Heller 1984
Heller 1984
Heller 1984
SOCIOLINGUISTICS: THEORY
Monica Heller
1. INTRODUCTION
47
48 MONICA HELLER
The concept of social network is, of course, not new in itself (cf.,
e.g., Bott 1971, Barnes 1972, Boissevain and Mitchell 1973). Gumperz
(1982a; cf., also Blom and Gumperz 1972) and Gal (1979) were already
applying the concept in sociolinguistics in the 1970s, as was Labov in his
studies of Martha's Vineyard and of adolescent boys' gangs (Labov 1972).
More recently, stimulated largely by Milroy's (1980) work in Belfast, it
has increasingly been applied in variationist work (Labov, et at. 1983), and
also in the area of language contact (Blanc, et al. 1982).
that people who interact, who bear social relationships to each other, come
to share knowledge, or interpretations of how the world works, and this is
reflected in shared patterns of language use which conventionally reflect
shared knowledge. The notion of network is important in that it provides a
means of linking the establishment of shared knowledge in face-to-face
interaction to social processes—to the macrosociolinguistic context—
because it provides an explanation for how social interaction is itself
patterned and constrained. It thus permits sociolinguistics to link social
interactions to each other, in order to explain how knowledge pertinent to
individuals is tied to knowledge shared by larger groups. Finally, networks
provide a link to larger-scale political and economic processes, since those
processes can be seen to affect social organization (including social
networks) in general.
Breitborde (1983) has argued that the concept of network does not do
the job of linking micro-level and macro-level processes, because it does not
account for the ambiguity of multiple statuses which may obtain in any given
situation. This is best accounted for, he argues, by appealing to Fortes'
concept of domains of social life, each of which may have its own principles
of organization and sets of statuses. Ambiguity of multiple statuses is
explained by "degree of overlap between domains" (p. 26); Brice Heath, Gal and
Milroy, in their comments on Breitborde's paper a few pages further along,
contend that networks can indeed address this problem through the concepts of
the density and multiplexity of networks. In addition, Milroy and Gal argue
that networks, but not domains, can account for the definition, transmission,
and change of the opinions and values which inform individual speakers' use
of linguistic resources in the accomplishment of social goals. In this
respect, networks link micro-level and macro-level processes in more ways
than do domains.
associated with the use of each language, and they show how variation in one
language is reduced as the range of activities in which an individual
participates using that language is restricted. (See also Lambert and Freed
(1982) for a discussion of language loss or attrition.)
In the following section I will deal with the substantive areas of focus,
of language change and language use, and their contributions to recent socio-
linguistic theory.
4.1. Studies of children's discourse styles have begun to take the approach
that speech situation interacts with social background to produce a range
of discourse styles appropriate to certain kinds of speakers under certain
kinds of social circumstances (Cazden, Michaels, and Tabors in press,
Hemphill 1984). In other words, children learn different ways of speaking
according to their social background and their social role: socioeconomic
class, ethnocultural background, and sex are all factors that seem to be
linked to ways of speaking. None of this is new, but what is new is the
idea that, when confronted with new language use situations (such as they
might find when they start school), children develop strategies for dealing
54 MONICA HELLER
with this new situation based on the cultural conventions of language use
that they already have. Thus, communicative situations may present specific
communicative constraints, which speakers have to deal with based on the
communicative resources they already possess. Again, the concept of verbal
repertoire is invoked, but inter- and intra-speaker variation is linked
both to the community in which ways of speaking are learned and to the
challenges and constraints imposed by new experiences as social networks and
activities become more elaborated or change. Heller (1982) has discussed
changes in conversational routines which are linked not to an individual's
passage into new roles (i.e., change on the level of individual experience)
but to social change which is experienced (although experienced differently)
by all members of the community. Social change alters the background
knowledge about social relationships which conversational routines reflect
and symbolize, and so speakers must tap their (socio—) linguistic resources
for strategies to negotiate new frames of reference and new routines.
5. CONCLUSION
UNANNOTATED BIBLIOGRAPHY
Agar, M. 1982. Whatever happened to cognitive anthropology? Human
organization. 41.1.82-85.
. 1984. Ethnography doesn't live here anymore. Paper presented at the
Fifth Annual Forum on Ethnography in Education. Philadelphia,
University of Pennsylvania. 1 April.
Alatis, J. E. (ed.) 1980. Current issues in bilingual education. Washington,
DC: Georgetown University Press. [Georgetown University Round Table.]
Auer, J. C. P. 1984. On the meaning of conversational codeswitching. To
appear in J. C. P. Auer and A. di Luzio (eds.) Interpretive soaio-
linguistics. Tubingen. [TS cited; no pagination available.]
and A. di Luzio. 1983. Structure and meaning of linguistic
variation in Italian migrant children in Germany. In R. Bauerle,
C. Schwarze, and A. von Stechow (eds.) Meaning, use and interpretation
of languages. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter. 1-21.
Barnes, J. 1972. Social networks. Addison-Wesley module in anthropology.
Indianapolis: Addison-Wesley. [No. 26.]
Bauerle, R., C. Schwarze, and A. von Stechow (eds.) Meaning, use and
interpretation of languages. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter. 1-21.
Bauman, R. and J. Sherzer (eds.) 1980. Language and speech in American
society. Austin, TX: Southwest Educational Development Laboratory.
Bell, A. 1984. Language style as audience design. Language in society.
13.2.145-204.
Bennett, A. 1984. Voices of others: Literacy in an East Harlem school.
Paper presented at the Fifth Annual Forum on Ethnography in Education.
Philadelphia, University of Pennsylvania. 30 March.
Blanc, M., et di. 1982. Reseaux de communication. In A. Prujiner (ed.)
Variation du comportement langagier lorsque deux langues sont en
contact. Quebec: C.I.R.B. 73-77.
Blom, J. P. and J. Gumperz. 1972. Social meaning in linguistic structures:
Code-switching in Norway. In J. Gumperz and D. Hymes (eds.) directions
in sociolinguistics: The ethnography of communication. New York:
Holt, Rinehart and Winston. 407-434.
Boissevain, J. and J. Mitchell. 1973. Network analysis. The Hague: Mouton.
Bott, E. 1971. Family and social network. London: Tavistock. [Revised
edition.]
Breitborde, L. 1983. Levels of analysis in sociolinguistic explanation:
Bilingual codeswitching, social relations, and domain theory.
International journal of the sociology of language. 39.5-43.
56 MONICA HELLER