Dungfly Copulation
Dungfly Copulation
Dungfly Copulation
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide
range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and
facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
https://about.jstor.org/terms
British Ecological Society is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access
to Functional Ecology
This content downloaded from 152.118.231.148 on Wed, 20 Mar 2019 03:12:46 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Functional
Ecology 1992
Sexual conflict in the dung fly Sepsis cynipsea
6, 649-653
P. I. WARD, J. HEMMI and T. ROOSLI
Zoologisches Museum der Universitat Zurich, Winterthurerstr. 190, 8057 Zurich, Switz
Summary
1. The interests of a male and a female over whether mating should occur are often in
conflict and in insects conflict resolution usually favours the female.
2. A male dung fly Sepsis cynipsea guards a female while she oviposits in fresh cow
dung.
3. The amount of time a female spends laying depends only on the number of eggs
laid.
4. When she leaves the dung, there is a conflict over whether or not mating should
occur.
5. A discriminant analysis shows that features of both the female and male behaviour
influence the outcome of the conflict. The proportion of time the female spends
shaking to dislodge the male and how long she spends laying were more important
than the male behaviour apparently aimed at establishing genital contact.
6. If a pair went on to copulate, only the duration of the conflict influenced the copula
duration.
7. We conclude that the female largely determines if copula occurs, possibly on the
basis of her sperm reserves, but if copula does take place then the male determines
the duration.
8. A female may in general avoid copula because of the risk of internal injury by the
male's armoured genitalia.
This content downloaded from 152.118.231.148 on Wed, 20 Mar 2019 03:12:46 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
650 differences between pairs which did and did not 30
P. 1. Ward et al. copulate in both female and male behaviour but did
not attempt to assess the relative importance of each.
j20 *
At the end of copula the male releases the female
with his legs and twists around the still-united Eb .0. 0
genitalia two or three times before he disengages
> 10 0
from the female (Parker 1972a). Ward (1983) showed
-J 0
that a male's body size influences his behaviour and 0~~~~
This content downloaded from 152.118.231.148 on Wed, 20 Mar 2019 03:12:46 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
651 d 2
was wrongly predicted. This is a correct classification
Dungfly sexual ' a rate of 97*9% and should be contrasted with the
conflict ,, 0 *
random expectation of 41%, the percentage of cases
E Ad08
E~R0_8 .0_00 in the smaller group.
Copula duration was only related to the duration
E~.4 006
o0 of the conflict and the regression was positive
(b=032?008, t=4*04, P<0O001; model
02
0 0 F1 17=16*34; P<0.001) (Fig. 4).
cL 0-0 _ __
&00
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70
No. of eggs laid
deviations for pairs which did and did not copulate. which would go on to copulate from those which
The variables were used in a stepwise discriminant would not. The time the female had spent laying her
analysis to identify the combination which best eggs was the next factor in importance. These factors
discriminated pairs which copulated from those had much greater influence than the two male traits
which did not, i.e. copula or no copula was the aimed at attaining genital contact which entered into
2). The first two variables entered had marked effects k 10.
on Wilks' X for the model as a whole and were
E 0
features of the female's behaviour; the proportion of 6
time she spent shaking and the time she had spent
2 4-
laying. The next two were the male traits apparently
0
2
aimed at establishing genital contact; the number of
strokes and temporary genital contacts per minute,
but these had much smaller effects on the model. The -2-
I 1-05 1. 1-15 1-2 1-25 1-3 1-35 1-4
overall canonical correlation was 0*881, equivalent to
Female hind tibia length (mm)
the Pearson correlation between the discriminant
Fig. 3. The relationship between the female's hind tibia
score and the grouping variable. The classification
length and the number of strokes, which are almost
using these discriminant results contained only one
certainly attempts to attain genital contact, made by the
error. All the pairs which did copulate were success- male. The sizes of the circles reflect the degree of overlap
fully identified and one pair which did not copulate amongst points.
Table 1. The means and SD for the variables used in the discriminant analysis of pairs which did or did not copulate. n= 19
copulated and n=27 did not copulate
Variable x SD x SD
This content downloaded from 152.118.231.148 on Wed, 20 Mar 2019 03:12:46 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
652 40- genital contact, per minute of the conflict the larger
P. I. Ward et al. 0 the female with which he was paired. This makes
.E3*E 30 -* evolutionary sense as larger females produce larger
o20 -~~~ ' batches of eggs than smaller ones (Ward 1983). A
al20- * *0 male will therefore probably fertilize more eggs in the
female's next batch the larger the female. A male's
size was not found to be significantly related to any of
C)
Wilks'
Step Variable entered X Equivalent F df P
This content downloaded from 152.118.231.148 on Wed, 20 Mar 2019 03:12:46 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
653 Acknowledgements Ridley, M. (1990) The control and frequency of mating in
insects. Functional Ecology 4, 75-84.
Dung fly sexual Thanks to R. Semlitsch, A. Porter and an anonymous
Sokal, R.R. & Rohlf, F.J. (1981) Biometry, 2nd edn. W.H.
conflict referee for comments on Freeman,
the San manuscript.
Fraficisco.
Spieth, H.T. (1952) Mating behaviour within the genus
Drosophila. Bulletin of the American Museum of Natural
References History 99, 395-474.
SPSS (1990) SPSS Reference Guide. SPSS, Chicago.
Eberhard, W.G. (1985) Sexual Selection and Animal Geni- Thornhill, R. (1984) Alternative hypotheses for traits
talia. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Massa- believed to have evolved by sperm competition. Sperm
chusetts. Competition and the Evolution of Animal Mating Systems
Halliday, T. & Arnold, S.J. (1987) Multiple mating by (ed. R.L. Smith), pp. 151-178. Academic Press, New
females: a perspective from quantitative genetics. York.
Animal Behaviour 35, 939-941. Thornhill, R. & Alcock, J. (1983) The Evolution of Insect
Parker, G.A. (1972a) Reproductive behaviour of Sepsis Mating Systems. Harvard University Press, Cambridge,
cynipsea (L.) (Diptera: Sepsidae) I. A preliminary Massachusetts.
analysis of the reproductive strategy and its associated Waage, J.K. (1979) Dual function of the damselfly penis:
behaviour patterns. Behaviour 41, 172-206. sperm removal and transfer. Science 203, 916-918.
Parker, G.A. (1972b) Reproductive behaviour of Sepsis Ward, P.I. (1983) The effects of size on the mating
cynipsea (L.) (Diptera: Sepsidae) II. The significance of behaviour of the dung fly Sepsis cynipsea. Behavioral
the precopulatory passive phase and emigration. Behav- Ecology and Sociobiology 13, 75-80.
iour 41, 242-250. Ward, P.I. & Simmons, L. (1991) Copula duration and
Parker G.A. (1979) Sexual selection and sexual conflict. testes size in the yellow dung fly, Scathophaga stercoraria
Sexual Selection and Reproductive Competition in Insects (L.): the effects of diet, body size, and mating history.
(eds. M.S. Blum & N.A. Blum), pp. 123-166. Academic Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 29, 77-85.
Press, New York.
Pont, A.C. (1979) Sepsidae. Royal Entomological Society Received 13 January 1992; revised 26 May 1992; accepted I
of London, London. June 1992
This content downloaded from 152.118.231.148 on Wed, 20 Mar 2019 03:12:46 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms