Written Report Lingiustic

Download as docx, pdf, or txt
Download as docx, pdf, or txt
You are on page 1of 25

BATANGAS STATE UNIVERSITY

COLLEGE OF ARTS AND SCIENCES


Pablo Borbon Main I, Batangas City

Written Report on Sound Patterns of Language

One of the core fields that compose the discipline of linguistics,

which is the scientific study of language structure. One way to understand

the subject matter of phonology is to contrast it with other fields within

linguistics. A very brief explanation is that phonology is the study of sound

structure in language, which is different from the study of sentence

structure (syntax), word structure (morphology), or how languages change

over time (historical linguistics). The pronunciation of a given word is also a

fundamental part of the structure of the word. And certainly, the principles

of pronunciation in a language are subject to change over time. So,

phonology has a relationship to numerous domains of linguistics

Researchers and parents alike have long been intrigued by how and

when children begin to learn their first words. Although this is often thought

to occur when a child first says something like “baba,” and a parent thinks

they have (obviously) intended “mama” or “papa,” the child probably first

recognizes their own name around the age of six months (Bortfeld,

Morgan, Golinkoff, & Rathbun, 2005 and may have many words in their

receptive lexicon before they actually produce their first words, typically

between 11 to 16 months.

Phonetics – the manifestation of language sound

Ashby and Maidment (2005) provide a detailed introduction to the

subject area of phonetics. From the phonetic perspective, “sound” refers to

mechanical pressure waves and the sensations arising when such a


1
Page

pressure wave strikes your ear. In a physical sound, the wave changes
BATANGAS STATE UNIVERSITY
COLLEGE OF ARTS AND SCIENCES
Pablo Borbon Main I, Batangas City

continuously, and can be 2 introducing phonology graphed as a waveform

showing the amplitude on the vertical axis and time on the horizontal axis.

Phonology: the symbolic perspective on sound

Physical sound is too variable and contains too much information to

allow us to make meaningful and general statements about the grammar of

language sound. We require a way to represent just the essentials of

language sounds, as mental objects which grammars can manipulate. A

phonological representation of an utterance reduces this great mass of

phonetic information to a cognitive minimum, namely a sequence of

discrete segments. The basic tool for converting the continuous stream of

speech sound into discrete units is the phonetic transcription.The idea

behind a transcription is that the variability and continuity of speech can be

reduced to sequences of abstract symbols whose interpretation is

predefined, a symbol standing for all of the concrete variants of the sound.

Phonology

The study of higher-level patterns of language sound, conceived in

terms of discrete mental symbols, whereas phonetics is the study of how

those mental symbols are manifested as continuous muscular contractions

and acoustic waveforms, or how such waveforms are perceived as the

discrete symbols that the grammar acts on.

The IPA chart is arranged to suit the needs of phonetic analysis.

Standard phonological terminology and classification differ somewhat from

this usage. Phonetic terminology describes [p] as a “plosive,” where that


2
Page
BATANGAS STATE UNIVERSITY
COLLEGE OF ARTS AND SCIENCES
Pablo Borbon Main I, Batangas City

sound is phonologically termed a “stop”; the vowel [i] is called a “close”

vowel in phonetics, but a “high” vowel in phonology.

The three most important properties for defining vowels are height,

backness, and roundness. The height of a vowel refers to the fact that

the tongue is higher when producing [i] than it is when producing [e] (which

is higher than when producing [æ]), and the same holds for the relation

between [u], [o], and [a]. Three primary heights are generally recognized,

namely high, mid, and low, augmented with the secondary distinction

tense/lax for non low vowels which distinguishes vowel pairs such as [i]

(seed) vs. [ɪ] (Sid), [e] (late) vs. [ε] (let), or [u] (food) vs. [ʊ] (foot), where [i,

e, u] are tense and [ɪ, ε, ʊ] are lax. Tense vowels are higher and articulated

further from the center of the vocal tract compared to their lax

counterparts. It is not clear whether the tense/lax distinction extends to low

vowels.

Independent of height, vowels can differ in relative frontness of the

tongue. The vowel [i] is produced with a front tongue position, whereas [u]

is produced with a back tongue position. In addition, [u] is produced with

rounding of the lips: it is common but by no means universal for back

vowels to also be produced with lip rounding. Three phonetic degrees of

horizontal tongue positioning are generally recognized: front, central, and

back. Finally, any vowel can be pronounced with protrusion (rounding) of

the lips, and thus [o], [u] are rounded vowels whereas [i], [æ] are

unrounded vowels. With these independently controllable phonetic


3

parameters – five degrees of height, three degrees of fronting, and


Page

rounding versus non-rounding – we have the potential for up to thirty


BATANGAS STATE UNIVERSITY
COLLEGE OF ARTS AND SCIENCES
Pablo Borbon Main I, Batangas City

vowels, which is many more vowels than are found in English. Many of

these vowels are lacking in English, but can be found in other languages.

This yields a fairly symmetrical system of symbols and articulatory

classifications, but there are gaps such as the lack of tense/lax distinctions

among central high vowels

Where the IPA term for consonants like [p b] is “plosive,” these are

referred phonologically as “stops.” Lateral and rhotic consonants are

termed “liquids,” and non-lateral “approximants” are referred to as “glides.”

Terminology referring to the symbols for implosives, ejectives, diacritics,

and suprasegmentals is generally the same in phonological and phonetic

usage. Other classificatory terminology is used in phonological analysis to

refer to the fact that certain sets of sounds act together for grammatical

purposes.Plain stops and affricates are grouped together, by considering

affricates to be a kind of stop (one with a special fricative-type release).

Fricatives and stops commonly act as a group, and are termed obstruent's,

while glides, liquids, nasals, and vowels likewise act together, being

termed sonorants.

Rules for combining sounds.

Another aspect of language sound which a phonological analysis

takes account of is that in any language, certain combinations of sounds

are allowed, but other combinations are systematically impossible. It is

also a fact that there is no word *[blɪk] in English, even though the similar

words blink, brick do exist.


4
Page

The question is, why is there no word *blick in English?


BATANGAS STATE UNIVERSITY
COLLEGE OF ARTS AND SCIENCES
Pablo Borbon Main I, Batangas City

Answer: the nonexistence of this word is simply that it is an accidental

gap – not every logically possible combination of sounds which follows the

rules of English phonology is found as an actual word of the language.

While there are words that begin with sn like snake, snip, and snort, there

are no words beginning with bn, and thus *bnick, *bnark, *bniddle are not

words of English. There simply are no words in English which begin with

bn. Native speakers of English have a clear intuition that hypothetical

*bnick,*bnark, *bniddle could not be words of English.

Similarly, there are no words in English which are pronounced with pn at

the beginning, a fact which is not only demonstrated by the systematic lack

of words such as pnark, pnig, pnilge, but also by the fact that the word

spelled pneumonia which derives from Ancient Greek (a language which

does allow such consonant combinations) is pronounced [nʌ̍monjə] without

p. A description of the phonology of English would provide a basis for

characterizing such restrictions on sequences of sounds.

Variations in pronunciation

In addition to providing an account of possible versus impossible words in

a language, a phonological analysis will explain other general patterns in

the pronunciation of words. This pattern of pronunciation is not limited to

the plural, so despite the difference in spelling, the possessive suffix s 2 is

also subject to the same rules of pronunciation: thus, plural bushes is

pronounced the same as the possessive bush’s, and plural churches is

pronounced the same as possessive churches. This is the sense in which


5

phonology is about the sounds of language.


Page
BATANGAS STATE UNIVERSITY
COLLEGE OF ARTS AND SCIENCES
Pablo Borbon Main I, Batangas City

From the phonological perspective, a “sound” is a specific unit which

combines with other such specific units, and which represents physical

sounds. What phonology is concerned with is how sounds behave in a

grammar.

Allophonic relations

learn of predictable variants of basic sounds in English

learn about the concept’s “phoneme” and “allophone”

discover that similar relations between sounds exist in other languages

begin to learn the general technique for inducing phonological rules from

data that come from a language which you do not know be introduced to

writing phonological rules. Certain facts about pronunciation simply cannot

be predicted by rule, for example that in English the word sick is

pronounced [sɪk] and sip is pronounced [sɪp].

Hence one fundamental component of a language is a lexicon, a list of

words (or morphemes – parts of words), which must provide any

information which cannot be predicted by rules of the language. Any

information which cannot be predicted by rules of the language. However,

much about the pronunciation of words can be predicted. For example, in

the word tick the initial voiceless consonant t is phonetically aspirated, and

is phonetically [th ɪk].

This aspiration can be demonstrated visually by dangling a tissue in

front of the mouth when saying the word: notice that when you pronounce
6
Page

t, the tissue is blown forward. In comparison, t in the word stick is not


BATANGAS STATE UNIVERSITY
COLLEGE OF ARTS AND SCIENCES
Pablo Borbon Main I, Batangas City

aspirated (thus, the tissue is not blown forward), so this word is transcribed

as [stɪk]. This fact can be predicted by rule, and we now consider how this

is done.

English consonantal allophones

While the physical difference between t and th in English is just as real as

the difference between t and d, there is a fundamental linguistic difference

between these two relationships. The selection of t versus d may constitute

the sole difference between many different words in English: such words,

where two words are differentiated exclusively by a choice between one of

two segments, are referred to as minimal pairs. The difference between [t]

and [d] is contrastive (also termed distinctive) in English, since this

difference – voicing – forms the sole basis for distinguishing different

words (and thus, [t] and [d] contrast).The choice of a voiceless aspirated

stop such as [th ] versus a voiceless unaspirated stop such as [t], on the

other hand, never defines the sole basis for differentiating words in

English. The occurrence of [t] versus [th ] (also [k] versus [kh ], and [p]

versus [ph ]) follows a rule that aspirated stops are used in one

phonological context, and unaspirated stops are used in all other contexts.

In English, [t] and [th ] are predictable variants of a single abstract

segment, a phoneme, which we represent as /t/.

Purely predictable variants are termed allophones – the sounds are in

complementary distribution because the context where one variant

appears is the complement of the context where the other sound appears.
7
Page
BATANGAS STATE UNIVERSITY
COLLEGE OF ARTS AND SCIENCES
Pablo Borbon Main I, Batangas City

As we have emphasized, one concern of phonology is determining

valid relations between pronounced segments and the abstract mental

constructs that they derive from, the phonemes, which represent the unity

behind observed [t] and [th ] etc. The implicit claim is that despite there

being actual differences, [t] and [th ] (also [k] and [kh ], [p] and [ph ]) are in

a fundamental sense “the same thing.” We reduce the output sounds [t th k

kh p ph ] to just the set of sounds /t k p/, and a rule provides the

information “realized as [t] vs. [th ]” to account for these regularities.

Aspiration

We will turn our attention to rules of pronunciation in English, starting with

aspiration, to see what some of these regularities are. In the first set of

words below, the phonemes /p, t, k/ are aspirated whereas they are not

aspirated in the second set of words.

Unaspirated stops

spool [spuwl] stool [stuwl] school [skuwl] spit

[spɪt] stick [stɪk] skid [skɪd]

sap [sæp] sat [sæt] sack [sæk]

spray [spɹej] stray [stɹej] screw [skɹuw]

split [splɪt] sclerosis [skləɹowsɪs] spew [spjuw] skew

[skjuw]

The selection of an aspirated versus an unaspirated voiceless stop is

determined by the context in which the stop appears.


8
Page
BATANGAS STATE UNIVERSITY
COLLEGE OF ARTS AND SCIENCES
Pablo Borbon Main I, Batangas City

Aspirated stops appear at the beginning of a word, whereas unaspirated

stops appear after [s]; aspirated stops appear before a vowel or a sonorant

consonant, whereas unaspirated stops appear at the end of a word. This

collection of contexts can be expressed succinctly by referring to the

position of the consonant in the syllable: aspirated stops appear at the

beginning of the syllable and unaspirated stops appear elsewhere. We

assume that the voiceless stops are basically unaspirated in English, and

explain where aspirated segments appear by having a rule that assigns

aspiration to voiceless stops, only when the stop is at the beginning of the

syllable: the rule can be stated as “voiceless stops become aspirated at

the beginning of a syllable.” We don’t need a second special rule to derive

unaspirated stops in other environments, because that follows directly from

our assumption that the basic or underlying form of the voiceless stops in

English is unaspirated, and they will therefore be pronounced as such

unless they are specifically changed by a rule.

[p], [t], and [k] in the middle of the word are not aspirated, even though the

consonant is between vowels or syllabic sonorants – between syllable

peaks – and therefore is presumably at the beginning of a syllable

1. ˈhæpij happy ˈkhæmpɪŋ camping ˈhɛlpɪŋ helping

ˈlʌkij lucky ˈsʌkɹ̩ sucker ˈ saltij salty

Compare these words with seemingly analogous words where there

is aspiration on the stop between vowels, such as [əˈthæk] attack, [ə

ˈkhjuwmjəˌlejt] accumulate, [ ˈlejˌthɛks] latex,[ əˈphɛndɪks] appendix. The


9
Page

important difference in these words is the location of stress. In all of the


BATANGAS STATE UNIVERSITY
COLLEGE OF ARTS AND SCIENCES
Pablo Borbon Main I, Batangas City

words in example 1, where a voiceless consonant is not aspirated in

syllable-initial position, the consonant is followed by an unstressed vowel.

In other words, these data force us to refine our statement of the rule for

assignment of aspiration, to be “voiceless stops become aspirated at the

beginning of a stressed syllable.” voiceless stop! aspirated / [ˈσ

This statement introduces the method of writing rules, which will be

used in the book. Rules generally take the form “A!B/C_D,” where A, C, D

are variables that stand for single segment like[l]or[d]or phonetic classes

such as “voiceless stop,” and B describes the nature of the change, some

phonetic parameter such as “voiceless” or “nasal. The conditioning context

might involve only a preceding element in which case “D” would be

missing, it might involve only a following element in which case “C” would

be missing, or the applicability of the rule might depend on both what

precedes and what follows. The arrow means “becomes,” the slash means

“in the environment” where the context is what follows the slash. The

notation “[ˈσ” means “beginning of a stressed syllable,” thus “voiceless

stops become aspirated when they are preceded by the beginning of a

stressed syllable

Alternations involving aspiration

Certain word-formation processes in English change the location of

stress ,for example in atom the stress is on the first syllable of the root and

in the related adjective atomic the stress is on the second syllable


10

[əˈphlaj] apply [ˌæpləˈkhejʃn̩ ] application


Page
BATANGAS STATE UNIVERSITY
COLLEGE OF ARTS AND SCIENCES
Pablo Borbon Main I, Batangas City

[səˈphowz] suppose [ˌsʌpəˈzɪʃn̩ ] supposition

[əˈkhwajɹ] acquire [ˌækwəˈzɪʃn̩ ] acquisition

Pronunciation of novel utterances

When English speakers are faced with a new word which they have never

heard before, for example one coming from a foreign language, voiceless

consonants will be aspirated or unaspirated according to the general rule

for the distribution of aspiration. The pronunciation of unfamiliar foreign

place names provides one simple demonstration. The place names Stord

in Norway and Palma Mozambique will be pronounced by English

speakers as [stɔɹd] and [phalmə], as predicted by the aspiration rule.

The name Stavanger (Norway) may be pronounced many ways – [stə

ˈvæŋɹ̩ ], [ˈstævəndʒɹ̩ ], [stəˈvændʒɹ̩ ], [ˈstævəŋɹ̩ ], and so on, but consistently

throughout this variation, the /t/ will remain unaspirated because of its

position in the syllable.

When English speakers attempt to learn a language, which does not have

the same distribution of aspirated and unaspirated consonants as in

English, they encounter difficulties in pronunciation that reflect the effect of

the rule of aspiration. Even in native English words, unaspirated stops can

show the effect of the aspiration rule in hyper-slow, syllable-by-syllable

pronunciation. Notice that in the normal pronunciation of happy [ˈhæpij],

only the first syllable is tressed and therefore [p] remains unaspirated.

However, if this word is pronounced very slowly, drawing out each vowel,
11

then both syllables become stressed, and as predicted the stop p is


Page

aspirated – [ˈhæ::]... [ˌphi::j]. All of these facts are explained by one simple
BATANGAS STATE UNIVERSITY
COLLEGE OF ARTS AND SCIENCES
Pablo Borbon Main I, Batangas City

hypothesis, that in English the occurrence of aspiration on stops derives

from applying a rule.

Flapping

Another rule.

A phonetic characteristic of many North American dialects of English

is “flapping,” where /t/ and /d/ become the flap [ɾ] in certain contexts, for

example in [ˈwaɾɹ̩ ] water. It is clear that there is no contrast between the

flap [ɾ] and any other consonant of English.

water ˈwejɾɹ̩ waiter; wader waɾɹ̩ ˈæɾm̩

atom; Adam ˈ æɾəˌthuwd attitude

ˈhɪt hit ˈhɪɾɪŋ hitting ˈpʊt put ˈ

pʊɾɪŋ putting sɛt set ˈsɛɾɪŋ settin

We may state the rule of flapping as follows: “an alveolar stop becomes a

flap when it is followed by an unstressed syllabic and is preceded by a

vowel or glide.”

You will see how vowels and glides are unified in the next chapter: for the

moment, we use the term vocoid to refer to the phonetic class of vowels

and glides. It is again important to note that the notion of “vowel” used in

this rule must include syllabic sonorants such as [ɹ̩ ] for the preceding

segment, and [ɹ̩ ] or [m̩ ] for the following segment. Flapping is not limited to

the voiceless alveolar stop /t/: underlying /d/ also becomes [ɾ] in this same
12

context.
Page
BATANGAS STATE UNIVERSITY
COLLEGE OF ARTS AND SCIENCES
Pablo Borbon Main I, Batangas City

Base verbs ‘One who V-s’‘ V-ing’

ˈbɪd ˈ bɪɾɹ̩ ˈbɪɾɪŋ bid

hajd ˈ hajɾɹ̩ ˈ hajɾɪŋ hide

wejd ˈwejɾɹ̩ ˈwejɾɪŋ wade

Glottal stop

There is one context where flapping of/t/does not occur when preceded by

a vowel and followed by an unstressed syllabic segment (vowel or syllabic

sonorant), and that is when /t/ is followed by a syllabic [n̩ ]. Consider, first,

examples such as [ˈbʌʔn̩ ] button and [ˈkaʔn̩ ] cotton. Instead of the flap

that we expect, based on our understanding of the context where flapping

takes place, we find glottal stop before syllabic[n̩ ]

[rat] rot [ˈraʔn̩ ] rotten [hajt] height

[ˈhajʔn̩ ] heighten [lajt] light [ˈlajʔn̩ ] lighten

[fæt] fat [ˈfæʔn̩ ] fatten

The bare roots on the left show the underlying /t/ which has not changed to

glottal stop, and on the right, we observe that the addition of the suffix /n/

conditions the change of /t/ to [ʔ] in the context.Finally, notice that in casual

speech, the gerundive suffixɪŋ may be pronounced as [n̩ ]. When the verb

root ends in /t/, that /t/ becomes [ʔ] just in case the suffix becomes [n̩ ], and

thus provides the crucial context required for the glottal stop creation rule.

Take note:
13
Page
BATANGAS STATE UNIVERSITY
COLLEGE OF ARTS AND SCIENCES
Pablo Borbon Main I, Batangas City

Some speakers have [ʔ] only before syllabic [n̩ ], so their rule is different.

Not all American dialects have this rule – it is lacking in certain Southern

dialects, and instead the flapping rule applies. Some British dialects have a

rule which applies in a rather different context, e.g. [lεʔə] letter.Contrastive

aspects of pronunciation cannot be predicted by rule, but allophonic details

can be. Allophonic changes are a type of rule-governed phonological

behavior, and phonology is concerned with the study of rules. The practical

concern of this is understanding the method for discovering those rules.

The linguist looks for regularities in the distribution of one sound versus

others, and attempts to reduce multiple surface segments to one basic

segment, a phoneme, where the related segments derive by applying a

rule to the underlying phoneme in some context.

Assuming that sounds are in complementary distribution, you need to

determine which variant is the “basic” underlying one, and which derives

by rule. The decision is made by comparing the consequences of

alternative hypotheses.

Feature theory

One of the scientific questions that need to be asked about language is:

What is a possible speech sound?

Humans can physically produce many more kinds of sounds than are

used in language.No language employs hand-clapping, finger-snapping, or

vibrations of air between the hand and cheek caused by release of air from
14

the mouth when obstructed by the palm of the hand (though such a sound
Page

can easily communicate an attitude).


BATANGAS STATE UNIVERSITY
COLLEGE OF ARTS AND SCIENCES
Pablo Borbon Main I, Batangas City

The pronunciation of a given word is also a fundamental part of the

structure of the word. And certainly the principles of pronunciation in a

language are subject to change over time. So phonology has a relationship

to numerous domains of linguistics

A goal of a scientific theory of language is to systematize such facts

and explain them; thus we have discovered one limitation on language

sound and its modality – language sounds are produced exclusively within

the mouth and nasal passages, in the area between the lips and larynx.

Even staying within the vocal tract, languages also do not, for example,

use whistles or inhalation to form speech sounds, nor is a labiolingual trill

(a.k.a. “the raspberry”) a speech sound in any language.The “raspberry” in

American culture communicates a contemptuous attitude; in parts of

coastal East Africa and Scandinavia, inhaling with the tongue in the

position for schwa expresses agreement. Such noises lie outside of

language, and we never find plurality indicated with these sounds, nor are

they surrounded by other sounds to form the word dog. raspberry” in

American culture communicates a contemptuous attitude; in parts of

coastal East Africa and Scandinavia, inhaling with the tongue in the

position for schwa expresses agreement. Such noises lie outside of

language, and we never find plurality indicated with these sounds, nor are

they surrounded by other sounds to form the word dog.

The central question is:


15
Page
BATANGAS STATE UNIVERSITY
COLLEGE OF ARTS AND SCIENCES
Pablo Borbon Main I, Batangas City

What is the basis for defining possible speech sounds? Do we use

our “speech anatomy” in every imaginable way, or only in certain

well-defined ways?

Varieties of phonetic [i] vs. [ɪ].

Many languages have this pair of vowels; for example, Matuumbi

has [i] and [ɪ]. But the actual pronunciation of [i] vs. [ɪ] differs between

English and Matuumbi(spoken language in Tanzania) [i] is higher than in

English, and Matuumbi. Matuumbi [ɪ] is a bit lower than English [ɪ] – to

some people it almost sounds like [e] (but is clearly different from [e], even

the “pure” [e] found in Spanish) (language spoken in Tanzania). This might

force us to introduce new symbols, so that we can accurately represent

these distinctions. You do not have to go very far in to exotic languages to

find this phonetic difference, for the difference between English [i] and

German [i] is also very noticeable, and is something that a language

learner must master to develop a good German or English accent.

Other variants of sounds.

Similar variation exists with other phonetic categories. The retroflex

consonants of Telugu, Hindi, and Koti are all pronounced differently. Hindi

has what might be called “mild” retroflexion, where the tip of the tongue is

placed just behind the alveolar ridge, while in Telugu, the tip of the tongue

is further back and contact is made between the palate and the underside

of the tongue (sublaminal); in Koti, the tongue is placed further forward, but
16

is also sublaminal.
Page
BATANGAS STATE UNIVERSITY
COLLEGE OF ARTS AND SCIENCES
Pablo Borbon Main I, Batangas City

One thing that phonological theory wants to know is: what is a possible

phoneme?

How might we answer this?

We could look at all languages and publish a list. A monumental difficulty

with that is that there are nearly 7,000 languages, but useful information on

around only 10 percent of these languages

This could only say what phonemic contrasts happen to exist at the

present. A scientific account of language does not just ask what has been

actually observed, it asks about the fundamental nature of language,

including potential sounds which may have existed in a language spoken

1,000 years ago, or some future language which will be spoken 1,000

years hence

Analogously, phonological theory seeks to discover the laws for building

phonemes, which predict what phonemes can be found in languages

Distinctive feature theory

Just saying that rules are defined in terms of phonetic properties is

too broad a claim, since it says nothing about the phonetic properties that

are relevant. Consider a hypothetical rule, stated in terms of phonetic

properties:

all vowels change place of articulation so that the original difference in

formant frequency between F1 and F3 is reduced to half what it originally

was, when the vowel appears before a consonant whose duration ranges
17
Page

from 100 to 135 ms.


BATANGAS STATE UNIVERSITY
COLLEGE OF ARTS AND SCIENCES
Pablo Borbon Main I, Batangas City

The Acquisition of prosodic words

Well-formed prosodic words in English must contain two moras of

structure, that is, either a long/tense vowel or diphthong, or a short vowel

and a coda consonant if a disyllable (2b), or be disyllabic (2c). Words with

just a short vowel (2a) in English are ill-formed. Nicknames are a

productive way to show this: one can shorten Philip to Phil /fɪl/ (containing

a short vowel plus coda consonant), and Susan to Sue /su/ (containing a

long vowel), but not /fɪ/ or /sʌ/, respectively—both with only a short vowel.

But how and when do children learn that English

Many other languages have such a constraint as well. For example,

Bantu languages like Sesotho (Doke & Mofokeng, 1957), spoken in

southern Africa, add another mora to monomoraic words, so that ill-formed

*ja! “eat!” becomes eja! or jaa! But this is not universal: languages like

French have no such constraint, permitting words that contain only one

mora of structure, like o “water.”. As we have already mentioned, many

languages have a word minimality constraint, but this is not universal.

Rather, it must be learned. The previous discussion suggests that learners

of English have an emerging awareness of this constraint before the age of

two, and that it appears fairly stable by two to three years. The phonetic

properties which are the basis of phonological systems are general and

somewhat abstract, such as voicing or rounding, and are largely the

categories which we have informally been using already: they are not the

same, as we will see.


18
Page
BATANGAS STATE UNIVERSITY
COLLEGE OF ARTS AND SCIENCES
Pablo Borbon Main I, Batangas City

The hypothesis of distinctive feature theory is that there is a small set,

around two dozen, of phonetically based properties which phonological

analysis uses.

These properties, the distinctive features, not only define the possible

phonemes of human languages, but also define phonological rules.

Major class features

One of the most intuitive distinctions which feature theory needs to

capture is that between consonants and vowels. There are three features,

the so-called major class features, which provide a rough first grouping of

sounds into functional types that includes the consonant/vowel distinction.

syllabic (syl): forms a syllable peak (and thus can be stressed).

sonorant (son): sounds produced with a vocal tract configuration in which

spontaneous voicing is possible.

consonantal (cons): sounds produced with a major obstruction in the oral

cavity.

Place of articulation

Features to define place of articulation are our next functional set.

We begin with the features typically used by vowels, specifically the

[+syllabic, -consonantal, +sonorant] segments, and then proceed to

consonant features, ending with a discussion of the intersection of these

features.
19

Vowel place features. The features which define place of articulation for
Page

vowels are the following


BATANGAS STATE UNIVERSITY
COLLEGE OF ARTS AND SCIENCES
Pablo Borbon Main I, Batangas City

high: the body of the tongue is raised from the neutral position.

low: the body of the tongue is lowered from the neutral position.

back: the body of the tongue is retracted from the neutral position.

round: the lips are protruded.

tense: sounds requiring deliberate, accurate, maximally distinct gestures

that involve considerable muscular effort.

advanced tongue root: produced by drawing the root of the tongue forward.

Manner of articulation

Other features relate to the manner in which a segment is produced, apart

from the location of the segment’s constriction. The manner features are:

continuant (cont): the primary constriction is not narrowed so much that

airflow through the oral cavity is blocked.

delayed release (del.rel): release of a total constriction is slowed so that a

fricative is formed after the stop portion.

nasal (nas): the velum is lowered which allows air to escape through the

nose.

lateral (lat): the mid section of the tongue is lowered at the side

Laryngeal features

Three features characterize the state of the glottis:

spread glottis (s.g.): the vocal folds are spread far apart.
20
Page

constricted glottis (c.g.): the vocal folds are tightly


BATANGAS STATE UNIVERSITY
COLLEGE OF ARTS AND SCIENCES
Pablo Borbon Main I, Batangas City

constricted. voice (voi): the vocal folds vibrate.

Prosodic features

Finally, in order to account for the existence of length distinctions,

and to represent stressed versus unstressed vowels, two other features

were proposed:

long: has greater duration.

stress: has greater emphasis, higher amplitude and pitch, longer duration.

These are obvious: long segments are [+long] and stressed vowels are

[+stress].A major lacuna in the Chomsky and Halle (1968) account of

features is a lack of features for tone.

Rule formulation and features

The most important function of features is to form the basis for writing

rules, which is crucial in understanding what defines a possible

phonological rule. A typical rule of vowel nasalization, which nasalizes all

vowels before a nasal, can be formulated very simply if stated in features:

(28) [+syllabic] ! [+nasal]/ __ [+nasal]

The idea that rules are stated in terms of the simplest, most general

classes of phonetically defined segments has an implication for rule

formulation. Suppose we encounter a rule where high vowels (but not mid

and low vowels) nasalize before nasal stops (n, m, ŋ), thus in!ĩn, uŋ!ũŋ,

and so on. We would formulate such a rule as follows


21

The Formulation of phonological rules


Page
BATANGAS STATE UNIVERSITY
COLLEGE OF ARTS AND SCIENCES
Pablo Borbon Main I, Batangas City

Many aspects of rule theory were introduced in our informal approach to

rule writing.The general form of a phonological rule is:

Language sounds can be defined in terms of a small set of universal

phonetically based features, which not only define the basic atoms of

phonological representations, but also play a central role in the formal

expression of rules.

An important theme of this chapter is the nature of scientific theories, such

as the theory of features, which make predictions both about what can

happen and what cannot happen.

The fundamental role of feature theory is to make specific predictions

about the kinds of segments and rules that we should find in human

languages.

One of the main concerns of phonological theory is finding the correct set

of features that define the sounds and rule systems of all human

languages.

The Idea behind Phonological Rules

Generative phonologists, who have worked extensively with phonological

rules, work on the basic assumption that every speaker has a mental

lexicon full of abstract entries of phonological forms in his or her head.

These abstract stored entries are underlying representations and serve as

input for the phonological rules. These underlying forms then undergo a

derivational process which is defined by the phonological rule. The output


22
Page
BATANGAS STATE UNIVERSITY
COLLEGE OF ARTS AND SCIENCES
Pablo Borbon Main I, Batangas City

of that process is the phonetic representation of the pronunciation

underlying representation.

Theory cannot be entirely avoided; indeed, it is impossible to state

generalizations about a particular language without a theory which gives

you a basis for postulating general rules. The very question of what the

raw data are must be interpreted in the context of a theory, thus analysis

needs theory. Equally, theories are formal models which impose structure

on data – theories are theories about data – so theories need data, hence

analysis. The theoretical issues discussed because they represent issues

which have come up many times in phonology, because they are

fundamental issues, and especially because they allow exploration of the

deeper philosophical issues involved in theory construction and testing.

Making a principled separation between phonetics and phonology is

difficult – just as it is difficult to make a principled separation between

physics and chemistry, or sociology and anthropology. While phonetics

and phonology both deal with language sound, they address different

aspects of sound. Phonetics deals with “actual” physical sounds as they

are manifested in human speech, and concentrates on acoustic

waveforms, formant values, measurements of duration measured in

milliseconds, of amplitude and frequency. Phonetics also deals with the

physical principles underlying the production of sounds, namely vocal tract

resonances, and the muscles and other articulatory structures used to

produce those resonances. Phonology, on the other hand, is an abstract


23

cognitive system dealing with rules in a mental grammar: principles of


Page

subconscious “thought” as they relate to language sound.


BATANGAS STATE UNIVERSITY
COLLEGE OF ARTS AND SCIENCES
Pablo Borbon Main I, Batangas City

Brief explanation is that phonology is the study of sound structure in

language, which is different from the study of sentence structure (syntax),

word structure (morphology), or how languages change over time

(historical linguistics). But this is insufficient. An important feature of the

structure of a sentence is how it is pronounced – its sound structure.

The pronunciation of a given word is also a fundamental part of the

structure of the word. And certainly, the principles of pronunciation in a

language are subject to change over time. So, phonology has a

relationship to numerous domains of linguistics.

References:

https://linguistics.ucla.edu/people/keating/IPA/IPA_charts_2018.html?

fbclid=IwAR1sUvyYsBJDkusupLMtPT0OuUUGhgu3Uen1Rl7cGzvs8SlaZM

b2CtfVxBA

https://www.sciencedirect.com/topics/psychology/phonemes?

fbclid=IwAR0MhoJ_Pm8RyUxJTeOIfQW6MnIu_d3b6ipUJ8Krjy2Ppcjy8KN

eIQ9b8wc

https://www.sciencedirect.com/topics/psychology/phonemes?

fbclid=IwAR19bJdYoGGLOzCbcKMEHf3Nc3YxDX3BDJzEaUWus5I6e81is

0dtl-RHBwg

https://www.mq.edu.au/about/about-the-university/faculties-and-

departments/faculty-of-human-sciences/departments-and-

centres/department-of-linguistics/our-research/child-language-
24

acquisition/child-language-lab/publications/2018-Demuth-OH-
Page
BATANGAS STATE UNIVERSITY
COLLEGE OF ARTS AND SCIENCES
Pablo Borbon Main I, Batangas City

Psycholinguistics_Ch29.pdf?

fbclid=IwAR0z1mUfReymTyQno3tNSr80lvI25EmaODC3GI_k3DdaZWEHjy

6mEOAcarc

http://www.sfu.ca/~mcrobbie/Ling221/%236.pdf?

fbclid=IwAR3Hmncheu6xSlS7MAcYj1erPHvz5e8oRAc3kAHXPfXQQRbPv

RBB88varbE

file:///C:/Users/Anne/Downloads/tipa-162%20(2).pdf

https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/8797/5115c23ce6f931b3ae8a2b7cda2f68

31e7fe.pdf?fbclid=IwAR3cx3rJMARdOhWY-

R614aF3S_Gv_xsnvwhBRr16fsBp8LzoOtSn0D0HVqY

file:///C:/Users/Anne/Downloads/Odden.%20Introducing%20Phonology

%202nd%20ed%20(1).pdf

25
Page

You might also like