, deference, and
camaraderie. Women's language is characterized by formal and deference politeness, whereas
men's language is exemplified by camaraderie.
In setting out a list of what she calls sociolinguistic universal tendencies, Holmes
[44] does offer some testable claims. There are five of these:
According to Graham of University of Kentucky [200], there are the following differences in
communication characteristics between men and women:
98
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Latin.
The English language makes certain distinctions of a gender-based kind, e.g., actor-actress,
waiter-waitress, and master-mistress. In some Native American languages, grammatical forms of
verbs are inflected differently according to the gender of the speaker.
The term ‘sexism’ was coined in the 1960s, probably by analogy with the term racism, to
describe ‘discrimination within a social system on the basis of sexual membership’ [Wodak,
1997, as cited in [61, p.13]. Sexist language can be defined as one type of discriminatory
language (use); it refers to linguistic expressions that exlude, trivialize or insult (maily)
women. Sexism makes sense within a historically hierarchical relationship between men and
women, where one is the norm, and the other marked as ‘other’ or inferior, and in relation to a
wide range of social practices where women (and in some cases men) are exploited, manipulated or
constrained because of their sex.
There are a number of ways in which it has been suggested that the English language
discriminates against women [61, pp.13-18]:
In English language, there are many words, which are clearly male-orientated in that they
contain the element “-man” while they can in fact apply to both sexes, e.g. chairman, newsman,
policeman, mailman, congressman.
Many words reinforce the view of women as deviant, abnormal or subordinate group.
Sexism in language is also showed in that the noun of feminine gender can only be obtained by
adding a certain bound morpheme to the noun. For example, English morphology generally takes
the male form as the base form and add a suffix to signal female. Examples are:
100
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
The use of an additional suffix to signal femaleness is seen as conveying the message that
women are deviant or abnormal.
It has also been suggested that suffixes like –ess and –ette trivialize and diminish women.
Generic structures provide further evidence to support the claim that the English language
maginalised women and treat them as abnormal. In fact, words like’generic’ he and man can be said
to render women invisible. The basis for claims that English renders women invisible is the use of
he and man as generic forms. It is also clear that the word man is associated with male images even
though when it is used generically.
Second, most obviously, perhaps, in the semantic area the English metaphors available to
describe women include an extraordinarily high number of derogatory images compared to those
used to describe men.
Animal imagery is one example of an area where the images of women seem considerably
less positive than those for men. Consider the negativity of bitch, old biddy and cow, compared to
stud and wolf. Animal imagery which refers to men often has at least some positive component
(such as wiliness or sexual prowess). Birds are widely regarded as feather-brained and flighty!
Even the more positive chick and kitten are sweet but helpless pets.
Women may also be described or referred to in terms of food imagery, which is equally
insulting. Saccharine terms, such as sugar, sweetie, honey, are mainly, though not exclusively,
used for addressing women. Less complimentary terms such as crumpet and tart, however, are
restricted to female referents. They illustrate a common evolutionary pattern in the meaning of
words referring to women. Terms which were originally neutral or affectionate eventually acquire
negative connotations as they increasingly refer only to women and as their meanings focus on
women as sexual objects. By contrast, there appears to be less food imagery which is
appropriate for referring only to men.
Third, newspapers [61, p.15], for example, are full of examples of wordings which are used
to portray women in negative or limiting ways, i.e. wordings which:
a-depict women as sex objects and on the basis of their appearance rather than their intellect
or capabilities (e.g. ‘a blonde’);
b-define women in terms of home, family, and domestic roles (e.g. ‘mother of three’), in
ways that are seldom used for men;
101
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
c-trivialize women (e.g. using ‘girl’ for a much wider age range than ‘boy’ would be used;
also ‘weathergirl’; judge women , ‘career woman’).
Sexist wordings portray women as sex objects and, judging on the basis of their appearance
rather than intellect or capabilities, they define women in terms of home, family and domestic
roles, and trivialize women.
102
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
The dynamic or social constructionist approach is, as Coates describes, the most current
approach to language and gender. Instead of speech falling into a natural gendered category, the
dynamic nature and multiple factors of an interaction help a socially appropriate gendered
construct. As such, West and Zimmerman (1987) describe these constructs as "doing gender"
instead of the speech itself necessarily being classified in a particular category. This is to say that
these social constructs, while affiliated with particular genders, can be utilized by speakers as they
see fit.
Women are generally believed to speak a better "language" then men do. This is a
constant misconception, but scholars believe that no gender speaks a better language, but that each
gender instead speaks its own unique language. This notion has sparked further research into the
study of the differences between the way men and women communicate.
Wardhaugh [107, pp.326-327] has three claims, the first of which seems reasonable: “The
first claim is that men and women are biologically different and that this difference has
serious consequences for gender.”
“The differences between women and men in ways of interacting may be the result of
different socialisation and culturation patterns. If we learn the ways of talking mainly in
single sex peer groups, then the patterns we learn are likely to be sex-specific. And the
kind of miscommunication which undoubtedly occurs between women and men will be
attributable to the different expectations each sex has of the function of the interaction, and
the ways it is appropriately conducted” [Holmes,1992, as cited in [107, p.328].
“Sex varieties, then, are the result of different social attitudes towards the behaviour of
men and women, and of the attitudes men and women themselves consequently have to
language as a social symbol” [102, p.94].
Follow-up activity 2.7.2. True/False: Discuss with your friends and decide whether
the following statements are true or false:
1-There are many linguistic differences between male’s language and female’s language.
2-Gender-exclusive differences in language use involve the form of language use by only
one gender.
103
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
3-Tannen (1990) states that women use conversation as “rapport talk” and men use it as
“report talk”.
4-English males often use emotional adjectives such as super, lovely, sweet,
exclamations such as goodness, oh dear, and intesifiers such as so, such...
5-There are gender differences in language use because there are social and physical
differences between males and females.
CHAPTER II REVIEW I
I-Answer the following questions:
1-A language variety is a specific set of ‘linguistic items’ or ‘human speech pattern’ which
we can uniquely associate with some external factor.
2-A language variable is a linguistic item which has various forms (variants).
3-The dialects of a single language may be defined as mutually intelligible forms of the
language which differ in systematic ways from each other.
4-Dialects originally meant varieties based on social classes.
5-A dialect has prestige which a language lacks.
104
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
6-The standard language is the variety of a language which has the highest status in a
community or nation, and which is usually based on the speech and writing of educated native
speakers of the language.
7-Standard varieties are codified varieties.
8-A standard language is ususlly not used in education and media.
9-Standardisation refers to the process by which a language has been codified in some way.
That process usually involves the development of such things as grammars, spelling, dictionaries….
10-According to Haugen, the process of language standardization are: a-selection, b-
codification, c-elaboration and d-acceptance.
11-An idiolect is the language variety spoken in one region of a country.
12-English is the language of Indo-European family.
13-The word English is from Old English Englics that comes from the name of the Angles.
The Angles were named from Engle, their land of origin.
14-In broad term, British English is English used throughout the United Kingdom, but it is
often more narrowly understood as the English of England.
15-Two important regional dialects of England are Northern and Southern.
16-The major difference between Northern English and Southern English is in pronunciation.
17-American English originated from Australian English.
18-General English is the standard pronunciation of British English.
19-The terms Network English, Network Standard or General American refer to standard
American.
20-There is a difference in the pronunciation of the vowel sound of such words as dance,
fast, grass… between British English and American English.
21-North American has given the English language many thousands of words, meanings and
phrases.
22-There are no grammatical and spelling differences between British English and American
English.
23-Professor Hans Kurath grouped American dialects into Northern dialect and Southern
dialect only.
24-British English, American English and Australian English can be considered to be
different territorial dialects of the English language.
105
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
25-From geographical viewpoint we may say that the country wide standard form of written
English was derived from the speech of London. Socially, it originated from the speech of middle
class educated people.
26-The dialect we term standard English is spken with only one accent: RP.
27-International English is the concept of the English language as a global means of
communication in numerous dialects, and also the movement towards an international standard for
the language.
28-A sociolect is a variety of language used by people belonging to a particular social class
or it can be defined as the linguistic differences associated with definable social groups in a single
geographical area.
29-Social stratification gives rise linguistically to social class dialects or sociolects.
30-Gender-exclusive differences in language use means the relative frequency with which
men and women use the same lexical items or other linguistic features.
106
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Children begin to produce one-word utterances between the ages of 12 and 18 months. A
basic property of these one - word utterances is that they can be used to express the type of
meaning that would be associated with an entire sentence in adult speech. Thus, a child might
use the word dada to mean I saw daddy’s hat, more to mean give me more. Such utterances are
called holophrases. A striking feature of holophrastic utterances is children’s skill in
communicating complex messages with a single word.
In the years following the telegraphic stage, children continue to acquire complex grammar
that underlies adult linguistic competence.
At the earlier age, conversations are often very erratic and disjointed, with parents doing
most of the ‘work’, and children using sequences of utterances, many of which are not obviously
directed to any listener. The effect is a curious mixture of monologue and dialogue.
By three years old, it is plain that children have learned many aspects of conversational
strategy. They are able to initiate a dialogue, can handle several of the conventions of turn-taking.
They know a great deal of how to respond appropriately.
These skills develop greatly between three and five years old. In particular, there is a major
development in child awareness of the social factors that govern a successful conversations: correct
use forms of address, making requests, turn-taking, repairing..
7.3.1.6. Language development in school
When children arrive in school, the educational setting presents them with a variety of
unfamiliar, subject related styles of language. They have to learn a new range of linguistic
skills - reading, writing, and spelling.
They find themselves having to talk about what they are doing, which requires that they
learn a special technical vocabulary – a ‘language for talking about language’, or metalanguage.
The child uses its LAD to make sense of the utterances heard around it, deriving from
this ‘primary linguistic data’ hypotheses about the grammar of the language - what the sentences
are, and how they are constructed.
This knowledge is then used to produce sentences that, after a process of trial and error,
correspond to those in adult speech: the child has learned a set of generalizations, or rules.
Governing the way in which sentences are formed.
d-Cognition
The main alternative account argues that language acquisition must be viewed within the
context of a child’s intellectual development. Linguistic structures will emerge only if there is an
already-established cognitive foundation.
Several early child language scholars maintained that such a relationship exists, but the most
influential account stems from the model of cognitive development proposed by the Genevan
psychologist Jean Piaget (1896-1980)
7.3.2. Adolescence language/ Teenage talk
7.3.2.1. Language development in adolescence [93]
Although the majority of language development occurs in the critical infant through
preschool years, development continues into the adolescent years. Because the development is less
acute, the study of this linguistic period in a child's life is relatively new.
According to KidsHealth at [ 137] , adolescence is the period of a child's life as he
approaches adulthood and can begin anywhere from eight to fourteen years of age. It is marked by
numerous physical changes as well as certain cognitive developments.
Linguistics development during this time is subtle but important. This development
includes learning to use more complex language and to communicate differently depending on the
situation.
For the most part, pragmatics and semantics are the linguistic features which are developed
during adolescence. Pronunciation and phonology are primarily formed during the younger years.
At this time, children grow their vocabulary and learn the proper and underlying use of
each word. They also develop syntactic use and sentence form.
Also, as she develops socially, she will learn subtle societal differences in how certain
groups communicate and will be able to adapt her language to that situation. These abilities
prepare a child for further learning and for a growing community of peers and mentors.
109
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
However, the language of adolescents is distinguished from the language of children by its
intended purposes, functions, and consequences for behaviour and experience.
For children, language is the medium through which the world is discovered and the means
by which society and people are integrative and inclusive. The linguistic forms of adolescents have
their function as markers of social identities and group exclusiveness, and are sometimes used as
weapons to oppose, confuse, or offend.
In order to maintain its first function as a marker of group distinctiveness and solidarity,
and to minimize the potential encroachments of adults, adolescent terminology constantly change:
old terms replaced by new ones to refer to roughly the same phenomena, thereby preserving the
secret codes by which in-group members indentify and communicate with one another.
110
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
The use of slang, taboo words and alliteration, the style of articulation, and other kinds of
verbal play, proclaim for the adolescent membership in, and allegiance to, a particular group, and
serve as forms of resistance to adult control.
ii-to be a means of expressing the values and experiences of group members.
The second function of adolescent language is its use as a means of expressing the values and
experiences of group members.
The use of a special language, whether employed by adults (e.g., medical students,
academics, gamblers, etc.) or by adolescents, has much the same function for both groups, namely,
to facilitate communication among individuals who share common interests and concerns, and who
need a common vocabulary for talking about such matters.
A good part of the interests and concerns of adolescents revolve around such forbidden topics
as sexual activities, drinking, drugs, and lawbreaking, for which a special vocabulary unintelligible
for the adults is required.
Slang and elliptical utterances, vague in reference, are shorthand devices to save time and
effort by summing up complex and recurrent phenomena and experiences.
iii-to establish status and associational patterns within the adolescent social system
The third function of AL is to establish status and associational patterns within the adolescent
social system. Adolescent society provides a status system and terminology which enable youths to
engage in predictable and consistent interactions with their peers, and to acquire a sense of personal
worth based on sub-cultural standards rather than the norms of adult society.
The status terminology employed is affectively coded such that a youth’s status in the local
prestige hierarchy is partly a function of the meanings associated with the terms peers use to connote
his or her character and group affiliations.
These terms have laudatory or pejorative connotations and are in part related to the
adolescent’s interpersonal skills and accomplishments, as measured by the norms, standards, and
values of the peer group.
7.3.2.3. Gender-specific uses of adolescent language
Beyond the general functions of adolescent language noted above are gender – specific uses
of peer group language. Differential patterns of socialization and extensive interaction in single-sex
peer groups account for some of the gender-specific uses of language found among adolescent.
Since boys and girls typically interact in homegeneous groups and in different social
contexts, they tend to develop different genres of speech and skills for doing things with words.
The peer-group networks of adolescent female are typically smaller, and games or other
recreational activities are generally cooperative and noncompetitive. Closeness, intimacy, mutual
commitment, loyalty, equality, cooperation and support are girls’ important features.
111
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Girls learn to deal with conflicts through the use of language in several ways:
a-to create and maintain relationships of closeness and equality,
b-To criticize others in acceptable way,
c-to interpret accurately the speech of other girls.
Boys’ groups are typically larger and more hierachically structured than girls’group.
Posturing and counterposturing is a prominent feature of the male adolescent social world,
where patterns of speech are designed to accomplish at least three objectives:
Use of speech for the expression of dominance can be done through verbal duelling, story
telling, joke-telling, narrative performance, getting the floor to perform, maintaining his audience
in the midst of a performance, and successfully completing his performance.
Boys learn to assert their identity and opinions in such context by side comment, putdowns
and challenges.
112
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
f-Ritual conflict
The core type of ritual conflict is the ritual insult consisting of competitive sequences
where the participants exchange insults directed at the listener’s mother (the your mum is X
format). Ritual insult is primarily a male activity, some forms of ritual conflict are also found
among adolescent females.
Development slows in adulthood but continues through the lifetime unless hindered by
poor health, accident, or injury. As an adult, a person becomes the example for children and sets
the standards for mature behavior. As an adult, a person adds new skills, new words, and new
problem-solving skills to the formidable ones already possessed.
Social and communicative abilities adapt subtly to the many different environments in
which an adult functions. With development of a truly impressive set of pragmatic and
interactional skills, an adult learns to maneuver in the complex worlds of family, profession, and
community, and increasingly in the international multiethnic realm.
As a language user, anadult gains increasing flexibility. The organization of his or her huge
vocabulary enables an adult to access concepts both effectively and efficiently. Increased social
skills help an adult to choose the most appropriate words and syntactic structures for any given
situation. This doesn’t mean that language will be error-free or that an adult is an effective
communicator in every situation—especially cross-cultural ones—but as you mature you will be
even more skilled than you are already.
113
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
With aging, there is a slow decline in both oral and written language comprehension,
understanding syntactically complex sentences, and inferencing (Nicholas, Connor, Obler, &
Albert, 1998). Decline may be related to either overload or processing difficulties in working
memory. Although comprehension of figurative language seems unimpaired in healthy older
adults, the ability to explain figurative expressions does decline (Gregory & Waggoner, 1996).
The evolution of processing strategies may be reflected in the shifting recall patterns that
occur with adult changes. The free recall of complex linguistic material decreases with age. These
changes in cognitive operations may be more quantitative than qualitative. The elderly have
more difficulty with linguistic processing that requires greater organization in order to recall. In
general, the elderly are more sensitive to theme or underlying meaning but are less able than young
adults to recall syntax.
The incidence of hearing loss increases with age, being both more common and more
severe for the participants in their 80s than for those in their 70s. In addition, men are more
affected than women.
2-By 18 months, most children are thought that they can speak about 50 words and
understand as five times as many.
3-There are two contexts for the development of adolescent language: a-family and b-
school
114
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
5-In adulthood, the rate of language development slows, and language mature and deepens.
Adults are very flexible language users, in part, because of the variety of forms and functions
available. Although there are some signs of language and communication decline in some seniors,
most adults continue to be effective communicators well into their retirement years.
The term "taboo" comes from the Tongan tapu or Fijian tabu ("prohibited", "disallowed",
"forbidden"), related among others to the Maori tapu, Hawaiian kapu, Malagasy fady. Its English
use dates to 1777 when the British explorer James Cook visited Tonga. Describing the cultural
practices of the Tongans, he wrote:
“Not one of them would sit down, or eat a bit of any thing.... On expressing my surprise at
this, they were all taboo, as they said; which word has a very comprehensive meaning; but, in
general, signifies that a thing is forbidden.
When anything is forbidden to be eaten, or made use of, they say, that it is taboo” [192].
The term was translated to him as "consecrated, inviolable, forbidden, unclean or
cursed". Tabu itself has been derived from alleged Tongan morphemes ta ("mark") and bu
("especially"), but this may be a folk etymology (note that Tongan does not actually have a
phoneme /b/), and tapu is usually treated as a unitary, non-compound word inherited from Proto-
Polynesian tapu, in turn inherited from Proto-Oceanic tabu, with the reconstructed meaning
"sacred, forbidden". In its current use on Tonga, the word tapu means "sacred" or "holy", often
in the sense of being restricted or protected by custom or law.
A taboo is a vehement prohibition of an action based on the belief that such behavior is
either too sacred or too accursed for ordinary individuals to undertake. Taboo itself also contains a
binary opposition, referring to human experiences, words, or deeds that are unmentionable because
they are either ineffably sacred (like the name of God) or unspeakably vile (like incest).
“Taboos exist in all known cultures, referring to certain act, objects, or relationships which
society wishes to avoid-and thus to the language used to talk about them” [20, p.8)]
“Taboo words are those that are to be avoided because they are deemed unfit for normal
linguistic usage and by community consensus are banned in everyday language in the
public domain” [3].
Taboo words are to be avoided because they are powerful and can cause unforeseen
consequences. Underlying the avoidance of taboo words are cultural values and belief systems
according to which attitudes are formed and judgements are made regarding the offending,
dangerous, or sacred nature of certain words.
Since there are cross-cultural variations in attitudes and belief systems, there are also
differences concerning the semantic range and topical nature of taboo words.
Taboo words are avoided by paraphrases, synonyms, euphemism, code words, silence.
The use of taboo words is generally associated with strong emotions. Therefore, speakers use
them to express strong feelings and listeners respond with equally strong feelings and
reactions. The repeated use of taboo words diminishes their tabooness.
Whether or not a taboo word belonging to any of the above categories is used in speech
116
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
depends on the social cultural contexts. The use of the above words should be avoided in the
formal language in the public domain. Taboo words are likely to be used in social interactions
among friends in a private domain where an informal speech style prevails. The socio-cultural
contexts of speech and the backgrounds of the participants in social interaction determine the
use of taboo words. Such factors as enculturation, socialization, age, gender, social status,
educational level determines the use of taboo words in speech. Taboo words vary from country to
country, culture to culture, person to person.
Formal swearing is a ritual of social compliance and obligation: in marriage, in court, for
high office, and as allegiance to the state. On the other hand, informal swearing constitutes a
transgression of social codes ranging from the merely impolite to the criminal.
Swearing now includes so many varied and developed forms that some broad distinctions
need to be made at the outset. Let us start with differences between mode and content. In terms of
mode, we swear by some higher force or somebody; we swear that something is so; we swear to do
something; we swear at something or somebody; and we swear simply out of anger,
disappointment, or frustration. These different modes can be retermed by various unfamiliar
classical terms, such as asseveration, invocation, imprecation, malediction, blasphemy, profanity,
obscenity, and ejaculation (in its old sense of “exclamation”). The figure Varieties of Swearing and
Word Magic is designed to give the reader a basic map of the territory, showing the hierarchical
separation between the binary opposites of “sacred,” “profane,” and “taboo,” divided by the “line
of acceptability” on which stands “oaths,” since they can be either sacred or profane. The
categories of “obscenity,” “foul language,” and “ethnic slurs” stand below the line because they
are purely secular and have no sacred equivalent.
117
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
At base, swearing is governed by “sacral” notions of word magic; that is to say the belief
that words have the power to change the world. These beliefs tend to be very powerful at primitive
stages of society, manifesting themselves in charms, spells, invocations, and curses so that taboos
or prohibitions have grown up around dangerous or offensive usages. Swearing is, in one sense, a
violation of these taboos: the “high” varieties violate the taboo of invoking the name of the
deity, while the “low” are often violations of sexual taboos, especially those concerning copulation
and incest. This dualistic juxtaposition of the binary opposites of the sacred and the profane, the
high and the low, symbolically represents the angelic and the diabolical potentialities of man.
a-Prayer [184]
Prayer (from the Latin precari "to ask earnestly, beg, entreat") is an invocation or act that
seeks to activate a rapport with an object of worship through deliberate communication.
Prayer can be a form of religious practice, may be either individual or communal and take
place in public or in private. It may involve the use of words, song or complete silence. When
language is used, prayer may take the form of a hymn, incantation, formal creedal statement, or a
spontaneous utterance in the praying person. There are different forms of prayer such as petitionary
prayer, prayers of supplication, thanksgiving, and praise. Prayer may be directed towards a deity,
spirit, deceased person, or lofty idea, for the purpose of worshipping, requesting guidance,
requesting assistance, confessing transgressions (sins) or to express one's thoughts and emotions.
Thus, people pray for for the sake of others.
e.g. The Lord’s prayer as it occurs in the ESV version of Matthew 6:9–13 [173]
“Our Father in heaven, hallowed be your name. Your kingdom come, your will be done, on
earth as it is in heaven. Give us this day our daily bread, and forgive us our debts, as we also have
forgiven our debtors. And lead us not into temptation, but deliver us from evil.'"
118
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
In the law, testimony is a form of evidence that is obtained from a witness who makes a
solemn statement or declaration of fact. Testimony may be oral or written, and it is usually
made by oath or affirmation under penalty of perjury. Unless a witness is testifying as an expert
witness, testimony in the form of opinions or inferences is generally limited to those opinions or
inferences that are rationally based on the perceptions of the witness and are helpful to a clear
understanding of the witness' testimony. In testimony the form of the words is traditional, as in
“I solemnly swear to tell the truth, the whole truth, and nothing but the truth, so help me God.”
c-Oath [177]
i-Traditionally an oath (from Anglo-Saxon āð, also called plight) is either a statement of
fact or a promise with wording relating to something considered sacred as a sign of verity. A
common legal substitute for those who conscientiously object to making sacred oaths is to give an
affirmation instead. Nowadays, even when there's no notion of sanctity involved, certain promises
said out loud in ceremonial or juridical purpose are referred to as oaths. To swear is a verb used to
describe the taking of an oath, to making a solemn vow. We should bear in mind that the oldest of
to swear was simply “to take an oath or to give a solemn undertaking,” and that for several
centuries (from about 900 to 1400) this was the only sense.
ii-Divine oath
Usually oaths have referred to a deity significant in the cultural sphere in question. The
reciter's personal views upon the divinity of the aspects considered sacred in a predictated text of an
oath may or may not be taken in to account. There might not be alternative personal proclamations
with no mention of the sacred dogma in question, such as affirmations, to be made. This might
mean an impasse to those with unwillingness to edify the dogma they see as untrue and those
who decline to refer to sacred matters on the subject at hand.
A person taking an oath indicates this in a number of ways. The most usual is the explicit "I
swear," but any statement or promise that includes "with as my witness" or "so help me," with
being something or someone the oath-taker holds sacred, is an oath. Many people take an oath by
holding in their hand or placing over their head a book of scripture or a sacred object, thus
indicating the sacred witness through their action: such an oath is called corporal. However, the
119
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
chief purpose of such an act is for ceremony or solemnity, and the act does not of itself make an
oath.
Some oaths of office are a statement of loyalty to a constitution or other legal text or to a
person or other office-holder (e.g., an oath to support the constitution of the state, or of loyalty to
the king). Under the laws of a state it may be considered treason or a high crime to betray a
sworn oath of office.
The word 'oath' and the phrase 'I swear' refer to a solemn declaration to God. For those
who choose not to, the alternative terms 'solemn promise' and 'I promise' are sometimes used.
Before he enters on the Execution of his Office, he shall take the following Oath or
Affirmation: "I do solemnly swear (or affirm) that I will faithfully execute the Office of President
of the United States, and will to the best of my ability, preserve, protect and defend the Constitution
of the United States."[So help me God]
120
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
7.4.2.4. Informal swearing: to use rude or offensive word, usually because you are angry
In informal swearing, rude or offensive words are used to swear at somebody, usually
because you are angry, then the mode changes to profanity, blasphemy, imprecation, or
malediction. The distinction between profanity and blasphemy is quite complex and hinges largely
on intention, in that profanity is usually regarded as habitual, whereas blasphemy is more
obviously intentional or deliberate. However, both involve the violation of the taboos against the
use of holy names and referents (Hughes, 2006, pp. xvi-xx).
To curse means to swear, to say rude things to sb, to use a majic word or phrase against
somebody in order to harm them.
c-Blasphemy [145]
Blasphemy is the act of insulting or showing contempt or lack of reverence to a deity, to
religious or holy persons or sacred things, or toward something considered sacred or inviolable.
d-Profanity [185]
Profanity, as defined by Merriam-Webster, is "an offensive[a] word" or "offensive
language". It is also called bad language, strong language, coarse language, foul language, bad
words, vulgar language, lewd language, swearing, cursing, cussing, or using expletives. This
use is a subset of a language's lexicon that is generally considered to be strongly impolite, rude or
offensive. It can show a debasement of someone or something, or show intense emotion. Profanity
in this sense takes the form of words or verbal expressions that fall into the category of formulaic
language.
In its older, more literal sense, the term "profanity"refers to "offensive words, or
religious words", used in a way that shows the user does not respect "God or holy things", or
behaviour showing similar disrespect.
e-Ethnic slur
An ethnic slur is a term designed to insult others on the basis of race, ethnicity, or
121
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
nationality.
g-Four-letter words
In informal swearing and cursing, there are notions of four-letter words and dirty
words. Swear-words and curse-words may include four-letter words such as fuck, ball..
h-Dirty words
Dirty words are words referring to sexual organs and acts, body elimination processes such
as defecation, urination, and excretary substance. The term dirty words indicate a very personal
and subjective attitudes.
The functions of informal swearing are complex. Most obviously, it is an outlet for
frustration or pent up emotion and a means of releasing nervous energy after a sudden shock. It
has also been credited with various social functions as a marker of group identity and
solidarity, and as a way of expressing aggression without resort to violence. In these social
contexts swearing can become a dominant linguistic trait, with sentences often containing many
taboo words.
7.4.3. Euphemism:
7.4.3.1. Euphemism
Euphemisms are mild, agreeable, or roundabout words used in place of coarse, painful, or
offensive ones [160]. A euphemism is a generally innocuous word or expression used in place of
one that may be found offensive or suggest something unpleasant. Euphemisms are used to refer to
122
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
taboo topics (such as disability, sex, excretion, and death) in a polite way, or to mask profanity.
Euphemism comes from the Greek word εὐφημία (euphemia), meaning "the use of words
of good omen", which in turn is derived from the Greek root-words eû (εὖ), "good, well" and
phḗmē (φήμη) "prophetic speech; rumour, talk". Etymologically, the eupheme is the opposite of
the blaspheme "evil-speaking." The term euphemism itself was used as a euphemism by the
ancient Greeks, meaning "to keep a holy silence" (speaking well by not speaking at all).
The word lavatory has, naturally, produced many euphemisms. Here are some of them:
powder room, washroom, restroom, retiring room, (public) comfort station, ladies' (room),
gentlemen's (room), water-closet, w.c., public conveniences.
Pregnancy is another topic for "delicate" references. Here are some of the euphemisms
used as substitutes for the adjective pregnant: in an interesting condition, in a delicate
condition, in the family way, with a baby coming, (big) with child, expecting.
The adjective drunk, for instance, has a great number of such substitutes, some of them
"delicate", but most comical. E. g. intoxicated (form.), under the influence (form.), tipsy,
mellow, fresh, high, merry, flustered, overcome, full (coll.), drunk as a lord (coll.), drunk as
an owl (coll.), boiled (sl.), fried (sl.), tanked (sl.), tight (sl.), stiff (sl.), pickled (sl.), soaked (sl.),
three sheets to the wind (sl.), high as a kite (sl.), half-seas- over (sl.), etc.
The word God, due to other considerations, also had a great number of substitutes which
can still be traced in such phrases as Good Lord!, By Heavens/, Good Heavens!, (My)
goodness!, (My) goodness gracious!, Gracious me!
Even in our modern emancipated times, old superstitious fears still lurk behind words
associated with death and fatal diseases. People are not superstitious nowadays and yet they are
surprisingly reluctant to use the verb to die which has a long chain of both solemn and
humorous substitutes. E. g. to pass away, to be taken, to breathe one's last, to depart this life, to
close one's eyes, to yield (give) up the ghost, to go the way of all flesh, to go West (sl.), to kick
off (sl.), to check out (sl.), to kick the bucket (sl.), to take a ride (sl.), to hop the twig (sl.), to
join the majority (sl.).
Mental diseases also cause the frequent use of euphemisms. A mad person may be
described as insane, mentally unstable, unbalanced, unhinged, not (quite) right (coll.), not all
there (coll.), off one's head (coll.), off one's rocker (coll.), wrong in the upper storey (coll.),
having bats in one's belfry (coll.), crazy as a bedbug (coll.), cuckoo (sl.), nutty (sl.), off
one's nut (sl.), loony (sl.), a mental case, a mental defective, etc.
According to Rawson [80, pp.1-11] euphemisms can be divided into two general types—
positive and negative. The positive ones inflate and magnify, making the euphemized items
seem altogether grander and more important than they really are. The negative euphemisms
deflate and diminish. They are defensive in nature, offsetting the power of tabooed terms and
otherwise eradicating from the language everything that people prefer not to deal with directly.
123
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Positive euphemisms include the many fancy occupational titles, which salve the egos of
workers by elevating their job status: custodian for janitor (itself a euphemism for caretaker),
counsel for lawyer, the many kinds of engineer (exterminating engineer, mattress engineer,
publicity engineer, ad infinitum), help for servant (itself an old euphemism for slave), hooker and
working girl for whore, and so forth.
Other kinds of positive euphemisms include the many institutional euphemisms, which
convert madhouses into mental hospitals, colleges into universities, and small business
establishments into emporiums, parlors, salons, and shoppes.
Examples in this category include such nowstandard terms as cemetery (from the Greek
word for sleeping place. It replaced the more deathly graveyard), and the names of various
barnyard animals, including the donkey (the erstwhile ass), the sire (or studhorse), and the
rooster (for cock, and one of many similar evasions, e. g., haystack for haycock, weather vane
for weathercock, and Louisa May Alcott, whose father changed the family name from the nasty-
sounding Alcox.
Into this category, too, fall such watered-down swear words as cripes, Jiminy Cricket,
gee, and gosh, all designed to avoid taking holy names in vain and now commonly used without
much awareness of their original meaning, particularly by youngsters and by those who fill in
the balloons in comic strips. Then there are the words for which no honest Anglo-Saxon (often a
euphemism for "dirty") equivalents exist, e. g., brassiere, which has hardly anything to do with
the French bras (arm) from which it derives, and toilet, from the diminutive of toile (cloth).
Conscious euphemisms constitute a much more complex category, which is hardly
surprising, given the ingenuity, not to say the deviousness, of the human mind. This is not to imply
that euphemisms cannot be employed more or less honestly as well as knowingly. For example,
garbage men are upgraded routinely into sanitation men, but to say "Here come the sanitation
men" is a comparatively venial sin. The meaning does come across intelligibly, and the listener
understands that it is time to get out the garbage cans.
By the same token, it is honest enough to offer a woman condolences upon "the loss of her
husband, “where loss stands for death. Not only are amenities preserved: By avoiding the
troublesome term, the euphemism actually facilitates social discourse.
124
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Follow-up activity 2.7.4.True/False: Discuss with your friends and decide whether
the following statements are true or false:
1-Taboo words are those that are to be avoided because they are deemed unfit for normal
linguistic usage and by community consensus are banned in everyday language in the public domain.
Taboo words can be avoided by using paraphrases, synonyms, euphemisms, and code words.
2-Formal swearing constitutes a transgression of social codes ranging from a merely impolite
to the criminal.
3-Informal swearing involves the use of rude or offensive words.
4-Euphemisms are mild, agreeable, or round about words used in place of course, painful, or
offensive ones.
5-According to Rawson [80, pp.1-11], euphemisms can be divided into two general types:
positive and negative.
cohesiveness within a group or with a trend or fashion in society at large”. Accordingly, sharing the
same slang vocabulary helps both to gain acceptance in a group and to preserve group identity. On
the other hand, slang is said to serve anti-social purposes such as marking social differences,
opposing authority, and hiding secret information or improper behavior from them. In particular,
slang is viewed as an in-group vocabulary that certain sub-classes in society (e.g. criminals or drug
addicts) cultivate to keep the content of their conversations private, or which such specific subgroups
as adolescents or college students adopt to keep the other generation at a distance.
The two conflicting – social and anti-social tendencies of slang are evident in the effects it
may produce: If sometimes it appears playful and amusing, it may, some other times signal the
speaker’ s intention to startle the audience or even to be aggressive.
b-The stylistic approach
Within the stylistic approach, slang is justaposed to formal language: particularly, it is below
standard discourse and the neutral stylistic level, and typical of informal, relaxed speech. On the
other hand, slang is also justaposed to other non-standard varieties: it is neither dialect nor register,
nor can it be restricted to the concepts of cant (the specialized and usually secret language of
thieves, professional beggars, and other groups operating on the springes of society ) , argot (a
secret language used by various groups, e.g. schoolmates, outlaws, colleagues… to prevent the
outsiders from understanding the conversation), or jargon (the specialized vocabulary and
phraseology of a set of people sharing a trade or profession). Slang can be rather vieved as a short-
lived ephemeral vocabulary that is expecting either to pass into disuse or to have a more standard
status.
c-The linguistic approach
From the linguistic point of view, slang is regarded as the use of ordinary words in
extraordinary senses or of extraordinary words in ordinary senses. Jesperson pineers this position,
stating that slang “finds amusement in the creation and propagation of new words”.
Within the linguistic approach, slang is distinguished from the standard language in both its
morphology and its semantics. In morphology, it is characterized by clear insubordination as regards
the standard word-formation rules, as in semantics, it not only renames every day object, but also
enriches, qualifies and complexifies them. Hence, Sorings (1981), [as cited in [65] definition of
slang:
Slang is, as it were, a language in statu nascendi (or at least a lexicon) in the making. Slang
is essentially an experimental language.
d-The lexicographic definition
In dictionaries, there is no unique clear-cut definition of slang because this concept has
acquired different senses in different periods of time. Originally, the term was used to refer to the
language of criminals, thieves and vagabonds. The OED, for instance, states that:
In the mid-eighteen century, it labelled “the special vocabulary used by any set of a low or
disreputable character”, and Webster & Mc Kecnie (1963) [as cited in [65] specify that in the
126
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
beginning it referred to the specialized vocabulary and idioms of criminals, tramps, etc. the purpose
of which was to disguise from outsiders the meaning of what was said”. Accordingly, there seems to
be an overlap between the original sense of slang and the current concept of cant.
But soon after the mid-eighteenth century, the term “slang” gradually broadened to include
the language of other sub-groups, not necessarily of lower culture, but rather connected by their
profession, like lawers, scientists, historians, artists and poets (OED), or by a common way of life.
In this sense, the term became more specialized and nearly synonymous with jargon.
Lastly, in the early years of the nineteen century, slang acquired the more general sense of
colloquial of vocabulary which is outside of conventional or standard usage, and which belongs
rather to familiar conversation than to written language.
Sandburg states that “slang is language which takes off its coat, spits on its hands- and goes
to work” [as cited in [64, p.64].
127
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
out. It is also used by people sharing the same occupation (like military men and computer users) to
increase efficiency in communication; or by those sharing the same living conditions (like prisoners
and criminals) to hide secret information from people in authority. Lastly, it is used by people
sharing an attitude or lifestyle (like drug addicts and homosexuals) to reinforce their group
cohesiveness. Items like rock (a crystallized form of cocain), smack (a drug, spec. heroin) and
smoke (opium, marijuana) are likewise specific, as they belong to the vocabulary of drug addicts
and drug dealers, but they have a different meaning in the standard language.
General slang, on the other hand, is language that speakers deliberately use to break with the
standard language and to change the level of discourse in the direction of informality. It signals the
speaker’s intention to refuse conventions and their need to be fresh and startling in their expression,
to ease social exchanges and induce friendliness, to reduce excessive seriousness and avoid clichés,
in brief, to enrich the language. General slang words have a wider circulation as they are neither
group-nor subject-restricted: for example, items like bevvy (a drunk), caff (a café) and footy
(football) are much more likely to get established as informal or colloquial English.
Yet, some slang words are both specific and general.
7.5.2.3. Rhyming slang [187]
Rhyming slang is a form of slang word construction in the English language that uses rhyme.
It is often humorous and especially prevalent in the UK, Ireland and Australia. It may have started
in the late 18 century or early 19 century and is closely associated with the Cogney working-class
population, hence its alternative name, Cogney rhyming slang.
The construction of rhyming slang involved replacing a common word with a phrase of two
or more words the last of which rhymes with the original word; the in almost all cases, omitting,
from the end of the phrase, the secondary rhyming word, making the origin and the meaning of the
phrase elusive to listeners not in the know.
Examples of the rhyming slang which are familiar to most British people are apples and
pears (stairs), trouble and strife (wife), plates and meat (feet) and dicky and dirt (shirt).
Sometimes the rhyming part of the phrase has been dropped. Somebody may say, for instance, that
they are going to take a butcher’s (have a look at something). The original expression was to take
a butcher’s hook which rhyme with look. Similarly, a person may say use your loaf (think about
something/ use your head). Originally, the original phrase was use your loaf of bread, which
rhymes with head.
7.5.3. Characteristics of slang [65, pp.41-45]
7.5.3.1. The linguistic properties of slang
a-Phonology
At the phonological level, slang plays with sounds and manipulates word pronunciation
through onomatopia, echoism, jocular mispronunciation of words, assimilation, Cockney rhyming
slang.
128
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
b-Morphology
At the morphology level, it is claimed that “the same ordinary word-building processes that
give rise to the general vocabulary also shape slang expressions” (Eble, 1996, as cited in [65].
c-Grammar
Not much attention has been devoted so far to the grammar of slang.
d-Semantics
The semantics of slang has attracted the attention of almost all pertinent studies. In
particular, Eble [1996, as cited in Mattiello, 2008) underline the tendency of slang to name things
indirectly or figuratively, especially through metaphor, e.g. bird (an areoplane), metonymy, e.g.
tinie/-y (a can of beer), synechdoche, e.g. wheels “ a car”)…
Eble [1996, as cited in Mattiello, 2008) also argues that slang items often diverse from
standard usage in predictable ways, especially by such opposite semantic processes as
generalization, specialization, amelioration and perjoration.
However, it is not always possible to identify a logical connection between a word’s standard
meaning and those added by slang.
7.5.3.2. The sociological properties of slang [65, pp.46-60]
a-The sociological properties of slang [65, pp.46-55]
Slang is associated with many sociological properties, which derive from both its varied
nature and its multifunctionality.
Table 2.30. The Sociological properties of slang [65, p.60]:
Speaker-oriented Hearer-oriented
Group-restriction Playfulness
Subject-restriction Humour
Secrecy Freshness
Privacy Novelty
Informality Desire to impress
Debasement Faddishness
Vulgarity Colour
Obscenity Musicality
Time-restriction Impertinence
129
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Ephemerality Aggressiveness
Localism
The table shows that the sociological properties of slang may be classified in relation to
either the speaker or the hearer.
The speaker-oriented properties of slang characterize the speaker [65, p.59]:
i-as member of a particular group (group-restriction, individualities), often as exclusive one
(secrecy, privacy, culture-restriction, prestige);
ii-as someone with a prescise occupation / activity (subject-restriction, technicality);
iii-as someone having a low cultural status (informality, debasement), or using low / bad
language (vulgarality, obscenity);
iv-as an individual of certain age or generation (time-restriction, epherality) or coming
from a specific regional area (localism).
The hearer-oriented properties are rather meant to produce some effect upon the hearer [65,
p.60], viz:
i-to amuse the hearer or to make him laugh (playfulness, humour);
ii-to release him from the monotony of neutral style (freshness, novelty,
unconventionality);
iii-to impress the hearer (desire to impress), esp. with bizarre expressions (fadiness), or to
attract his attention with colourful words (colour) and their sounds(musicality);
iv-to mock, offend or challenge the hearer (impertinence, offensiveness, aggressiveness).
7.5.4. Functions and uses of slang
Slang has so many uses that it is difficult to choose one as central. According to Eric Partridge
(1933) [as cited in [20, p.53], there are at least fifteen reasons why people often use slang: a-for the
fun of it, b-as an exercise in wit or ingenuity, c-to be different, d-to be picturesque, e-to be arresting,
f-to escape from clichés, g-to enrich the language, i-to add concreteness to speech, j-to reduce
seriousness, k- to be colloquial, h-for ease of social interaction, l-to induce intimacy, m-to show that
one belongs, n-to exclude others and o-to be secret.
Whatever the function and use of slang, one theme recurs among all these reasons: the use of
slang as a means of marking social or linguistic identity. In Patridge’s book Slang: Today and
Yesterday [1933, as cited in [20], the group-identity function in fact provides the basis for most of
130
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
the detailed illustration, which comes from a wide range of geographical areasand occupational
activities.
7.3.5. Slang formation
Slang formation follows different word-formation patterns. Many of them are obtained via
extra-grammatical morphological operations, i.e. word-formation mechanisms which violate various
universal properties of grammatical morphology. Yet, some slang formation can be assigned to
recognized grammatical morphological rules, i.e. rules which are productive in the grammatical
morphology of standard English. A third type of slang formation appears to lie in-between, since it
exhibits some regularity, but extra-grammaticality in the base forms, or vice versa, i.e. it shows the
regularity of the base, but extra-grammaticality in the mechanism of formation.
Follow-up activity 2.7.4.True/False: Discuss with your friends and decide whether
the following statements are true or false:
1-The term slang is used to refer to formal standard words.
2-Slang can be classified into specific and general slang.
3-The sociological properties of slang can be speaker-oriented properties and hearer-oriented
properties.
4-The chief use of slang is metalinguistic.
5-Slang expressions are created by the same process that affect ordinary speech.
7.6. Style
By style, we can mean the degree of formality that characterizes a speaker’s language as
influenced by the social contexts.
Joos [1967, as cited in [79, pp.24-34] in his book The Five Clock distinguishes 5 styles:
a-Frozen: the most careful and elegant variety, reserved for very important or symbolic
moment.
b-Formal: our generally serious levels of language use.
c-Consultative: the plain, every day style
d-Casual: our normal, relaxed style, appropriate to conversations with friends.
e-Intimate: the most grammatically and phonologically reduced style, used exclusively with
our closest friends and family.
Much of frozen style may be exactly the same as formal in its carefully planned and often
elaborate structure. When spoken it is often intoned or elongated rather than carefully pronounced.
However, here and there in the frozen style, certain fixed expressions are required. A judge must
say to a returning jury: ”Ladies and gentlemen of the jury, have you reached a verdict?” or “Ladies
132
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Frozen style is seldom interactive. Usually the speaker is involved in monologue, not
conversation. Some few exceptions to this are always occasions in which each member of the
interaction has a certain (or even fixed) verbal role to play. Meetings which produce such
formulas as point of order, second, call the question, and so on are examples of such interactive
frozen style. The language is frozen because it must not be altered in any way, perhaps acarryover
from the inalterability of religious and / or legal documents.
Ex.1.Judge: Ladies and gentlemen of the jury, have you reached a verdict.
Ex.2.Minister: our father, who art in heaven.......
Ex.3.
Chairperson: Do I hear a motion?
Member 1: I so move the resolution. Chairperson: Is there a second?
Member 2 : I second it.
Chairperson: Any further discussion?
Member 1 : Call the question.
Chairperson : All in favour of the resolution, signify by saying “Aye.”
Member 3 : Nay
Chairperson : The Ayes have it. The resolution passes
The pronunciation at this level is clear and precise. The vocabulary and syntax are varied
and complicated, but there are no formulaic utterances that could be characterized as “flowery,”
“elaborated,’ or “ordinate.” Variation, rather than formulaic repetition, is a characteristic of
formal style.
Formal style is usually single topic oriented. Wondering from one topic to another or
introducing a number of asides, marginally connected remarks, or afterthoughts is not
characteristic of formal style, either in speech or writing. In some cases this singleness of theme is
related to the fact that much formal writing is technical. Most scholarly or technical reports, and
even popular accounts of such subject matter, are prepared in formal style. In the spoken
formal style, the pronunciation is careful and precise.
133
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Ex.2: Chairperson: Ladies and gentlemen, the company cannot ascertain why this has
happened. We’re unable to explain the drop in prices, but we do not feel that the trend will
continue.
Shopping, selling, requesting for information, answering the telephone calls, ordinary
speaking are carried out in consultative style. It is the style most open to the give-and- take of
everyday conversation. Both the formal and frozen styles are likely to occur in monologues, but
the consultative is for conversation.
134
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
the other hand, when the casual is expected and one uses the consultative or even formal, the use of
the inappropriate style seems to suggest that the friendship is strained or that the speaker is angry or
upset with the listener.
The language of the casual is devoid of specialist jargon. There is a great deal of
generalisation and use of pronouns. First names or even nicknames rather than titles and last
namese are used in addressing one another. The pronunciation is rapid, often slurred. Slang and
profanity occur with a great deal of regularity, as do proverbial and folk phrases that are fully
integrated into conversation. Syntactic structures may be considerably reduced.
Linguistically, intimate language, filled as it is with deletion, ellipsis, rapid and slurred
pronunciation, non-verbal communication, and private code characteristics, is often unitelligible
outside the smallest social units.
Style can also refer to a particular person’s use of speech or writing at all times or to a way
of speaking at a particular period of time, e.g. Dicken’s style, the Style of Shakespear, or an 8th
century style of writing. It can be used to refer to those features of a text, and more
especially of a literary text, which identify it as being as the product of an author. We talk, for
example, of the style of Jane Austen as being characteristically different from that of Charlotte
Bronte.
Two main functiona styles are a-informal style and formal style:
a-Informal style or colloquial style serving phatic function in informal situations. This FS is
used in conversations, dialogues, personal letters, diaries..
b-Formal Styles used in formal situations. They include:
i-Belles-letters styles serving aesthetic cognitive function in stories, novels, folklores,
poems, plays.
ii-Publicistic style serving conative, persuasive functions used in news report,
advertisements, annoucement, religion, speeches.
iii-Legal style serving directive functions used in legal documents, diplomatic documents,
business letters,..
iv-Scientific style serving informative functions used in scientific research, lectures,
instructions, prescriptions.
In the English literary standard we distinguish the following major functional styles (FS) [30,
p.33]:
Follow-up activity 2.7.6. True/False: Discuss with your friends and decide whether
the following statements are true or false:
1-Style is a product of individual choices and patterns of choices among linguistic
possibilities.
2-The formal style is the plain, everyday style.
3-The intimate style is the most careful and elegant varieties, reserved for every important
symbolic movements.
4- Style can also refers to a particular person’s use of speech or writing at all times or to a
way of speaking at a particular period of time.
5- A functional style of language is a system of interrelated language means which serves a
definite aim in communication.
8. CODE CHOICE
8.1. Multilingualism [126]
Pre-lecture-activity 2.8.1. Discuss
1-How many languages can you speak?
2-Can you think of the countries in which people can speak two or three languages?
Multilingualism is the act of using, or promoting the use of, multiple languages,
either by an individual speaker or by a community of speakers. Multilingual speakers
outnumber monolingual speakers in the world's population. Multilingualism is becoming a social
phenomenon governed by the needs of globalization and cultural openness. Thanks to the ease of
access to information facilitated by the Internet, individuals' exposure to multiple languages is
getting more and more frequent, and triggering therefore the need to acquire more and more
languages.
than one language, be it actively (through speaking, writing, or signing) or passively (through
listening, reading, or perceiving). More specifically, the terms bilingual and trilingual are used to
describe comparable situations in which two or three languages are involved. A multilingual
person is generally referred to as a polyglot. Poly (Greek: πολύς) means "many", glot (Greek:
γλώττα) means "language".
Multilingual speakers have acquired and maintained at least one language during
childhood, the so-called first language (L1). The first language (sometimes also referred to as the
mother tongue) is acquired without formal education. Children acquiring two languages in this way
are called simultaneous bilinguals. Even in the case of simultaneous bilinguals one language
usually dominates over the other.
A further possibility is that a child may become naturally trilingual by having a mother
and father with separate languages being brought up in a third language environment. An example
of this may be an English-speaking father married to a Mandarin Chinese speaking mother
with the family living in Hong Kong, where the community language (and primary language of
education) is Cantonese. If the child goes to a Cantonese medium school from a young age, then
trilingualism will result.
Even if someone is highly proficient in two or more languages, his or her so-called
communicative competence or ability may not be as balanced. Linguists have distinguished
various types of multilingual competence, which can roughly be put into two categories:
For compound bilinguals, words and phrases in different languages are the same concepts.
These speakers are usually fluent in both languages.
For coordinate bilinguals, words and phrases in the speaker's mind are all related to their
own unique concepts. In these individuals, one language, usually the first language, is more
dominant than the other, and the first language may be used to think through the second language.
A sub-group of the latter is the subordinate bilingual, which is typical of beginning second
language learners.
The distinction between compound and coordinate bilingualism has come under scrutiny.
When studies are done of multilinguals, most are found to show behavior intermediate between
compound and coordinate bilingualism. Some authors have suggested that the distinction should
only be made at the level of grammar rather than vocabulary, others use "coordinate bilingual"
as a synonym for one who has learned two languages from birth, and others have proposed
dropping the distinction altogether.
not all speakers need to be multilingual. When all speakers are multilingual, linguists classify the
community according to the functional distribution of the languages involved:
Bipart-lingualism: if more than one language can be heard in a small area, but the large
majority of speakers are monolinguals, who have little contact with speakers from neighbouring
ethnic groups, an area is called 'bipart-lingual'. An example of this is the Balkans.
Some multilinguals use code-switching, a term that describes the process of 'swapping'
between languages. In many cases, code-switching is motivated by the wish to express loyalty to
more than one cultural group, as holds for many immigrant communities in the New World. Code-
switching may also function as a strategy where proficiency is lacking.
Bilingual interaction can even take place without the speakers switching. In certain areas, it
is not uncommon for speakers each to use a different language within the same conversation.
This phenomenon is found, amongst other places, in Scandinavia. Most speakers of Swedish and
Norwegian, and Norwegian and Danish, can communicate with each other speaking their
respective languages, while few can speak both (people used to these situations often adjust their
language, avoiding words that are not found in the other language or that can be misunderstood).
The causes of multilingualism may be politics, religion, culture, education, economy and
natural disaster.
Follow-up activity 2.8.1. True/False: Discuss with your friends and decide whether
the following statements are true or false:
1-Multilingualism is the act of using or promoting the use of, multiple languages, either by
139
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
2-If someone is highly proficient in two or more languages, his or her so-called
communicative competence or ability is always balanced.
3-For co-ordinate bi-linguals, words and phrases in the speaker’s mind are all related to
their own unique concepts. In these individuals, one language, usually the first language, is more
dominant than the other.
1-Why do people change from one language to another, one variety of language to
another, or one style of speaking to another?
8.2.1. Code
According to Richards, Platt and Weber [82, p.42) and Saville-Troike [86, p.48], code is a
term which is used instead of a language, a speech variety or even a dialect.
8.2.2. Code-switching, style-switching and code-mixing
8.2.2.1. Code-swiching
a-What is code-switching?
In linguistics, code-switching occurs when a speaker alternates between two or more
languages or language varieties, in the context of a single conversation. The term “code- switching
refers to change in languages (or language varieties) within a single speech event” [86, p.48].
Speakers practice code-switching when they are each fluent in two languages or more than two
languages. Both in popular usage and in sociolinguistic study, the name code-switching is
sometimes used to refer to switching among dialects, styles or registers. Bilinguals often switch
between their two languages in the middle of their conversation. These code switches can take
place between or even within sentences, involving phrases or words or even parts of words. Here is
one example from Saville-Troike [86, p.48):
within the same speech event, because she had identified the caller as a Spanish speaker.
b-Types of code-switching
Scholars use different names for various types of code-switching.
i-Intersentential switching [147]
Intersentential switching occurs outside the sentence or the clause level (i.e. at sentence or
clause boundaries). It is sometimes called "extrasentential" switching. In Assyrian- English
switching one could say, "Ani wideili. What happened?" ("Those, I did them. What happened?").
ii-Intra-sentential switching [147]
Intra-sentential switching occurs within a sentence or a clause. In Spanish-English
switching one could say, "La onda is to fight y jambar." ("The in-thing is to fight and steal.").
iii-Tag-switching [147]
Tag-switching is the switching of either a tag phrase or a word, or both, from one
language to another, (common in intra-sentential switches). In Spanish-English switching one could
say, "Él es de México y así los criaron a ellos, you know." ("He's from Mexico, and they raise them
like that, you know.").
iv-Intra-word switching [147]
Intra-word switching occurs within a word itself, such as at a morpheme boundary. In
Shona-English switching one could say, "But ma-day-s a-no a-ya ha-ndi-si ku-mu-on-a. ("But
these days I don't see him much.") Here the English plural morpheme -s appears alongside the
Shona prefix ma-, which also marks plurality.
v-Situational code-switching
Situational code-switching occurs when the language used change according to the
situations in which the conversants find themselves: they speak one language in one situation and
another in a different one [107, p. 104].
Instances of situational code-switching are usually fairly easy to classify for what they are.
What we observe is that one variety is used in a certain set of situations and another in an entirely
different set.
In a multilingual country like Singapore, the ability to shift from one language to another is
accepted as quite normal. Singapore has four official languages: English, the Mandarin variety of
Chinese, Tamil, and Malay, which is also the national language. National policy promotes English
as a trade language, Manderin as the international ‘Chinese’ language, Malay as the language of the
region, and Tamil as the language of the important ethnic groups in the republic. In Singapore,
people change languages according to the situations.
141
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
vi-Metaphorical code-switching
When a change of topic requires a change in the language used we have metaphorical code-
switching [107, p. 104].
According to Saville-Troike [86, p.49] metaphorical code-switching occurs within a single
situation, but adds meaning to such components as the role-relationships which are being
expressed. When a change of topic requires a change in the language used we have metaphorical
code-switching. Metaphorical code-switching has an affective dimension to it. You change the code
as you redefine the situation: formal to informal, official to personal, serious to humourous, and
politeness to solidarity. Since speaking different languages is an obvious marker of differential
group membership, by switching languages bilinguals often have the option of choosing which
group to identify with in a particular situation, and thus can convey the metaphorical meaning
which goes along with such choice as well as whatever denotative meaning is conveyed by the code
itself.
8.2.2.2. Style-shifting
Style-shifting is a term in sociolinguistics referring to alternation between styles of
speech included in a linguistic repertoire of an individual speaker.
8.2.2.3. Code-mixing
Code-mixing refers to the mixing of two or more languages or language varieties in speech.
Some scholars use the terms "code-mixing" and "code-switching" interchangeably, especially in
studies of syntax, morphology, and other formal aspects of language. Others assume more specific
definitions of code-mixing, but these specific definitions may be different in different subfields of
linguistics, education theory, communications etc.
Code-mixing is similar to the use or creation of pidgins; but while a pidgin is created
142
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
across groups that do not share a common language, code-mixing may occur within a multilingual
setting where speakers share more than one language.
Follow-up activity 2.8.2. True/False: Discuss with your friends and decide whether the
following statements are true or false:
5-Intersentential switching is the change in language which occurs within a single sentence.
143
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
The origin of the word pidgin is uncertain. Pidgin first appeared in print in 1850 and
there are many sources to which the word may be attributed. For example [45, pp.85-86):
Pidgin has variously called “makeshift”, “marginal”, or “mixed language”. The creation of
a pidgin usually requires [127]
a-Prolonged, regular contact between the different language communities.
b-A need to communicate between them
c-An absence of widespread proficiency in a widespread, accessible interlanguage.
Pidgin languages are created from the combined efforts of people who speak different
languages. All languages involved may contribute to the sounds, the vocabulary and the grammatical
features, but to different extents, and some additional features may emerge which are unique to the
new variety. Nevertheless, it has been found that when one group speaks a prestigious world
144
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
language and the other groups use local vernaculars, the prestige language tends to supply more of
the vocabulary, while vernacular languages have more influence on the grammar of the
developing pidgin. The language which supplies most of the vocabulary is known as the lexifier (or
sometimes superstrate) language, while the languages which influence the grammatical structure are
called the substrate.
Initially, pidgins develop with a narrow range of functions. Those who use them speak other
languages, so the pidgin is an addition to their linguistic repertoire used for a specific purpose,
such as trade or perhaps administration. They are typically used for quite specific functions like
buying and selling grain, or animal hides, rather than to signal social distinctions or express
politeness.
Because pidgins develop to serve a very narrow range of functions in a very restricted set of
domains, they tend to have a simplified structure and a small vocabulary compared with fully
developed languages. Consequently, the structure of a pidgin is generally no more complicated than
it needs to be to express these functions.
Pidgin languages are created from the combined efforts of people who speak different
languages. All languages involved may contribute to the sounds, the vocabulary and the
grammatical features, but to different extents, and some additional features may emerge which are
unique to the new variety. Nevertheless, it has been found that when one group speaks a prestigious
world language and the other groups use local vernaculars, the prestige language tends to supply
more of the vocabulary, while vernacular languages have more influence on the grammar of the
developing pidgin.
Because pidgins develop to serve a very narrow range of functions in a very restricted set of
domains, they tend to have a simplified structure and a small vocabulary compared with fully
developed languages.
Since a pidgin language is a fundamentally simpler form of communication, the grammar
and phonology are usually as simple as possible, and usually consist of [127]:
145
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Example 3. Ok, you take cyclo. One you, one dollar, two you, two dollar. If you OK you sit
down, if you no OK you go [111].
Pidgins have a limited vocabulary, a reduced grammatical structure, and a much narrower
range of functions. They are the native language of no one, but they are nonetheless a main means
of communication for millions of people. Most pidgins are based on European languages: English,
French, Spanish, Dutch and Portuguese, reflecting the history of colonialism.
Pidgins may start out as or become trade languages, such as Tok Pisin. Trade languages are
often full blown languages in their own right such as Swahili. Trade languages tend to be "vehicular
languages", while pidgins can evolve into the vernacular.
Some pidgins have become so powerful as a means of communication that they have
developed a more formal role, as regular auxiliary languages. They maybe given official status by
a community as lingua francas. These are known as “expanded pidgins”
As with other lingua francas, the uses and occasions for using a pidgin will partly depend on
how widely it is known. This will determine the degree of expansion of the language. Pidgins can
be classified into different types according to their phase of development: jargon, stable pidgin,
extended pidgin, and creole. Each stage is characterised by a gradual increase in overall
complexities
Acoording to Holmes [45, p.89), a pidgin language has three identifying characteristics:
146
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
As a result of their status as some group’s first language, creoles also differ from pidgins in
their range of functions, in their structure and in some cases in the attitudes expressed towards them.
A creole is a pidgin which has expanded in structure and vocabulary to express the range of
meanings and serve the range of functions required of a first language [45, p.90].
Once a creole has developed it can be used for all the functions of any language – politics,
education, administration, original literature, and so on. Creoles have become accepted standard
and even national and official languages. Once developed there is no evidence in their linguistic
structure to reveal their pidgin origins [45, p.93].
Table 2.32. Example of creoles [20, p.336]
French Guyanese Creole Krio English
Mangez Māʒe Chop Eat
J’ai mange Mo māʒe A chop I ate
Il/Elle a mange Li māʒe I chop He/She ate
McWhorter (1998), [as cited in [153] has proposed the following list of features to indicate a
creole prototype:
a-a lack of inflectional morphology (other than at most two or three inflectional affixes),
147
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
8.3.4. Distribution
Pidgin and creole languages are distributed mainly in the equatorial belt around the
world, usually in places with direct or easy access to the oceans. They are found mainly in the
Caribean and around the north and east coasts of South America, around the coasts of Africa,
particularly the west coast, and across the Indian and Pacific Oceans.
The names given to pidgin and creole languages by linguists refer to their location and their
principal lexifier language (i.e. the language from which they draw most of their vocabulary). Thus,
Jamaican Creole English refers to the creole spoken in Jamaica, which is English-based or draws
most of its vocabulary from English. Haitian Creole French is the name given to the creole language
spoken in Haity.
8.3.5. Where do pidgins and creoles come from?
There are a variety of theories on the origin of creole languages, all of which attempt to
explain the similarities among them. Arends, Muysken & Smith (1995) [as cited in [153] outline a
fourfold classification of explanations regarding creole genesis:
148
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
b-https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Creole_language
Follow-up activity 2.8.3. True/False: Discuss with your friends and decide whether
the following statements are true or false:
2- The language from which the pitgin is derived are simplified, and vocabulary and
grammatical complexities have been reduced.
3- Pidgins can be classified into different types according to their phase of development:
jargon, stable pidgin, extended pidgin, and creole.
5- A creole is used in restricted domains and functions and has a simplified structure
compared to the source language.
CHAPTER II REVIEW 2
I-Answer the following questions
1-What are the stages of child language development in the first year after birth?
2-What are the three main functions of adolescent language?
3-What are the main features of adult language?
4-What is a taboo word? How do we avoid using taboo words in everyday communication?
5-What is formal swearing? informal swearing?
6-What is euphemism? Give some examples.
7-What is slang? How are slang expressions formed?
149
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
150
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
15-In bilingual or multilingual communities, people may change from one language to
another in the course of conversation. This phenomenon is called language mixing, language
switching or simply “code-switching”.
16-The term code-switching refer to change in languages within a single speech event.
17-Situational code-switching occurs when one changes from formal to informal language
within a single event.
18-Intersentential switching is the change which occurs between sentences or speech acts.
19-A pidgin is a simplified language that develops as a means of communication between
two or more groups that do not have a language in common.
20-A creole has a limited vocabulary, a narrow range of functions and is only used in trading.
151
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
152
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
about whom, whether formally or informally, why, when and where). Language varies
according to the change in social factors or in other words, according to the social context in
which it occurs. The basic assumption here is that language users adapt the properties of their
language use (such as intonation, lexical choice, syntax, and other aspects of formulation) to the
current communicative situation.
The notion of context was invoked by Malinowski to account for the way language was
used among the Trobriand islanders in the Western Pacific [105]. Firth [1957, as cited in [105]
takes up the notion of context of situation and turns it into a key concept in his linguistic theory
by giving it a more abstract character, and, more significantly, by incorporating language within
it.
According to Firth [1957a, as cited in [105], “context of situation” is best used as a
suitable schematic construct to apply to language events. He suggests the following categories
to relate context of situation to language events:
a-The relevant features of participants: persons, personalities.
i-The verbal action of the participants.
ii-The non-verbal action of the participants.
b-The relevant objects.
c-The effect of the verbal action.
A similar approach is found in Hymes (1964, 1974) where further details of context are
specified. Hymes lists the components of speech as follows [as cited in [87, pp.110-111]: a-the
genre, b-the topic, c-the purpose or function, d-the setting, e-the key, f-the participants, g- the
message form, h-the message content, i-the act sequence, j-the rules for interaction, k-the norms
of interpretation.
Pervin [1978), as cited in [105] summarizes the structure of situations in terms of the
following categories: a-place, b-time, c-people, and d-activities.
Argyle [1978, as cited in [105], proposes a more complex list of situation categories: a-
elements of behaviour, b-goals or motivations, c-rules of behaviour, d- social or formal roles, e-
physical setting and equipment, f-salient cognitive concepts, and g- relevant skills.
Probably the most detailed contribution to the study of the structure of social situations
in social psychology has been provided in an influential paper by Brown and Fraser [1979, as
cited in [105, p.38]. Their schema is inspired by the situational factors that explain
sociolinguistic variation, and they provide not only a theoretical sketch, but also a selective
review of the sociolinguistic literature in which such factors have been observed. Their schema
of situation structure combines a major Scene category with that of Participants, where a
Setting (locale, time, bystanders) is part of the Scene, more or less as in other approaches. They
see Purpose as the “motor” of social situations, but place it under Scene, and define it in terms
of activity type (goals, activated roles) and subject matter (task, topic).
153
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Figure 3.1. Components of a situation [Brown and Fraser, 1979, as cited in [105, p.38].
Saville-Troike [86, pp.42-43] states that language choice depends on such social factors
as domain, topic, setting and function.
According to Samovar, Porter, Mcdaniel and Roy [84, p.311], there are three important
communication variables found in every communication setting. These are general rules that
apply across all cultures and all social settings. In every social context, you will find culture-
based communication rules that apply to a-the appropriate degree of formality and informality,
b- the influence of assertiveness and interpersonal harmony, and c-the influence of power
distance relationship. Each variable plays an important role in how people respond to their
interpersonal and organizational environment.
According to Holmes [45, pp.8-9], in any situation, linguistic choices generally indicate
people’s awareness of the influence of one or more of the following components:
a-The participants
i-who is speaking, and
ii-who are they speaking to?
b-The setting or social context of the interaction: where are they
speaking?
c-The topic: what is being talked about?
d-The function: Why are they speaking?
154
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
155
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
codes in different situations, and their use of Spanish and Guaraní fell into a pattern for different
domains. This was useful though it still leaves considerable areas of language use unspecified.
Table 3.1. Domains of language use in Paraguay [45, p.23]
Domain Addressee Setting Topic Language
Family Parent Home Planning a Guarani
family party
Friendship Friend Cafe Funny anecdote Guarani
Religion Priest Church Choosing the Spanish
Sunday liturgy
Education Teacher Primary school Telling story Guarani
Education Lecturer University Solving a maths Spanish
problem
Administration Official Office Getting an Spanish
import licence
Domain is clearly a very general concept which draws on three important social factors in
code choice – participants, setting and topic. It is useful for capturing broad generalisations about
any speech community. Using information about the domains of use in a community, it is
possible to draw a very simple model summarising the norms of language use for the
community. This is often particularly useful for bilingual and multilingual speech communities.
1.1.2.2. Topic
Topic, as discussed in this part, refers to discourse topic: the subject matter of a speech,
text, meeting, discourse, etc. It is what is talked or written about. Discourse topic categorizes,
reduces, summarizes, and organizes the generic structure and the semantic information of
discourse.
Topics, topic categories, topic nature (safe, easy, taboo, familiar…) will influence the
language choice such as spoken or written discourse, the generic structure of discourse, styles of
language, speech act choice, register, discourse makers and many others.
Once a topic is chosen, the speaker or writer has to speak or write topically. S/he thinks
first of the generic structure of discourse. Then, there is the problem of topicality. Every clause,
sentence, paragraph, discourse is organised around an element that is taken as its point of
departure: the discourse topic. The topic of a discourse captures “what a document is about”,
i.e., the meaning of the text. A discourse can be represented by a “bag of words” for several
purposes. This is the issue of lexical cohesion and lexical/ conceptual field of discourse. A topic
consists of a cluster of words that frequently occur together. Examples are:
a-Arts: architecture, fine art, dancing, fashion, film, museum, music, …
b-Business: advertising, e-commerce, capital, finance, investment, …
156
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
In diglossic communities, choice of when H will be used and when L will be used
depends on the topic chosen.
In addition to topic, appropriate language choice may depend on setting (including locale
and time of day), purposes and participants.
1.1.2.3. Function
The term function refers to the purpose for which an utterance or a unit of language is
used [82, p.113]. In linguistics, language functions are described in the speech act theory: a
speech act is an utterance as a functional unit in communication. Depending on the context of
communication, one utterance has a certain function in communication. The purpose of using
the utterance, together with other aspects of social context, will decide which kind of speech act
the speaker is performing. The speech act realization differs from culture to culture.
In a spoken discourse, the structure of a spoken discourse is normally organized around
the organization of the speech acts relevant to the discourse topic. For example, when you hear:
Good morning, Erm... I’m wondering what the Vietnam Airlines flights are from Hanoi to
HoChiMinh city every day? You know that the discourse topic is buying airline tickets (fare).
The topic framework is: Location (a Vietnam airline ticket office in Hanoi), participants: (sale
and and English-speaking customer), time (in the morning), language: English, discourse type
(transaction discourse), lexical field: (flight, time of depature, air part, price…). The speech acts
will be about buying the airline ticket at an Vietname airline office in Hanoi for a morning
flight from Hanoi to HoChiMinh City.
157
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Functions can be the purpose for which a language unit is used. It will decide the generic
/ the overall structure and the functional style of a discourse. If we view genres as social
processes, then the functions of these social processes can be to describe, to explain, to instruct,
to argue, to narrate. These social processes with their functions will result in different types of
genre and their generic structures: personal descriptions, accounts, instructions, essays, narrative.
For example, to narrate a story, you will make use of the generic structure of narrative which has
the following structure: abstract, orientation, complicating event, resolution and coda.
Functions or purposes can decide the functional styles of the discourse or text.
1.1.2.4. Setting [105]
a-Setting: time, location, circumstances and situation
We shall use the term setting here in two senses: that of situation and that of time and
place / space. Thus, situations include a family breakfast, a faculty meeting, a party,
Thanksgiving dinner, a lecture, a date. Social situations may be restricted by cultural norms
which specify the appropriate participants, the physical setting, the topics, the functions of
discourse, and the style.
b- Location: Types of places
Of the vast number of possible places, we shall, therefore, only consider three basic
types: i-personal and inter-personal places/spaces, ii-social places/spaces and iii-geographical
places/ spaces.
Personal and interpersonal place and space define where I am now, as well as my
interlocutors, that is, a space that organizes our interaction, perspective and discourse in direct,
face-to-face interaction. Preferred personal and interpersonal space may vary in size between
cultures.
Social places locate our joint and collective activities in everyday life, at home, at work,
and during times of leisure, and hence tend to be defined in terms of what people do in such
places. Such places may be variously categorized, as follows: i-outdoor places and spaces
(streets, squares, parks, lakes, rivers and beaches), ii-residential places (houses, apartment
buildings, homes), iii-commercial places (shops, department stores, shopping malls), iv-
commercial service places (hotels and restaurants), v-community service places (city halls,
police stations and post offices, government agencies; hospitals, stations and airports), vi-
educational places (schools and universities), vii-leisure places (movie houses, theatres and
gyms), viii-workplaces (offices, workshops and factories)
Social places are also defined by the social categories of the people who work there (the
professionals), those who visit such places (the users or clients), as well as the typical
transactions they engage in. And conversely, social places thus seem to be involved in the
assignment of social identities to members, especially to the professionals who work there.
A third kind of place may be called geographical, but actually embodies and combines
social, political and cultural dimensions. They may be represented by their scope, range, size or
level, and are progressively inclusive, for instance as follows: i-Home, ii-Street / Neighborhood,
iii-City, iv-State, Province, v-Country, vi-Region / Continent, vii-World.
158
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
As part of the representation of place in context models, these categories not only tell us
where we (and others) are, and what happens where, but also provide a sociocultural dimension
and identity, which influences language choice in communication.
So far we have conceptualized the three types of places as wholes. Many of them,
however, are structured, organized, ordered in many ways, and such structures are important for
interaction. Teachers traditionally stand in front of a classroom so that they can see and control
the students, and a judge usually sits in front of a courtroom, and on a higher bench, both to
symbolize his / her power.
b-Time
There are many forms of discourse and communicative events that are organized by time,
with fixed beginnings and endings, as is the case for most meetings, broadcast programs,
lectures, parliamentary debates, doctors’ visits, and so on. This is the case for most institutional
discourse, but also in informal everyday conversations. People do not have limitless time, and
organize part of their discourse, and especially ending it, by negotiating termination with
reference to time. Communicative events may take place between fixed time points, or have a
maximum duration.
All this shows that participants in many communicative events, at least in many modern
cultures, must be aware of time, and keep monitoring it – starting with the obligatory presence at
a certain place at a certain time (that is, being “on time” for work, a class, a meeting, a train ride
or a concert).
We have seen that settings are a fundamental aspect of the structure of situations in
general, and of contexts in particular. Joos [1967, as cited [79, p.24] has given a classification of
five major setting varieties in his own cultural system on the basis of the degrees of formality of
the settings; these he defines by style types as intimate, casual, consultative, formal, and frozen.
1.1.2.5. Participants
a-participants’ social attributes
In any act of communication, there is a sender and a receiver who together may be
called interlocutors. In addition, there may be present an audience which is not the primary
addressee of the message. These are the participants in a communicative event.
For most sociolinguistic analyses the important features of participants will be
sociological attributes. Participants’ social identities involves many types of identity and roles,
such as [105]:
i-Categories: gender (women, men, etc.), age (young, old, etc.), appearance (black,
white, etc.), etc.
ii-Ethnicity: European, Asian-American, etc.
iii-Nationality: Dutch, Spanish, etc.
iv-Profession/Occupation: baker, dentist, journalist, etc.
v-Social positions: boss, employee, etc.
159
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
160
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
older than, parent of, richer than, stronger than, and noble than are all asymmetrical power
relations.
Solidarity is a relationship which is based on similarity or even sameness of salient
characteristics in two (or more) persons [Thomas, 1998, as cited in [10]. Such relationships
are reciprocal, i.e. they obtain equally for both individuals. The relationship is symmetrical.
Attending the same school or having the same parents, or practicing the same profession are
a set of relations which are symmetrical.
Solidarity concerns the social distance between the characteristics they shared [Hudson,
1960, as cited in [10]: How much experience they have shared, how many social characteristics
they share (religion, sex, region of origin, race, occupation, interest, etc.). Solidarity forms
express intimacy and familiarity [Polly Sterling, 2000, as cited in [10].
Power and solidarity are two types of social relationships which influence the language
choice in communication. In communication, people often vary their language according to the
communicative contexts to achieve their communicative purposes. One of the most important
function of language variation is to enable people to identify with a social group (to show
solidarity with a social group / to be in solidarity with the group) or to separate themselves from
the group (to convey the idea that there is a difference in social distance between the
interlocutors). In the first case, the speaker means “I am one of you”. In the second case, the
speaker means “I am different from you”. The two notions Power Relation and Solidarity
Relation were introduced by Brown and Gillman [10]. Solidarity relation is the relation of
social equality, of social closeness, of being friends. The language used to show solidarity
tends to be informal. It means: ”I am one of you”. Power relation is the relation of difference
in social distance. This relation is characterised by power factor (political, economic or social).
One interlocutor has higher power (higher in status) whereas the other is lower in power (lower
in status). There is a difference in social distance between the interlocutors: higher status-lower
status, higher power-lower power, old-young, rich-poor, employer-employee. The difference in
social distance between them will result in the use of different verbal–nonverbal language
varieties.
Power governs asymmetrical relationships where one is subordinate to another;
solidarity governs symmetrical relationships characterized by social equality and
similarity. We can display power-solidarity relationship as follows:
Power Solidarity
Asymmetry Symmetry
Hierarchy Equality
Distance Closeness
________________________________________
Linguistic signals of power and solidarity can be words, phrases, pronunciation, or the
whole language.
One example of linguistic signals of power and solidarity is T-V usage. In
sociolinguistics, a T–V distinction (from the Latin pronouns tu and vos) is a contrast, within one
161
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
language, between various forms of addressing one's conversation partner or partners [195].
Brown and Gilman argued that the choice of forms of address is governed by either relationships
of power and / or solidarity, depending on the culture of the speakers. Brown & Gilman (1960)
defined social relationships in terms of POWER and SOLIDARITY. Forms which indicate
POWER establish who has authority and how great that authority is. Forms which indicate
SOLIDARITY establish the degree of intimacy in the relationship. T is the familiar form and V
is the polite form. T and V forms are used to control social interactions by indicating the degree
of power and solidarity. Non-reciprocal T/V usage indicates power relationship. Reciprocal T or
V usage indicates solidarity relationships, reciprocal V usage: “politeness” and reciprocal T
usage: “intimacy”.
1.1.2.6. Society as social context
Society, among other things, provides the social contexts and social situations for
language use. Social situations are restricted by cultural norms which specify the appropriate
participants, the physical setting, the topics, the functions of discourse, and the style.
In addition, society controls speech [49, p.119]:
a-by providing a set of norms, which we learn to follow.
b-by providing the motivation for adhering to these norms, for putting efforts into speech.
c-by providing a set of concepts for thinking and talking about.
162
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Australian anthropologist Roger Keesing [73, p.46] argues that culture provides people
with an implicit theory about how to behave and how to interpret the behavior of others. People
from different cultures learn different implicit theories. These theories are learned through
socialization. And through socialization, individuals also learn the dominant values of their
particular culture and their self-identities.
163
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
cultural values and the individual values they hold. Cultural values provide broad guidelines
about what are acceptable means for achieving end states in different situations. Individual
values provide specific guidelines for behavior across situations. Feather (1995) demonstrated
that the values individuals hold are linked to the valences they attach to different behaviors.
When individuals are socialized, they learn various patterns of interaction that are based
on the norms, rules, and values of their culture. These patterns of interaction form the basis for
individuals' communication styles.
According to Gudykunst [38] cultural individualism-collectivism influences
communication in a culture through the cultural norms and rules associated with the major
cultural tendency (e.g. the United States tends to have individualistic norms / rules, Asian
cultures tend to have collectivistic norms / rules). In addition to cultural norms / rules,
individualism-collectivism also influences the ways individuals are socialized in their cultures.
Individuals in a culture generally are socialized in ways consistent with the cultural-level
tendencies. Cultural individualism-collectivism, therefore, indirectly influences communication
through characteristics individuals learn when they are socialized. At the least three individual
characteristics mediate the influence of cultural individualism-collectivism on individuals’
communication: their personalities, their individual values, and their self-construals. These
individual-level characteristics are related to cultural-level individualism-collectivism and can be
used to explain variability in communication within cultures.
Thus, the culture in which individuals are raised influences the way individuals are
socialized in terms of individualistic and collectivistic tendencies. Cultural individualism-
collectivism (I-C) has a direct effect on communication because it affects the norms and rules
that guide behavior in individualistic and collectivistic cultures. The individualistic or
collectivistic tendencies that individuals learn when being socialized into their cultures in turn
also influence individual level factors such as the way individuals conceive of themselves and
the values individuals hold. Cultural I-C, therefore, has both a direct effect on communication
behavior and an indirect effect on communication behavior that is mediated through individual-
level factors such as self construals and values. The theoretical position outlined is summarized
in Figure 3.2.
164
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
According to Merkin [66] individuals know people mostly from their own cultural
network. Consequently, identifying and understanding underlying cultural values is essential for
successful intercultural interactions. Studies have examined how different cultural values
determine the way people communicate to accomplish goals while also maintaining their
dignified, self-confident manner. Considerations occur because it is difficult to decipher how to
communicate effectively with people from other cultures; particularly when one is balancing
attaining goals and maintaining composure with people who see things differently. The different
perspectives people from dissimilar cultures hold are referred to as cultural norms. Cultural
norms condition people to relate to others in patterned ways. One classic example is how initial
business meetings in places like the US get straight to the point. In contrast, in Japan, when
business partners first meet, much time is spent socializing initially before plunging into the
substance of a business deal.
These cultural patterns reflect fundamental values that underlie communication. Hence,
the values behind getting straight to work reflect the individualistic value of consulting the
other (as a courtesy). In contrast, the value sharing contexts before signing a contract, reflects
the collectivistic notion that face must be attended to first. Establishing harmony gives face
which founds good business relationships. Thus, collectivists need harmonious
communication.
The following part will look at Hall (1976, 1983)’s idea of low-context cultures and high-
context cultures and Hofstede’s dimensions of national cultures (2010, 2011) as the cultural
frameworks which guide and influence people’s behaviour and communication.
1.2.2. The frameworks of Low-context cultures (LCC) and High-context cultures (HCC) and
communication styles.
Communication styles are behaviors that consistently occur in the “way one verbally,
nonverbally, and paraverbally interacts to signal how literal meaning should be taken,
interpreted, filtered, or understood” [Norton, 1983, as cited in [75].
Hall [1976, 1983, as cited in [81, pp.134-138] distinguished between cultures according
to how communication takes place: low / high context cultures. Ting-Toomy [99, p.101] presents
the Low-Context Communication (LCC) and High-Context Communication (HCC)
characteristics as follows:
Ting-Toomey [99, pp.100-101] states that by low-context communication we emphasize
how intention or meaning is best expressed through explicit verbal messages. By high-context
communication we emphasize how intention or meaning can best be conveyed through the
context (e.g. social roles or position) and the nonverbal channels (e.g. pauses, silence, tone of
voice) of the message.
In general, low-context communication refers to communication patterns of direct verbal
mode: straight talk, nonverbal immediacy, and sender-oriented values (i.e. the sender assumes
the responsibility to communicate clearly). In low-context communication, the speaker is
expected to be responsible for constructing a clear, persuasive message that the listener can
decode easily.
165
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
166
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Table 3.4. Major differences between Individualistic and Collectivistic cultures [99, p.67;
42; 41, p.113]
Individualistic cultures Collectivistic cultures
“I” identity “We” identity
Individual goals Group goals
Interindividual emphasis Ingroup emphasis
Voluntary reciprocity Obligatory reciprocity
Low-context communication High-context communication
Management of individuals Management of groups
Small-power distance prevails Large-power distance prevails
Symmetrical interaction Asymmetrical interaction
Examples: United States, Austrlia, United Guatemala, Indonesia, Pakistan, China,
Kindom, Canada, Netherlands, New Zealand, Japan, Vietnam
France, Germany
Ting-Toomey [99, p.67] maintains that basically, individualism refers to the broad value
tendencies of a culture in emphasizing the importance of individual identity over group identity,
individual rights over group rights, and individual needs over group needs. Individualism
promotes self-efficiency, individual responsibilities, and personal autonomy. Collectivism
refers to the broad value tendencies of a culture in emphasizing the importance of the ‘we’
identity over the ‘I’ identity, group rights over individual rights, and in-group-oriented needs
over individual wants and desires. Collectivism promotes relational interdependence, in-
group harmony, and in-group collaborative spirit. Individualistic and collectivistic value
tendencies are manifested in everyday family, school, and workplace interaction.
In collectivist cultures, high-context communication prevails. In individualistic cultures,
low-context communication prevails. Individualism is also related to small-power distance
whereas collectivism is related to a large-power distance.
Cultural individualism-collectivism influences communication in a culture through the
cultural norms and rules associated with the major cultural tendency (e.g. the United States tends
to have individualistic norms / rules, Asian cultures tend to have collectivistic norms / rules). In
addition to cultural norms / rules, individualism-collectivism also influences the ways individuals
are socialized in their cultures. Individuals in a culture generally are socialized in ways
consistent with the cultural-level tendencies. Cultural individualism-collectivism, therefore,
indirectly influences communication through characteristics individuals learn when they are
socialized. At the least three individual characteristics mediate the influence of cultural
individualism-collectivism on individuals’ communication: their personalities, their
individual values, and their self-construals. These individual-level characteristics are related
to cultural-level individualism-collectivism and can be used to explain variability in
communication within cultures. Figure 3.3. schematically illustrates how the influence of
cultural individualism-collectivism on communication is mediated by individual-level factors
that mediate their effects.
167
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Cultural I-C influences the major cultural values individuals learn and the ways members
of cultures acquire conceptions of themselves (Gudykunst et al, 1996). Cultural I-C has a direct
influence on behavior (e.g., through norms/rules used to guide behavior), but it also influences
behavior indirectly through the values and self-construals that individual members learn
when being socialized into the culture. To understand individual behavior, both cultural-level I-C
and individual-level factors that mediate the influence of cultural I-C must be taken into
consideration.
Individuals learn their values through the socialization process. The values that are
predominant in the culture influence the value s that individuals learn, but individual
value structures are different from cultural value structures. Individuals' behavior is affected by
cultural values and the individual values they hold. Cultural values provide broad guidelines
about what are acceptable means for achieving end states in different situations. Individual
values provide specific guidelines for behavior across situations. Feather (1995) demonstrated
that the values individuals hold are linked to the valences they attach to different behaviors.
When individuals are socialized, they learn various patterns of interaction that are based
on the norms, rules, and values of their culture. These patterns of interaction form the basis for
individuals' communication styles.
According to Ting-Toomey [99, p.68], the top individualist values are freedom, honesty,
social recognition, comfort, hedonism, and personal equity. The top collectivist values are
harmony, face-saving, fifial piety (respect and conformity of parents’ wishes), equality in the
distribution of rewards among peers (for the sake of group harmony), and fulfillment of
other’s needs.
168
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
According to Merkin [66] a large body of research highlights how individualists prefer
direct communication while collectivists prefer indirect communication, particularly in face-
threatening situations.
Direct communication can be defined as messages that have the meaning included within
them, including logical viewpoints, direct declarations, and expressiveness. Direct speech is
efficient because it is concise, evident, and unequivocal. However, it commits the speaker to
what is said which could easily cause people to lose face. So why would individualists prefer
direct communication? Individualists favor direct communication partly because they can assert
control over their relationships. Direct messages employ low-context communication –
communication where the meaning is explicitly stated. In fact, findings with regard to facework
– behavioral actions enacted to protect one’s face – indicate that individualism is responsible for
more dominating and less other-oriented facework than collectivism. Direct communication
strategies are risky and could destroy relationships if not purposefully considered. However,
individualists’ need to manage relationships is so strong that they are willing to risk taking
responsibility for their direct assertions.
In cultures with a high collectivism maintenance of the harmony in the group is highly
important. In those cultures one seldom argues with people and one avoids that a member of the
group will loose face. Communication is very implicit because all members of the group know
each other well and they don't need words to convey a message.
In collectivistic cultures, the self is interdependent, which means that people feel linked
to others in their primary group. Accordingly, collectivists must maintain connection and adjust
their comportment to conform and be accepted by others; otherwise, they risk losing face.
169
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Consequently, they prefer to use indirect communication. The distinctive feature of indirect
communication is that the meaning is outside the message. Indirect communication styles
consist of influencing through face work or third parties or using ambiguity. However, the
ambiguities of indirect speech can be a source of misunderstanding and conflict in relationships
which could also lead to a loss of face.
170
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
171
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
People in small power distance cultures tend to value equal power distributions, equal
rights and relations, and equitable rewards and punishments based on performance. People in
large power distance cultures tend to accept unequal power distribution, hierarchical rights,
asymmetrical role relations, and rewards and punishments based on age, rank, status, title and
seniority. For small power cultures, equality of personal rights represents an ideal to strive
toward in a system. For large power cultures, respect for power hierarchy in any system is a
fundamental way of life.
i-Power distance difference among countries: Roots in the Family [41]
Most people in the world are born into a family. All people started acquiring their mental
software immediately after birth, from the elders in whose presence they grew up, modeling
themselves after the examples set by these elders.
In the large-power-distance situation, children are expected to be obedient toward their
parents. Sometimes there is even an order of authority among the children themselves, with
younger children being expected to yield to older children. Independent behavior on the part of a
child is not encouraged. Respect for parents and other elders is considered a basic virtue;
children see others showing such respect and soon acquire it themselves. There is often
considerable warmth and care in the way parents and older children treat younger ones,
especially those who are very young. They are looked after and are not expected to experiment
for themselves. Respect for parents and older relatives lasts through adulthood: parental authority
continues to play a role in a person’s life as long as the parents are alive. Parents and
grandparents are treated with formal deference even after their children have actually taken
control of their own lives. There is a pattern of dependence on seniors that pervades all human
contacts, and the mental software that people carry contains a strong need for such dependence.
When parents reach old age or if they become otherwise infirm, children are expected to support
them financially and practically; grandparents often live with their children’s families.
In the small-power-distance situation, children are more or less treated as equals as soon
as they are able to act, and this may already be visible in the way a baby is handled in its bath.
The goal of parental education is to let children take control of their own affairs as soon as they
can. Active experimentation by the child is encouraged; being allowed to contradict their parents,
children learn to say “no” very early. Behavior toward others is not dependent on the other’s age
or status; formal respect and deference are seldom shown. Family relations in such societies
often strike people from other societies as lacking intensity. When children grow up, they start
relating to their parents as friends, or at least as equals, and a grown- up person is not apt to ask
his or her parents’ permission or even advice regarding an important decision. In the ideal
family, adult members are mutually independent. A need for independence is supposed to be a
172
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
major component of the mental software of adults. Parents should make their own provisions for
when they become old or infirm; they cannot count on their children to support them, nor can
they expect to live with them.
ii-Power distance at school [41]
In most societies today, children go to school for at least some years. In the more affluent
societies, the school period may cover more than twenty years of a young person’s life. In school
the child further develops his or her mental programming. Teachers and classmates inculcate
additional values, being part of a culture that honors these values. It is an unanswered question as
to what extent an education system can contribute to changing a society. Can a school create
values that were not yet there, or will it unwittingly only be able to reinforce what already exists
in a given society? In a comparison of schools across societies, the same patterns of differences
that were found within families resurge. The role pair parent-child is replaced by the role pair
teacher-student, but basic values and behaviors are carried forward from one sphere into the
other. And of course, most schoolchildren continue to spend most of their time within their
families.
In the large-power-distance situation, the parent-child inequality is perpetuated by a
teacher-student inequality that caters to the need for dependence well established in the student’s
mind. Teachers are treated with respect or even fear (and older teachers even more so than
younger ones); students may have to stand when they enter. The educational process is teacher-
centered; teachers outline the intellectual paths to be followed. In the classroom there is
supposed to be a strict order, with the teacher initiating all communication. Students in class
speak up only when invited to; teachers are never publicly contradicted or criticized and are
treated with deference even outside school. When a child misbehaves, teachers involve the
parents and expect them to help set the child straight. The educational process is highly
personalized: especially in more advanced subjects at universities, what is transferred is seen not
as an impersonal “truth,” but as the personal wisdom of the teacher. The teacher is a guru, a term
derived from the Sanskrit word for “weighty” or “honorable,” and in India and Indonesia this is,
in fact, what a teacher is called. The French term is a maître à penser, a “teacher for thinking.” In
such a system the quality of one’s learning is highly dependent on the excellence of one’s
teachers.
In the small-power-distance situation, teachers are supposed to treat the students as basic
equals and expect to be treated as equals by the students. Younger teachers are more equal and
are therefore usually more liked than older ones. The educational process is student-centered,
with a premium on student initiative; students are expected to find their own intellectual paths.
Students make uninvited interventions in class; they are supposed to ask questions when they do
not understand something. They argue with teachers, express disagreement and criticisms in
front of the teachers, and show no particular respect to teachers outside school. When a child
misbehaves, parents often side with the child against the teacher. The educational process is
rather impersonal; what is transferred are “truths” or “facts” that exist independently of this
particular teacher. Effective learning in such a system depends very much on whether the
supposed two-way communication between students and teacher is, indeed, established. The
entire system is based on the students’ well-developed need for independence; the quality of
learning is to a considerable extent determined by the excellence of the students.
173
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
174
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Reisinger [81, p.184] maintains that the power distance (PDI) dimension – the extent to
which a society accepts the unequal distribution of social power - also influences intercultural
communication. In the large-power distance cultures societies people show respect for elderly
people, the boss and teachers. They consider them as a kind of father. They expect them to tell
what to do and they will hardly argue with them. In these countries it is held that people are not
equal and that everyone has a rightful place, people obey the rules of authority and supervisors;
decisions are made autocratically. They are aware of status and role relationships and use
separate appropriate forms of communication style, forms of language and ways of addressing
those from different social classes. They tend to have formal and asymmetrical interaction and
expect to receive diections. In low-power distance cultures societies in which there is no social
hierarchy people consult with each other, use the same communication style for everybody, and
focus on independence and personality. People tend to have informal and symmetrical
interaction.
1.2.3.3. The Uncertainty avoidance dimension
“Uncertainty avoidance can be defined as the extent to which the members of a culture
feel threatened by ambiguous or unknown situations” [41, p.191].
According to Ting-Toomey [99, p.72], while members in weak uncertainty avoidance family
situations prefer informal rules to guide their behaviour, members in strong uncertainty avoidance
family situations tend to prefer formal structure and formal rules. Rules and laws are established to
counteract uncertainties in social interaction. In weak avoidance family situations, roles and
behavioural expectations are actively negotiated. Children are given more latitude to explore their own
values and morals. In strong uncertainty avoidance family situations, family roles are clearly
established and family rules are expected to be followed closely.
In weak uncertainty work situations, there is a greater tolerance of innovative ideas and
behaviour. Conflict is also viewed as a natural part of organizational productivity. In strong uncertainty
avoidance work situations, there is a greater resistance to deviant and innovative ideas. Career mobility
is high in weak uncertainty avoidance cultures, whereas career stability is a desired end goal in strong
uncertainty avoidance cultures. In strong uncertainty avoidance organizations, conflict is viewed as a
threat to organizational effectiveness.
Hoftede [1980, as cited in [99] uses the following statements to represent the basic
characteristics of strong uncertainty avoidance organizations: a- most organizations would be
better off if conflict could be eliminated; b-it is important for a manager to have at hand precise
answers to most of the questions that his or her subordinates may raise about their work; and c-
when the respective roles of the members of a department become complex detailed job
descriptions are essential.
Reisinger [81, p.184] states that in the high uncertainty avoidance cultures (Greece,
Portugal, Guatemala, Uruguay, Japan) people avoid conflict in communication, seek consensus
in conversation, and security through written rules, regulations, and face saving. Members of
these societies are anxious, aggressive, emotionally restrained and loyal to group decisions.
175
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
They are often nationalistic and suspicious toward foreigners. In the low uncertainty avoidance
cultures (Singapore, Denmark, Sweden, Hong Kong, the United Kingdom, India, the United
States, New Zealand), people tolerate ambiguity and uncertainty in conversations, talk about new
ideas, and need few rules as possible to guide them in communication. They believe conflicts
and disagreements are natural. They accept foreigners with different ideas and are optimistic
about the future.
Table 3.8. Major differences between Weak-uncertainty-avoidance and Strong- uncertainty
avoidance cultures [99, p.72]
WEAK UNCERTAINTY AVOIDANCE STRONG UNCERTAINTY AVOIDANCE
CULTURES CULTURES
Uncertainty is valued Uncertaintyis a threat
Career change Career stability
Encourage risk taking Expect clear procedures
Conflict can be positive Conflict is negative
Expect innovations Preserve status quo
Countries: Singapore, Jamaica, Denmark, Countries: Greece, Portugal, Guatemala, Uruguay,
Sweden, Hong Kong, United States, Japan, France, Spain, South Korea, and Japan
Canada, Norway, and Australia.
176
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
feminine organization tends to emphasize environmental issues above and beyond business
performace [99, p.73].
Table 3.9. Major differences between “Feminine” and “Masculine” cultures [99]
FEMININE CULTURES MASCULINE CULTURES
Flexible sex roles Complementary sex roles
Emphasize nurturance Emphasize achievements
Quality of work life Economic growth
Work in order to live Live in order to work
Environmental issues Business performance
Countries: Sweden, Norway, Netherlands, Countries: Japan, Austria, Venezuela, Italy,
Denmark, Costa Rica, and Finland Mexico, and Philippines
177
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
178
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
tradition, and reciprocation of greetings, favors, and gifts–reflect distractions from the
Confucian work dynamism, and thus characterize people who have a short-term orientation
toward life. After finding that people in Asian countries with a strong link to Confucian
philosophy acted differently from people in Western cultures, Hofstede added a fifth dimension
based on Confucian dynamism, Long-Term Orientation – LTO, to his model.
According to Ting-Toomey [99, p.75], the following two principles guide Confucian
philosophy: 1-superiors in the workplace must act with virtue, and those in inferior positions
must obey their superiors; 2-one should act dutifully toward one’s parents and elders,
reciprocally in one’s obligations, and respectfully in role differentiation.
Confucianism includes core values such as “servility, frugality,
abstinence,…diligence…hardwork, patriarchal leadership, entrepreneutrial spirit, and devotion
to family” [Engholm, 1994, as cited in [99, p.75]. The Confucian dynamism is reflective of the
collectivism and large power distance dimensions. Additionally, Confucian dynamism
emphasizes both traditional values and adaptation to economic change in the environment.
Finally, based on Confucian philosophy, the Chinese concept of ‘Face’ was derived.
Face, in the Chinese context, means projected social image and social self-respect. Group
harmony, and thus in-group interdependence, is achieved through the maintenance of everyone’s
face in the society and trying hard not to cause anyone to lose “face.” The theme of ‘facework’
permeats many cultures and profoundly influences how Asian cultures conduct business with
their counterparts [99, p.75].
How does Confucianism influence communication?
According to Samovar, Porter, Mcdaniel and Roy [84, pp.168-169], Confucianism
influences perception and communication in a variety of ways. Let us mention four of those
ways that most directly relate to intercultural communication. First, Confucianism teaches
empathy as it encourages people to understand the feelings of others. Second, when
communicating with someone who adheres to the Confucian philosophy, we should be aware of
status and role relationships. Remember, it was the goal of Confucius “to make social
relationships work without strife.” To accomplish that goal it was important that proper status
and role relationships be maintained. We should note that Confucianism prescribes different
obligatory requirements for different role relationships; for example, loyalty of the ruled to their
ruler, filial piety of sons and daughters to their parents, respect for brothers, and trust for friends.
Even today, these different role behaviors influence such things as using language that shows
respect and status, how leaders are selected, and seating arrangements in business and
educational settings. Third, Confucian principles manifest great concern for ritual and protocol.
Social etiquette was an important part of Confucian teaching. In Confucius’s view, attentive
performance of social ritual and everyday etiquette shapes human character in accordance with
archetypal patterns. In the business context, ritual and protocol are manifested in the fact that
when negotiating, the Chinese feel uncomfortable if there is not structure, form, and correct
manners. They believe these characteristics will preserve harmony among the participants.
Finally, Confucian philosophy tends to encourage the use of indirect instead of direct language.
In the United States people often ask direct questions, are sometimes blunt, and frequently use
the word “no.” Confucian philosophy, on the other hand, encourages indirect communication.
179
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
For example, in Chinese culture, requests often are implied rather than stated explicitly for the
sake of relational harmony and face maintenance. The Confucian legacy of consideration for
others and concern for proper human relationships has led to the development of communication
patterns that preserve one another’s face. Indirect communication helps to prevent the
embarrassment of rejection by the other person or disagreement among partners.
Confucianism influences communication and especially much on Asian communication
styles [75]. The emphasis on high context communication in Asian culture may be explained by
the influence of Confucianism. The influence of Confucianism has been widespread in Asian
countries, particularly in East Asia. Within Confucianism, the purpose of communication is to
develop and maintain harmony within relationships rather than pursuing the outcomes that may
come out of having relationships. Social status and the particular context of interpersonal
relationships structures how individuals relate to one another. For example, Asians may use
different linguistic codes (i.e., plain, polite, honorific) depending on the social status, degree of
intimacy, age, sex, and level of formality of the participants who are engaged in communication.
The indirect communication style helps to facilitate the Confucian value of maintaining
harmony within interpersonal relationships. Searle [1969, as cited in [75] posited that indirect
communication occurs when “the speaker communicates to the hearer more than he actually
says by way of relying on their mutually shared background information, both linguistic and
nonlinguistic, together with the general powers of rationality and inference on the part of the
hearer”. Within this dynamic, the communicator expresses negative feelings or disagreement in
an ambiguous manner so that the receiver has flexibility in interpreting the message in a negative
or non-negative manner. Even though the receiver may accurately interpret the negative feelings
based on the context of the message, the relationship is still protected since the negative message
was encoded in a way so that others may not be able to interpret the negative connotation of the
message. The use of indirect communication may differ based on the communication context.
Brew and Cairns [2004, as cited in [75] examined indirect communication in the workplace and
found that East Asians used more indirect communication with superiors but not with
subordinates. Perhaps, Asians may use more indirect communication with higher status
individuals in order to protect the superior from embarrassment or disagreement.
When indirect communication is used, there is a need for the receiver to be sensitive
toward and be able to infer meaning from the communication context in order to negotiate the
meaning of the indirect message. For example, Koreans use a communication strategy called
Noon-chi, which is the ability to infer the intention, desire, mood state, and attitudes of the other
without having these inner states expressed explicitly to them. Japanese have a similar
communication strategy called Sasshi, which translates to “guessing what someone means”. In
both Korean and Japanese cultures, the ability to infer meaning is considered a valuable
communication skill.
Traditional Asian communication norms devalue the openness and expressiveness in
communication. In Japanese culture, a person who speaks much is considered “light” and a
reticent person is trusted more than a person who is gregarious. In addition, Chinese people may
limit disclosure of personal information, especially within public settings, in fear that their face
may be threatened. Asians tend to disclose personal information to those whose trust has been
proven [Kao, Nagata, & Peterson, 1997, as cited in [75].
180
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
181
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
interpreted, filtered, or understood. Culture can influence social behaviour and communication
style. This can be seen in American culture, Asian cultures and Vietnamese culture.
1.4.1. Advanced Reading: Please, read the section 2 of the article :”Ảnh hưởng của yếu tố
văn hóa truyền thống đối với văn hóa hành chính Việt Nam hiện nay” published in Tạp chí
Quản lí Nhà nước (No. 221, June 2014, pp.39-42)
2. Ảnh hưởng của văn hóa truyền thống đối với văn hóa hành chính Việt Nam
[109, pp.39-42]
Văn hóa truyền thống dân tộc, nơi mà các tổ chức đang tồn tại và hoạt động có ảnh
hưởng quan trọng đối với văn hóa của tổ chức đó. Geert Hofstede (1980) là tác giả nổi tiếng đầu
tiên trong lĩnh vực này đã khám phá những ảnh hưởng của văn hóa dân tộc tới văn hóa của tổ
chức thông qua 5 tiêu chí là: xu hướng về khoảng cách quyền lực, xu hướng cá nhân/tập thể, xu
hướng nam giới/nữ giới, xu hướng ổn định/năng động, xu hướng tránh né những bất định.
- Khoảng cách quyền lực (Power distance): Tiêu chí này liên quan đến mức độ bình
đẳng/bất bình đẳng giữa người với người trong một xã hội bất kỳ nào đó. Một quốc gia có điểm
khoảng cách quyền lực lớn sẽ chấp nhận và kéo dài sự bất bình đẳng giữa người và người, tháp
quyền lực cao và nhọn, do đó việc một người muốn di chuyển từ chân tháp lên đỉnh tháp sẽ rất
khó khăn và hạn chế.
- Chủ nghĩa cá nhân/ tập thể (Invidualism/Collectivism): Xu hướng này liên quan đến
mức độ mà một xã hội chấp nhận chủ nghĩa cá nhân hay yêu cầu mọi cá nhân phải sống vì tập
thể. Tâm lý cá nhân xuất hiện ở những xã hội nơi mỗi người tự chịu trách nhiệm về hành vi của
mình, mỗi người tự lo cho mình và quan hệ giữa các cá nhân lỏng lẻo. Tâm lý tập thể tồn tại ở
những xã hội nơi mà con người có sự gắn kết với nhau, sống dựa vào nhau.
- Tính nam quyền/nữ quyền (Masculinity/Womanizer): Nam quyền/nữ quyền chỉ mức độ
rõ ràng của các vai trò giới giữa nam và nữ.
- Xu hướng dài hạn, coi trọng sự ổn định (Long- term Orientation): Xu hướng này chỉ
mức độ mà tư tưởng nhìn xa hay nhìn gần là phương hướng chủ đạo trong cuộc sống và gắn liền
với khái niệm của Khổng học về “đạo đức” được Hofstede đối lập với phương Tây chỉ quan tâm
đến “sự thực”.
- Xu hướng tránh né những bất định (Uncertainty Avoidance): Xu hướng này nói lên mức
độ sẵn sàng chấp nhận những thay đổi, những điều mới mẻ của một cộng đồng. Một quốc gia có
điểm số cao về tránh bất định sẽ không sẵn sàng chấp nhận những điều mới lạ, những thay đổi
mà họ chưa từng trải nghiệm.
Theo 5 tiêu chí này, văn hóa Việt Nam thuộc loại khoảng cách quyền lực cao, có xu
hướng theo chủ nghĩa tập thể, chỉ số tránh bất định ở mức trung bình. Các chỉ số về văn hóa Việt
Nam tương đồng với các chỉ số văn hóa Trung Quốc, thể hiện qua bảng sau:
Table 3.11. Some indices of national cultures
Việt Nam 70 20 40 30 80
182
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Hàn Quốc 60 18 39 85 75
Nhật Bản 54 46 95 92 80
Mỹ 40 91 62 46 29
Đức 35 67 66 65 31
Thụy Điển 31 71 5 29 33
183
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
kiến cá nhân một cách trực tiếp. Vai trò cá nhân không được đề cao, nhân viên luôn chờ đợi ý
kiến chỉ đạo của cấp trên tạo nên tình trạng trì trệ, ỷ lại vào tập thể, thiếu chủ động, thiếu ý thức
trách nhiệm trong công việc. Các nhân viên ít quan tâm đến chất lượng, hiệu quả thực thi, coi tài
sản tập thể như “của chùa” cũng như thói quen chi tiêu bừa bãi, lãng phí theo kiểu “cha chung
không ai khóc” là tình trạng thường gặp ở nhiều tổ chức hành chính hiện nay.
2.3 Tính nam quyền/ nữ quyền
Do ảnh hưởng của chế độ mẫu hệ nên văn hóa hành chính Việt Nam mang nhiều yếu tố
nữ quyền. Người Việt Nam sống khiêm tốn và nhường nhịn. Các tổ chức hành chính Việt Nam
coi trọng tính ổn định, tránh xung đột. Điều này xuất phát từ nhận thức “giữ thể diện”, và xu
hướng tránh xung đột trong các mối quan hệ.
Cái lý cao nhất của văn hóa cộng đồng hay văn hóa làng là: “thương người như thể
thương thân”, “tình làng nghĩa xóm”, là “người trong một nước phải thương nhau cùng”. Do ảnh
hưởng sâu sắc của nền văn hóa truyền thống, văn hóa hành chính ở nước ta lấy tiêu chí đoàn kết,
thống nhất, tinh thần đùm bọc,… trong tổ chức quan trọng hơn là sự ganh đua mạnh mẽ, để tạo
ra hiệu quả cao hơn. Điều này làm cho đặc trưng văn hóa các tổ chức hành chính ở Việt Nam, ở
khía cạnh này mang “nữ tính” nhiều hơn. Trong khi đó, văn hóa các nước Nhật Bản, Anh, Hoa
Kỳ lại chú trọng nhiều tới tính hiệu quả và cạnh tranh, thể hiện rõ tính chất mạnh mẽ, “nam
tính”.
Tuy nhiên, sự mềm dẻo, linh hoạt, thiên về cảm xúc trong ứng xử cũng như giải quyết
công việc hàng ngày có thể dẫn đến sự tuỳ tiện, thiếu nguyên tắc trong thực thi công vụ. Điều
này được thể hiện trong cách giao tiếp thân mật đời thường cũng như cách xưng hô kiểu gia
đình: chú – cháu, chú – bác,… khi làm việc. Việc coi trọng tình nghĩa, chú trọng thâm niên và
tuổi tác cũng có thể dẫn tới sự thiếu dứt khoát, thiếu công bằng trong xử lí công việc.
184
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
thể làm cho các nhà quản lí khó khăn hơn trong việc thu nhận được những thông tin phản hồi
thực chất về các vấn đề trong tổ chức.
1.4.2. America communication style:
American culture is considered to be an individualistic, small-power distance, weak
certainty, short-term orientation, low-context communication culture. Therefore, as analysed
above, in the American cultural context, the features of American commmunication style would
be freedom of speech, aggressiveness, individual-face concern, consultation, informality,
overtness, explicitness, directness, verbal-based understanding, symmetrical interaction, and
two-way communication.
American culture is individualistic culture. American English is replete with words and
phrases that promote the individual. In the United States children are continually told they need
to be “independent’ and “self-reliant,” and hear phrases like “stand on your own two feet” and
“don’t depend on others.” Grounded on a strong belief in individualism and equality, the
United States has long been considered an informal culture. In North America people tend to
treat others with informality and directness. Informality is manifested in a host of ways. For
example, regardless of the social position, most Americans will quickly move to using first
names when meeting the strangers.
American is considered a low-context communication culture, one where the meaning of
a given statement is taken literally, and does not depend on the context. In a low- context culture
such as the American one, communication tend to be explicit and direct, and getting to the point
quickly is critical.
The dominant style of communication in the American context has the following
characteristics:
a-A majority of the verbal information is explicitly communicated. For example, an
apology must be clearly articulated, whereas in a high context culture the same message can be
communicated through a variety of nonverbal gestures such as a smile, a sigh, a shrug, or a
frown.
b-Directness, openness, and honesty are valued, as is freedom of emotional expression.
c-Questioning and challenging authority are both acceptable and encouraged.
Questioning authority figures suggests one has personal power and can help bring about change.
Independence, self-determination, and personal power are highly valued.
d-In the North American context, time is considered a valuable commodity and of great
importance. Being on time is valued and tardiness is considered disrespectful.
1.4.3. For better understanding of Vietnamese communication style, please read Tran Ngoc
Them [117, pp.155-165].
185
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
The macro levels of culture in language use can be broken down into more specific
features which show points of articulation between language and culture directly convertible into
teachable material.
World knowledge in a foreign culture corresponds to what has been meant traditionally
by ‘teaching culture’. It encompasses the teaching of the general cultural traits and ways of life
of a society, including literature, critical literacy, history, geography, institution and arts. This
‘cultural’ world knowledge is closer to culture than to language per se.
Culture in spoken and written genres is embedded in the general structure of text. For
example, culture is found in the way official or intimate letters are written in different countries,
the type of information which ought to come first and last, what is acceptable content, etc... A
speech, (as a genre of oral text), also reflects culture in the way it has been structured.
Pragmatic and interactional norms refer to the way culture is manifested in spoken and
written language. In pragmatic norms, culture is visible in shorter units of texts such as speech
acts (e.g. thanking in Japanese differs from thanking in Anglo-Australian). Interactional norms
refer more to the way units of speech such as openings or closings in a conversation are
organised.
In grammar, lexicon, kinetics, prosody and pronunciation, culture is also present
interwoven into linguistic structures, words, syntax and non-verbal language.
[Crozet & Liddicoat, as cited in [24]
In addition, attention should be paid to aspects of non-verbal language.
186
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Follow-up activity 3.1. True/False: Decide whether the following statements are true or
false:
1-The social context is the linguistic units which occur before or after a word, a phrase, or
even a longer utterance or a text.
2-According to Holmes (2013), the context of situation includes the following factors:
participant, setting, topic and function.
3-Participant relationships do not influence language choice in communication.
4-Culture provides the overall framework in which human beings learn to organize their
thought, emotions, and behavior in relation to their environment.
5-The cultural context enters through the psychological context and social context.
2. PERSONAL NAMES
Pre-lecture-activity 3.2. Discuss
1-What is your full name? What are the parts of your full name?
2-What is the meaning of each part of your name? How was your given name chosen?
187
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
patronymics or matronymics, for instance, via a middle name as with Pyotr Ilyich Tchaikovsky
(son of pater Ilya), or via a last name as with Björk Guðmundsdóttir (daughter of pater
Guðmund) or Heiðar Helguson (son of mater Helga). Similar concepts are present in Eastern
cultures.
A person's full name usually identifies that person for legal and administrative purposes,
although it may not be the name by which the person is commonly known; some people use only
a portion of their full name, or are known by titles, nicknames, pseudonyms or other formal or
informal designations.
Naming conventions are strongly influenced by culture, with some cultures being more
flexible on naming than others. However, for all cultures where historical records are more
available, the naming rules are known to change over time.
2.1.2. Functions of personal names
Functions of human names are
a-Social differentiation / social identity
b- Aesthetic function.
c-Gender differentiation
2.1.3. Categories of personal names
There are the following types of personal names:
a-Given, First, Christian, Forename.
b-Family, Surname, Last name
d-Middle Name
e-Nick name, Pen name……
2.1.4. Order of personal names
2.1.4.1. Western name order
The order given name – family name is commonly known as the Western order and is
usually used in most European countries and in countries that have cultures predominantly
influenced by Western Europe (North and South America, North, East, Central and West India,
Australia, New Zealand and the Philippines).
Table 3.13. Order of personal English names
Given name Middle name Smith
First name Middle name Last name
David Michael Smith
Within alphabetic lists and catalogues, however the family name is generally proponed,
with the given names following, separated with a comma (e.g. Smith, John), representing the
"lexical name order". This convention is followed by most Western libraries, as well as on many
administrative forms.
188
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
When East Asian names are transliterated into the Latin alphabet, some people prefer to
convert them to the Western order, while others leave them in the Eastern order but write the
family name in capital letters. To avoid confusion, some always write a family name in capital
letters, especially when writing for an international audience. This habit is commonly used in the
international language Esperanto. In Hungarian, for example, Japanese or Chinese names are
most frequently used in the Western order, however, they sometimes remain in the same order as
those of Hungarians.
In contemporary Western societies (except for Iceland, Hungary, and sometimes
Flanders, depending on the occasion), the most common naming convention is that a person must
have a given name, which is usually gender-specific, followed by the parents' family name.
Different cultures have different conventions for personal names.
Surname or family name is a name added to a given name. In many cases, a surname is
a family name and many dictionaries define "surname" as a synonym of "family name". In the
Western Hemisphere, it is commonly synonymous with last name because it is usually placed at
the end of a person's given name.
In some cultures, including those of most Western countries, the surname or family name
("last name") is placed after the personal or given name ("first name"). In other cultures, the
surname is placed first, followed by the given name or names; this is the case in Hungary,
Andhra Pradesh in South India, Sri Lanka and countries in the Chinese cultural sphere including
Japan, Korea, Vietnam and China.
A family name is typically a part of a person's personal name which, according to law or
custom, is passed or given to children from one or both of their parents' family names. The use of
family names is common in most cultures around the world, with each culture having its own
rules as to how these names are formed, passed and used.
189
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Surname laws vary around the world. Traditionally in many European countries for the
past few hundred years, it was the custom or law that a woman would on marriage use the
surname of her husband and that children of a man would have the father's surname.
In English-speaking cultures, family names are often used by children when referring to
adults, but it's also used to refer to someone in authority, the elderly, or in a formal setting, and
are often used with a title or honorific such as Mr., Mrs., Ms., Miss, Dr, and so on. Generally,
the given name, first name, forename, or personal name is the one used by friends, family, and
other intimates to address an individual. It may also be used by someone who is in some way
senior to the person being addressed.
2.2.2. Formation
Name etymologists classify European surnames under five categories, depending on their
origin: given name, occupational name, location name, nickname, and ornamental name.
In some cultures, such as Greek, Bulgarian, Russian, Slovak, Czech, etc. surnames
change form depending on the gender of the bearer. For example, in Greece, if a man called
Papadopoulos has a daughter, she will likely be named Papadopoulou (if the couple have decided
their offspring will take the father's surname), since that name has a female version. In Poland, if
the husband is named Podwiński, and his wife takes his surname, her last name, and those of
their unmarried daughters, would be Podwińska. The sons would be known as Podwiński. In
Lithuania, if the husband is named Vilkas, his wife will be named Vilkienė and his daughter will
be named Vilkaitė. In Slovakia and Czech Republic alike, if a man is called Novák, the wife adds
a feminine suffix "-ová" to his surname after the marriage, hence Nováková. The same is true for
daughters which almost always inherit the father's surname with the feminine suffix.
In most European (and Europe-derived) cultures, the given name usually comes before
the family name (though generally not in lists and catalogs), and so is known as a forename or
first name; but the family name traditionally comes first in Hungary, parts of Africa and most of
East Asia (e.g. China, Japan, Korea and Vietnam). In East Asia, even part of the given name may
be shared among all members of a given generation in a family and the family's extensions, to
differentiate those generations from other generations.
190
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Given names are often used in a familiar and friendly manner in informal situations. In
more formal situations the surname is used instead, unless it is necessary to distinguish between
people with the same surname. The idiom "on a first-name basis" (or "on first-name terms")
alludes to the familiarity of addressing another by a given name.
A child's given name or names are usually chosen by the parents soon after birth. If a
name is not assigned at birth, one may be given at a naming ceremony, with family and friends in
attendance. In most jurisdictions, a child's name at birth is a matter of public record, inscribed on
a birth certificate, or its equivalent. .
2.3.3.1. Aspiring personal traits (external and internal). For example, the name Clement means
"merciful". English examples include Faith, Prudence and August.
2.3.3.2. Occupations, for example George means "farmer".
2.3.3.3. Circumstances of birth, for example Thomas meaning "twin" or the Latin name Quintus,
which was traditionally given to the fifth male child.
2.3.3.4. Objects, for example Peter means "rock" and Edgar means "rich spear".
2.3.3.5. Physical characteristics, for example Calvin means "bald".
2.3.3.6. Variations on another name, especially to change the sex of the name (Pauline, Georgia)
or to translate from another language (for instance, the names Francisor Francisco that come
from the name Franciscus meaning "Frenchman").
3.2.3.7. Surnames, for example Winston, Harrison, and Ross. Such names often come from
families that are frequently intermarried with the family bearing the individual's surname.
2.3.3.8. Places, for example Brittany and Lorraine.
2.3.3.9. Time of birth, for example day of the week, as in Kofi Annan, whose given name means
"born on Friday", or the holiday on which one was born, for example, the name Natalie meaning
"[born on] Christmas day" in Latin.
2.3.3.10. Combination of the above, for example the Armenian name Sirvart means "love rose."
2.3.3.11. Names of unknown or disputed etymology, for example Mary.
In many cultures, given names are reused, especially to commemorate ancestors or those
who are particularly admired, resulting in a limited repertoire of names that sometimes vary by
orthography.
Most common given names in English (and many other European languages) can be
grouped into broad categories based on their origin:
191
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Frequently, a given name has versions in many different languages. For example, the
biblical name Susanna also occurs in its original biblical Hebrewversion, Shoshannah, its
Spanish and Portuguese version Susana, and its French version, Suzanne, and its Polish version,
Zuzanna.
2.3.4. Gender
Most names in English are specifically masculine or feminine, but there are many unisex
name as well, such as Jordan, Jamie, Jesse, Alex, Ashley, Chris, Hilary/ Hillary, Kim, Leslie /
Lesley, Joe / Jo, Jackie, Pat, Sam. Often, one gender is predominant; often a particular spelling is
more common for each of the two genders, even when the pronunciation is the same.
Many culture groups, past and present, did not or do not gender names strongly, so that
many or all of their names are unisex. On the other hand, in many languages including most
Indo-European languages (but not English), gender is inherent in the grammar.
The term Christian name is often used as a general synonym for given name. Strictly
speaking, the term applies to a name formally given to a child at an infant baptism or
"christening".
Where births are required to be officially registered, the name entered onto a birth
register or birth certificate may by that fact alone become a legal name. The assumption in the
Western world is often that the name from birth, or perhaps from baptism or bris, persists to
adulthood in the normal course of affairs. Some possible changes concern middle names, uses of
diminutive forms, adoption, choice of surname as parents divorce or were not married. Matters
are very different in some other cultures, where a name at birth is only a childhood name rather
than the default choice for later life.
"Birth name", or now sometimes birthname, can mean name at birth, or the more elusive
concept of personal name (that is, name before taking a professional name such as stage name,
pen name, ring name, assumed name, alias name, nickname, or some recognised name change
process that de jure alters names). The term "Birth name" is sometimes used for the name before
marriage of a woman – in cultures where a married woman's name customarily changes – by
those who find maiden name to be an old-fashioned usage with the wrong connotations.
The term "Birth name" is also applied to mean the family name of the mother of a child
adopted at birth, and is thus likely to be used with more flexibility than the loan-words née and
192
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
né, accepting it even when the name being referred to was acquired by adoption (at or long after
birth), or made in connection with a change of nationality, or changed in any of many other
(rarer) circumstances.
People's names in several cultures include one or more additional names often but not
necessarily placed between the first given name and the surname.
In some English speaking countries such names are specifically referred to as middle
name(s); in most European countries they would simply be regarded as second, third, etc. given
names. In some countries there is usually only one middle name, and in the United States it is
often abbreviated to the middle initial (e.g. Mary Lee Bianchi becomes Mary L. Bianchi, which
is usually standard for signatures or omitted entirely in everyday use (e.g. just Mary Bianchi). In
the United Kingdom she would usually be referred to either as Mary Bianchi, M. L. Bianchi or
Mary Lee Bianchi, or she may choose Lee Bianchi, and informally there may be familiar
shortenings. An individual may have more than one given name, or none. In some other
countries, the term middle name is only used for names that are originally last names, but not
part of the last name of the bearer (for instance one can have one's mother's maiden name as a
middle name).
In countries that primarily speak English, such as Australia, Canada, Ireland, New
Zealand, the United States and the United Kingdom, the forename of a relative is often used as
one's middle name to honor familial heritage.
Multiple middle names are common, e.g. George H. W. Bush. Often, middle names are
names of famous and influential people throughout history.
Vietnamese names generally consist of three parts: a family name, a middle name, and
a given name, used in that order. Persons can be referred to either by the whole name, given
name, or a hierarchic name in normal usage.
Due to the ubiquity of the major family names such as Nguyen, Pham, and Le, a person is
often referred to by their middle name along with their given name in Vietnamese media and
youth culture.
The Vietnamese language is tonal, and so are Vietnamese names. The same spelling
with different tones are different names, which can confuse non-Vietnamese people when the
diacritics are dropped when used outside of Vietnam.
193
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
The family name, positioned first, is passed on by the father to his children (patronymic
naming system). It is estimated that there are around one hundred family names in common use,
although some are far more common than others. The name Nguyễn is estimated to be used by
almost 40% of the Vietnamese population. The top three names are so popular because people
tended to take the family name of kings, to show their favor and loyalty. Over many generations,
the family names became permanent.
The most common family names among the Vietnamese are the following (the Chinese
characters following each name are Hán tự). Added together these 14 names account for 99% of
the people.
Nguyễn 阮 (39%)
Trần 陳 (11%)
Lê 黎 (9.5%)
Phạm 范 (7.1%)
Huỳnh/Hoàng 黃 (5.1%)
Phan 潘 (4.5%)
Vũ/Võ 武 (3.9%)
Đặng 鄧(2.1%)
Bùi 裴 (2%)
Đỗ 杜 (1.4%)
Hồ 胡 (1.3%)
Ngô 吳 (1.3%)
Dương 楊 (1%)
Lý 李 (0.5%)
In Vietnamese cultural practice, women always keep their family names once they marry,
just as in other East Asian cultures, including Chinese culture, to the north and northeast.
Some Vietnamese have dual family names. This dual family name is usually passed
through all people in the family, but sometimes through the male or female line only. In many
cases the mother's family name is added behind the father's as a middle name, which does not
make a dual family name, as the mother's family name isn't passed through to the next
generation.
194
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Most Vietnamese have one middle name, but it is quite possible to have two or more, or
even no middle names at all.
In the past, the middle name was selected by parents from a fairly narrow range of
options. Almost all women had Thị (氏) as their middle name, and many men had Văn (文).
More recently, a broader range of names have been used, and people named Thị sometimes omit
their middle name.
a-To indicate a person's generation — brothers and sisters share the same middle name,
which distinguish them from the generation before them and the generation after them (see
generation name).
b-To separate branches of a big family. For example, "Nguyễn Hữu", "Nguyễn Sinh",
"Trần Lâm". However, this usage is still controversial. Some people consider them to be dual
family names, not family name + middle name. Some families may, however, set up arbitrary
rules about giving a different middle name to each generation.
c-To indicate a person's position in the family, also known as birth order. This usage is
less common than others.
However, nowadays most middle names do not have those usages. They can either have a
meaning or just be there to make the full names more euphonious.
In most cases, formally, the middle name is actually a part of the given name. For
example, the name "Đinh Quang Dũng" is separated into the surname "Đinh" and the given
name "Quang Dũng". In a normal name list, these two parts of the full name are put in two
different columns. However, in daily conversation, the last word in a given name with a title
before it is used to address a person, for example "Ông Dũng", "Anh Dũng", etc. where "Ông"
and "Anh" are words to address the person which depend on age, social position, etc.
The given name is the primary form of address for Vietnamese. It is chosen by parents
and usually has a literal meaning in the Vietnamese language. Names often represent beauty,
such as bird or flower names, or attributes and characteristics that the parents want in their child,
such as modesty (Khiêm).
Typically, Vietnamese will be addressed with their given name, even in formal situations,
although an honorific equivalent to "Mr.", "Mrs.", etc. will be added when necessary. This
contrasts with the situation in many other cultures, where the family name is used in formal
situations, and is a practice similar to Icelandic usage and, to some degree, to Polish practice. It
is similar to the Latin-American and southern European custom of referring to some people as
"Don" along with their first name. It contrasts with Japanese custom, where the given name is
195
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
used only by close friends, especially children and young people (and even here the family name
is often used) and some family members (though in the family, hierarchical role names, such as
'older brother', are often preferred).
Addressing someone by his or her family name is rare though not unheard of. In the past,
married women in the north have been called by their family name, with Thị 氏 as a suffix. In
recent years, doctors are more likely than any other social group to be addressed by their family
name, though this form of reference is more common in the north than in the south. Some
extremely well-known people are sometimes referred to by their family names, such as Hồ Chí
Minh ("Uncle Hồ") (however, his real surname is Nguyễn), Trịnh Công Sơn ("the musician
Trịnh"), and Hồ Xuân Hương ("the poetess with the family name Hồ"). In the old days, people in
Vietnam, particularly North Vietnam, addressed parents using the first child's name; for example,
Mr and Mrs Anh or Master Minh.
When being addressed within the family, children are sometimes referred to by their birth
number, starting from one in the north but starting with two in the south. This practice is less
common recently, especially in the north.
Example
Nguyễn Tấn Dũng is the former Prime Minister of Vietnam. Nguyễn is his family name,
Tấn is his middle name, and Dũng is his given name. In formal usage, he is referred to by his
given name ("Mr. Dũng"), not by his family name ("Mr. Nguyễn").
196
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
“Tên các vị Vua Tiền Triều đều dùng bộ Thuỷ, từ đời Đức Thế Tôn Hiếu Võ Hoàng Đế
dùng cả bộ Nhật và bộ Thuỷ, và Triều Vua Gia Long trở về sau mới chuyên dùng bộ Nhật.
Năm 1823, Vua Minh Mạng có lựa sẵn hai mươi chữ (toàn bộ Nhật) để đặt tên cho các vị
Vua kế thống, sau này Ngài lại làm một bài thơ “Ngự Chế Mạng Danh Thi” gồm có 20 bộ, các
Triều Vua sau cứ noi theo thế thứ mà đặt tên cho các Hoàng Tử:
197
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Tên các Vị Hoàng Tử con Vua Minh Mạng đều dùng bộ Miên, còn Vua Thiệu Trị bộ
Nhơn, đến Đức Đông Cung Hoàng Thái Tử Bảo Long là bộ Phụ (bộ thứ 6 của bài “Ngự Chế
Mạng Danh Thi”.
Follow-up activity 3.2.True/False: Discuss with your friends and decide whether the
following statements are true or false:
3-The order of personal names in English and Vietnamese are the same:
Surname+(Middle name)+Given name.
5-A Given name, in Western Context often referred to as First Name, is a personal name
that specifies and differentiates between members of a group of individuals, especially in a
family, all of whose members usually have the same family name (Surname).
3. KIN(SHIP) TERMS
Pre-lecture Activity 3.3. Discuss
1-What are the kin relations in the family?
2-What kin(ship) terms are used to refer to these relations in English? In Vietnamese?
198
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
199
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
relationship between two people, while a classificatory term represents one of many different
types of relationships. For example, the word brother in English speaking societies indicates a
son of the same parent; thus, English speaking societies use the word brother as a descriptive
term. But a person's male first cousin could be the mother's brother's son, mother's sister's son,
father's brother's son, father's sister's son, and so on; English speaking societies therefore use the
word cousin as a classificatory term.
Morgan discovered that a descriptive term in one society can become a classificatory
term in another society. For example, in some societies one would refer to many different people
as "mother" (the woman who gave birth to oneself, as well as her sister and husband's sister, and
also one's father's sister).
Kroeber, in a 1929 paper, exploded the notion of any kind of simple classificatory /
descriptive typology [136]. This was quite an important paper. He looked at the principles that
were used in separating kinds of kin, and suggested eight: generation, affinity, collaterality,
gender of relative, bifurcation, gender of speaker, relative age, and decedence. Lowie added
a ninth - polarity. He examined the kinds of differences which can be employed to distinguish
kin. For example, in English and American kinship terminology, father and son differ only on
the dimension of generation. Father and father-in-law differ in that father-in-law is an affine,
that is, a relative by marriage. Father and uncle are distinguished by collaterality. The criterion
of collaterality rests on the distinction between siblings and lineal relatives. In English, only
cousin ignores the distinction of collaterality. Cousin also ignores the distinction made
concerning the gender of the relative concerned - the female child of our mother's brother is
called by the same term as his male child. Grouping lineal and collateral relatives under the same
term is technically called "merging", and in kinship systems in general the relatives most
frequently merged are a parent and sibling of the same gender, a sibling and parallel cousin, or a
son or daughter and nephew and niece.
Bifurcation means "forking", and recognizes that relatives may be traced through either a
male or female connecting relative. Our own kinship system ignores this - an uncle may be
traced through either parent, a grandparent is a parent of either or our parents.
Polarity recognizes that a relationship consists of two parties, and thus two terms - aunt,
niece. If this criterion is ignored, the two parties call each other by the same name. In English,
cousin is an example. "Brother is not”, although both brothers use the same term to indicate each
other. Brother results from the fact that the same criteria is being used to assign both people to
the classification. In English, we almost always acknowledge polarity. In some kinship systems,
this is not the case: it is, for example, fairly common for grandfather and grandson to be called
by the same term.
The last three criteria are much less commonly used - the criteria of relative age
recognizes that within a generation people differ in age. They have different terms for elder
brother and younger, elder brother of the father and younger brother of the father, for example.
Some groups have different terms depending on the sex of the speaker - male speakers use
different terms than female speakers. Decedance assigns a different term to a relative depending
on whether the relative is alive or dead.
200
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
201
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
i-Person
KinDEEP recognizes the semantic component Person, with values of {First} and
{Second}. It would be easy to suggest a third-person form, e.g., "their mother".
The major patterns of kinship systems that are known which Lewis Henry Morgan
identified through kinship terminology in his 1871 work Systems of Consanguinity and
Affinity of the Human Family are [168]:
a-Iroquois kinship (also known as "bifurcate merging")
b-Crow kinship (an expansion of bifurcate merging)
c-Omaha kinship (also an expansion of bifurcate merging)
d-Eskimo kinship (also referred to as "lineal kinship")
e-Hawaiian kinship (also referred to as the "generational system")
f-Sudanese kinship (also referred to as the "descriptive system)
202
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
203
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
204
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
The term cousin is the most classificatory term meaning the children of uncles or aunts.
One can further distinguish cousins by degrees of collaterality and by generation. Two persons of
the same generation who share a grandparent count as "first cousins" (one degree of
collaterality); if they share a great-grandparent they count as "second cousins" (two degrees of
collaterality) and so on. When the cousins are not the same generation, they are described as
"removed". If two persons share an ancestor, one as a grandchild and the other as a great-
grandchild of that individual, then the two descendants class as "first cousins once removed"
(removed by one generation); if they shared ancestor figures as the grandparent of one individual
and the great-great-grandparent of the other, the individuals class as "first cousins twice
removed" (removed by two generations), and so on. Similarly, if they shared ancestor figures as
the great-grandparent of one person and the great-great-grandparent of the other, the individuals
class as "second cousins once removed". Hence one can refer to a "third cousin once removed
upwards"[161].
When the cousins are not the same generation, they are described as "removed". In this
case, the smaller number of generations to the common ancestor is used to determine the degree,
and the difference in generations determines the number of times removed. Note that the ages of
the cousins are irrelevant to the definition of the cousin relationship. Consider the following
[161]:
First cousins
Joseph Nancy
Sam Ryan
205
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
A child ("Sam") of one person ("Laura") and a child ("Ryan") of that one person's sibling
("Robert") are first cousins. First cousins share grandparents ("Joseph" and "Nancy"). Sam and
Ryan are first cousins because they are non-siblings who share a pair of grandparents.
Second cousins
James Mary
Sam Susannah
The children of first cousins share a second cousin relationship. Second cousins share
great-grandparents. People occasionally mistake the child of their first cousin as their "second
cousin" - however that would actually be a first cousin, once removed. The removal denotes the
generational difference. Sam and Susannah are second cousins because they are non-first
cousins and non-siblings who share great-grandparents. In other words, Sam and Susannah's
parents are cousins.
Frank
Two people for whom a first cousin relationship is one generation removed. The child of
one's first cousin or the cousin of one's parent. Frank and his father's first cousin, Emma, are
first cousins once removed. First cousins once removed are often erroneously referred to as
"second cousins." In some cultures, especially in Latin America and India, the relationship is
viewed as being the same as an aunt-uncle and niece-nephew relations
206
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Frank Felicity
Harry
207
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
208
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
209
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
The Vietnamese lineal system of nine generations is very rare in the world. In addition,
the words used to denote each generation are monosyllabic words.
3.4.3. Vietnamese kinship terms
Professor Nguyen Van Khang [112] states that the Vietnamese kinship system are
established on the following kinship categories:
a-relations of consanguinity (blood relation) forming consanguinity (blood relation)-
affinity (non-blood relation) opposition, e.g. bác, chú, cô, cậu, dì (blood-related /
consanguine)/ mợ, thím, dượng (not-blood-related / affinal),
b-relations of generation forming same generation-different generation opposition,
e.g. father (G-1) / ego (G-0),
c-Relations of gender forming male-female opposition, e.g. anh (elder brother) / chị
(elder sister).
d-relations of consanguinity forming lineal-collateral opposition, e.g. cha, mẹ, con, anh,
chị (lineal); bác, chú, cô (colateral),
e-relations of consanguinity hierarchy forming the above rank-the bellow rank
opposition.
The notion of rank is very important in kinship hierarchy in Vietnamese. Rank is based
on kinship hierarchy. It denotes differences in age and generation.
For example, the addressor has to address his / her mother’s senior sister’s daughter as
senior sister even though this is just a baby girl. This little baby can be addressed as great-
grandmother if she is in equal family hierarchy with her relative who has great-grand children.
The baby is of the higher rank than the addressor.
In Vietnamese, there are many cases in which an old man has to address a boy as ông /
bác/ chú…because the boy is higher in rank
f-relations of paternal side-maternal side consanguinity forming nội (paternal side)-
ngoại (maternal side) forming opposition, e.g. ông nội (paternal grand father) / ông ngoại
(maternal grand father).
Another criterion in Vietnamese kinship system is relative age distinguishing elder/
younger, first-born / second-born / third –born.
210
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
211
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
bà gì ruột
15 Ông / ông bác Older brother of grand 49 Anh (ruột) Older brother
father
18 Ông / ông bác Husband of older sister 52 Chị (ruột) Older sister
of grand father
/ ông dượng
dượng
212
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
cậu
Bu, Cái , Mợ
26 Bố/ mẹ/ anh Wife ’s father/mother/ 60 Cháu nội (gái) Paternal grand daughter
older brother/older
/chị / em vợ sister/younger
sister/younger brother
trai
213
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
33 Thím Father’s younger 67 Chắt nội trai Paternal great grand son
brother’s wife
The Vietnamese kinship system reflects the patrilineal organization in the Vietnamese
family.
There are corresponding kinship terms on each rank. The kinhip term on the rank of kị
(great great grand parent) includes kị; on the rank of cụ (great grand parents) are cụ, cụ ông, cụ
bà; on the rank of ông bà (grand parents) are ông bà, ông nội , bà nội, ông ngoại, bà ngoại,
ông (older brother of grand parents), bà ( older sister of grand parents), ông trẻ (younger
brorher of grand parents), bà trẻ (younger sister of grand parents); on the rank of cha mẹ
(parents) are bố mẹ, bố, mẹ, bố đẻ, bối ruột, mẹ đẻ, mẹ ruột, bố chồng, mẹ chồng, bố vợ, mẹ
vợ, bố nuôi, mẹ nuôi, gì ghẻ, mẹ ghẻ, dượng. On the rank of bác chú, cô, cậu gì (uncle and
aunt) are bác, bác ruột, bác họ, bác trai, bác gái, bác dâu; chú, cô, cậu , gì, thím, mợ; cô chú,
chú thím, cậu mợ, chú bác, cô gì; chú ruột, chú họ; cậu ruột, cậu họ; cô ruột, cô họ, gì ruột,
gì họ; on the rank of vợ chồng (wife and husband) are vợ chồng; vợ, chồng, vợ cả, vợ lẽ, vợ
hai, vợ ba; On the rank of siblings and cousins are anh, anh trai, anh họ, anh chồng, anh vợ,
anh rể; chị, chị gái, chị họ, chị chồng, chị vợ, chị dâu; em, em trai, em gái, em chồng, em
vợ, em rể, em dâu, em họ; on the rank of con cháu ( children, grand children, great grand
214
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
children) are con, con trai, con gái, con đầu, con trưởng, con cả, con thứ, con út; con dâu,
con rể; con nuôi, con đẻ, con riêng, con chồng, con vợ; cháu, cháu trai, cháu gái, cháu nội,
cháu ngoại, cháu họ, cháu rể, cháu dâu; chắt, chắt trai, chắt gái, chăt nội, chắt ngoại.
There may be up to four generations living in the family in a house in Vietnam. The
kinship terms used in a Vietnamese family would include ông bà nội, ông bà ngoại, chồng, vợ,
con; con trưởng, con thứ, con út; con một. bố chồng, mẹ chồng, con dâu, bố vợ, mẹ vợ, con
rể, em chồng, em vợ; anh chồng, anh vợ; anh rể , em rể; chị dâu, em dâu, bác (wife or
husband of bác), chú (husband of cô), thím (wife of chú), mợ ( wife of cậu), gì ghẻ, mẹ kế, bố
dượng, dượng.
Table 3.22. Vietnamese family kinship terms
Kinship English equivalent Kinship terms English equivalent
terms
1 Ông bà nội Paternal grand 23 Anh rể Older brother of husband
parents
2 Ông nội Paternal grand father 24 Em rể Younger brother of
husband
3 Bà nội Maternal grand 25 Chị dâu Older sister of wife
mother
4 Ông bà ngoại Maternal grand 26 Em dâu Younger sister of wife
parents
5 Bà ngoại Maternal grand 27 Dượng Step father
mother
6 Ông ngoại Maternal grand 28 Mẹ kế / dì ghẻ Step mother
mother
7 Chồng Husband 29 Bác Older brother of father
8 Vợ Wife 30 Bác Wife of older brother of
father
9 Con trai Son 31 Bác Older sister of father
10 Con gái Daughter 32 Bác Husband of older sister of
father
11 Cháu trai Nephew 33 Chú Younger brother of father
12 Cháu gái Niece 34 Thím Wife of younger brother
of father
13 Chắt trai Grand nephew 35 Cô Younger sister of father
14 Chắt gái Grand niece 36 Chú Husband of younger sister
215
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
of father
15 Bố chồng Paternal father in law 37 Bác Older brother of mother
16 Bố vợ Maternal father in 38 Bác Wife of older brother of
law mother
17 Mẹ chồng Paternal mother in 39 Bác Older sister of mother
law
18 Mẹ vợ Paternal mother in 40 Bác Husband of older sister of
law mother
19 Con rể Son in law 41 Cậu Younger brother of
mother
20 Con dâu Daughter in law 42 Mợ Wife of younger brother
of mother
21 Em chồng Younger brother of 43 Dì Younger sister of mother
husband
22 Em vợ Younger sister of 44 Chú Husband of younger sister
wife of mother
216
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
217
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
c-gender is a factor taken in account by both Vietnamese and English kinship terms
system. Hence, a female parent is called “mother” in English and “mẹ” in Vietnamese,
meanwhile, a male one is called “father” and “cha” respectively.
3.5.2. Differences:
3.5.2.1. Degree of descriptiveness
Belonging to the Eskimo pattern, English kinship terminology system consists of both
descriptive and classificatory terms. Descriptive terms such as “mother, father, brother, etc”
are used to express lineal relations. Meanwhile, collateral relations are referred to mostly by
classificatory terms. For instance, the term “aunt” can mean mother’s sister, father’s sister,
father’s brother’s wife and mother’s brother’s wife.
Table 3.24. Collateral relations in English
English terms Relation to the Ego
Uncle Father’s older brother
Father’s younger brother
Mother’s Brother
Aunt Father’s Sister
Mother’s Sister
In contrast, the Vietnamese system makes use of many more descriptive terms than the
English one. There are not many Vietnamese kin terms expressing more than one kind of
relations. One noticable exception is the term “cháu” which means grandchildren as well as
niece or nephew. In addition, unlike English, descriptive terms are not only used to express lineal
relations but also collateral ones. For instance: “cô” can only be used to call one’s father’s
younger sister.
Table 3.25. Collateral relations in Vietnamese
218
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
219
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Follow-up activity 3.3. True/False: Discuss with your friends and decide whether the
following statements are true or false:
1-Kin(ship) is the term used to denote human relations based on biological decent and
marriage.
4-In the Vietnamese society, there is a kinship system of only two generations. It belongs
to Eskimo type.
4. ADDRESSING
Pre-lecture activity 3.4. Discuss
1-How do you
220
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
221
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
“When we look at what is involved in addressing another, it seems that a variety of social
factors usually governs our choice of terms: the particular occasion; the social status or
rank of the other; gender; age; family relationship; occupational hierarchy;
transactional status (i.e., a service encounter, or a doctor–patient relationship, or one of
priest–penitent); race; or degree of intimacy” [107, p. 288].
4.1.3. Common forms of address in English
The common forms of address in English are: Tittle (e.g. My Lord, Mr. Chairman, Prime
Minister), Tittle LN (e.g. professor/ Dr., Father Smith), Mr. LN (e.g.Mr. Smith), Mrs. LN
(e.g.Mrs. Smith), Miss LN (e.g.Miss Smith), kin tittle FN (e.g.Aunt Mary)/ Kin tittle (e.g.aunt,
father, dad), FN (e.g.Mary, Robert), nickname, short name, ø (no name), personal-emotive
means of the direct address (endearments and abusives).
According to Crystal [20, p.44], in English the basic choice is between first name (FN) or
tittle with last name (TLN).
Brown and Ford [1961, as cited in [107, pp.282-283], basing in their study of naming
practices in English report that the asymmetric use of title+last name, and first name (TLN/FN)
indicated inequality in power, that mutual TLN indicated inequality and unfamiliarity, and that
mutual FN indicated equality and familiarity. The switch from mutual TLN to FN is also usually
initiated by the more powerful member of the relationship. The idiom"on a first-name basis" (or
"on first-name terms") alludes to the familiarity of addressing another by a given / first name.
In English speaking countries, the modern tendency is to come to first name terms as soon as
possible, especially if status and age are the same.
In English, when we are in doubt as to how to address another we can actually avoid the
difficulty by not using any address term at all. We, therefore, have the possibility of the
avoidance of an address term, that is, Ø use, or of a choice between familiar and polite [107,
p.285].
4.1.4. Direct address system [64, pp.53-56]
The direct address is a name or designation of the person or persons… to whom the
speech is addressed [64, pp.53].
K.A. Dolinin propopses to distinguish between two classes of the direct address:
a-Socially orienting forms and b- personal emotive forms
4.1.4.1. Socially orienting forms
a-The honorific terms: sir or madam
They are never followed by a name, except when sir is an aristocratic tittle used before
the name of a knight or baronet, such as Sir Robert Chiltern. When sir is a tittle it is always
used with the first name only, and they say Sir Robert, never Sir Chiltern. Sir and Madam
used alone are meant to show respect for position and seniority. Shopkeepers, waiters and
servants call their customers and masters sir or madam.
222
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Sir is an honorific term to be used by persons belonging to various social groups when
addressing their superiors: schoolchildren-their men teachers, soldiers, sailors, policemen, etc.-
their officers, young men-older men.
There are other honorific terms of address. For further information, you can refer to the
the following website link: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Style_(manner_of_address)
i-Profesional tittles
Professional tittles: (Doctor, Inpspector, Colonel, Nurse, Professor)
The professional title is used with or without a surname: T + (LN / Surname).
ii-Courtesy tittles Mr., Mrs., Miss, Ms. + LN
Addressing by surnames with the prefixed courtesy titles Mr., Mrs, Miss (Ms.) + Last
Name is usual among acquaintances. It signals the equality of relations and also a certain social
distance, absence of intimacy. Using a bare surname may imply the addressee’s inferior
position. Using a bare surname is also usual among unrelated male friends, not among near
relatives.
iii-First Name (FN) or Christian / Given name
Addressing by first (Christian) names is current among near relatives, friends, lovers.
The modern tendency is to come to first name terms as soon as possible, especially if status and
age are the same. Addressing somebody by his or her first name in its short, diminutive or
familiar form or using pet names express a greater intimacy and affection.
There are the following short forms for Elizabeth: Bess, Bet, Beth, Eliza, Elsa, Lisa,
Lisbet, Lisbeth, Liz, Liza. The pet forms are: Bessi, Bessy, Betsy, Bette, Betty, Elsie, Libby,
Lizzie, Lizzy.
iv-Kinship terms:
Persons may be also address by the names of the family members which denote the
family status and his relation to the speaker (i) without any other name (e.g. father, dad, pa) and
ii-with a proper name added (e.g. uncle George, auntie Ruth). Group (i) is more inimate than
group (ii).
v-Words of address may also employ ‘an expression of the addressee’s human
classification (of age, gender, generation), e.g. baby, boy, kiddy, old chap, mate, buddy.
4.1.4.2. Personal–emotive means of the direct address
They draw on the stock of evaluative words.
a-Endearments
They express a positive attitude of the speaker to the listener, some of them are capable
of linking up with the forms of socially address (e.g. dear, darling), many of them are familiar
tropes, mostly metaphors (e.g. honey, ducky). The addition of the possessive ‘my’ increases the
emotives.
b-Abusives
223
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Terms of abuse (bad names) are “depreciative (pejorative) words or phrases with a
dominant appeal function with which the speaker addresses another person with the intention to
offend or discredit him socially.” Examples are bastard, idiot, slut…
Diagram 3.1. Factors constraining the polite choice of address terms in British English
[84, p. 286]
The diagram assumes the speaker knows the addressee’s name and the first box takes
account of whether the addressee is a child or an adult. Where the line between adult and
child is drawn differs between communities, and even within communities for different
purposes (e.g. baby-sitting, buying fireworks, marrying, owning a firearm, voting). If the
addressee would be classified as a child in the relevant social interaction, then, following the
minus exit from the box, the flow chart indicates FN is used in British English, e.g. Hey Jill.
224
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Box 2, labelled ‘marked setting’, refers to formal settings such as Parliament and law
courts where people act in role, and where speech is governed by relatively explicit
discourse rules. Forms of address are derived from identity in the context, e.g. Your
honour, Prime Minister. The boxes marked ‘kin’ and ‘well acquainted’ are self-
explanatory – though who qualifies in each case will of course be culture-dependent and
context-dependent.
The box marked ‘higher rank’ refers to social superiors or people of higher status in a
hierarchy. The ‘ascending generation’ is the generation above the speaker–parents and parents’
sisters, brothers and cousins, for instance. A ‘dispensation’ refers to the fact that a superior may
absolve the speaker from the obligation of using TLN and permit them to use FN, e.g. Dr
Worth is far too formal. Please call me Helen. The box marked ‘profession title’ refers to
occupational or courtesy titles such as Doctor, Professor, Sister for a nun, Father for a priest,
and so on.
We can cause offence by treating someone, from their perspective, too familiarly, or by
treating them too distantly. Being polite means getting the linguistic expression of social
distance right as far as your addressee is concerned. This is very variable from one speech
community to another. In the north of England, many newspaper vendors, bus conductors and
people selling railway tickets call everyone love regardless of how well they know them, and
often regardless of their gender. By contrast, mutual TLN (Mrs Landy, Mr Duncan) is usual
between upper-working-class neighbours who live close to each other, but who are not friends
and do not see each other socially. In North America, it is usual to use first names to people you
work with, regardless of how little or much you like them. In other English- speaking cultures,
mutual FN is experienced as too familiar. Mutual TLN is appropriate until you have worked
together for a period of time or develop a friendship. Issues like how long do you have to know
someone, or how close a friendship does it have to be, to use FN raise further complications, of
course. The answers will be different for different communities.
In earlier centuries, the norms were simpler. Status was the major consideration. In
general, people used TLN (or an appropriate kin-term) upwards to superiors, and FN downwards
to subordinates, no matter how well they knew them. Solidarity became relevant only
between equals. Equals used mutual TLN with people they did not know well, and they used
mutual first names to friends. In addition, between some (mainly male) people, mutual LN was
the norm (Elementary my dear Watson!), and there was also a non-reciprocal pattern involving
madam/sir upwards and LN downwards to subordinates (clean that blackboard, Hadley).
Today, however, the interaction between social status and social distance in many
Western societies is more complicated. This results in a conflict of norms in two situations: (a)
high status with high solidarity and (b) low status with low solidarity. If your addressee is of
lower status and you know them well, then either way you use FN to them. But what form do
they use to you? The norms conflict. Because they know you well, they could assume FN is
appropriate, but their subordinate status predicts TLN. How can this dilemma be resolved? One
common solution is to avoid address terms altogether. When an address term must be used,
however, FN is generally used in such a situation. In other words, in many Western
communities in the twenty-first century, the solidarity dimension was tended to be given
greater weight. So secretaries, for instance, generally use FN to their bosses if they have worked
225
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
for them for some time. Workers use FN to the factory floor supervisor. Civil servants use FN
to their policy-making bosses if they talk to them regularly.
What of the second conflicting situation? There is no problem when you do not know
someone well, and they are your superior. Both factors predict that you will use TLN. But when
someone is your subordinate and you don’t know them well, what do you use? This is a tricky
area but, in general, once again the solidarity dimension tends to win out: degree of solidarity (or
social distance) is what counts most. British office workers tend to use TLN to the caretaker
whom they rarely see. Middle-class women generally use TLN to their cleaning women,
especially when the woman is older and rarely encountered face-to- face. The following
American example describes a different pattern, however, and illustrates the complexity of the
factors which may be relevant.
Another factor which contributes to the assessment of social distance, and hence to the
appropriate way of being polite, is the type of relationship involved. In many communities,
transactional relationships favour TLN. Shopkeepers and customers may exchange mutual
TLN even when they have known each other for a long time. Doctors and patients similarly
tend to use mutual TLN. These relationships put the emphasis on the social distance dimension
in that, even when they are long-standing, they do not involve intimacy. To know someone
‘well’ involves more than just knowing them for a long time. Mutual TLN is also a marker of
mutual respect, as example 20 indicates, and it may be especially relevant to express this in
certain contexts. Transactional relationships are usually one-dimensional. When they shift to
become more personal, they often also shift to mutual FN.
One factor which seems to override these patterns – as indicated by its primary position
in figure above – is relative age. Adults use FN to children on first meeting. Young people are
more likely to receive FN in any context. A young shop assistant, hairdresser or cleaner or an
office junior will receive FN from a customer or client, and will often be expected to use TLN
back, especially if the person is a generation or more older.
4.1.5.2. Honorific forms of address in the United Kingdom [190]
Table 3.27. Honorific forms of address in the United Kingdom
On Salutation in
Position Oral address
envelopes letter
For more honorific forms of address in the United Kingdom, please refer to [190].
226
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Diagramme 3.2. American address system [after S. Ervin-Trip, 1972, as cited in [20, p. 44]
Several studies have attempted to explicate these factors. The flow-chart was devised by Susan
Ervin-Tripp (1972) as a means of specifying the factors that condition a speakers’s choice of
address in American English. The chart is simply a logical statement of the various possibilities,
given a context such as ’Look,-,it’s time to leave’; it is not an account of what goes on in the
227
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
speaker’s mind. The knowledge structure represented is that of an American academic; but
dialect differences, idiosyncratic preferences, and other variants are not taken into account.
The entrance point to the diagram is at the bottom left. Each part through the diagram
leads to one of the possible modes of address, listed vertically at the right. Alternative
realizations of these address modes are not given (e.g. a first name may alternate with a
nickname). For example, as one enters the diagram, the first choice which has to be made is
whether the addressee is a child (-Adult) or an adult (+Adult). If the former, one follows the
line downwards, where the only distinction drawn is that between name known (+) or not (-). If
not, one does not use a name at all (ø). The diagram does not give criteria for deciding when a
child becomes an adult.
Along the adult path, several decisions have to be made. ‘Status-marked setting’ refers
to special occasions (such as a courtroom) where forms of address are rigidly prescribed (e.g.
your honour, Mr. Chairman). The ‘identity set’ refers to the list of occupational or courtesy
tittles that may be used alone to mark social identity (e.g. Father, Doctor, Mr., Miss).
In addressing people whose names are known, kinship is a major criterion. If the speaker
is related to the addressee (‘alter’), two factors are relevant ‘ascending generation’ (e.g. aunt
as opposed to cousin) and age. If the speaker is not related to the alter, the factor of familiarity
is relevant: whether or not alter is a friend or colleague. If familiarity applies, the next factor is
social rank, here defined with reference to a professional hierarchy. A senior alter has the option
of offering or accepting FN, instead of TLN (‘dispensation’- Call me Mike), though this
situation is often ambiguous. Age difference is not significant until there is a gap of nearly a
generation (15 years apart).
Addressing by Title alone is the least intimate form of address in that Titles usually
designate ranks or occupations, as in Colonel, Doctor, or Waiter. They are devoid of ‘personal’
content. Doctor Smith is more intimate than Doctor.
Knowing and using another’s First Name is a sign of considerable intimacy or at least of
a desire for such intimacy. Using a nick name or pet name shows an even greater intimacy.
When someone uses your first name alone in addressing you, you may feel on occasion that that
person is presuming an intimacy you do not recognize, or alternatively, is trying to assert power
over you.
The use of a person’s first name in North America does not necessarily indicate
friendship or respect. First Names are required among people who work closely together, even
though they may not like each other at all. First Names may even be used to refer to public
figures, but contemtuously as well as admiringly.
The asymmetric use of names and address terms is often a clear indicator of a power
differential. School classrooms are almost universally good examples; John and Sally are likely
to be children and Miss or Mr. Smith to be teachers.
In English, when we are in doubt as to how to address another we can actually avoid the
difficulty by not using any address term at all. Kinship terms are also used as terms of address.
As your age and your family relationships change, issues of naming and addressing may arise.
228
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Most current and former elected federal and state officials and judges in the U.S. are
styled "The Honorable [full name]" in writing, (e.g., "The Honorable Bill de Blasio, Mayor
of the City of New York"). Many are addressed in conversation as "Mister [title]" or "Madam
[title]" ("Mr. President," "Madam Mayor") or simply by (title)+(name) e.g., "Senator Jones"
or "Commissioner Smith".
In academic fields, it is customary in the U.S. to refer to those holding any level of
professorship (professor, assistant professor, associate professor, adjunct professor, etc.) as
"Professor" – as in "Professor Jones" – orally or in writing. In writing, "professor" is often
abbreviated as "Prof.", as in "Prof. Jones". Those holding academic doctorates are frequently
referred to as "Dr. Jones."
For more details on honorific terms of address in the USA, please refer to [190].
4.2. Vietnamese terms of address
4.2.1. Forms of address in the Vietnamese language
“Vietnamese forms of address are diverse, coming from different sources and used
flexibly from the domain of family communication to social communication, in both
formal and informal registers” [112, p.38].
Professor Nguyen Van Khang [111, p.366] states that there exist two notions in
Vienamese: Xưng (self-calling / self-referring / first-person referring) and Hô / gọi (second-
person addressing). Accordingly, forms / terms of adress in the Vietnamese language include từ
xưng (hereafter referred to as first-person referring term/form of address) and từ hô / gọi
(hereafter referred to as second-person referring term/form of address). Thus, Vietnamese
forms / terms of address will include three categories: a-first-person-referring term of address,
b-second-person-referring term of address and c-term of third-person reference.
Professor Nguyen Văn Khang has the following remarks [111, pp.366 – 367]:
a-Vietnamese terms of address are of various categories. Many types of words have been
used as terms of address, noteworthy of which are the Vietnamese kin(ship) terms. Kinship terms
229
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Professor Nguyễn Văn Khang states that there are the following modes of address [111,
p.362]:
a-Surname and given names
i-Given name
ii-Surname
iii-Middle Name + Given name
iv-Surname + Given name
v-Surname+Middle name+Given name
b-All the possible terms of address
vi-Personal pronouns ii-Kin(ship) Terms
vii-Kinship terms
viii- Other types of words used as terms of address, such as reflexive pronouns (e.g.
mình), demonstrative pronouns (e.g. đằng này, đằng ấy…)
c-Tittles
ix-One of the tittles
x-Many or all of the tittles
d-Substitute terms of address
xi-Words used as substitute terms of address, e.g. bố thằng Nam ơi.
e-a combination of i, ii, iii, or iv
xii-Different types of combination (Tittle+LN), (Tittle+full name)..
230
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Luong [115] is of the opinion that forms of person reference in the Vietnamese language
can be: a-common nouns (kinship and status terms), b-people’s names and c- personal pronouns.
Main Vietnamese terms of address are presented in the following two tables:
Table 3.29. First-person-referring terms of address in the Vietnamese language [111,
pp.367-368]:
First-person-referring Communicative context Note
terms of address
1 Tên -bằng vai+thân mật+trẻ -Con với bố mẹ;
tuổi cháu với ông bà;
Name em với anh chị
Same rank +familiar
+young Child addressing
parent, (great)
-vai dưới+gần gũi thân grand child
mật + nhỏ tuổi addressing grand
Lower rank + familiar + parent, younger
younger sibling or cousin to
older sibling or
-không muốn hạ vai cousin
Does not want to lower -ít gặp; đặc biệt
the rank trong trường hợp
chủ thể giao tiếp là
lãnh đạo nhưng ít
tuổi hơn khách thể
giao tiếp
Special, when the
addressor is the
boss but younger
than the addressee
in age.
231
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
232
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
233
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
2 Từ (a)Cụ, Ông, bà, -Vai dưới gọi vai trên -Dùng trong xã
bố, mẹ (gia đình+Xã hội) + hội + gần gũi
Xưng tôn kính (nhằm tạo ra gần
Hô gũi)
Lower rank
addressing higher Adsressing in
rank (in family and society + familiar
Terms / Forms society) + respect (creating familiar
of Address atmosphere as the
-bằng vai gọi nhau relatives)
+thân mật+xuồng xã
(Kinship terms (ông, bà) -Thí dụ: vợ gọi
& pronouns chồng bằng bố
Same rank + familiar
(personal, + casual e.g. wife
reflessive, addressing the
-gọi thay con, cháu husband as the
234
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
235
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
236
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
237
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
238
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
their meaningful middle names and their given names preceded by a kinship status term (e.g. co
Ngoc Lan- female master teacher Ngoc Lan). In a highly formal situation, people are addressed
by their full names preceded by a status term. Our famous president Hồ Chí Minh is addressed as
“Chủ tịch Hồ Chí Minh” (President Ho Chi Minh) with high respect.
Using the first names as first-person-referring terms of address is not common. However,
when it is used, it would imply a cautious attitude of the speaker to avoid offending the other
interactant for being unsure of his or her social status or when the interactants are of different
status. Another case when the first name is used as first-person-referring terms of address is
when the addressor knows his/her senior (or junior) status but wants to deny it when addressing a
junior (or senior) person. In a public-domain interaction, it represents an implication of
informality and solidarity.
The use of family name as terms of address was popular before the 20th century.
However, as discussed above, it would still be disrespectful to address people even by their
family names without a kinship or status term. President Ho Chi Minh is called Bác Hồ (Uncle
Hồ) by Vietnamese people with an implication of both respect (by using his family name Ho)
and intimacy (by using kinship term “Uncle”).
Nicknames are used as another reference to show informality and solidarity. When it is
used for young children, it even shows affection.
239
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
của chồng, con vợ/con của vợ); cháu (grand child) for all terms relating to về cháu (cháu trai, cháu gái, cháu nội,
cháu ngoại, cháu họ, cháu rể, cháu dâu).
This manner of addressing is in accordance with the tendency of condensity, economy and possible
syllabic structure reduction in terms of address in Vietnamese communication. In addition, it indicates the
tendency of showing repect and affection, avoiding biase in differentiating between paternal side and maternal
side, son-adopted son, in-laws. The kinship terms in Vietnam vary according to regions.
4.2.3.2. The kinship terms as terms of address in family communication
According to professor Nguyen Van Khang (2014b, p.41), the following basic terms of kinship are used
as first-person-referring terms of address and second-person-referring terms of address: cụ, ông, bà, bố, mẹ, gì,
dượng, bác, chú, cô, cậu, thím, mợ, anh, chị, em, con, cháu in family communication. Kị and chắt
are not used as terms of address in the family. The two terms anh-em are used as terms of
address for vợ-chồng.
The compound terms of kinship are used terms of address in limited contexts. They
include: a- comprehensive compound terms of kinship, e.g. ông bà, cha mẹ, bố mẹ, chú cô, cô,
chú, chú bác, chú thím, cậu mợ, cô gì; vợ chồng, anh chị, anh em, chị em, con cháu (These
terms are used as terms of address when the addressor wants to imply something or to invite, e.g.
Mời ông bà, mời bố mẹ xơi cơm! Two terms cháu chắt and cụ kị are not used as terms of address
and b- subordinate compound terms of kinship such as cụ ông, cụ bà, ông nội , bà nội, ông
ngoại, bà ngoại, bác họ, chị gái, cháu trai, cháu gái, cháu nội,… are used as terms of address
in certain limited contexts, e.g. Con gái ăn cơm rồi học bài đi nhé!
The use of kinship terms as terms of address is governed by rigid hierarchy of
Vietnamese kinship system.
Table 3.31. Common terms of Vietnamese kinship used as terms of address in the
Vietnamese family
First-person- English Translation Note Second-person-
referring referring terms of
terms address address
cha father Many other terms are used, con
depending on the dialect: ba, bố,
tía, thầy
mẹ mother mẹ is the Northern form, má is con
used in the South. Many other
terms are used, depending on the
dialect: u, bầm, mạ
anh older brother em
chị older sister em
em younger sibling or anh or chị
cousin of the same
generation
240
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
241
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Second-person- First-
referring terms person-
of address referring
terms of
address
242
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
following compound terms of kinship: ông bà, cô chú, chú bác, anh chị, anh em, chị em are used as
terms of second-person referent only. The use of kinship terms as terms of address is closely governed by
such factot as gender, age and then social position.
4.2.3.4. Kinship terms used as term of address in public services communication:
In public services communication, professional tittles (e.g. giám đốc, chủ tịch, viện trưởng….) and
terms such as đồng chí, and recently ông, ngài, quý vị are are used as honorific terms of second-person address.
The pattern is Tittle alone or Tittle + LN or Tittle + full Name. In addition, kinship terms are largely used as
terms of address in public communication with the choice of kinship terms as terms of address in accordance with
the characteristics of public services communication. The most common kinship terms used as terms of address
are em – anh / chị, tôi – anh / chị, cháu – chú / cô. The less common are con – bố / mẹ, con – cô / chú, cháu –
bác, con- dì. The Vietnamese people have the habit of using the term bác + professional /honoric tittle to
address high-ranking officials, e.g. bác Tổng bí thư, bác Chủ tich nước. In diplomatic reception, the terms
ngài, quý vị have been much used in addition to the term đồng chí.
Terms of address in public services communication can vary according to domains. In schools and
universities, the pattern is em / con – thầy / cô. In hospitals the common patern can be tôi / em / cháu – bác sĩ.
In media communication, especially on television the pattern is tôi / chúng tôi – các đồng chí / các bạn / các bác
/ các cô / quý vị khán thính giả. VTV1 addresses the audience as “kính chào quý vị, xin chào các bạn.”
4.2.3.5. Precise and imprecise addressing
According to professor Nguyen Van Chien [as cited in [111, p.367] there exist two modes
of addressing with kinship term in the Vienamese language: precise addressing (e.g. Cháu-cô)
and imprecise addressing (e.g. con-cô).
Table 3.33. Kinship terms as precise and imprecise terms of address in the Vietnamese
language (precise addressing: (+); imprecise addressing: +)
Terms of Con Chá em chị an cô Cậu chú bác ông bà cụ
second- u h
person
referent
Terms of
first-person
referent
Con + + + + + + +
Cháu (+) (+) (+) (+) (+) (+) (+)
Em (+) (+) + + + + + + +
Chị (+) + + +
Anh (+) + + +
Cô + (+) + + + +
Cậu + (+) +
243
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Chú + (+) +
Bác + (+) +
Ông + (+) +
Bà + (+) +
Cụ + (+)
Tôi +
Persons Number
tao mày, mi
ta mi
mình bạn
244
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
/ mình
họ,
chúng tao chúng mày, bây,
e.g.1. Tôi nghe nói bạn sắp đi du học à? (Tôi, bạn: personal pronoun)
e.g.2. Nam ơi, cho mình hỏi cái này một tí. (mình: reflexive pronoun)
e.g. 3. Đằng ấy có rảnh không? (đằng ấy: demonstrative pronoun)
245
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Swedish Du Ni
French Tu Vous
In sociolinguistics, a T–V distinction (from the Latin pronouns tu and vos) is a contrast,
within one language, between various forms of addressing one's conversation partner or partners
that are specialized for varying levels of politeness, social distance, courtesy, familiarity, age or
insult toward the addressee. Languages such as modern English that, outside of certain dialects,
have no morphosyntactic T–V distinction may have semantic analogues to convey the mentioned
attitudes towards the addressee, such as whether to address someone by given or surname, or
whether to use "sir" or "ma'am". Under a broader classification, T and V forms are examples of
honorifics.
The terms T and V, based on the Latin pronouns tu and vos, were first used in a paper by
the social psychologist Roger Brown and the Shakespearian scholar Albert Gilman. This was a
historical and contemporary survey of the uses of pronouns of address, seen as semantic markers
of social relationships between individuals. The study considered mainly French, Italian, Spanish
and German. The paper was highly influential and with few exceptions, the terms T and V have
been used in subsequent studies.
4.3.2. Origin
In Latin, tu was originally the singular, and vos the plural, with no distinction for
honorific or familiar. According to Brown and Gilman, usage of the plural to the Roman emperor
began in the fourth century AD. They mention the possibility that this was because there were
two emperors at that time (in Constantinople and Rome), but also mention that "plurality is a
very old and ubiquitous metaphor for power". This usage was extended to other powerful
figures, such as Pope Gregory I (590–604). However, Brown and Gilman note that it was only
between the twelfth and fourteenth centuries that the norms for the use of T- and V-forms
crystallized. Less commonly, the use of the plural may be extended to other persons, such as the
"royal we" (majestic plural) in English.
Brown and Gilman argued that the choice of form is governed by either relationships of
'power' and/or 'solidarity', depending on the culture of the speakers, showing that 'power' had
been the dominant predictor of form in Europe until the twentieth century. Thus, it was quite
normal for a powerful person to use a T-form but expect a V-form in return. However, in the
twentieth century the dynamic shifted in favour of solidarity, so that people would use T-forms
with those they knew, and V-forms in service encounters, with reciprocal usage being the norm
in both cases.
In the Early Middle Ages (the 5th century to the 10th century), the pronoun vos was used
to address the most exalted figures, emperors and popes, who would use the pronoun tu to
246
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
address a subject. This use was progressively extended to other states and societies, and down
the social hierarchy as a mark of respect to individuals of higher rank, religious authority, greater
wealth, or seniority within a family. The development was slow and erratic, but a consistent
pattern of use is estimated to have been reached in different European societies by the period
1100 to 1500. Use of V spread to upper-class individuals of equal rank, but not to lower class
individuals. This may be represented in Brown and Gilman's notation:
These choices were available not only to reflect permanent relationships, but to express
momentary changes of attitude. This allowed playwrights such as Racine, Molière, Ben Johnson,
Marlowe and Shakespeare to express a character's inner changes of mood through outward
changes of pronoun.
For centuries, it was the more powerful individual who chose to address a subordinate
either with T or with V, or to allow the subordinate to choose. For this reason, the pronouns were
traditionally defined as the pronoun of either condescension or intimacy (T) and the pronoun of
reverence or formality (V). Brown and Gilman argue that modern usage no longer supports these
definitions.
247
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Developments from the nineteenth century have seen the solidarity semantic more
consistently applied. It has become less acceptable for a more powerful individual to exercise the
choice of pronoun. Officers in most armies are not permitted to address a soldier as T. Most
European parents cannot oblige their children to use V. The relationships illustrated above have
changed in the direction of the following norms:
The tendency to promote the solidarity semantic may lead to the abolition of any choice
of address pronoun. During the French Revolution attempts were made to abolish V. In
seventeenth century England the Society of Friends obliged its members to use only T to
everyone, and some continue to use T (thee) to one another. In most Modern English dialects the
choice of T no longer exists outside of poetry.
The Old English and Early Middle English second person pronouns thou and ye (with
variants) were used for singular and plural reference respectively with no T–V distinction. The
earliest entry in the Oxford English Dictionary for ye as a V pronoun in place of the singular thou
exists in a Middle English text of 1225 composed in 1200.[11] The usage may have started among
the French nobility in imitation of French. It made noticeable advances during the second half of
the thirteenth century. During the sixteenth century, the distinction between the subject form ye
and the object form you was largely lost, leaving you as the usual V pronoun (and plural
pronoun). After 1600, the use of ye in standard English was confined to literary and religious
contexts or as a consciously archaic usage .
b-by the upper classes when talking to each other, even if they were closely related
248
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
The T–V distinction was still well preserved when Shakespeare began writing at the end
of the sixteenth century. However, other playwrights of the time made less use of T–V contrasts
than Shakespeare. The infrequent use of T in popular writing earlier in the century such as the
Paston Letters suggest that the distinction was already disappearing from popular speech. In the
first half of the seventeenth century, thou disappeared from Standard English, although the T–V
distinction was preserved in many regional dialects. When the Quakers began using thou again in
the middle of the century, many people were still aware of the old T–V distinction and
responded with derision and physical violence.
In the nineteenth century, one aspect of the T–V distinction was restored to some English
dialects in the form of a pronoun that expressed friendly solidarity, written as y'all. Unlike earlier
thou, it was used primarily for plural address, and in some dialects for singular address as well.
The pronoun was first observed in the southern states of the US among African-American
speakers, although its precise origin is obscure. The pronoun spread rapidly to white speakers in
those southern states, and (to a lesser extent) other regions of the US and beyond. This pronoun
is not universally accepted, and may be regarded as either nonstandard or a regionalism.
Yous(e) (pron. /juːz/, /jəz/) as a plural is found mainly in (Northern) England, Scotland,
Ireland, Australia, New Zealand, South Africa, northern Nova Scotia and parts of Ontario in
Canada and parts of the northeastern United States (especially areas like Boston where there was
historically Irish immigration). It also occurs in Scouse (the regional dialect of the Liverpool
area). Yous(e) as a singular is found in Philadelphia, New York, Boston and scattered throughout
working class Italian-American communities in the American Rust Belt.
Follow-up activity 3.4.True/False: Discuss with your friends and decide whether the
following statements are true or false:
1-Terms of address are the words used to address (call by name or title) in speech or
writing.
249
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
2-According to Crystal (1994a), in English the basic choice ofterms of address is between
first name (FN) or tittle + last name (TLN).
4-Kinship terms are not used in the Vietnamese language as terms of address.
5-Vietnamese people follow the principle of “Xưng khiêm, Hô tôn” (Refer to yourself
with humbleness, address the other with high respect) in addressing.
250
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
251
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
17-Culture provides the context within which words and, more generally, grammatical
systems are interpreted.
18-The context of culture is instantiated in or through more specific contexts of situation.
19-Culture is described as an inherent part of situation.
20-The characteristics of low-context cultures are overt and explicit communication.
21-The characteristics of high-context cultures are line logic, verbal message, explicit and
precise instructions, signs, rules and clear intentions.
22- In collectivism, harmony should be always maintained.
23- In individualistic cultures, high-context communication prevails.
24-In collectivistic cultures, low-context communication prevails.
25- Confusian ideas influence communication in many countries in Asia. They give
guide to harmony and indirectness in communication.
26-The cultural contexts enter through the psychological and social contexts.
27-The more social side of culture enters into communication through social identities of
the communicator.
28-Linguistic communication is always carried on in a context which is, in a large part,
culturally constituted.
31-The order of personal names in English and Vietnamese are the same:
Surname+(Middle name)+Given name.
32-A Given name, in Western Context often referred to as First Name, is a personal name
that specifies and differentiates between members of a group of individuals, especially in a
family, all of whose members usually have the same family name (Surname).
33-Kin(ship) is the term used to denote human relations based on biological decent and
marriage.
35-Matrilineality, also known as the male line, is a common kinship system in which an
individual's family membership derives from and is traced through his or her father's lineage
252
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
36-KinDEEP is a detailed framework for defining kinship terms from different language.
37-The system of English-language kinship terms falls into the Eskimo type.
38-Vietnamese has a very complex system of kinship terminology with a lot of variations
among different regions. Vietnamese kinship is of the Sudanese type, highly descriptive.
40-Age in the Vietnamese kinship system is distinguished by combining with one of the
following words: ông /bà (e.g. ông nội / bà nội), trai / gái (e.g. con trai / con gái), dâu / rể (e.g.
con dâu / con rể).
41-Terms of address are the words used to address (call by name or title) in speech or
writing.
42-A variety of social factors usually governs our choice of terms: the particular
occasion; the social status or rank of the other; gender; age; family relationship; occupational
hierarchy; transactional status (i.e., a service encounter, or a doctor–patient relationship, or one
of priest–penitent); race; or degree of intimacy.
43-According to Crystal (1994, p.44), in English the basic choice is between first name
(FN) or tittle with last name (TLN).
44-Most current and former elected federal and state officials and judges in the U.S. are
styled "The Honorable [full name]" in writing.
45-Sir or Madam used alone are meant to show respect for position and seniority.
46-The asymmetric use of names and address terms is often a clear indicator of a power
differential.
47-Knowing and using another’s first name is a sign of considerable intimacy or at least
of a desire for such intimacy.
48-Professor Nguyen Van Khang states that there exist two notions in Vienamese: Xưng
(self-calling / self-referring / first-person referring) and Hô / gọi (second-person addressing).
49-In general, to show respect in addressing a person in Vietnamese, the given name
must be preceded by a status or kinship term if the addresee is older or of higher status
253
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
1. Ethnography of Communication
4. Politeness Theories
1. ETHNOGRAPHY OF COMMUNICATION
The general aims of this qualitative research method include being able to discern which
communication acts and/or codes are important to different groups, what types of meanings
groups apply to different communication events, and how group members learn these codes, in
order to provide insight into particular communities. This additional insight may be used to
enhance communication with group members, make sense of group members’ decisions, and
distinguish groups from one another, among other things.
Dell Hymes proposed the ethnography of communication as an approach towards
analyzing patterns of language use within speech communities, in order to provide support
254
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
for his idea of communicative competence, which itself was a reaction to Noam Chomsky's
distinction between linguistic competence and linguistic performance.
The term ethnography of communication is meant to be descriptive of the
characteristics that an approach towards language from an anthropological standpoint must take.
Namely, according to Dell Hymes, it must a-"investigate directly the use of language in contexts
of situations so as to discern patterns proper to speech activity" and b-"take as context a
community, investigating its communicative habits as a whole." In other words, rather than
divorcing linguistic form from its function, the analysis of a culture's or community's
communication, linguistic and otherwise, must occur with respect to the socio-cultural context of
its use and the functions of the meanings conveyed.
Ethnography of communication can be used as a means by which to study the
interactions among members of a specific culture or "speech community." Philipsen [2001, as
cited in [159] explains that "Each community has its own cultural values about speaking and
these are linked to judgments of situational appropriateness." The meaning and understanding of
the presence or absence of speech within different communities will vary. Local cultural patterns
and norms must be understood for analysis and interpretation of the appropriateness of speech
act situated within specific communities. Thus, "the statement that talk is not anywhere valued
equally in all social contexts suggests a research strategy for discovering and describing
cultural or subcultural differences in the value of speaking. Speaking is one among other
symbolic resources which are allocated and distributed in social situations according to
distinctive culture patterns"[Philipsen, 2001, as cited in [159].
For our purpose, the term ethnography of speaking/communication is used to refer to
the study of the nature and function of communicative behavior in the context of culture
1.2. Communication
The term communication is difficult to define because it has been used in a variety of
ways for different purposes [81]. Among early communication scholars, communication was
defined as a process of conveying information from one person to another. This process was
differently conceived as one–way, two-way, or circular process of exchange of information or
interactions between a sender, receiver, medium, and message. The objective of the
communication process was information transfer. Such communication was fairly easy because
the information transferred could be simplified by resorting to gestures.
The process of communication has been described by Jakobson [1960, as cited in [64,
pp.15-16] as follows:
255
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
“ The addressor sends a message to the Addressee. To be operative the message requires
a Context referred to, seizable by the addressee, and either verbal or capable of being
verbalized; a Code fully, or at least partially, common to the addresser and addressee;
and finally, a Contact, a physical channel and psychological connection between the
addresser and addressee, enabling both of them to enter and stay in communication.”
According to Reisinger [81, p.166], since the 1960s, communication theories have
defined “communication as the interpretation of meaning through symbols and signs or as
meaning-making process heavily influenced by culture”. Lustig and Koester [1993, as cited in
[81, p.166] summarized these (post)modern definitions into a contemporary definition of
communication as “symbolic, interpretive, transactional, contextual process in which people
create shared meanings.”
256
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
257
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
b-Proxemics
Proxemics deals with the concept of space element in communication. Proxemics
explains four zones of spaces; namely, intimate, personal, social and public. This concept differs
with different culture as the permissible space vary in different countries.
d-Artifactics
Artifactics studies about the non-verbal signals or communication which emerges from
personal accessories such as dresses or fashion accessories worn and it varies with culture as
people of different countries follow different dressing codes.
e-Chronemics
Chronemics deal with the time aspects of communication and also include importance
given to the time. Some issues explaining this concept are pauses, silences and response lag
during an interaction. This aspect of communication is also influenced by cultural differences as
it is well known that there is a great difference in the value given by different cultures to time.
f-Kinesics
Kinesics mainly deals with the body languages such as postures, gestures, head nods, leg
movements etc. In different counties same gestures and postures are used to convey different
messages. Sometimes even a particular kinesic indicating something good in a country may have
a negative meaning in any other culture.
Valdes [104, p.66] states that for simplicity, the nonverbal aspects of communication may
be divided into three classes:
a-body language, comprising movement, gesture, posture, facial expression, gaze, touch, and
distancing;
b-object language, including the use of signs, designs, realia, artifacts, clothing…
258
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Patterning occurs at all levels of communication: societal, group, and individual. At the
societal level, communication usually patterns in terms of its functions, categories of talk, and
attitudes and conceptions about language and speakers. Communication also patterns according
to particular roles and groups within a society, such as gender, age, social status, and occupation:
e.g., a teacher has different ways of speaking from a lawyer, a doctor, or an insurance salesman.
Ways of speaking also pattern according to educational level, rural or urban residence,
geographic region, and other features of social organization. Finally, communication patterns at
the individual level, at the level of expression and interpretation of personality.
Although we have listed societal, group, and individual levels of patterning separately,
there is an invisible web of interrelationships among them, and indeed among all patterns of
culture.
The concern for pattern has always been basic in anthropology, with interpretations of
underlying meaning dependent on the discovery and description of normative structure or design.
More recent emphasis on processes of interactions in generating behavioral patterns extends this
concern to explanation as well as description.
Speech is patterned. For example, in a conversation we have the beginning, body and
end. The conversation in many cases begins by greetings and ends with such routines as good-
bye. The structure of the lesson, interview, conversation can be: Discourse-Transaction-
Exchange-Move-Act. In conversation we use different types of speech acts and the speech acts
would form adjacency pairs. People should know the rules of turns. And we also make use of
non-verbal language. All of the above rules and patterns depend on the socio-cultural contexts.
259
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
been extended to the patterning of a community's entire range of speech situations and speech
events.
According to Crystal [20, p.48], telephone conversations can differ in different countries.
In British English, for example, the normal sequence for a call to private residence is as follows:
260
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
261
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
262
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
263
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
The speech community is the largest descriptive unit. Within a speech community, from
one view constitutive of it, are speech events, locally defined contexts for speaking, each of
which has an internal structure which differentiates it from other events in a community. Hymes
(1962) extended Jakobson's (1960) model of a speech event by increasing the number of
constitutive factors and functions from six to seven. Thus, any speech event is comprised of
seven factors, including minimally, a sender, who sends a message to a receiver. The message
is sent via a physical channel, implying as well some psychological connection between or
among the interlocutors, and is expressed in a code which is at least partially shared by the
sender and receiver. The message is about something, i.e., its topic. And the event occurs in a
particular time and place, its setting. As the factors which make up any act of verbal
communication, these are factors to attend to in describing indigenous speech events and the
speech acts which comprise them.
Corresponding to these factors are seven types of functions. The expressive function
focuses on the attitude which the 'sender' expresses toward what he is speaking about or toward
the situation itself. The directive function, sometimes called the conative or persuasive
264
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
function, focuses on what the sender is asking the 'receiver' to do, in responding to the verbal
message. The poetic function focuses on the form of the 'message,' with particular emphasis on
its artistic or aesthetic value to the interlocutors. Whether contact is established, and whether the
'channel' is opened and maintained between or among interlocutors, is the phatic function, with
emphasis on verbal contact being established or maintained. Whenever the interlocutors turn
their attention to the 'code' itself (or the codes) being used, a metalinguistic function is
performed. A focus on 'topic,' the subject of the verbal communication, signals attention to the
referential function. The 'setting' may be the focus of emphasis in an act of verbal
communication, as when attention turns to the social context or social relationship which forms a
backdrop to the speech event, or which becomes the object of the speech event, as when
interlocutors use speaking to define or redefine their social relationship; in these cases, a
contextual (or situational) function is emphasized. Although all features of a speech event may
participate in all the functions, there may be specifiable linkages of factor and function, to be
investigated in given cases.
Hymes's (1962) framework was proposed tentatively in the hope that it would provide a
basis for empirical studies. The factors in speech events were reformulated in the acronym
SPEAKING, thus: Setting or Scene; Participants; Ends; Act Characteristics, including both
the form and content of the message; Key or Tone of the event; Instrumentalities, including
Channels and Codes; Norms of Interaction and of Interpretation; and Genres.
a-Speech community: a community sharing rules for the conduct and interpretation of
speech
b-Language field and speech field: the total range of communities of a speaker
c-Speech situation: contexts of situation, such as ceremonies, meals, etc., not to be
reduced to types of speech events – because such situations may also have non-verbal
dimensions
d-Speech event: activities governed by rules of speech
e-Speech act
f-Speech style
g-Ways of speaking: the overall terms of capturing diversity of speech
h-Components of speech
e-Rules (relations) of speaking.
265
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Less frequently employed terms for related concepts include language field, speech field,
and speech networks [Hymes, 1972, as cited in [86]. The term language field refers to all those
communities in which an individual is able to communicate adequately by virtue of
knowing the languages and language varieties serving the communities. The concept of
speech field parallels that of language field but involves the knowledge of rules of speaking
rather than knowledge of languages. The last term (speech network) refers to linkages
between persons from different communities who share language varieties as well as rules
for speaking. To give an example, in addition to her mother tongue, a woman knows four
languages well enough to read books and newspapers published in them; a total of five languages
make up her language field. However, the same woman is able to communicate easily in only
one foreign language in addition to her native language; the communities within which she
functions effectively in the two languages make up her speech field. Within that speech field the
woman has special rapport with those persons, regardless of where they may come from, who
share with her the two languages, rules for speech, and a professional interest in, say,
archaeology; the linkages with these people make up her speech network.
266
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
While the SPEAKING model is a valuable model to EOC, as well as the descriptive
framework most commonly used in ethnography of communication, Cameron cautions that
Hymes' model should be used more as a guide than a template, because adhering to it too
narrowly may create a limiting view of the subject of its study. Ethnography of communication,
according to Cameron, should strive not only to address such 'descriptive' questions as 'what
speech events occur in such-and-such a community?' and 'what are the components of speech
events X, Y, and Z?'", but also to explain "why particular events occur and why they have
particular characteristics.”
The communicative events selected initially for description and analysis by one learning
to use this approach should be brief self-contained sequences which have readily identifiable
beginnings and endings. Further, they should be events which recur in similar form and with
some frequency, so that regular patterns will be more easily discernible: e.g. greetings, leave-
takings, prayers, condolences, jokes, insults, compliments, ordering meals in restaurants. More
complex and less regular events yield themselves to analysis more readily after patterns of use
267
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
and norms of interpretation have already been discovered in relation to simpler and more regular
communicative events.
SETTING:
If mid-afternoon with a hot sun overhead, under trees
If in the late afternoon or during evening hours, in the village common place
KEY: Serious
PARTICIPANTS:
All of the male inhabitants of the village
P1 – Chief
P2 – Herald
P3 – Active inhabitants (age 45+)
P4 – Semi-active inhabitants (age 21–45) P5 – Passive inhabitants (age 14 –20)
ACT SEQUENCE:
P1 recites agenda
P2 transmits agenda to assembly
P3 (one) asks for floor
P2 transmits request to P1
P1 grants consent or rejects request
P2 transmits consent or rejection to speaker P3
P3 gives opinion (if P1 consents)
P2 transmits opinion to P1 and assembly
[Acts 3 –8 are repeated as active members (P3s) take turns giving their opinions]
P1 summarizes the debate and makes a proposal
P2 transmits the summary and proposal to the assembly
268
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
NORMS OF INTERPRETATION:
Direct speech (laconic and clear) means the speaker is defending a point.
Indirect speech (e.g. riddles and parables) means the speaker is opposing a point.
The people in the assembly are serious.
The Herald is not necessarily being serious.
Canale and Swain [1980, as cited in [1, pp.26-27] adapted Hymes’s notion of
communicative competence to language teaching concerns. They proposed that communicative
competence has four components:
a-Grammatical competence: the knowledge of correct syntactic and phonological forms
and general vocabulary.
b-Sociolinguistic competence: the knowledge of how to use language appropriately in
different social contexts
c-Strategic competence: the ability to communicate with limited linguistic resource - to
get the meaning across even though one is not fluent in the language.
d-Discourse competence: the ability to comprehend and produce text that is cohesive and
can be understood.
The following outline summarizes the broad range of shared knowledge that is involved
in appropriate communication. From the ethnographer’s perspective, this inventory also indicates
the range of linguistic, interactional, and cultural phenomena which must ultimately be
accounted for in an adequate description and explanation of communicative competence [86, pp.
20-21].
a-Linguistic knowledge
i-Verbal elements
269
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
ii-Nonverbal elements
iii-Patterns of elements in particular speech events
iv-Range of possible variants (in all elements and their organization)
v-Meaning of variants in particular situations
b-Interaction skills
i-Perception of salient features in communicative situations
ii-Selection and interpretation of forms appropriate to specific situ- ations, roles, and
relationships (rules for the use of speech)
iii-Discourse organization and processes
iv-Norms of interaction and interpretation
v-Strategies for achieving goals
c-Cultural knowledge
i-Social structure (status, power, speaking rights)
ii-Values and attitudes
iii-Cognitive maps/schemata
iv-Enculturation processes (transmission of knowledge and skills)
270
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
The major characteristics of this definition include the following concepts: symbolic
exchange, process, different cultural communities, negotiate shared meanings, and an
interactive situation.
The first characteristics, symbolic exchange, refers to the use of verbal and nonverbal
symbols between a minimum of two individuals to accomplish shared meanings.
The second characteristics, process, refers to the independent nature of the intercultural
encounter. Once two cultural strangers make contact and attempt to communicate, they enter
into a mutually interdependent relationship. The concept of process refers to two ideas: the
transactional nature and the irreversive nature of communication. The transactional nature of
intercultural communication refers to the simultaneous encoding (i.e. the sender choosing the
right words or nonverbal gestures to express his or her intentions) and decoding (i.e. the receiver
translating the words or nonverbal cues into comprehensible meanings) of the encoding
messages. When the decoding process of the receiver matches the encoding process of the
sender, the receiver and the sender of the message have accomplished shared content meanings
effectively.
The third characteristics, different cultural communities, is defined as a broad concept.
A cultural community refers to a group of interacting individuals within a bounded unit who
uphold a set of shared traditions and ways of life. This unit can refer to a geographical locale
with clear-cut boundaries such as a nation. Broadly speaking, a cultural community can refer to
a national cultural group, an ethnic group, or a gender group.
The fourth characteristic, negotiate shared meanings, refers to the general goal of any
intercultural communication encounter. In intercultural business negotiations or intercultural
romantic relationships, our first level of concern is that we want our messages to be understood.
The word ”negotiate” connotes the creative give-and-take nature of the fluid process of human
communication. The three layers of meanings that are critical to our understanding of how
people express themselves in a communication process are content meaning, identity meaning,
and relational meaning.
Content meaning refers to the factual information that is being conveyed to the receiver
through an oral channel or other communication medium. When the intended content meaning of
the sender has been accurately decoded by the receiver, the communicators have established a
level of mutually shared content meanings. Identity meaning refers to the following questions:
”Who am I and Who are you in this interaction scene?”. Identity meaning involves issues such as
the display of respect or rejection and is thus much more subtle than overt, content meaning.
The verbal and nonverbal cues, the interaction styles, and the salient identities of the
communicators are part of the identity meaning negotiation process. Relational meaning offers
information concerning the state of the relationship between the two communicators. Relational
meanings are inferred via nonverbal intonations, body movements, or gestures that accompany
the verbal content level. It conveys both power distance (i.e. equal-unequal) meanings and
relational distance (e.g. personal-impersonal) meanings.
271
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
One last characteristic, an interactive situation, refers to the interaction scene of the
dyadic encounter. An interactive scene includes both the concrete features and psychological
features of a setting. Every communication episode occurs in an interactive situation. Burgoon,
Buller, and Woodall [1996, as cited in [99] conclude that an interactive situation typically
includes the following gestalt components:
i-Elements of behaviour: These are the specific verbal and nonverbal behaviours that occur
in a situation.
ii-Goals or motivations of the participants. For example, is this a business get-acquainted
situation or a business negotiation situation?
iii-Rules of behaviour: The rules for getting acquainted differ from a baigaining /
concession-seeking negotiation situation.
iv-Different roles that people must play: individuals have different prescribed roles to play in
different interactive situations.
v-The physical setting and equipment: For, example, a classroom environment with
chalkboard and straight-row seating is different from an office environment with a desk, file
cabinets, and personal objects.
vi-Cognitive concepts: The psychological features of the situation such as the public -private
dimension, formal informal dimension, task-social dimension, competitive-cooperative
dimension.
vii-Relevant social skills: Appropriate and effective skills are needed to achieve interaction
goals in the situations.
The interpretation that we attach to various components of an interactive situation are
strongly influenced by the meanings we attach to these components. We acquire the meanings to
these situational components via the primary socialization process within our own culture. For
example, whether we define different room in our home environment as “public” or “private”
spaces (reserved for guests or family members) can vary tremendously from one culture to the
next. Furthermore, our expectations of what interactions scripts (i.e. patterns of communication
or activities) and how interaction sequences should be carried out are highly culturally and
situationally based.
1.12.1.2. Intercultural communication and cross-cultural communication
The term cross-cultural communication is typically used to refer to the study of a
particular idea or concept within many cultures. The goal of such investigations is to conduct a
series of intracultural analyses in order to compare one culture with another on the attributes of
interest. For example, someone interested in studying the marriage rituals in many cultures
would be considered a cross-cultural researcher. Scholars who study self-disclosure patterns,
child-rearing practices, or educational methods as they exist in many different cultures are doing
cross-cultural comparisons. Whereas intercultural communication involves interactions among
272
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
273
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
within a culture and in a particular interaction situation. The identity negotiation perspective
emphasizes eight identity domains in influencing our everyday interactions as presented in the
following table:
Table 4.3. Identity negotiation perspective: eight identity domains [99, p. 29]
Primary Identities Cultural identity
Ethnic identity
Gender identity
Personal identity
Situational Identities Role identity
Relational identity
Facework identity
Symbolic interaction identity
274
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
cultural or ethnic differences, and the preparedness to experiment with creative avenues of
decision making and problem solving. The concept of mindfulness can serve as the first
effective step in integrating our theoretical knowledge with the identity-based outcome
dimensions.
To be mindful communicators, individuals need to learn the value systems that influence
the others’ self-conceptions. They need to be open to a new way of identity construction. They
neeed to be prepared to perceive and understand a behaviour or a problem from others’ cultural
and personal stand-points. Mindful communicators need to be on the alert that multiple
perspectives typically exist in interpreting a basic phenomenon.
Mindful intercultural communication is defined as the process and outcome of how two
dissimilar individuals negotiate shared meanings and achieve desired outcomes through
appropriate and effective behaviours in an intercultural situation. The components, criteria and
outcomes of the mindful intercultural communication are as follows:
Table 4.4. A mindful intercultural communication model: Components, criteria, and
outcomes [99, p.49]:
Components Criteria Outcomes
Knowledge Factors
M Cultural/personal values, language &
verbal communication, in-group
I
&out-group boundary, relationship
N development, conflict management, Appropriateness Being Understood
intercultural adaptation
F
Motivational Factors
U Effectiveness Being Respected
Mindful of identity domains, mindful
L of identity needs, mindful of
N ethnocentric tendencies Satisfaction Being Supported
E Skill Factors
The identity negotiation theory emphasizes two ideas: the first is that mindful
intercultural communication has three components – knowledge, motivation, skills; the second is
that mindful intercultural communication refers to the appropriate, effective, and satisfactory
management of desired shared meanings and goals in an intercultural episode.
275
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Follow-up activity 4.1. True/False: Discuss with your friends and decide whether the
following statements are true or false:
1-The nature and function of communicative behaviour in the context of culture is the
subject of ethnography of communication.
3-Units of speech behaviour are speech situation, speech event, and speech act.
5-In the interpretation of speech, SPEAKING factors (Setting and Scene, Participant,
Ends, Act sequence, Key, Instrumentality, Norm, Genre) are important.
2. 1. Talk
276
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Categories of talk in speaking may vary in different languages. However, the main
categories of talk in English include: conversation, lecture, oratory, gossip, joking, story-telling,
and preaching.
Categories of talk in each language have different functional distribution, and most are
limited to a particular situation, or involves constraints on who may speak them, or what topic
may be addressed, who initiates the talk… Their description is thus of interest not only because
of the linguistic phenomena which distinguish one from another, but also because these
categories may provide clues to how other dimensions of the society are segmented and
organized. The following table gives partial answer to the question:
277
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Speech is patterned and rule-governed. Speaking can be described in terms of rules and
system. Choices as to which language or varieties of language to use in a particular situation,
how to address an interlocutor, whether to speak or to remain silent, are not free to variation, but
are patterned, according to the rules which are part of the social knowledge of a particular
community.
To explain speaking activities requires reference to the settings, participants, ends, act
sequences, topics, and so forth, which comprises the social situation. This goes beyond the rules
of language structure to a consideration of rules specifying who may say what to whom, in what
language or style, to what ends, and on what occasions.
We may have to consider choice of written or spoken language, genres, address terms,
choice of language styles, choice of register, choice of appropriate speech acts in different speech
situations or speech events. The choice is culture-specific and context-dependent. In other
words, we should have appropriate speech behaviour in different contexts and situations.
Speech is patterned. For example, in a conversation we have the beginning, body and
end. The conversation in many cases begins by greeting and ends with such routines as good-
bye. The generic structures of spoken discourse in general, and of conversations in particular,
can differ according to the communicative purposes, discourse types and cultures. Although the
generic structures of spoken discourse differ, the basic pattern of a spoken discourse would be
the same: Discourse-Transaction-Exchange-Move-Act. This is the basic hierarchical pattern
of a spoken discourse.
In speaking we have to obey many rules: co-operative principle, the politeness rules,
rules of face-saving, rules of turn-taking and many others. These rules can differ from culture to
culture. Culture helps determine the appropriate communicative behavior within a variety of
social and physical contexts by prescribing certain rules. When communicating with members of
278
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
our own culture, we rely on deeply internalized cultural rules that delineate the normative
behaviours for specific communication situations. These rules facilitate our ability to
communicate effectively with each other. And since they are integrated into our personality, we
do not have to think consciously about which rules to use. There are three basic assumptions
about human communication that apply directly to communication [84, pp.311]: a-
communication is rule-governed, b-context prescribes appropriate communication rules, and c-
communication rules are culturally diverse
2.2. Silence
Silence, perhaps because it seems the antithesis of linguistic form, has long been
neglected in the study of language. Nevertheless, it forms an essential part of communication
and speech communities differ as much as the uses and interpretations they give to silence as
they do in regard to the linguistic forms that they use. Silence is thus better seen as the
complement to sound; an awareness of its potential functions, structures and meaning, therefore,
become relevant to the study of linguistic communication.
279
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
this patterning is determined by the institutions of a specific society, and the functional meaning
of silence can only be understood in relation to particular institutional contexts. These contexts
may be as various as physical locations, ritual performances, or the enactment of social roles /
relationships: Communication may be proscribed, for example, between a commoner and a
chief, or a man and his mother-in-law, while membership in certain religious groups may require
a vow of silence.
Many societal patterns of silence are also determined by members of a group in relation
to dynamics of social organization. Patterns may be situational as when access to speaking
privilege in public forums is allocated by group decision and others must remain silent, or
normative, as when differential speaking privileges are allocated to individuals or classes of
individuals.
The amount of talk versus silence that is prescribed is closely tied to social values and
norms. The relative value of talk or silence in a society may be partly inferred from whether one
or the other is ascribed to its rulers, priests, and sages. The value of silence may also be found in
proverbs: e.g., ‘Silence is golden’ (English), ‘Because of the mouth, the fish dies” (Spanish); ‘The
way your eyes look can say more than your mouth’ (Japanese); ‘Man becomes wise through the
ear’ (Persian).
280
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
related to participants’ purposes and needs. Bruneau (1973) terms these psycholinguistic and
interactive silences, which include an array of functions ranging from defining the role of
auditor in a communicative exchange, to providing social control, to demonstrating deference, to
indicating emotional closeness, to managing personal interaction. Jensen (1973) presents a
similar array, categorizing functions as linkage, affecting, revelational, judgemental and
activating.
Components of the interpretive frame that defines interactional silence include the
physical and temporal setting, the type (or genre) of event, its general purpose and topic, the
identity and relationship of participants, and the textual sequence (what has preceded and what is
expected to follow). This interpretive frame is situated within the larger context of societal
institutions, beliefs, and norms. Noninteractional silence may merely indicate absence of
activity, but it too is imbued with conventional meaning when it is the frame for such
contemplative and meditative events as that invoked in the Judeo Christian call to worship: The
Lord is in His holy temple; let all the earth keep silence before Him. Other sociocultural
functions include marking boundaries of events and serving as a background against which
speech and other activity is interpreted.
Some interactional functions of silence may be viewed as primarily sociocontextual in
nature: defining (e.g. status and role), structuring (e.g. situations), tactical (e.g.
nonparticipation, avoidance, disapproval, mitigation, image manipulation), and phatic
(emotional sharing); some as primarily linguistic: discursive (e.g. prayer, fantasizing,
rehearsing) or propositional (e.g. negation, affirmation, refusal, acknowledgement); and some as
primarily psychological (e.g. expression of anger, sorrow, embarrassment, joy, or fear). Some
noninteractional functions of silence involve contemplative / mediatative states, while others are
inactive in nature.
281
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
to remain silent by being denied speaking rights, their silence becomes a sign of oppression, and
may even, to the discerning observer, ‘speak louder than words.’
2.2.5. Functions of silence [72]:
A basic distinction should be made between silences which carry meaning, but not
propositional content, and silence communicative acts which carry their own illocutionary force.
The former includes the pauses and hesitations that occur within and between turns of talking –
the prosodic dimension of silence. The latter is used as a kind of speech act and conveys either
emotional or propositional content. In different patternings (social, special, small group and
individual), silence can be used with many functions as dicussed above. Now, we will discuss
different functions of silence from Nakene (2007)’s views.
282
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
c-Conversational styles through pause length, frequency, speed of talk and overlapping:
The literature also discusses how silence serves to form conversational styles. Tannen
(1985) demonstrated how features of discourse such as preference of overlap to silent switching
pauses and relatively fast rate of speech characterise the conversational style of New York
Jewish people. Her analysis of interaction over a Thanksgiving dinner among three New
Yorkers, two Californians and one Briton showed that the two groups had different
conversational styles characterized by different levels of tolerance of silence. Moreover, as
Scollon & Scollon (1981, 19830, and Scollon (1985) argue, different orientations to silence can
become a cause of negative stereotyping.
In performing a Face Threatening Act, when the risk of threat to face is too great, one
may decide not to perform that FTA at all, and this is called the strategy of ‘Don’t do the FTA’.
Therefore, the assumption is that silence would be the equivalent of this ‘don’t do the FTA’
strategy.
Sifianou (1997) argues that silence can be used as a positive politeness strategy when it
functions as a sign of solidarity and good rapport, while it can also be a negative politeness
strategy if it functions as a distancing tactic. In addition, it is also possible to use silence as an
off-record strategy when it functions as the most indirect form of speech act.
Silence as a politeness strategy can also be used in communication by people who have a
limited verbal communication capacity. Jaworski & Stephens (1988) revealed that avoidance of
talk was used not only to avoid loss of face due to their inability to capture the speech content
but also to avoid imposition on others by requiring them to repeat their speech.
283
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Follow-up activity 4.2. True/False: Discuss with your friends and decide whether the
following statements are true or false:
2-In conversation, there are usually the beginning, body and end of the conversation.
284
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
3-In speaking people do not have to obey the rule of turn-taking, that is, all speakers can
speak at the same time.
4-At the societal level, patterning of silence occurs in relation to expression and
interpretation of personality and to microfunctions related to participants’s purposes and needs.
5-Rules for appropriate interpretation and production of speech include knowing the
properties relating to silence.
1-How do you greet, introduce, or thank (formally and informally) in English and
Vietnamese? What factors help you choose what to say in those situations?
Routines must be learned, as well as analyzed, as single units, although they may vary in
length from single syllables (e.g. Hi) to phrases (e.g. How do you do?) to a sequence of
sentences. They may be uttered by an individual, or may require cooperation between two or
more persons, as in a greeting sequence or in minister / congregation alteration in the reading of
scriptures.
Understanding routines requires shared cultural knowledge because they are generally
metaphoric in nature, and must be interpreted at a non-literal level. They include greetings, leave
takings, curses, jokes, condolences, prayers, compliments, and other formlaic languages.
285
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
3.1.2. Uses
a-to ease interaction
b-to establish group membership
c-to reinforce group identity
d-to indicate attitude
e-to make conventional comment / response
3.1.3. Characteristics
a-Routines are highly ritualized: ritual forms are used in relation to cultural or social
practices, such as baptism, funerals, confessional meal times, rememberance services, wedding
initiation ceremonies, cleasing rites, oaths, vows, and the blessing of people, objects in places.
The language of the routines is fixed, and the linguistic formulae themselves are expected to
exert some control over the supernatural.
b-Routines are idiomatic. The meanings of routines are interpreted in the context of the
meaning of the ritual situation.
c-Routines are reccurent sequences
d-Routines are predictable exchanges
e-Routines are accompanied with non-verbal behaviour
f-Routines are formulaic or conventional behavior
g-Perhaps the most important characteristic of routines and ritual is that truth value is
largely irrelevant. Their meaning is dependent on shared beliefs and values of the speech
community coded into communicative patterns, and they can not be interpreted apart from social
and cultural context.
286
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
3.1.6.2. in discussion
a-to attract the listener’s attention/ to intterupt the communication / to agree, disagree,
evaluate, thank,
b-for leave-taking, closings
c-for preparing for leave-taking
d-leave-taking
3.1.7. The importance of the study of linguistic routines in the ethnography of communication
3.1.7.1. Linguistic Routines show the nature and function of the ethnography of communication
3.1.7.2. The study helps us to understand that:
a-Routines occur in every part of communication
b-Linguistic routines are determined by cultural rules.
Magical incantations provide one example of ritual: the language is fixed, and the
linguistic formulae themselves are expected to exert some control over the supernatural. Parts of
a spell have no meaning uttered by themselves; the whole must always be recited in full to have
effect. Paralinguistic features of production are clearly differentiated from “normal” language,
with spells often recited in a sing-song manner, and with distinctive rhythm and pitch.
Comparable to the sing-song of magical incantation, intoned speech is common for
expressing grief, and both intoned speech and chanting are often used in religious rituals. These
varieties of language are on a speech-song continuum, with the song end of the continuum used
in more formal contexts.
As routines often mark the boundaries of speech events by opening and closing them,
rituals serve as boundary markers for major changes in social status: puberty rites, weddings,
287
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
funerals, and graduation ceremonies. Perhaps the most important characteristic of routines and
rituals is that truth value is largely irrelevant. Their meaning is dependent on shared beliefs and
values of the speech community coded into communicative patterns, and they cannot be
interpreted apart from social and cultural context.
Follow-up activity 4.3. True/False: Discuss with your friends and decide whether the
following statements are true or false:
4-Linguistic routines show the nature and function of the ethnography of communication.
4. POLITENESS THEORIES
Pre-lecture activity 4.4. Discuss:
1-How do you understand the term politeness?
2-Give examples of polite expressions in English and Vietnamese.
According to Kasper [54, p.3206], in ordinary language use, politeness refers to proper
social conduct and tactful consideration of others. Politeness in this non-technical sense
contrasts with rudeness. What counts as polite in any given context is socially and historically
determined. For example, the lexical li in Chinese was connected with notion of politeness and
underwent important semantic changes. In Confucius’s writing (551-479 BC), li referred to the
slavery-based social hierarchy of the Zhou dynasty. The modern sense of li is equated with the
demonstration of self-denigration and respect for the other person, especially in vertical
relationships. In English, the notion of polite dates back to the 15th century meaning polished.
In the seventeenth century, a polite person was of refined courteous manners. Politeness was
thus associated with the norms of social conduct extant in the upper class.
In the following part we will examine Fraser [28]’s four views of politeness: a-the social
norm view, b-the conversational maxim view, c-the face-saving view and d-the conversational
contract view.
4.1. The social norm view
The social norm view is one reflected in ordinary language use: proper social conduct and
tactful consideration of others. According to Fraser [28], the social norm view reflects the
historical understanding of politeness generally embraced by the public within the English-
288
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
speaking world. Briefly stated, it assumes that each society has a particular set of social norms
consisting of more or less explicit rules that prescribe a certain behavior, a state of affairs, or a
way of thinking in a context. A positive evaluation (politeness) arises when an action is in
congruence with a norm, a negative evaluation (impoliteness=rudeness) arises when the action is
to the contrary. This normative view historically considers politeness to be associated with “good
manners” of speech. Politeness in this view is equivalent to a normative notion of
appropriateness [78, p.679]. Being polite is defined as having or showing good manners and
respect for the feelings of others [47, p.1122]. Politeness involves contributing to social
harmony and avoiding conflict [45, p.285].
4.2. The conversational-maxim view
4.2.1. Grice ‘s cooperative principle and conversational implicatures
The conversational-maxim perspective relies principally on the work of Grice [1975,
ascited in [28]. In an attempt to clarify how it is that speakers can mean more than they say,
Grice argued that conversationalists are rational individuals who are, all the other things being
equal, primarily interested in the efficient conveying of the message. To this end, he proposed his
general Cooperative Principle which provides that you should “make your contribution such as
required, at the stage at which it occurs, by the accepted purpose or direction of talk exchange in
which you are engaged.” To do so, the conversationalists have to obey one or all of the four
conversational maxims in conversation: quality, quantity, relevance and manner. Grice assumes
that the Cooperative Principle is always observed and that any real or apparent violations of the
maxims will signal conversational implicatures: the implicit messages intended by the speaker to
be inferred by the hearer. The intentional violation of a conversational maxim may be accepted
as signaling certain speaker intentions. The conversational implicatures help the
conversationalists to be polite and not lose his/her face in generating intended implied messages
to be inferred by the listeners. Being polite here means intentionally generating conversational
implicatures for the listeners to infer in conversation.
4.2.2. Lakoff (1973)’s rules of politeness
Lakoff (1973) was among the first to adopt Grice’s construct of conversational principle
in an effort to account for politeness. She sees politeness to be the avoidance of offense. In one
of her work, she refers to politeness as “a device used in order to reduce friction in personal
interaction” [Lakoff, 1979, as cited in [28]. Lakoff (1973) suggests two rules of Pragmatic
Competence:
a-Be clear (essentially Grice’s maxim)
b-Be polite
She takes these to be in opposition to each other, and notes that they are at times
reinforcing, at other times in conflict. In addition she posits 3 sub-maxims as follows:
Rule 1: Don’t impose (used when formal / impersonal politeness is required)
Rule 2: Give options (used when informal politeness is required)
289
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Rule 3: Make A (the hearer) feel good (used when intimate politeness is required)
Each of these rules are oriented to make the hearer feel good.
4.2.3. Leech (1983)’s politeness principle
The most comprehensive proposal of a politeness principle was formulated by Leech
(1983). The position of Leech (1983) is a grand elaboration of the conversational maxim
approach to politeness. Like Lakoff, Leech adopts the framework initially set out by Grice:
there exists a set of maxims and sub-maxims that guide and constrain the conversation of rational
people. He opts to treat politeness within the domain of a rhetoric pragmatics: his account of
goal-directed linguistic behavior.
Leech’s principle of politeness, adopted here can be stated as the following: “Other
things being equal, minimize the expression of beliefs which are unfavourable to the hearer and
at the same time (but less important) maximize the expression of beliefs which are favourable to
the hearer” [as cited in [28].
Like Grice, Leech provides a finer differentiation within his principles. He proposes six
Interpersonal Maxims:
Table 4.6. Leech’s (1983) politeness maxims and sub-maxims [Leech, 1983, as cited in [7,
p.48]
Maxim Positive politeness Negative politeness
Tact Maxim Minimize hearer costs Maximize hearer benefit.
Generosity maxim Minimize your own benefit Maximize your hearer's benefit
Approbation Maxim Minimize hearer dispraise Maximize hearer praise.
Modesty maxim Minimize self-praise Maximize self-dispraise
Agreement Maxim Minimize disagreement Maximize agreement between
between yourself and others yourself and others.
290
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
[99, p.75]. Face, in the Chinese context, means projected social image and social self-respect.
Group harmony, and thus in-group interdependence, is achieved through the maintainance of
everyone’s face in the society and trying hard not to cause anyone to lose face. Goffman [34]
went on to introduce the Chinese concept of face into academia through his theories of face and
facework. Although politeness has been studied in a variety of centuries for many years, Brown
and Levinson’s politeness theory has become very influential. In 1987, Brown and Levinson
proposed that politeness was a universal concept [9]. It is the expression of the speakers’
intention to mitigate face threats carried out by certain face-threatening act toward the speaker
and the listener. Being polite can be an attempt for the speaker to save his/her own face or the
face of whom he/she is talking to.
Politeness theory accounts for the redressing of affronts to a person’s face by face-
threatening acts. Goffman [34] defines face as being “the positive social value a person
effectively claims for himself by the line others assume he has taken during a particular contact.
Face is an image of self-delineated in terms of social attributes.” Face is best understood as an
individual’s feeling of self-worth or self-image [9]
Face, in Goffman [34] and Brown and Levinson [9]’s concepts, is an individual’s
publicly manifest self-esteem: a public self-image. Brown and Levinson [9] sub-divided face
into positive face and negative face, which can be summarized as follows:
a-Negative face: the want of every “competent adult member” that his/her action be
unimpeded by others.
b-Positive face: the want of every member that his/her wants be desirable to at least some
others.
According to Brown and Levinson [9], members of a given society treat face not as
norms or values which members of that society subscribes to, but as basic wants which every
member of a society, on some level, knows every other member desires, and which in general are
in the interests of every other member to be (at least) partially satisfied.
Face is the public self-image that every person tries to protect. Brown and Levinson [9]
defined positive face two ways: as "the want of every member that his wants be desirable to at
least some others executors" [9, p.62], or alternatively, "the positive consistent self- image or
'personality' (crucially including the desire that this self-image be appreciated and approved of)
claimed by interactants" [9, p.61]. Negative face was defined as "the want of every 'competent
adult member' that his actions be unimpeded by others"[9, p.62], or "the basic claim to
territories, personal preserves, rights to non-distraction—i.e. the freedom of action and freedom
from imposition" [9, p.62]. Whereas positive face involves a desire for connection with others,
negative face needs include autonomy and independence.
Ten years later, Brown characterized positive face by desires to be liked, admired,
ratified, and related to positively, noting that one would threaten positive face by ignoring
someone [183]. At the same time, she characterized negative face by the desire not to be imposed
upon, noting that negative face could be impinged upon by imposing on someone. Positive face
291
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
refers to one's self-esteem, while negative face refers to one's freedom to act. These two aspects
of face are the basic wants in any social interaction; during any social interaction, cooperation is
needed amongst the participants to maintain each other's face. Participants can do this by using
positive politeness and negative politeness, which pay attention to people's positive and negative
face needs respectively
As seen, social members are endowed with two kinds of face: negative face, the want of
self-determination and positive face, the want of approval. Face is something which might be
lost, saved, maintained and improved through interaction with others. Therefore, in
communication, we should act so that we do not lose our face and the person whom we are
talking to does not lose his / her face. There is a working hypothesis that face is constantly at
risk. Consequently, any kind of linguistic act which has a relational dimension is seen as
inherently face-threatening, and needs to be counterbalanced by appropriate doses of politeness.
These doses of politeness are called politeness strategies.
4.3.2. Face-threatening acts (FTA)
Face is something that can be lost, maintained, or enhanced, and any threat to face must
be continually monitored during an interaction. And, since face is so vulnerable, and since most
participants will defend their face if threatened, the assumption is made that it is generally in
everyone's best interest to maintain each other's face and to act in such ways that others are made
aware that this is one's intention.
The organizing principle for their politeness theory is the idea that some acts are
intrinsically threatening to face and thus require softening. To this end, each group of language
users develops politeness principles from which they derive certain linguistic strategies. It is by
the use of these so-called politeness strategies that speakers succeed in communicating both their
primary message(s) as well as their intention to be polite in doing so. And in doing so, they
reduce the face loss that results from the interaction.
Whereas Leech proposes that certain types of acts are inherently polite or impolite,
Brown & Levinson [9] propose that such acts are inherently face-threatening to the speaker, to
the hearer, or to both. In their terminology, such acts are called Face-threatening acts or FTAs.
They propose the following four-way analysis: a-Acts threatening to the hearer's Negative Face:
(e.g. ordering, advising, threatening, warning) b-Acts threatening to the hearer's Positive Face
(e.g. complaining, criticizing, disagreeing, raising taboo topics), c-Acts threatening to the
speaker's Negative Face (e.g. accepting an offer, accepting thanks, promising unwillingly), and
d-Acts threatening to the speaker's Positive Face (e.g. apologizing, accepting compliments,
confessing).
Thus, a face-threatening act is an act that inherently damages the face of the addressee or
the speaker by acting in opposition to the wants and desires of the other. Face-threatening acts
can be verbal (using words/language), paraverbal (conveyed in the characteristics of speech such
as tone, inflection, etc.), or non-verbal (facial expression, etc.). Based on the terms of
conversation in social interactions, face-threatening acts are at times inevitable. At minimum,
292
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
there must be at least one of the face-threatening acts associated with an utterance. It is also
possible to have multiple acts working within a single utterance.
4.3.3. Brown and Levinson [9] politeness strategies
Politeness strategies are used to formulate messages in order to save the hearer's
positive face when face-threatening acts are inevitable or desired. Brown and Levinson [9]
suggest that certain illocution acts inherently threaten either aspect of the face of another person.
In their terminology, such acts are called Face-threatening acts or FTAs. They propose five
super strategies for mitigating FTAs (facework):
293
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
294
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
By using any of the innumerable ways to convey in-group membership, S can implicitly
claim the common ground with H that is carried by that definition of the group. These include in
group usages of address forms, of language or dialect, of jargon or slang, and of ellipsis.
Address forms: Other address forms used to convey such in-group membership include
generic names and terms of address like Mate, honey, dear, babe, mom, brother, sister, cutie,
sweetheart, guys. Using such in group kinds of address forms with imperatives. For example:
“Come here, honey” indicates that S considers the relative P (power, status difference) between
himself and the addressee to be small thus softening the imperative by indicating that it isn’t a
power-backed command.
Use of in-group language or dialect: Another type of code-switching phenomenon is the
switch in English into a spurious dialect, or a dialect not normally used by S or H, to soften an
FTA or turn it into a joke.
Use of jargon or slang: Use brand names in a request may stress that S and H share an
(in-group) reliance on the required object.
Contraction and Ellipsis: S and H must share some knowledge about the context that
makes the utterance understandable (for example that S and H are cooperating in building a
house and S has the hammer in his hand).
Strategy 5: Seek Agreement (safe topics, repetition):
Safe topics: The raising of ‘safe topics’ allows S to stress his agreement with H and
therefore to satisfy H’s desire to be ‘right’, or to be corroborated in his opinions.
Repetition: Agreement may also be stressed by repeating part or all of what the
preceding S has said in the conversation and by using that function to indicate emphatic
agreement (‘yes’, ‘Really’, etc) whenever someone is telling story.
e.g. A:There was flood in my hometown.”
B: Oh my God. Flood!
Positive Strategy 6. Avoid disagreement (token agreement, pseudo-agreement, white
lies, hedging opinions):
Token agreement: S may go in twisting their utterances so as to appear to agree or to
hide disagreement-to respond to a preceding utterance with ‘yes, but…..' in effect, rather than a
blatant ‘No’.
e.g. A:How the girl looked like, beautiful?
B:Yes, I think she is quite, but not really beautiful, she is certainly not really ugly.
Pseudo-agreement: Another example of apparent or pseudo-agreement is found in
English in the use of then as a conclusory marker.
e.g.I’ll meet you in front of the theatre just before 8.0, then.
295
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
White lies: S may do white lie to hide disagreement. By doing this, S is saving H’s face.
e.g. In response to a request to borrow a car, “Oh I can’t, my father will use it tonight.”
Hedging opinions: To soften FTA of suggesting, criticizing or complaining, hedges may
also be used.
e.g. I know you are sort of a polite person.
Strategy 7: Presuppose/ raise/ assert common ground (gossip, small talk, point of view
operations, presupposition manipulations).
Gossip or small talk: S is talking about unrelated topics to show that S is interested in H
as the mark of friendship and does not come only to impose him.
e.g. You look so bright today. It must be because MU had defeated Chelsea, right? By the
way, can you take me to the airport this afternoon?
Point-of-view operations: S may claim common ground by using cooperation point of
view. (S speaks as if H were S, or H’s knowledge were equal to S’s knowledge).
e.g. I had a really hard time learning to drive, didn’t I?
Presupposition manipulations: S presupposes something when he presumes that it is
mutually taken for granted.
e.g. Wouldn’t you like a drink?
Positive Strategy 8: Jokes
Jokes can be used to stress the fact that there must be some mutual background
knowledge and values that S and H share. That is why, the strategy of joking may be useful in
diminishing the social distance between S and H.
e.g. OK if I tackle those cookies now?
How about lending me this old heap of junk? (H’s new Cadillac)
b-Convey that S and H are co-operators
Positive Strategy 9: Assert or Presuppose S’s knowledge of and concerns for H’s wants.
It is the way to indicate that S and H are co-operators, and thus potentially to put pressure
on H to cooperate with S. S wants to assert and imply knowledge of H’s wants and willingness to
fit one’s own wants in with them.
e.g. I understand you can do it yourself, but this time, do what I suggested you.
Positive Strategy 10: Offer or promises
296
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
S and H are good co-operators that they share some goals or S is willing to help to
achieve those goals. Promise or offer demonstrates S’s good attention in satisfying H’s positive-
face wants, even if they are false.
e.g. I’ll go there sometimes.
Positive Strategy 11: Be optimistic
S assumes that H wants S’s wants for S (or for S and H) and will help to obtain them.
This usually happens among people with close relationship.
e.g. You’ll tell your father that you did it, I hope.
Positive Strategy 12: Include both S and H in the activity
Here, S manipulates the subject of an activity is done together. S uses an inclusive ‘we’
from when S actually means ‘you’ or ‘me’. Inclusive form ‘we’ is usually used in the
construction ‘let’s’.
e.g. Bring us the book. (i.e. me)
Let’s go downtown, uh? (i.e you)
Positive Strategy 13: Give (or ask for) reasons
S uses H as the reason why S wants something so that it will seem reasonable to the
hearer. S assumes (via optimism) that there are no good reasons why H should not or cannot
cooperate.
e.g.Why not lend me your car for the weekend?
Positive Strategy 14: Assume or assert reciprocity
S asks H to cooperate with him by giving evidence of reciprocal rights or obligations
between S and H. Thus, S may say, in effect, “I’ll do x for you if you can do y for me”.
e.g. I’ll tell you what it looks like if you tell me where she is now.
c-Fulfil H’s want for some X
Positive Strategy 15: Give gifts to H (goods, sympathy, understanding, cooperation)
S satisfies H’s Positive Face want by giving gift, not only tangible gifts, but human
relation wants which are the wants to be liked, admired, cared about, understood, listened to, etc.
in other words, this strategy is usually used for the benefit of H.
e.g. I’m sorry to hear that.
4.3.3.3. Negative politeness strategies
Negative politeness strategies are oriented towards the hearer's negative face and emphasize
avoidance of imposition on the hearer. By attempting to avoid imposition from the speaker, the
297
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
risk of face-threat to the hearer is reduced. The FTA is performed utilising strategies oriented
towards redressing the negative face threat to the hearer. Negative politeness strategies suggest
distance by accentuating the hearer’s right to territorial claims and freedom from imposition
(referred to as deference strategy). The linguistic output strategies include [9, pp.129–211]:
a-Be direct
Negative politeness strategy 1: Be conventionally indirect
In this strategy, the speaker is being indirect and on-record to communicate the FTA.
e.g. Can you please pass me the salt?
This example shows that the speaker avoids being too direct through phrase “Can you
please?” as a device of indirectness. Consequently, the speaker can avoid imposing the hearer
by not being too direct.
e.g.Would you know where Oxford Street is?
b-Don’t presume / assume
Negative politeness strategy 2: Question, hedge (hedge on illocutionary force,
prosodic/kinesic hedges):
In this strategy, the speaker can use hedges or questions to show negative politeness. The
use of hedges is to modify the level of predicate or noun phrase in which the level of predicate is
partial, or true in some particular aspects, or more true and complete than what expected by the
interactants.
e.g. I’m pretty sure I’ve read that book before.
I rather think it’s hopeless
She might hate you for what you have done
The words pretty, rather, and might function as hedges to indicate the speaker does not
presume something. In this case, the speaker makes the utterance be a little vague.
e.g. Perhaps, he might have taken it, maybe.
Could you please pass the rice?
c-Don’t coerce H
Negative politeness strategy 3: Be pessimistic
The speaker can redress the FTA on the hearer’s negative face by expressing something
doubtfully.
e.g. You could possibly lend me your lawnmower, could you?
298
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
By using negative form, the speaker can be polite by being pessimistic in performing an
FTA. In this case, the speaker assumes that the hearer probably will not do something for him.
Thus, the coercion toward the hearer can be minimized.
e.g.You couldn't find your way to lending me a thousand dollars, could you?
Negative politeness strategy 4: Minimize the imposition, Rx
In communication, the speaker can impose the hearer when asking the hearer to do
something. Therefore, the speaker has to be aware of Rx (the intrinsic of seriousness of
imposition) as considered polite.
e.g. I just want to ask you if you could lend me a single sheet of paper?
The example shows the word “just” is slightly restricting the extent of FTA that means
“only” in which the speaker takes account of the hearer’s negative face and avoids coercing the
hearer.
e.g. It's not too much out of your way, just a couple of blocks.
Negative politeness strategy 5: Give deference
By treating the hearer as superior, the speaker may show deference to the hearer. In
English, it can be done by referent honorific (T/V) that is associated with the hearer such as eat /
dine, man/gentleman, give/bestow that may give greater honour to the person, thing or activity.
e.g. We look forward very much to dining with you.
The example shows that the word dining as referent honorific may give deference to the
hearer. Giving deference is a strategy to show negative politeness.
d-Communicate ‘S want to not imposition, Rx
Negative politeness strategy 6: Apologize (admit the impingement, indicate reluctance,
give overwhelming reasons, beg forgiveness)
Apology can be used to omit an impingement between speaker and hearer. It can be
expressed by admitting the impingement, indicating reluctance, giving overwhelming reasons,
and giving begging forgiveness.
e.g. I am sorry to bother you. I want to borrow some money from you.
By aplologizing for doing an FTA, the speaker can indicate his reluctance to impinge on
addressee’s negative face and thereby partially redress the impingement.
e.g. I'm sorry; it's a lot to ask, but can you lend me a thousand dollars?
Negative politeness strategy 7: Impersonalize S and H (use performatives, imperatives,
impersonal verbs, passive and circumstantial voices, replace the pronouns ‘I’ and ‘you’ by
indefinites, pluralize the ‘I’ and ‘you’ pronouns, use point-of-view distancing):
299
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
In the conversation, hiding who the speaker is, or hearer is, can be a way to be polite.
Speaker is not considered as alone or the addressee is not the hearer.
e.g. Do this for me.
The example shows that speaker avoids I and You pronouns to impersonalize the speaker
and hearer. When the speaker is unmentioned in a conversation, it means that the speaker does
not want to impinge the hearer. In negative politeness, it can be the strategy.
Negative politeness strategy 8: State the FTA as a general rule
In this strategy, the speaker generalizes the expressions of FTA to addressee.
e.g. Passengers will please refrain from flushing toilets on the train.
The example shows that speaker communicate the FTA to hearer as not to impinge on the
address’s face. By using the word “passengers” and avoiding you propnoun, the speaker states
the FTA as general rule.
Negative politeness strategy 9: Nominalize
This strategy is a way to show formality that the speaker nominalizes the expression in
order to make sentence or speaker’s utterance in the form of nominal phrase to show negative
politeness.
e.g. Your good performance on the examinations impressed us favourably.
This example shows that the subject of the sentence above is nominalized by making the
utterance in the form of nominal phrase. Therefore, this example ca be a way to show negative
politeness.
e-Redress other wants of H’s
Negative politeness strategy 10: Go on record as incurring a debt, or as not indebting H
Indebtedness of speaker can be claimed by the speaker to hearer as the way to redress
FTA. When the hearer requests or offers the speaker something, the speaker does it as not a debt
of the hearer.
e.g. I could easily do it for you.
The example shows that the speaker disclaim indebtedness of the hearer by offering. He
states that he is willing to do something for the hearer as not indebting the hearer and thereby
avoid the FTA
4.3.3.4. Off-record strategies
The FTA is performed ‘Off Record’, typically through the deployment of an indirect
illocutionary act which has more than one interpretation and, thus, allows for plausible
deniability on the part of the utterer if the in- tended recipient takes offence at the face threat
inherent in the utterance. The linguistic output strategies include [9, pp. 211–227]:
300
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
CHAPTER IV REVIEW
I-Answer the following questions
1-What is ethnography of speaking / communication?
2-What is communication?
3-What are the units of speech behaviour in communication?
4-Is speaking culturally distinctive? Give examples.
301
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
302
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
303
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
304
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
linguistic change is found in the story of individual languages, and in the regular
correspondences that exist between different languages and dialects. Genetically related
languages “descend” from a common “parent language” through linguistic change. An early
stage in the history of related languages is that they are the dialects of the parent language.
305
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
c-Archaic Vietnamese, the state of the language upon adoption of the Sino-Vietnamese
vocabulary, c. 10th century AD.
d-Ancient Vietnamese, the language represented by chu nom characters (c. 15th
century) and the Chinese–Vietnamese glossary Hua-yi Yi-yu (c. 16th century). By this point a
tone split had happened in the language, leading to six tones but a loss of contrastive voicing
among consonants.
e-Middle Vietnamese, the language of the Vietnamese–Portuguese–Latin dictionary of
the Jesuit missionary Alexandre de Rhodes (c. 17th century).
f-Modern Vietnamese, from the 19th century.
Internal causes of language change are the causes from inside the language. Any change
which can be traced to structural requirement in a language and which is independent of
sociolinguistic factors can be classified as internal (linguistic) causes of language change. This
kind of language change is internally motivated change, that is the change which comes from
factors acting within the language, factors connected with the system of language.
Internally motivated change usually leads to balance in the system, the removal of
marked elements, the analogical spread of regular forms or the like. As language consists of
various modules on various levels, a change in one quarter may lead to an imbalance in another
and provoke a further change.
306
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
External causes of language change are the causes connected with the development of the
society, changes in social, political, economic, cultural life, in science and technology.
Language contact in the historical development of the English language has brought about many
borrowings, the loanwords, from different languages. In addition, with the development of the
society, changes in social, political, economic, cultural life, in science and technology, many new
words have been created. Words which are not in use have disappeared from the English
language.
307
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Analogy reflects the preference of speakers for regular patterns over irregular ones. It
typically involves the extension or generalisation of a regularity on the basis of the inference that
if elements are alike in some respects, they should be alike in others as well. Both phonological
and semantic characteristics can serve as basis for analogy. For example, children create forms
such as goed by analogy with the regular past tense forms like played.
Languages (as well as dialects) contact also results in another minor but nevertheless
important source of language change, hypercorrection. Hypercorrection occurs when a speaker
who is attempting to speak another dialect or language overgeneralizes particular rules.
2.3.2. Analogy:
Analogy reduces word forms by likening different forms of the word to the root.
308
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
2.3.6. Migration/Movement:
Speakers will change and create languages, such as pidgins and creoles.
309
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
b-Palatalisation:
Palatalization is the effect that front vowels and the palatal glide [ j ] typically have on
velar, alveolar, and dental stops, making their place of articulation more palatal. Paltalization is
often the first step in affrication, a change in which palatalized stops become affricates.
c-Nasalisation:
Nasalization refers to nasalising effect that a nasal consonant can have on adjacent vowel.
d-Umblaut:
Umblaut is the case when the effect of a vowel or sometimes a glide in one syllable can
have on the vowel of another syllable, usually a preceding one. Umlaut (resulting in front
rounded vowels [y] and [ø]) played an important role in Old English and is the source of
irregular plurals such as goose / geese and mouse / mice in Modern English. For example, the
plural of the pre-Old English words gōs goose and mūs mouse was formed by adding a suffix –
[i]. As a result, umblaut of the vowel in the preceding syllable occurred in the plural form but
not in the singular forms. By early Old English, the suffix –[i] had been lost in a separate
change, leaving the umblauted vowel as the marker of the plural form. This is called i-mutation.
(Subsequent changes included the derounding of the umblauted vowels [ȳ] and [ø] yielding [ī]
and [ē] respectively by Middle English and the Great Vowel Shift)
3.1.1.2. Dissimilation:
Dissimilation is the process whereby one segment is made less like another segment in its
environment. This type of change typically occurs when it would be difficult to articulate or to
perceive two similar sounds in close proximity.
3.1.1.3. Epenthesis:
Epenthesis involves the insertion of a consonant or vowel into a particular environment.
In these examples, the epenthetic [b]. [d], or [p] has the place of articulation of the
preceding nasal but agrees with the following segment in terms of voice and nasality. The
epenthetic segment therefore serves as a bridge for the transition between the segments on either
side.
310
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
3.1.1.4. Metathesis:
Metathesis is a change in the relative positioning of segments. This change, like
assimilation and dissimilation, can affect adjacent segments or segments at a distance.
Consonant deletion is a very common sound change. For example, the word-initial
cluster [kn] was found in Old and Middle English, as the spelling of such words as knight, knit,
knot, and knee implies, but the [k] subsequently lost giving us our modern pronunciation.
b-Rhotacism:
Rhotacism is a common type of weakening which typically involves the change of [z] to
[r]. In Modern English, rhoticism is the source of the alternation between [z] and [r] in was and
were. The [r] resulted from earlier [z] which was originally intervocalic.
c- Consonant strengthening
Just as consonants weaken, they can also be strengthened.
311
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
In a phonological split, allophones of the same phoneme come to contrast with each other
due to the loss of the conditioning environment, with the result that one or more new phonemes
are created. The English phoneme /ŋ/ was the result of a phonological split. Originally, /ŋ/ was
simply the allophone of /n/ that appeared before a velar consonant. During Middle English,
consonant deletion resulted in the loss of [g] in word-final position after a nasal consonant,
leaving [ŋ] as the final sound in words such as sing.
The loss of the final [g] in words created minimal pairs such as sin (/sin/) and sing (/siŋ/),
in which there is a contrast between /n/ and /ŋ/. This example represents a typical phonological
split.
In a phonological merger, two or more phonemes collapse into a single one, thereby
reducing the number of phonemes in the language. The case of auditory-based substitution
discussed above has the effect in Cockney English, where all instances of the interdental fricative
/θ/ have become /f/. Consequently, the phonemes /θ/ and /f/ have merged into one (/f/) and
words such as thin and fin have the same phonological form (/fin/). Similarly, /v/ and /ð/ have
merged to /v/.
312
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
In the Old English period, a number of umlaut processes affected vowels in complex
ways, and unstressed vowels were gradually eroded, eventually leading to a loss of grammatical
case and grammatical gender in the Early Middle English period. The most important umlaut
process was i-mutation (c. 500 CE), which led to pervasive alternations of all sorts, many of
which survive in the modern language: e.g. in noun paradigms (foot vs. feet, mouse vs. mice,
brother vs. brethren); in verb paradigms (sold vs. sell); nominal derivatives from adjectives
("strong" vs. "strength", broad vs. breadth, foul vs. filth) and from other nouns (fox vs.
"vixen"); verbal derivatives ("food" vs. "to feed"); and comparative adjectives ("old" vs.
"elder"). Consonants were more stable, although velar consonants were significantly modified
by palatalization, which produced alternations such as speak vs. speech, drink vs. drench,
wake vs. watch, bake vs. batch.
The Middle English period saw further vowel changes. Most significant was the Great
Vowel Shift (c. 1500 CE), which transformed the pronunciation of all long vowels. This
occurred after the spelling system was fixed, and accounts for the drastic differences in
pronunciation between "short" mat, met, bit, cot vs. "long" mate, mete/meet, bite, coot. Other
changes that left echoes in the modern language were homorganic lengthening before ld, mb,
nd, which accounts for the long vowels in child, mind, climb, etc.; pre-cluster shortening, which
resulted in the vowel alternations in child vs. children, keep vs. kept, meet vs. met; and
trisyllabic laxing, which is responsible for alternations such as grateful vs. gratitude, divine vs.
divinity, sole vs. solitary.
Among the more significant recent changes to the language have been the development
of rhotic and non-rhotic accents (i.e. "r-dropping"); the trap-bath split in many dialects of
British English; and flapping of t and d between vowels in American English and Australian
English.
313
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
The vowel changes over time can be seen in the following example words, showing the
changes in their form over the last 2,000 years [164]:
314
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Language change can involve alphabet change. An example is the English language.
The English language was first written in the Anglo-Saxon futhorc runic alphabet, in
use from the 5th century. This alphabet was brought to what is now England, along with the
proto-form of the language itself, by Anglo-Saxon settlers.
Anglo-Saxon runes are runes used by the early Anglo-Saxons as an alphabet in their
writing. The characters are known collectively as the futhorc (or fuþorc), from the Old English
sound values of the first six runes. The futhorc was a development from the 24-character elder
futhark. Since the futhorc runes are thought to have first been used in Frisia before the Anglo-
Saxon settlement of Britain, they have also been called Anglo-Frisian runes. They were likely
used from the 5th century onward, recording Old English and Old Frisian.
After the 9th century, they were gradually supplanted in Anglo-Saxon England by the
Old English Latin alphabets introduced by Irish missionaries. Runes were no longer in common
use by the year 1000 and were banned under King Cnut (r. 1016–1036).
The Anglo-Saxon rune poem (Cotton Otho B.x.165) has the following runes, listed with
their Unicode glyphs, their names, their transliterations, and their approximate phonetic values in
IPA notation:
315
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
The Latin script, introduced by Christian missionaries, began to replace the Anglo-Saxon
futhorc from about the 7th century, although the two continued in parallel for some time. Futhorc
influenced the emerging English alphabet by providing it with the letters thorn (Þ þ) and wynn
(Ƿ ƿ). The letter eth (Ð ð) was later devised as a modification of dee (D d), and finally yogh (Ȝ ȝ)
was created by Norman scribes from the insular g in Old English and Irish, and used alongside
their Carolingian g.
The a-e ligature ash (Æ æ) was adopted as a letter in its own right, named after a futhorc
rune æsc. In very early Old English the o-e ligature ethel (Œ œ) also appeared as a distinct letter,
likewise named after a rune, œðel. Additionally, the v-v or u-u ligature double-u (W w) was in
use.
In the year 1011, a monk named Byrhtferð recorded the traditional order of the Old
English alphabet. He listed the 24 letters of the Latin alphabet first (including ampersand), then 5
additional English letters, starting with the Tironian note ond (⁊), an insular symbol for and:
ABCDEFGHIKLMNOPQRSTVXYZ&⁊ǷÞÐÆ
The Old English Latin alphabet generally consisted of 24 letters, and was used for
writing Old English from the 9th to the 12th centuries. Of these letters, 20 were directly adopted
from the Latin alphabet, two were modified Latin letters (Æ, Ð), and two developed from the
runic alphabet (Ƿ, Þ). The letters K, Q and Z were not in the spelling of native English words.
316
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
317
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
318
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
X x /ks/ oxa ox
319
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Notes
320
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
321
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
With the discontinuation of the Late West Saxon standard used for the writing of Old
English in the period prior to the Norman Conquest, Middle English came to be written in a wide
variety of scribal forms, reflecting different regional dialects and orthographic conventions. Later
in the Middle English period, however, and particularly with the development of the Chancery
Standard in the 15th century, orthography became relatively standardised in a form based on the
East Midlands-influenced speech of London. Spelling at the time was mostly quite regular (there
was a fairly consistent correspondence between letters and sounds). The irregularity of present-
day English orthography is largely due to pronunciation changes that have taken place over the
Early Modern English and Modern English eras.
The basic Old English Latin alphabet had consisted of 20 standard letters (there was not
yet a distinct j, v or w, and Old English scribes did not generally use k, q or z) plus four
additional letters: ash ⟨æ⟩, eth ⟨ð⟩, thorn ⟨þ⟩ and wynn ⟨ƿ⟩.
Ash was no longer required in Middle English, as the Old English vowel /æ/ that it
represented had merged into /a/. The symbol nonetheless came to be used as a ligature for the
digraph ⟨ae⟩ in many words of Greek or Latin origin, as did œ for ⟨oe⟩.
Eth and thorn both represented /θ/ in Old English. Eth fell out of use during the 13th
century and was replaced by thorn. Thorn mostly fell out of use during the 14thcentury, and was
replaced by ⟨th⟩. (Anachronistic usage of the scribal abbreviation ("þe", i.e. "the") has led to the
modern mispronunciation of thorn as ⟨y⟩ in this context; see Ye Olde.)
Wynn, which represented the phoneme /w/, was replaced by ⟨w⟩ during the 13th century.
Due to its similarity to the letter ⟨p⟩, it is mostly represented by ⟨w⟩ in modern editions of Old
and Middle English texts even when the manuscript has wynn.
Under Norman influence, the continental Carolingian script replaced the insular that had
been used for Old English. However, because of the significant difference in appearance between
the old insular g and the Carolingian g, the former continued in use as a separate letter, known as
yogh, written ⟨ȝ⟩. This was adopted for use to represent a variety of sounds: [ɣ], [j], [dʒ], [x], [ç],
while the Carolingian g was normally used for [g]. Instances of yogh were eventually replaced
by ⟨j⟩ or ⟨y⟩, and by ⟨gh⟩ in words like night and laugh. In Middle Scots yogh became
indistinguishable from cursive z, and printers tended to use ⟨z⟩ when yogh was not available in
their fonts; this led to new spellings (often giving rise to new pronunciations), as in McKenzie,
where the ⟨z⟩ replaced a yogh which had the pronunciation /j/.
Under continental influence, the letters ⟨k⟩, ⟨q⟩ and ⟨z⟩, which had not normally been
used by Old English scribes, came to be commonly used in the writing of Middle English. Also
the newer Latin letter ⟨w⟩ was introduced (replacing wynn). The distinct letter forms ⟨v⟩ and ⟨u⟩
322
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
came into use, but were still used interchangeably; the same applies to ⟨j⟩ and ⟨i⟩. (For example,
spellings such as wijf and paradijs for wife and paradise can be found in Middle English.)
The consonantal ⟨j⟩/⟨i⟩ was sometimes used to transliterate the Hebrew letter yodh,
representing the palatal approximant sound /j/ (and transliterated in Greek by iota and in Latin
by ⟨i⟩); words like Jerusalem, Joseph, etc. would have originally followed the Latin
pronunciation beginning with /j/, that is, the sound of ⟨y⟩ in yes. In some words, however,
notably from Old French, ⟨j⟩/⟨i⟩ was used for the affricate /dʒ/, as in joie (modern "joy"), used in
Wycliffe's Bible. This was similar to the geminate sound [ddʒ] which had been represented as
⟨cg⟩ in Old English. By the time of Modern English, the sound came to be written as ⟨j⟩/⟨i⟩ at the
start of words (like joy), and usually as ⟨dg⟩ elsewhere (as in bridge). It could also be written,
mainly in French loanwords, as ⟨g⟩, with the adoption of the soft G convention (age, page, etc.)
Many scribal abbreviations were also used. It was common for the Lollards to abbreviate
the name of Jesus (as in Latin manuscripts) to ihc. The letters ⟨n⟩ and ⟨m⟩ were often omitted
and indicated by a macron above an adjacent letter, so for example in could be written as ī. A
thorn with a superscript ⟨t⟩ or ⟨e⟩ could be used for that and the; the thorn here resembled a ⟨Y⟩,
giving rise to the ye of "Ye Olde". Various forms of the ampersand replaced the word and.
Numbers were still always written using Roman numerals, except for some rare
occurrences of Arabic numerals during the 15th century.
Although Middle English spelling was never fully standardised, the following table
shows the pronunciations most usually represented by particular letters and digraphs towards the
end of the Middle English period, using the notation given in the article on Middle English
phonology. As explained above, single vowel letters had alternative pronunciations depending on
whether they were in a position where their sounds had been subject to lengthening. Long vowel
pronunciations were in flux due to the beginnings of the Great Vowel Shift.
323
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
/k/, replaced earlier ⟨kk⟩ as the doubled form of ⟨k⟩ (for the phenomenon of
ck
doubling, see above).
d /d/
/e/, or in lengthened positions /eː/ or sometimes /ɛː/ (see ee). For silent ⟨e⟩, see
e
above.
ea Rare, for /ɛː/ (see ee).
/eː/, becoming [iː] by about 1500; or /ɛː/, becoming [eː] by about 1500. In Early
ee Modern English the latter vowel came to be commonly written ⟨ea⟩. The two
vowels later merged.
ei, ey Sometimes the same as ⟨ai⟩; sometimes /ɛː/ or /eː/ (see also fleece merger).
ew Either /ɛu/ or /iu/ (see Late Middle English diphthongs; these later merged).
f /f/
/ɡ/, or /dʒ/ before ⟨e⟩, ⟨i⟩, ⟨y⟩ (see ⟨g⟩ for details). The ⟨g⟩ in initial gn- was still
g
pronounced.
[ç] or [x], post-vowel allophones of /h/ (this was formerly one of the uses of yogh).
gh The ⟨gh⟩ is often retained in Chancery spellings even though the sound was starting
to be lost.
/h/ (except for the allophones for which ⟨gh⟩ was used). Also used in several
h digraphs (⟨ch⟩, ⟨th⟩, etc.). In some French loanwords, such as horrible, the ⟨h⟩ was
silent.
As a vowel, /i/, or in lengthened positions /iː/, which had started to be
i, j diphthongised by about 1500. As a consonant, /dʒ/ ( (corresponding to modern ⟨j⟩);
see above).
ie Used sometimes for /ɛː/ (see ee).
/k/, used particularly in positions where ⟨c⟩ would be softened. Also used in ⟨kn⟩ at
k
the start of words; here both consonants were still pronounced.
l /l/
m /m/
n /n/, including its allophone [ŋ] (before /k/, /g/).
/o/, or in lengthened positions /ɔː/ or sometimes /oː/ (see oo). Sometimes /u/, as in
o sone (modern son); the ⟨o⟩ spelling was often used rather than ⟨u⟩ when adjacent to
i, m, n, v, w for legibility, i.e. to avoid a succession of vertical strokes.
oa Rare, for /ɔː/ (became commonly used in Early Modern English).
oi, oy /ɔi/ or /ui/ (see Late Middle English diphthongs; these later merged).
oo /oː/, becoming [uː] by about 1500; or /ɔː/.
ou, ow Either /uː/, which had started to be diphthongised by about 1500, or /ɔu/.
p /p/
qu /kw/
r /r/
s /s/, sometimes /z/ (formerly [z] was an allophone of /s/). Also appeared as ſ (long s).
sch, sh /ʃ/
324
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
t /t/
/θ/ or /ð/ (which had previously been allophones of a single phoneme), replacing
th
earlier eth and thorn, although thorn was still sometimes used.
Used interchangeably. As a consonant, /v/. As a vowel, /u/, or /iu/ in "lengthened"
u, v positions (although it had generally not gone through the same lengthening process
as other vowels – see history of /iu/).
w /w/ (replaced Old English wynn).
wh /hw/ (see English ⟨wh⟩).
x /ks/
As a consonant, /j/ (earlier this was one of the uses of yogh). Sometimes also /g/. As
y a vowel, the same as ⟨i⟩, where ⟨y⟩ is often preferred beside letters with
downstrokes.
z /z/ (in Scotland sometimes used as a substitute for yogh; see above).
3.2.3.4. Sample
Table 5.15. The Canterbury Tales: The first 18 lines of the Prologue [134]
Here bygynneth the Book of the tales of Here begins the Book of the Tales of Canterbury
Caunterbury
1-When April with his showers sweet with fruit
1-Whan that aprill with his shoures soote
2-The drought of March has pierced unto the root
2-The droghte of march hath perced to the
roote, 3-And bathed each vein with liquor that has power
3-And bathed every veyne in swich licour 4-To generate therein and sire the flower;
4-Of which vertu engendred is the flour; 5-When Zephyr also has, with his sweet breath,
5-Whan zephirus eek with his sweete breeth 6-Quickened again, in every holt and heath,
6-Inspired hath in every holt and heeth 7-The tender shoots and buds, and the young sun
7-Tendre croppes, and the yonge sonne 8-Into the Ram one half his course has run,
8-Hath in the ram his halve cours yronne, 9-And many little birds make melody
9-And smale foweles maken melodye, 10-That sleep through all the night with open eye
10-That slepen al the nyght with open ye 11-(So Nature pricks them on to ramp and rage)-
325
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
11-(so priketh hem nature in hir corages); 12-Then do folk long to go on pilgrimage,
12-Thanne longen folk to goon on 13-And palmers to go seeking out strange strands,
pilgrimages,
14-To distant shrines well known in sundry lands.
13-And palmeres for to seken straunge
strondes, 15-And specially from every shire's end
14-To ferne halwes, kowthe in sondry londes; 16-Of England they to Canterbury wend,
15-And specially from every shires ende 17-The holy blessed martyr there to seek
16-Of engelond to caunterbury they wende, 18-Who helped them when they lay so ill and weal
In the orthography of Modern English, thorn (þ), eth (ð), wynn (ƿ), yogh (ȝ), ash (æ),
and ethel (œ) are obsolete. Latin borrowings reintroduced homographs of ash and ethel into
Middle English and Early Modern English, though they are not considered to be the same letters
but rather ligatures, and in any case are somewhat old-fashioned. Thorn and eth were both
replaced by th, though thorn continued in existence for some time, its lowercase form gradually
becoming graphically indistinguishable from the minuscule y in most handwriting. Y for th can
still be seen in pseudo-archaisms such as "Ye Olde Booke Shoppe". The letters þ and ð are still
used in present-day Icelandic while ð is still used in present-day Faroese. Wynn disappeared
from English around the 14th century when it was supplanted by uu, which ultimately developed
into the modern w. Yogh disappeared around the 15th century and was typically replaced by gh.
The letters u and j, as distinct from v and i, were introduced in the 16th century, and w
assumed the status of an independent letter, so that the English alphabet is now considered to
consist of the following 26 letters:
ABCDEFGHIJKLMNOPQRSTUVWXYZ
The variant lowercase form long s (ſ) lasted into early modern English, and was used in
non-final position up to the early 19th century.The modern English alphabet is a Latin alphabet
consisting of 26 letters (each having an uppercase and a lowercase form).
326
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26
Majuscule forms (also called uppercase or capital letters)
A B C D E F G H I J K L M N O P Q R S T U V W X Y Z
Minuscule forms (also called lowercase or small letters)
a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q r s t u v w x y z
3.3.1.2. Fusion
Not all new affixes are the result of borrowing. Words themselves can develop into
affixes in a process called fusion. If two words are frequently adjacent, over time they can
become fused together to form a single unit consisting of a stem and an affix. Fusion can result
in either prefixes or suffixes.
A number of Modern English suffixes are derived from earlier words by means of fusion.
327
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Loss of case endings in English is another example of loss of affixes. Old English had a
complex system of affixes marking case and gender. The English language once had an
extensive declension system similar to Latin, modern German and Icelandic. Nouns were divided
into three gender classes; masculine, neutral, and feminine. Each gender class was associated
with a different set of case endings. Old English distinguished between the nominative,
accusative, dative, and genitive cases, and for strongly declined adjectives and some pronouns
also a separate instrumental case (which otherwise and later completely coincided with the
dative). In addition, the dual number was distinguished from the singular and plural. By the
fifteenth century, English case endings had changed radically. Declension was greatly simplified
during the Middle English period, when the accusative and dative cases of the pronouns merged
into a single oblique case that also replaced the genitive case after prepositions. Consequently,
many of the earlier case and gender distinctions were obliterated. Nouns in Modern English no
longer decline for case, except for the genitive.
Table 5.20. The loss of case affixes in the English word hound [74, p.272]
O.E M.E Modern E.
Singular
Nominative hund hund hound
Accusative hund hund hound
Genitive hundes hundes hound’s
Dative hunde hunde hound
Plural
Nominative hundas hundes hounds
Accusative hundas hundes hounds
Genitive hunda hunde hounds’
Dative hundum hunde hounds
Whereas Old English had five distinctive affixes for cases, Middle English had only two
affixes, -e and -es which, with the loss of schwa, were ultimately reduced to a single suffix -s,
still used in Modern English for the plural and the possessive. This represents a typical example
of how sound change can result in modification to the morphological component of the grammar.
Analogy reflects the preference of speakers for regular patterns over irregular ones. It
typically involves the extension or generalisation of a regularity on the basis of the inference that
if elements are alike in some respects, they should be alike in others as well.
The Modern English plural hands can not be the direct consequence of sound change.
Rather, it is the result of earlier analogy with words such as hund (hound) which did form the
plural with the suffix -s (The Old English handa (hands) changed to handə (vowel reduction)
and then to hand (apocope). The suffix -s, whose earlier form -as was predominant even in Old
English, was extended by analogy to all English nouns with a few exceptions. Other plural
forms besides hands that were created on the basis of analogy include eye (eyen in Middle
328
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
English) and shoe (formally shooen). Each generation of English speaking children temporarily
extends the analogy still further by producing forms such as sheeps, gooses, and mouses.
e.g. S V O
Hē geseah ƿone mann
He saw the man
However, when the clause began with an element such as ƿa (then) or ne (not), the verb
occurred in the second position and preceded the subject.
e.g. V S O
ƿa sende sē cyning ƿone disc
Then sent the king the dish
Then the king sent the dish.
When the direct object was a noun, the subject-object-verb order was typical.
e.g. S O V
Hē hine lǣrde
She him advised
She advised him.
The subject-object-verb order also prevailed in embedded clauses, even when the direct
object was not a pronoun.
e.g. S O V
ƿa hē ƿone cyning sōhte, hē bēotode
when he the king visited, he boasted.
When he visited the king, he boasted.
Since case markings were lost during the Middle English period through sound change,
fixed subject-verb-object order (SVO) became the means of marking grammatical relations. As
329
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
table 5.20 shows, a major change in word order took place between 1300 and 1400, with verb-
object order (SVO) becoming dominant:
Evidence indicates the earliest form of Germanic (from which English descended), was
an SOV language. If the earliest Germanic was SVO and Modern English is firmly SVO, then
Old English represents a transitional syntactic type. The English language has changed from a
SOV language to SVO language.
Table 5.22. Grammatical case change in the English word hound [74, p.271]
O.E M.E Modern E.
Singular
Nominative hund hund hound
Accusative hund hund hound
Genitive hundes hundes hound’s
Dative hunde hunde hound
Plural
Nominative hundas hundes hounds
Accusative hundas hundes hounds
Genitive hunda hunde hounds’
Dative hundum hunde hounds
Table 5.23. Grammatical case change in first person personal pronouns [164]
330
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
As seen the English language has developed from a synthetic language into an analytic
language. While Old English was a highly inflected language, a few critical sound changes left
little inflectional morphology in Middle English. Modern English is thus an analytical language,
relying principally on word order, not on grammatical cases as in Old English, to express
grammatical relations that were formally marked inflectionally. The Old English grammatical
case system does not exist in Modern English.
In the first example the word order is different from that of Modern English, and there are
two negatives: na (a contraction of ne+a; not +ever =never) and ne. A double negative was
grammatical in Old English, although double negative are ungrammatical in Modern Standard
English.
In addition to the contraction of ne+a na, other negative contractions occurred in Old
English: ne could be attached to habb- (have), wes- (be), wit-(know), and will-(will) to form
nabb-, nes-, nyt, and nyll-, respectively.
In Old English, the negative element occurs at the beginning of the contraction, because it
typically preceded the auxiliary in sentences. The rules determining the placement of the
negative morpheme have changed in the historical development of the English language.
During the sixteenth and seventeenth centuries, the inversion rule was changed to apply
solely to auxiliary verbs
331
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Inversion (old form): the verb moves to the left of the subject
e.g. They speak. Speak they?
They can speak Can they speak?
Inversion (new form): the Auxiliaries moves to the left of the subject.
e.g. They speak *Speak they?
They can speak Can they speak?
With this change, structures such as Speak they the truth? were no longer possible.
The corresponding question came to be formed with the auxiliary do as in Do they speak
the truth?
332
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Derived adjectives
[wundor]N + ful wundorfull wonderful
[cid]N + isc cildisc childish
Many Old English compoundings and derived words are not used in Modern English.
Not all word formation processes available to Modern English speakers were also found in Old
English. For example, conversion was not possible in Old English. In fact, conversion is
typically not available to (synthetic) inflectional languages such as Old English since change in a
word category in such languages is usually indicated morphologically and conversion, does not
involve the use of affixes.
3.5.1.2. Borrowing
In the development of the English language, many words have been borrowed from other
language to enrich English vocabulary.
333
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Old English (called Anglo-Saxon) was formed by mostly Germanic word-stock plus
some Celtic words and Latin words. Anglo-Saxon words have been considered native words.
Then, the English language borrowed words from the Scandinavian languages (languages spoken
by the Danes, Swedes, Norwegians) in the 8th century, words from the Norman-French after
1066 (11th century), words from the Latin language and Greek language during the renaissance
(1500-1700) and words from many other languages in Modern English period.
The tendency of English to borrow words has never abated since the earliest times. Let's
review the main sources of borrowing.
a-North European aboriginal terms into Common Germanic (before 2000BC)
b-Latin terms from the Romans into West Germanic (100BC-400AD)
c-Christianized Latin terms into Anglo Saxon (after 587AD)
d-Old Norse into Anglo Saxon (700-900AD)
e-Norman French into Old English (1066-1300AD)
f-Ancient Latin and Greek into Modern English 1500- through the present)
g-Other languages in the modern periods (Italian, Spanish, German, Dutch, Slavic
languages, Ameridian languages, Hindi).
Table 5.26. Origin of the 5000 most frequent words in English [74, p.281]
First 1000 83 11 2 4
Second 1000 34 56 11 9
Third 1000 29 46 14 11
Fourth 1000 27 45 17 11
Fifth 1000 27 47 17 9
334
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
and unfamiliar items or concepts. This situation reflects the fact that it is usually the speakers of
the substratum language who inhabited the area first.
In some cases, French loanwords were used in conjunction with native English words to
convey distinctions of various sorts. For a minor crime, for example, the English word theft was
employed, but for a more serious breach of the law the French word theft was employed. The
English also kept their own words for domesticated animals, but adopted the French words for
the meat from those creatures.
Table 5.28. French loanwords used in conjunctions with native English words [74, p.280]
Adstratum influence refers to the situation where two languages are in contact and
neither one is clearly politically or culturally dominant. In a city such as Montreal with its large
number of bilingual speakers, English and French inevitably influence each other.
Earlier in the history of English, when the Scandinavian settled part of England beginning
in 800 A.D., there was substantial contact between the speakers of English and Scandinavian
resulting in an adstratum relationship. Adstratum contact usually results in the borrowing of
335
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
everyday words. In fact, without consulting a dictionary, most English speakers could not
distinguish between borrowings from Scandinavian and native English words.
Anger, cake, call, egg, fellow, gear, get, hit, husband, low, lump, raise, root, score, seat,
Borrowed words from many other languages attest to various types of cultural contact
and serve to fill the lexical gaps such contact inevitably brings.
3.5.1.3. Influence of borrowings on the vocabulary of the English language [114, pp.138-139]
Borrowed words have great influences on the vocabulary of the English language
ii-A borrowed word may restrict a native word to a narrower sphere of usage
iii- A borrowed word may become a stylistic synonym to a native word (the borrowed
word being more literary)
336
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
e.g. race (native word = running) – race (borrowed word = a distinct ethical stock)
arm (native word = part of the body) – arm (borrowed word = weapon)
v-As a result of semantic borrowing, new meanings appeared and the number of
polysemantic words increased.
iii-A noun may have an adjective formed by means of affixes and a corresponding
borrowed adjective
c-Etymological doublets
Etymological doublets are two words of the same language which were borrowed by
different routes (from different languages) but derived from the same basic word. They differ to
337
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
a certain degree in form, meaning and usage because they underwent different changes
depending on the time of borrowing.
iii-Still others were borrowed from the same language twice, but in different periods,
Just as words can be added to the lexicon, they can also be lost. Loss of a word
frequently occurs as a result of changes in society, particularly in the case where the object or
notion a word refers to has become obsolete.
338
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
3.6.3. Amelioration
In amelioration the meaning of a word becomes more positive or favourable.
3.6.4. Pejoration
In pejoration the meaning of a word becomes more negative or more unfavourable.
339
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
a-similarity of shape and appearance: head (of a cabbage), the teeth (of a saw), the neck
(of a bottle), the mouth (of a river)..
b-similarity of position: the tail (of a procession), the foot (of a mountain)
340
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
c-similarity of movement: to worm, to fall (in love), to come to (an agreement), to run
(into debt)…
d-similarity of function or use: finger (of instrument), the key (to the mistery)…
e-Similarity of size, midget (submarine), elephantine (task)…
f-similarity of temperature: hot (scent), cold (war), warm (words)…
g-similarity of sound and manner: to cough (to speak reluctantly), to bark (to say in a
sharp, commanding voice)…
f-similarity in quality: a bookworm (one who devotes too much to books and study), a
bee (a hard-working person), a goose (a stupid person), a fox (a cunning person).
341
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Just change sometimes begins with a small number of words, effects of a change often
appear first in the speech of a small number of people. Social pressures play an important role in
determining whether a particular innovation will spread through the entire linguistic community.
Since speakers can consciously or unconsciously alter the way they speak to approximate what
they perceive to be a more prestigious or socially acceptable variety of speech, once a change has
taken hold in the speech of a high prestige group it may gradually spread to other speakers and
ultimately affect the entire linguistic community.
CHAPTER V REVIEW
I-Answer the following questions
342
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
4-Articulatory pronunciation and spelling pronunciation are the two causes of language
change.
5-Sound change can involve sequential change, segmental change and auditory-based
change.
6-Umblout is the nasalising effect that a nasal consonant can have on adjacent vowel.
10- The grammatical case system in Old English were the same as it is in Modern
English.
13-Lexical change in the English language is by means of word formation, borrowing and
word loss.
14-In the development of the English language, many borrowings have been introduced
into the language to enrich the vocabulary.
15- Semantic broadening is the process in which the meaning of a word becomes less
general or less inclusive than its historical earlier form.
16-Semantic narrowing is the process in which the meaning of a word becomes less
general or less inclusive than its historical earlier form.
18-In perjoration, the meaning of a word becomes more positive and more favourable.
343
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
20-Language change is through the lexical diffusion and spread through population.
344
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
ENGLISH-VIETNAMESE TERMINOLOGY
1 Act sequence Chuỗi hành vi 133 Metonymy Hoán dụ
2 Addressing Xưng hô 134 Micro- Ngôn ngữ- Xã hội
sociolinguistics học vi mô
3 Adolescent Thanh thiếu niên 135 Middle name Tên đệm
4 Adstratum Tầng thêm 136 Modesty maxim Phương châm
khiêm tốn
5 Affinity Quan hệ thân tộc 137 Mutual face Thể diện chung
với bên vợ / chồng
6 Agreement maxim Phương châm tán 138 Mutual intelligibility Sự hiểu được lẫn
đồng nhau
7 American English Tiếng Anh Mỹ 139 Negative face Thể diện tiêu cực /
thể diện âm tính
8 Analogy Sự giống nhau 140 Negative politeness Lịch sự tiêu cực /
Lich sự âm tính
9 Anthroponomastics Danh nhân học/ 141 Negative politeness Chiến lược lịch sự
ngành khoa học strategy âm tính / tích cực
nghiên cứu tên
người
10 Apocope Hiện tượng mất âm 142 Non-verbal Giao tiếp bằng
chủ communication ngôn ngữ cử chỉ
11 Approbation Phương châm tán 143 Norm of interaction Chuẩn tương tác
maxim thưởng
12 Articulatory Sự đơn giản hóa về 144 Norms of Chuẩn luận giải /
simplication phát âm interpretation giải thích
13 Artifact Ngôn ngữ trang 145 Off-record Nói vòng vo, nói
phục gián tiếp
14 Ascribed status Địa vị cha truyền 146 Onomastics Ngành nghiên cứu
con nối tên riêng
15 Australian English Tiếng Anh Úc 147 On-record Nói thẳng vấn đề
16 Autonomy Tính độc lập 148 Orthography Chính tả
17 Blasphemy Sự / Lời báng bổ 149 Palatalization Hiện tượng vòm
345
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
hóa
18 British English Tiếng Anh Anh 150 Paralinguistics Cận ngôn ngữ /
ngôn ngữ giọng
nói
19 Cant Tiếng lóng của bọn 151 Participant Người tham gia
trộm cướp giao tiếp
20 Caste Đẳng cấp 152 Patrilineality Phụ hệ
21 Caste dialect Phương ngữ giai 153 Perjoration Nghĩa xấu đi
tầng
22 Chronomics Ngôn ngữ thời gian 154 Personal identity Bản sắc cá nhân
23 Class dialect Phương ngữ giai 155 Personal name Tên người
cấp
24 Code Mã 156 Person-oriented Tính định hướng
theo con người
25 Code choice Chọn mã 157 Pidgin Tiếng bồi, tiếng lai
tạp
26 Code-mixing Trộn mã 158 Politeness Lịch sự
27 Code-switching Chuyển mã 159 Positive face Thể diện tích cưc
/Thể diện dương
tính
28 Codification Điển chế hóa 160 Positive politeness Lịch sự dương tính
/ lịc sự dương tính
29 Collateral Bàng hệ 161 Positive politeness Chiến lược lịch sự
relationship strategy dương tính / tích
cực
30 Collectivism Chủ nghĩa tập thể 162 Posture Tư thế
31 Communication Sự giao tiếp 163 Post-vocalic -r r-sau nguyên âm
32 Communication Ngữ cảnh giao tiếp 164 Power Quyền lực
context
33 Communicative Vai giao tiếp 165 Power distance Khoảng cách
role quyền lực
34 Concentric circle Vòng tròn đồng 166 Power relation Mối quan hệ
tâm quyền lực
346
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
347
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
348
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
349
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
350
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
nghiệp
106 Jati Đẳng cấp 238 Superposed variety Biến thể trội
107 Karma Số phận 239 Superstratum Tầng trên
108 Key Phương thức 240 Surname Họ
109 Kin(ship) Thân tộc 241 Swearing Thề, tuyên thệ,
chửi thề, rủa
110 Kinetics Ngôn ngữ cơ thể 242 Sympathy maxim Phương châm cảm
thông
111 Kinship term Từ thân tộc 243 Synchronic Ngôn ngữ học
linguistics đồng đại
112 Language Ngôn ngữ 244 Syncope Sự rụng âm
113 Language change Sự thay đổi ngôn 245 Taboo Hiện tượng kiêng
ngữ kị
114 Language contact Tiếp xúc ngôn ngữ 246 Taboo word Từ kiêng kị
115 Language function Chức năng ngôn 247 Tact maxim Phương châm
ngữ khéo léo
116 Language variation Biến thể ngôn ngữ 248 Tag-switching Chuyển mã bằng
các từ hỏi chêm
vào cuối câu như
right, er…
117 Language variety Biển thể ngôn ngữ 249 Tangible culture Văn hóa vật thể
118 Last name Họ 250 Teenage language Ngôn ngữ thanh
thiếu niên
119 Linear logic Logic tuyến tính 251 Term of address Từ xưng hô
120 Linguistic Tính quyết định của 252 Territorial dialect Phương ngữ lánh
determinism ngôn ngữ thổ, phương ngữ
địa lý
121 Linguistic Tính tương đối của 253 Third cousin Anh
relativity ngôn ngữ
122 Linguistic structure Cấu trúc ngôn ngữ 254 Thought Tư duy
123 Linguistic variable Biến số ngôn ngữ 255 Tone Âm điệu, dấu
124 Linguistic variant Biến tố ngôn ngữ 256 Topography Địa hình
351
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
352
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
REFERENCES
I-English authors
[1] Adamson, H.D. (1993). Academic Competence. London: Longman Publishing Group.
[2] Allwood, J. (1990). On The Role of Cultural Content and Cultural Context in Language
Instruction. Gothenburg Papers in Theoretical Linguistics 60, University of Göteborg, Dept
of Linguistics.
[3] Asher, R.E. & Simson, J.M.Y. (1994). The Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics.
Oxford: Pergamon Press.
[4] Baugh, A.C., & Cable, T. (2002). A History of the English Language (3rd edition). London:
Routledge & Kegan Paul.
[5] Beal, J.C. (2010). An Introduction to Regional Englishes. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University
Press Ltd.
[6] Becker, A. & Bieswanger, M. (2006). Introduction to English Linguistics. Germany: UTB
basics.
[7] Bousfield, D. (2008). Impoliteness in interaction. Amsterdam: John Benjamin Publishing
Company.
[8] Brown, K. & Anderson, A (2006). The Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics. Boston:
Elsevier.
[9] Brown, P. & Levinson, S.C. (1987). Politeness: Some Universals in Language Usage.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
[10] Brown, R., & Gilman, A. (1960). The pronouns of power and solidarity. In T. A. Sebeok
(ed.), Style in Language (pp.253-276). Cambridge: MIT Press.
[11] Cameron, D. (2001). Working with Spoken Discourse. London: Sage Publications.
[12] Chambers, J.K., & Trudgill, P. (2004). Dialectology. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
[13] Cheshire, J., & Trudgill, P. (Eds.). (1998). The sociolinguistics reader, vol. 2: Gender and
Discourse. London: Arnold.
[14] Coates, J. (1986). Women, Men and Language: A sociolinguistic account of gender
differences in language. London: Longman.
[15] Coates, J. (1996). Women Talk: Conversation between women friends. Oxford: Blackwell.
353
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
[16] Cran, W., McCrum, R. & MacNeil, R. (1986). The Story of English. New York: Viking.
[17] Crawford, M. (1995). Talking Differences: On Gender and Language. London: Sage
[18] Crowther, J. (1999). Oxford Guide to British and American Culture. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
[19] Cruttenden, A. (2014). Gimson’s pronunciation of English. London and New York:
Routledge.
[20] Crystal, D. (1994a). The Cambridge Encyclopedia of Language. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press
[21] Crystal, D. (1994b). The Cambridge Encyclopedia of the English Language. London: BCA.
[22] Crystal, D. (2003). English as a Global Language. London: Cambridge University Press.
[23] Crystal, D. (2008). A Dictionary of Language and Linguistics. USA: Blackwell Publishing.
[24] Eckermann, A. K. (2001). Facilitating Cross-cultural Communication (Unpublished
Coursebook). Australia: University of New England
[25] Eckert, P. & McConnell-Ginet, S. (1992). Think practically and look locally: Language and
gender as community-based practice. Annual Review of Anthropology, 21, 461-290.
[26] Eckert, P. & McConnell-Ginet, S. (2003). Language and gender. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
[27] Ervin-Trip, S. (1964). An Analysis of the Interaction of Language, Topic, and Listener.
American Anthropologist, Vol. 66, No.6, Part 2, 87-102.
[28] Fraser, B. (1990). Perspectives on Politeness. Journal of Pragmatics 14 (1990), 219-236.
[29] Fromkin, V & Roadman, R. (1983). An Introduction to Language. New York: Holt,
Rinehart, and Winston.
[30] Galperin, I.R. (1981). Stylistics. Moscow: Moscow High school
[31] Galperin, I.R. (1988). Stylistics. Moscow: Moscow Highschool.
[32] Garcia-Carbonell, A., & Rising, B. (2006). Culture and Communication. USA: College of
Management Georgia Institute of Technology, Atlanta, Georgia.
[33] Gerritsen, M. (1998). The Role of Culture in Communication. South African Journal of
Linguistics, Volume 16, 1998.
[34] Goffman, E. (1967). Interaction Ritual: Essays on Face-to-Face Behaviour. New York:
Doubleday.
354
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
[35] Goshylyk.V.B. & Goshylyk N.S. (2010). Glossary of Intercultural Communication. Ivano-
Frankivsk
[36] Greenbaum, S. (1996). The Oxford English Grammar. Oxford: Oxford university Press.
[37] Gudykunst,W.B. et al. (1996). The Influence of Cultural Individualism – Collectivism, Self-
construals, and Individual Values on Communication Styles Across Cultures. Human June
Communication Research, volume 22, issue 4, June 1996, 510-543.
[38] Gudykunst, W. B. (1998). Cross-cultural and Intercultural communication. USA: Sage
Publications Inc.
[39] Halliday, M. (1991). The Notion of Context in Language Education. In Thao Le and M.
McCAUSLAND, Language Education: Interaction and Development (1-26). Tasmania:
University of Tasmania at Launceston.
[40] Haughen, E. (1966). Dialect, Language, Nation. American Anthropologist, New Series, Vol.
68, No. 4 (August, 1966), Blackwell Publishing, 922-935.
[41] Hofstede, G., Hoftede, G.J. & Minkov, M. (2010). Cultures and Organizations: Software of
the Mind. New York: Cambridge University Press.
[42] Hofstede, G. (2011). Dimensionalizing Cultures: The Hofstede Model in Context. The
Netherlands: University of Maastricht and Tilburg.
[43] Hogg, R. M. (1994). The Cambridge History of the English Language (Volume V).
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
[44] Holmes, J. (2000). Gendered Speech in Social Context. Victoria University Press
[48] Hotten, J. (1860). A Dictionary of Modern Slang, Cant, and Vulgar words: Used at the
Present Day in the Streets of London (2nd ed.). USA: Forgotten Books
355
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
Washington.
[52] Hymes, D. (1964). Introduction:Toward Ethnographies of Communication. American
Anthropologist, 66 (6), 1–34, doi:10.1525/aa.1964.66.suppl_3.02a00010.
[53] Hymes, D. (1976). Foundations in sociolinguistics: An ethnographic approach (8th ed.).
Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.
[54] Kasper, G. (1994). Politeness. In R.E. Asher & J.M.Y. Simson, The Encyclopedia of
Language and Linguistics (pp.3206-3211). Oxford: Pergamon Press.
[55] Keith, G.R. & Shuttleworth, J. (20000). Living Language: exploring advanced level English
language (2nd ed.). London: Holder & Stoughton Educational.
[56] Krachu, Y. and Smith, L.E. (2008). Cultures, Contexts, and World English. New York:
Routledge.
[57] Labov, W. (1966). The Social Stratification of English in New York City. New York:
Cambridge University Press.
[58] Labov, W. (2001). Principles of Linguistic change, II: Social factors. Oxford: Blackwell.
[59] Lakoff, R. (1973). Language and Women’s place. Language in Society, Vol.2, No.1 (Appril
1973), 45-80.
[60] Lerer, S. (2008). The History of the English Language (part 2). Standford University: The
Teaching Company.
[61] Litosseliti, L. (2013). Gender and Language. London and New York: Routlege..
[62] Littlejohn, S. W & Foss, K. A. (2011). Theories of Human Communication (10th ed.). Long
Grove, Ill.: Waveland Press.
[63] Lustig, M.W. & Koester J. (2010). Intercultural Competence. United States: Person
education, Inc.
[64] Maltzev, V.A. (1984). Essays on English Stylistics. Misk: Vysheishaya Shkola.
356
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
[70] Migut, P. (2010). The Study of Social Varieties of English with Special Emphasis on the
Am.E. CB slang. Germany: Grin Verlag.
[71] Moore, S. (1993). Sociology Alive. Great Britain: Stanley Thornes LTD.
[75] Park, Y.S. and Kim, B. S. K. (2008). Asian and European American cultural values and
communication styles among Asian American and European college students. Cultural
Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, Volume 14, No. 1, 44-56.
[76] Philipsen, G. (1975) "Speaking "like a man". In Teamsterville Culture patterns of role
enactment in an urban neighborhood, Quarterly Journal of Speech, Volume 61 No. 1, 13–
22. doi:10.1080/00335637509383264.
[77] Philipsen, G. (2001). Ethnography of Speaking. In R. Mesthrie, Concise Encyclopedia of
Sociolinguistics (pp. 154-160). New York: Elsevier..
[78] Pizziconi, B. (2006). Politeness. In K. Brown & A. Simson, The encyclopedia of Language
and Linguistics (pp. 679-684). Boston: Elsevier.
[79] Preston, D.P. and Roger, W.S. (1988). Varieties of American English. Washington D.C.:
English Language Program Division.
[80] Rawson, H. (1981). A Dictionary Euphemisms and Other Doubletalk. New York: Crown
Publishers, Inc.
[81] Reisinger, Y. (2009). International Tourism. USA: Elsevier Ltd.
[82] Richards, J., Platt, J. & Weber, H. (1985). Longman Dictionary of Applied Linguistics.
England: Longman Group Limited.
[83] Salzmann, Z. (1993). Language, Culture and Society. The United States of America.
[84] Samovar, L.A., Porter, R.E., Mcdaniel, E.R & Roy, C.S. (2013). Communication Between
Cultures. USA: Monica Eckman.
[85] Saville-Troike, M. (1994). Silence. In Asher and Somson, The Encyclopedia of Language
and Linguistics ( pp.3945-3947). Oxford: Pergamon Press.
357
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
[87] Saville-Troike, M. (2006). Silence: Cultural Aspects. In K. Brown and A. Anderson, The
Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics (pp.379-381). Boston: Elservier.
[88] Schilling, N. (2016). English in America: A Linguistic History. The United States of
America: The Great Courses
[89] Seargeant, P. and Swan, J. (2012). Worlds of English. USA: Routledge.
[90] Sponsky, B. (1998). Sociolinguistics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
[91] Stenström, A. B., Anderson, G. & Hasund, I.K. (2002). Trends in Teenage Talk. USA: John
Benjamins B.V.
[92] Stolley, K.S. (2005). The basics of sociology. The United States of America: Greenwood
Publishing Group, Inc.
[99] Ting-Toomy, S. (1999). Communicating Across Culture. New York: The Guilford Press
358
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
[105] Van Dijc, T.A. (2009). Discourse and Context. UK: Cambrdge University Press
[106] Victor, D.A. (1992). International Business Communication. New York: Harper Collins.
[107] Wardhaugh, R. (2010). An Introduction to Sociolinguistics. USA: John Wiley-Blackwell.
II-Vietnamese authors
[108] Tôn Thất Cổn. (1943). Hoàng Tộc Lược Biên. Huế.
[109] PhamThi Diễm and Vu Thi Thùy Dung. (2014). Ảnh Hưởng của Yếu tố Văn hóa
Truyền thống đối với Văn hóa Hành chính Việt nam. Tạp chí Quản lí Nhà nước, No.
221, June 2014.
[110] Nguyễn Văn Khang. (1999). Ngôn Ngữ Xã Hôii học. Nhà Xuất Bản Khoa học Xã hội.
[111] Nguyễn Văn Khang. (2014a). Ngôn Ngữ học Xã Hội. Nxb Giáo dục Việt nam.
[112] Nguyễn Văn Khang. (2014b). Giao Tiếp Xưng Hô Tiếng Việt Bằng Từ Thân Tộc Và
Việc Sử Dụng Chúng Trong Giao Tiếp Công Quyền. Ngôn Ngữ & Đời Sống. No. 10
(228), 2014.
[113] Lê Trung Hoa. (1992). Họ và Tên Người Việt nam. Ha nôi: Nhà Xuất Bản Khoa Học
Xã Hội.
[114] Nguyễn Thị Hồng and Nguyễn Thị Anh Đào. (1996). Giáo trình Từ Vựng Học Tiếng
Anh (unpublished coursebook). Thành phố Hồ Chí Minh: Trường Cao Đẳng Sư Phạm
TP. Hồ Chí Minh.
[115] Luong Van Hy (Hy Van Luong). (1988). Discursive Practices and Power Structure:
Person Referring Forms and Sociopolitical Struggles in Colonial Vietnam. American
Ethnologist , Vol. 15, No. 2, , 239-253.
[116] Dương Thị Liễu. (2008). Bài Giảng Văn Hóa Kinh Doanh. Ha noi: Nhà Xuất bản Kinh
Tế Quốc Dân
[117] Trần Ngọc Thêm. (1999). Cơ Sở Văn Hóa Việt Nam. Việt nam: Nhà Xuất Bản Giáo
Dục.
III-Websites
[118] http://boingboing.net/2013/12/24/what-we-can-learn-from-dialect.html
[119] http://dx.doi.org/10.9707/2307-0919.1014
359
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
[120] http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/American_English
[121] http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Australian_English
[122] http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/British_English
[123] http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Dialect
[124] http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/General_american
[125] http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Language_change
[126] http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Multilingualism
[127] http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Pidgin
[128] http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Sociolinguistics
[129] http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/The_English_language
[130] http://gas.hoasen.edu.vn/en/gas-page/understanding-relationship-between-language-and-
gender
[131] http://ic-migration.webhost.uits.arizona.edu/icfiles/ic/lsp/site/index.html
[132] http://ngonngu.net/?p=313
[133] http://quochoi.vn/
[134] http://sourcebooks.fordham.edu/source/CT-prolog-para.html
[135] http://thehistoryofenglish.com/index.html
[136] http://www.era.anthropology.ac.uk/Kinship/kinIntro.html
[137] http://www.kidshealth.org
[138] http://www.omniglot.com/writing/oldenglish.htm
[139] https://ealdaenglisc.wordpress.com/old-english-lessons/lesson-0-alphabet-and-pronunciation/
[140] https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Anglo-Saxon_runes&printable=yes
[141] https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=English_alphabet&printable=yes
[142] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/African-American_English
360
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
[143] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/American_anthropology
[144] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Basil_Bernstein
[145] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Blasphemy
[146] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Chicano_English
[147] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Code-switching
[148] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Communication
[149] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Communication_strategies_in_second-language_acquisition
[150] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Comparison_of_American_and_British_English
[151] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Consanguinity
[152] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Context_(language_use)
[153] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Creole_language
[154] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Culture
[155] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Curse
[156] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/English_alphabet
[157] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/English_language_in_England
[158] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/English_phonology
[159] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Ethnography_of_communication
[160] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Euphemism
[161] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Family
[162] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Foreign_language_influences_in_English
[163] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Given_name
[164] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/History_of_English
[165] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Hofstede%27s_cultural_dimensions_theory
361
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
[166] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Incantation
[167] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/International_language
[168] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Kinship_terminology
[169] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Language_and_gender
[170] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Language_and_thought
[171] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Language_change
[172] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/List_of_dialects_of_the_English_language
[173] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Lord%27s_Prayer
[174] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Middle_English
[175] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Middle_name
[176] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/New_York_Latino_English
[177] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Oath
[178] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Oath_of_office
[179] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Oath_of_office_of_the_President_of_the_United_States
[180] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Old_English_Latin_alphabet
[181] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Pennsylvania_Dutch_English
[183] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Politeness_theory
[184] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Prayer
[185] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Profanity
[186] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Regional_accents_of_English
[187] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Rhyming_slang
[188] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Society
362
An Introduction to Sociolinguistics
(A Coursebook)
[189] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Standard_language
[190] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Style(manner_of_address)
[191] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Surname
[192] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Taboo
[193] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Talk
[194] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Testimony
[195] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/T–V_distinction
[196] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Vietnamese_language
[197] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Vietnamese_name
[198] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/World_Englishes
[199] https://seahorseviet.files.wordpress.com/2011/09/family-tree.jpg
[200] http://www.uky.edu/ofa/sites/www.uky.edu.ofa/files/uploads/Gender%20Styles%20in%20C
ommunication.pdf
IV-Anonymous Authors
363