The Efficacy of The Mind Map Study Technique
The Efficacy of The Mind Map Study Technique
The Efficacy of The Mind Map Study Technique
net/publication/11344656
CITATIONS READS
278 12,517
3 authors, including:
SEE PROFILE
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
All content following this page was uploaded by Paul Andrew Farrand on 12 February 2019.
Objectives To examine the effectiveness of using the However this improvement was only robust after a
‘mind map’ study technique to improve factual recall week for those in the mind map group. At 1 week, the
from written information. factual knowledge in the mind map group was greater
Design To obtain baseline data, subjects completed a by 10% (adjusting for baseline) (95% CI –1% to
short test based on a 600-word passage of text prior to 22%). However motivation for the technique used was
being randomly allocated to form two groups: ‘self- lower in the mind map group; if motivation could have
selected study technique’ and ‘mind map’. After a been made equal in the groups, the improvement with
30-minute interval the self-selected study technique mind mapping would have been 15% (95% CI 3% to
group were exposed to the same passage of text previ- 27%).
ously seen and told to apply existing study techniques. Conclusion Mind maps provide an effective study
Subjects in the mind map group were trained in the technique when applied to written material. However
mind map technique and told to apply it to the passage before mind maps are generally adopted as a study
of text. Recall was measured after an interfering task technique, consideration has to be given towards ways
and a week later. Measures of motivation were taken. of improving motivation amongst users.
Setting Barts and the London School of Medicine and Keywords ANOVA; Education, medical, undergraduate/
Dentistry, University of London. *methods; educational measurement, *methods;
Subjects 50 second- and third-year medical students. London; *motivation; problem-based learning,
*methods.
Results Recall of factual material improved for both
the mind map and self-selected study technique Medical Education 2002;36:426–431
groups at immediate test compared with baseline.
were alternately assigned to the between subjects to return 30 min later. Subjects in the mind map
conditions (mind map vs. self-selected study technique) condition however, were given a 30-minute lesson in
based on the order in which they presented for the the mind map technique. The lesson used material
study. The allocation of students resulted in groups totally unrelated to the study text to demonstrate the
with similar characteristics with the exception that there best ways to produce and memorize mind maps and
were slightly fewer men in the mind map group (see gave subjects the opportunity to ask questions about the
Table 1). There was no subject drop-out from any of technique.
the sessions. During session 1, subjects were exposed to the study
text again for an additional 10-minute period. Subjects
in the mind map condition were advised to divide the
Materials
time between reading the study text and producing a
A 600-word sample of text was taken from an article mind map and studying it. Subjects forming the self-
which had appeared in Scientific American. The topic of selected study technique condition were advised to
the sample was unusual forms of transportation and had divide their time between reading the text and using
been chosen to minimize the possibility that subjects their existing study methods. None of the subjects in
had prior knowledge about the material to be tested. the self-selected study technique condition used mind
Three question sets containing 15 questions each were maps as their preferred method of study. Following a
developed from the study text. All questions were of a mental arithmetic task all subjects were again given
similar length and required the recall of a specific piece 5 minutes to complete the question set. Additionally all
of information presented in the text (for example, ‘Who subjects were requested to complete a 5-point scale
ordered the cancellation of the atomic plane program?’). which assessed level of motivation in studying the
Question sets were extensively piloted to ensure that material (1 ¼ very unmotivated, 5 ¼ very motivated).
ceiling or floor effects did not arise, and each set At the end of this session the mind maps were collected
reflected a similar level of difficulty. The order of from the mind map group and all the subjects were told
presentation of the question sets was fully randomized about session 2 and requested to attend 1 week later.
by both subject and session throughout the study. At session 2, all subjects were given 5 minutes to
complete the final question set without any additional
exposure to the study text.
Procedure
Table 2 Difference in mean correct answers (95% CI) between mind map and self-selected study technique groups, at session 1 (day 1)
and session 2 (day 7)
Session 1(day 1)
Adjusted for baseline 0Æ8 ()0Æ6 to 2Æ3) 5Æ5 ()4Æ3 to 15Æ9) 0Æ26
Adjusted for baseline and motivation 1Æ7 (0Æ3 to 3Æ1) 11Æ3 (2Æ1 to 20Æ4) 0Æ016
Session 2 (day 7)
Adjusted for baseline 1Æ6 ()0Æ2 to 3Æ4) 10Æ7 ()1Æ1 to 22Æ5) 0Æ07
Adjusted for baseline and motivation 2Æ3 (0Æ5 to 4Æ1) 15Æ3 (3Æ3 to 27Æ3) 0Æ013
Discussion
Analysis of the data indicates that, as a strategy to
improve memory for written information, the mind map
technique has the potential for an important improve-
ment in efficacy. With the mind map technique there is
an estimated increase of 10% without any change in
motivation, and an increase of 13% if there were such a
change. Mind maps are unlikely to worsen the learning
process as the lower end of the 95% confidence interval
excluded a deterioration of more than 1%, and it is likely
in practice that those who did not like the technique
would continue with the one they currently used. At
Figure 1 Mean unadjusted correct recall (maximum score ¼ 15),
both session 1 and session 2, the mind map technique
using mind map and self-selected study techniques. resulted in correct recall of a statistically significantly
higher number of items than the self-selected study
technique, when adjustments for baseline score and
study groups. For both session 1 and session 2, the motivation were made. Importantly, the recall ad-
mind map group recalled significantly more correct vantage with the mind map technique was robust over a
items than the self-selected study group, showing a week, resulting in a 24% proportional increase in correct
clear study advantage when the mind map was used, recall when compared with baseline score. Recall
which was robust over a 1-week time delay (see performance in the self-selected study technique was
Table 2). actually a little worse (–6%) over the week. That
That motivation may have differentially affected the improvements in recall at a week re-test remained evi-
mind map and self-selected study technique groups is dent with just a single exposure to the mind map tech-
further supported by a different pattern of partial nique suggests a real benefit of this study technique
correlations with baseline performance controlled. compared with other memory strategies where benefits
Motivation was significantly correlated with perform- have been confined only to tests of immediate recall.16
ance at both session 1 and session 2 for the self-selected That differences exist between the groups in
study technique group (rdf¼22 ¼ 0Æ57, P ¼ 0Æ002; memory performance suggests that improvements that
rdf¼22 ¼ 0Æ45, P ¼ 0Æ039, respectively). Correlations arise with mind maps are likely to be dependent upon
improvements in cognitive processing. Whilst it is interested in the material being studied.29 It should be
beyond the scope of this study to identify the nature of noted, however, that if successful efforts were made to
the cognitive processes supported when mind maps are counter the reductions in motivation encountered when
used, it is likely that mind maps encourage a deeper the mind map technique was used, significant further
level of processing than that obtained with the other, recall benefits of mind mapping would be expected.
more conventional study techniques adopted in the This paper has shown the efficacy of using mind
self-selected study technique. A deeper level of infor- maps as a study aid, even when use has been limited to
mation processing has been associated with better a single exposure. The increased use of mind maps, and
academic performance by medical students.20 It has the emergence of educational materials supporting the
also been acknowledged, however, that fostering a use of mind maps, within medical curricula, should
deep level of learning is very difficult because students therefore be cautiously welcomed. The mind map
do not spontaneously adopt strategies that foster such technique would seem to be particularly suited to
learning.21 medical curricula based around PBL, as both approa-
A highly surprising finding was that motivation in the ches support, and encourage students to adopt a deeper
self-selected study technique group was significantly level of learning. Nevertheless, before mind maps were
higher than that in the mind map group, even though recommended as a study technique, a way of providing
participants in the latter group received training in the effective training would need to be established so that
mind map technique. students were encouraged and enthusiastic about
Straightforward relationships between training and adopting this approach in preference to other more
motivation have been extensively reported,22,23 with conventional study techniques. With this aim in mind,
previous research also indicating that that the provision one possibility would be the incorporation of a mind
of task training is often accompanied by increased map training course into the first few terms of the
motivation in engaging with and using a study tech- medical curriculum, accompanying other sessions in
nique.24 Training is believed to focus individuals study skills often provided during the initial stages of a
towards directing their efforts to working on the task PBL curriculum.
given rather than dividing their attention between other
tasks, and to increase the amount of time the individual
Acknowledgements
chooses to spend completing an activity.25 For both
groups, the well-established relationship between Thanks are due to Richard Rowe for comments on an
motivation and recall performance26 was supported. earlier draft of this paper.
This relationship was weaker for the mind map group,
however, and was only marginally significant (P ¼ 0Æ07)
Contributors
at session 2.
A possibility which might account for the lower level Contributions to the paper were as follows: Fearzana
of motivation in the mind map group is that there is Hussain originated the idea behind the research and
resistance to using memory strategies as study aids. conducted the experimentation; Paul Farrand designed
Students, especially males, have been reported to be the experiment and wrote the paper, whilst Enid
reluctant to use many memory strategies, preferring to Hennessy conducted the statistical analysis.
adopt more conventional techniques such as rehearsal,
repetition and summary elaboration, techniques
Funding
exclusively adopted by the students in the self-selected
study group.27 Motivation in the mind map group may The research was completed without external funding.
therefore have been lower than in the self-selected study
group as students were trained and instructed to use a
References
strategy they were already reluctant to employ, which
may potentially raise other issues such as compliance in 1 McDermott P, Clarke DN. Mind Maps in Medicine. Edin-
using this study technique.28 Higher levels of motiva- burgh: Churchill Livingstone; 1998.
tion might have been obtained if the medical students 2 Buzan T, Buzan B. The Mind Map Book. London: BBC
used as participants had believed the mind map method Books; 1997.
3 Bellezza FS. The spatial arrangement mnemonic. J Educ
might improve their exam results, or if they had used it
Psychol 1983;75:830–7.
to study medically related information. Previous
4 Bellezza FS, Goverdhan RB. Mnemonic devices and natural
research indicates that students are only motivated in memory. Bull Psychon Soc 1978;11:277–80.
their use of memory strategies when they are also
5 Ross J, Lawrence KA. Some observations on memory artifice. 18 Albernese MA, Mitchell S. Problem-based learning: a review
Psychon Sci 1968;13:107–8. of literature on its outcomes and implementation issues. Acad
6 Jamieson DG, Schimpf MG. Self-generated images are more Med 1993;68:52–81.
effective mnemonics. J Ment Imag 1980;4:25–33. 19 Hemker HC. Critical perceptions of problem-based learning.
7 Bower G. Organizational factors in memory. Cognit Psychol Adv Health Sci Educ 1998;3:71–6.
1970;1:18–46. 20 McManus IC, Richards P, Winder BC, Sprotson KA. Clinical
8 Craik FIM, Lockhart RS. Levels of processing: a framework for experience, performance in final examinations, and learning
memory research. J Verb Learn Verb Behav 1972;11:671–84. style in medical students: prospective study. Br Med
9 Lockhart RS, Craik FIM. Levels of processing: a retrospective J 1998;316:345–50.
commentary on a framework for memory research. Can 21 Sandberg J, Yvonne B. Deep learning is difficult. Instruct Sci
J Psychol 1990;44:87–112. 1997;25:15–36.
10 Anderson JR, Reder LM. An elaborative processing explan- 22 Stipek DJ. Motivation and instruction. In: DC Berliner, RC
ation of depth of processing. In: LS Cermack, FIM Craik, eds. Calfee, eds. Handbook of Educational Psychology. London:
Levels of Processing in Human Memory. Hillside, NJ: Erlbaum; Prentice Hall International; 1996.
1979. 23 Lepper MR. Motivational considerations in the study of
11 Eysenck MW, Eysenck MC. Effects of processing depth, instruction. Cognit Instruct 1988;5:289–309.
distinctiveness and word frequency on retention. Br J Psychol 24 Ford ME. Motivating Humans: Goals, Emotions and Personal
1980;71:263–74. Agency Beliefs. London: Sage; 1992.
12 Tess DE, Hutchinson RL, Treloar JH, Jenkins CM. Bizarre 25 Kanfer R. Motivation theory. In: MD Dunnette, ML Hough,
imagery and distinctiveness: implications for the classroom. eds. Handbook of Industrial and Organizational Psychology, vol. 1.
J Ment Imag 1999;23:153–70. California: Consulting Psychologists’ Press; 1996.
13 Brooks JO, Friedman L, Pearman AM, Gray C, Yesavage JA. 26 Eagly AH, Chen S, Chaiken S, Shaw Barnes K. The impact of
Mnemonic training in older adults: effects of age, length of attitudes on memory: an affair to remember. Psychol Bull
training and type of cognitive pretraining. Int Psychogeriatr 1999;125:64–89.
1999;11:75–84. 27 Soler MJ, Ruiz JC. The spontaneous use of memory aids at
14 De Beni R, Mo A, Cornoldi C. Learning from texts or lec- different educational levels. Appl Cognit Psychol 1999;10:41–51.
tures: loci mnemonics can interfere with reading but not with 28 Campos A, Loepoz A, Perez MJ. Non-compliance with
listening. Eur J Cognit Psychol 1997;9:401–15. instructions in studies of the use of imagery as a memory aid.
15 Hunter IHL. Imagery, comprehension and mnemonics. Imag Cognit Personal 1999;18:241–9.
J Ment Imag 1977;1:65–72. 29 Krapp A. Interest, motivation and learning: an educational-
16 Thomas MH, Wang AY. Learning by keyword mnemonic: psychological perspective. Eur J Psychol Educ 1999;14:23–40.
looking for long-term benefits. J Exp Psychol App
1996;2:330–42. Received 14 April 2000; editorial comments to authors 1 August 2000
17 Davis MH, Harden RM. AMEE Medical education guide no. and 21 June 2001; accepted for publication 14 August 2001
15. Problem based learning: a practical guide. Med Teacher
1999;21:130–40.