Notes Phonology
Notes Phonology
Notes Phonology
PHONOLOGY
NOTES
Assembled by
Solomon Luvonga
@2023
TOPIC ONE:
1
What is Phonetics?
What is Phonology?
Phonology deals with the organization, grouping, patterning and distribution of the
basic sounds of natural languages (vowels and consonants). It studies the
restrictions and regular patterns of sound combinations. Hyman (1975) says that
Phonology is the study of how speech sounds structure and function in
languages. It is the task of phonology to study which differences in sound are
related to differences in meaning in a given language. It is, hence, a branch of
linguistics which studies the sound system of languages.
Hyman adds that phonology is the study of sound system. The reference to
‘system’ suggests that one is dealing with a complex whole whereby each part of
2
the system has a function. A system, therefore, has a structure because there are
functionally distinguishable parts.
Also, can be defined as the study of the inventory of sounds in a language and
of how speech sounds may pattern together, or contrast.
IMPORTANCE OF PHONOLOGY
We need phonology
to account for how sounds are grouped together despite being phonetically
distinct.
to explain how regular sound alternations occur (e.g. aspiration of /p t k/)
to explain how we extend these alternations to novel words
to explain how we extend these alternations to mistakes like
spoonerisms(misplacement of sounds in word pronunciation e.g.
ecexpt instead of except or expect, proscratination instead
of procrastination. Transposition)
to explain how we determine what possible words of our language are
to pronounce the sounds in the word correctly with respect to
stress, intonation and aspirated sounds.
to build confidence in speeches through correct
interpretation of sound basing on the fact that there are
silent and abstract sounds.
Teachers of foreign language uses the knowledge of phonology
to correct the pronunciation errors in learners
Speech therapist treats patients with speech defects or sound
failures to produce certain allophones of phonemes. pg76
3
However, Phonology studies the patterns of sounds within the system of
individual languages. It is concerned with the structure and function of
sounds. It is abstract and involves analyzing raw data to get a unified picture
of how sounds behave in the framework of a whole language system. It
refers to an insider’s view of the sound pattern.
A phonetic study tells us how the sounds of a language are made and
what the acoustic features are while in phonology, speech sounds cannot
be resolved by the study of phonetics alone. Speech sounds have both
psychological and physical dimensions. Phonetics only accounts for the
physical dimensions. The idea that speakers perceive words as discrete
sounds which occur in a sequence is not evident at the phonetic level.
4
Most linguists, until recently at least, have regarded the phonemes as one of the
basic units of a language. The term phoneme was first used by Kruzenski in late
1870s. Some linguists like Bloomfield and Daniel Jones have described phonemes
in purely physical terms. Others like Sapir have preferred psychological definition.
Some regard phonemes only as abstract fictitious units and argue that these are not
phonemes but allophones that exist in reality. Thus, we do not find the definition of
phonemes in the same way.
The change in phonological unit may be anywhere in the word which can bring
about a change in the meaning of the word. For example
b + in bi + n
p + in pi + n
t + in ti + n
s + in si + n
w + in wi + n
Allophone
5
is a common practice to write phone symbols between square brackets and
phonemes symbols between oblique brackets. Thus in English [t] and [th] are
allophones of the phoneme /t/. The variations of phoneme which cannot change
the meaning is called allophones. For example, ph, kh, th, etc are called aspirated
sounds in English. It does not bring about a change in meaning but only changes
the initial position in a word. PRACTICE: top[th] , stop[t], little [tr] and kitten
[t?].
1. Minimal Pair
When two sounds are in complementary distribution, they are barred from
occurring in identical environments. One sound appears in certain contexts and
the other in some different clearly defined contexts. This is when two or more
6
phonetically similar sounds never occur in exactly the same environment but in
complementary or mutually-exclusive environments. For instance:
Spat [p] – pool [ph]
Speak [p] – peak [ph]
tea [t] - too [tw] - eighth [tn].
The word “complementary” actually refers to the fact that the contexts in which the
allophones of a phoneme appear can never be the same and they cover the whole
range of possible environments in which the sound can occur.
3. Free Variation
Sometimes two different sounds can be freely used in the same position
without changing the meaning. Free variation is the ability of one sound to be a
substitute for another within a word without it affecting meaning. A substitution of
one for the other does not produce a different word but merely a different
pronunciation of the same word. Hence, two sounds are in free variation when:
they occur in the same environment, and
replacing one with the other does not change the meaning
7
PHONEME IDENTIFICATION
Sometimes it is not possible to find minimal pairs. In this case the phonologist has
to settle for something less rigorous which is called contrast in analogous
environments. Sounds are isolated as belonging to separate phonemes if they
occur in phonetically very similar though not identical environments provided
that differences between them cannot be reasonably attributed to the influence of
neighbouring sounds.
For example, we can regard /f/ and /v/ as separate phonemes in EWE (Nigerian
language) because they contrast in analogous environments in words like [evlo] ‘he
is evil’ and [efle] ‘he split off ‘( [e] symbolizes a nasalized [e] vowel). The
difference between nasalised [e] on the one hand and oral [e] and [o] on the other
cannot be the reason for the difference between voiceless [f] and voiced [v] since
nasal [e] as well as oral [e] and [o] are all voiced and should not affect in different
ways the voicing of neighbouring sounds.
3. Suspicious Pairs
A practising phonologist can normally safely assume that sounds like [n] and [m]
or [f] and [p] which show no phonetic resemblance are distinct phonemes. As a
rule, methods for determining whether or not sounds belong to the same
8
phoneme are only employed where SUSPICIOUS PAIRS have been identified,
that is to say, pairs of words containing sounds which only differ slightly and
which can plausibly be members of the same phoneme for example, [p/b], [t/d],
[l/r], [u/o]
Since only sounds which show considerable phonetic similarity can be grouped
together as allophones of the same phoneme, it would be pointless to comb through
an entire phonological system of language in search of minimal pair and examples
of contrast in analogous environment. Identifying pairs of words containing sounds
which only differ slightly and which can plausibly be members of the same
phoneme.
Suspicious pairs of sounds are sounds which are phonetically similar, making
their phonological status suspect. The sounds usually differ in only one, or
perhaps two, phonetic features. Because of their similarity, the two members of
the pair may prove not to be separate phonemes, but simply allophones of the same
phoneme. If this is the case, the phonetic differences between the sounds may be
explainable by the environments in which they occur. The identification of
suspicious pairs is based on linguists’ experience with a wide variety of languages.
Pairs of sounds which have turned out to be allophonic variants in other languages
are suspicious, and should be investigated closely when we work on a new
language, because similar patterns of behavior often reoccur. Admittedly, the
identification of suspicious pairs is somewhat subjective, and different pairs of
sounds will exhibit different degrees of suspiciousness.
For example
9
as phonemes. Redundant sounds are those which are predictable from a given
environment. These sounds are also termed as contextual variants or allophones.
Aspirated stops are found at the beginning of words e.g pin while unaspitated
stops are found after word initials thus the word spin is pronounced without
aspiration on [p]. This can be tested by putting the palm of our hand before our
mouth during the pronunciation of both spin and pin
In Thai, there are also two series of voiceless stop. The Thai words [phaa] ‘to split’
and [p’aa] ‘forest’ illustrate the same difference [ph] as in English words pin and
spin. In English the two different [p]s are found in different environments. The fact
that one [p] is aspirated and the other is not, is predictable from the place it falls
within a word. This is demonstrated thus:
The difference between English and Thai is that aspiration is not redundant in
Thai since [ph] and [p] both occur in exactly the same environment in ‘to split’ and
‘forest’. It is not possible to predict whether a given [p] will be aspirated or
unaspirated in this language.
Since both [p]s are capable of occurring in the same place in a word in Thai and
since the substitution of one for the other results in a word of different meaning
aspiration is said to be distinctive in Thai just like [b] and [ph] is distinctive in
English.
TOPIC THREE:
10
THE PHONEME THEORY
There are different views of the phoneme. Phonological representations are not
identical to phonetic transcription. In addition, the notion of distinctiveness, for
instance, that two languages can have the same inventory of phonetic sounds
(phones) but significantly different phonological system, for example, English and
Thai aspirated and unaspirated stops. Each language organizes sounds in different
ways. There are three approaches to this organization.
This refers to the fact that the phoneme represents a physical phonetic reality. A
sound which belongs to the same phoneme share important phonetic property
for example, /p/ may stand for [p], [ph], [pw], [p:]. Not all phonetic properties of
a sound are crucial in every case in distinguishing word meaning. Some properties
are redundant – predictable and non-distinctive. When writing phonological rules,
the convention of omitting redundant properties is normally observed.
The notion of the phoneme as a phonetic or physical reality sees the phoneme
as representing primarily a concrete segmental reality. It looks at the phoneme
at the level of segments. In other words, it is essentially concerned with the study
of phonemes in their distinctive and separate entities rather than in organized
speech (supra segmental phonology) The phoneme is here affirmed to be the sound
unit that can be ordinarily perceived and tried by experience to have an existence
not merely in appearance, thought or language. This is to say, phonemes can be
analyzed in a distinctive environment not in relation to how they overlap in rapid
speech.
The study of the phoneme as a phonetic or physical reality brings to focus the
examination of the notion of minimal pairs, complementary distribution, free
variation and suspicious pairs.
11
2. The phoneme as a Phonological Reality
The phoneme can be defined satisfactorily neither on the basis of its psychological
nature nor on the basis of its relation to the phonetic variants but purely and
solely on the basis of its function in the system of language. Thus a phoneme is
a minimal unit that can function to distinguish meanings. It is not a sound or even a
group of sounds but rather an abstraction, a theoretical construct on the
phonological level. It is defined in terms of contrasts within a system for example,
the /b/ in English has a contrast but Berber doesn’t.
Each time a speaker pronounces the sound [p], for example, it is acoustically never
quite the same as the last [p] the speaker must have internalized an image or
idealized picture, a target which he/she tries to approximate. For example,
American English [aI mis a] while the real intention is [aI mis yu]
1. Linguistic Intuitions
12
The brick – blick opposition shows that the two forms which both satisfy the
sequential constraints of a language (are well formed) can differ in that one can be
found in the dictionary while the other does not exist. Words which are well
formed but are not found in the dictionary (lexicon) are termed as accidental gaps.
On the other hand, the brick – blick opposition shows that two words not occurring
in the lexicon can differ in that one is well formed (brick) but the other is ill
formed (blick). Words which are formed and do not occur in the lexicon are termed
as systemic gaps.
2. Foreign Accents
It is well known that speakers substitute sounds of their own language for
sounds of foreign languages they attempt to speak. The result is that they
typically have foreign accents. For instance, speakers of Spanish tend to insert [E]
before English words beginning with [s] followed by another consonant e.g. I
speak Espanish. This is due to the Spanish E-insertion rule.
3. Speech Errors
4. Language Acquisition
TOPIC FOUR:
When we describe speech sounds, we use terms like: place, manner, voicing,
tongue height, lip rounding, tenseness. When we specify one of these
descriptions, we are defining the articulatory features of the particular sound. In
this theory the basic unit is the feature (not the phoneme) - features cannot be
broken into smaller units. Features are binary; all phonemes either have [+] or don't
have [-] a particular feature. Several feature systems have been proposed.
Features may be based on: Acoustic properties (e.g., strident, voice), Articulatory
properties (e.g., high, back, lateral, coronal) and Function in a syllable (e.g.,
consonantal, vocalic). Proponents of this theory believe that phonemes are stored
in the brain as “bundles of features”. Like phonemes, features are considered to be
abstract mental notions. Features are called “distinctive” because they allow us to
distinguish among phonemes. Two phones are different phonemes if at least one of
their features is different.
Several linguists have theorized the distinctive features from different perspectives.
These are discussed below.
14
1. Trubetzkoy’s Theory of Distinctive Oppositions
A fact about phonological systems is that segments typically group themselves into
phonetically definable classes. Trubetzkoy is one of the founders of Prague School
of Linguistics which developed in the decade preceding World War 2. Trubetzkoy
(1939) attempted a comprehensive taxonomy of the phonetic properties of the
distinctive contrasts employed by language.
It was interested not only in how /p/ differs from /b/ but also in what the nature of
the contrast was within the phonological system. In his book ‘Principles of
Phonology’ he classified distinctive oppositions (sound difference which results in
meaning difference) on the basis of:
a. Bilateral/Multilateral Oppositions
In Thai, on the other hand, one finds not only /p/ and /b/ but also /ph/. We can still
say that /p/ and /b/ started in bilateral opposition but we further specify the
properties they have in common as oral unaspirated labial stops. However, /ph/
and /b/ do stand in bilateral opposition as they are oral labial stops. Since there is a
third segment which shares the properties common to /ph/ and /b/, we ca say they
are in a multi-lateral opposition.The same phonetic segments distinguished by the
same phonetic features can stand in multilateral opposition in one language and in
bilateral in another language.
15
b. Proportional or Isolated oppositions
Another distinction made concerning which, in relation to the whole system are
either proportional or isolated. An opposition is proportional if the relation
between its members is identical with the relation between members of another
opposition or several other opposition of the same system. Otherwise, the
opposition is said to be an isolated one.
In English, the opposition between /p/ and /p/ is proportional because the relation
between its members is identical to the relation between /t/ and /d/ and between /k/
and /g/. On the other hand, the opposition between /l/ and /r/ is isolated since no
other segments in English stand in same relation as these two opposition members.
The proportions can be stated such as p:b=t:d=k:g. Therefore, whether an
opposition is proportional or isolated depends on the language.
c. Privative Oppositions
d. Gradual
Unlike vowels, where only the degree of vowel height is involved, different places
of articulation in consonants are obtained by discrete changes in the two
articulators. This third category of opposition is termed equipollent.
Although rat (advice) and rad ‘wheel’ are written differently both are pronounced
[ra:t]. The plural forms rate [re:ta] ‘advices’ and rader [rE: dar] ‘wheels’ shows a
contrast between /t/ and /d/ since these consonants are with the plural suffixes –e
and –er, no longer at the end of the word. The opposition between /t/ and /d/ is
therefore realized phonetically only in certain positions where only /t/ is found
phonetically, the opposition is said to be neutralized.
On the other hand, when two members of an opposition can occur in all positions,
there is no neutralization. Rather, the opposition is said to be constant e.g. tap –
dap; bad – pad; utter – udder
llll
2. JAKOBSON’S THEORY OF DISTINCTIVE FEATURES
Jakobson, another founding member of the Prague School hypothesized that the
presence of certain oppositions hinders the presence of other oppositions. He
classified sounds into the following:
a. Articulatory vs Acoustic Features
Articulatory features for consonants include where the sound is made (place),
how it is made (manner) and what the state of glottis is (voiced/unvoiced). The
17
features for vowels focus on which part of the tongue is raised (front, central,
back), how much it is raised (high, mid, low) and whether the lips are rounded or
not.
A feature can have only two values, one of which is designated as [+f] and the
other as [-f]. +f indicates the presence of a feature while –f is absence of a
given feature. In many cases, the binary approach is phonologically significant as
in those oppositions termed as privative. Thus a phoneme is either [+ nasal] or [-
nasal] though phonetically some sounds may be more heavily nasalized than
others. For example,
bet, den, ghoul, zinc (+voice) pet, ten, cool, sink (- voice)
However, binarism works well where there exists a two way opposition, but it
does not yield entirely satisfactory results where multilateral contrast is involved.
Trubetzkoy’s gradual oppositions seem to defy binary reinterpretation. Thus, the
vowels /I, e, E, ae / differ in degree of vowel height and would appear to require a
scale from say [1 vowel height] for /ae/ to [4 vowel height] for /i/.
The features used by Chomsky and Halle were defined primarily in articulatory
and not in acoustic ones as they were in the Jakobsonian model. This was not a
return to the ‘tradition”, but a reinterpretation of most of Jakobson’s features.
Chomsky and Halle argue that the priority given to an articulatory description
is a circumstantial one rather than one pertaining to the essence of their
theoretical approach (1968: 299).
18
The features – more numerous than Jakobson’s – were subdivided into five groups.
2. Cavity features:
coronal/noncoronal
anterior/nonanterior
body of the tongue features:
high/nonhigh
low/nonlow
back/nonback
rounded/nonrounded
distributed/nondistributed
covered/noncovered
glottal constrictions
secondary apertures:
nasal/nonnasal
lateral/nonlateral
3. Manner of articulation features:
continuant/noncontinuant (stop)
instantaneous/delayed release
supplementary movements
suction
pressure
tense/nontense (lax)
4. Source features:
heightened subglottal pressure
voiced/nonvoiced
strident/nonstrident
5. Prosodic features:
stress
pitch
length
19
Major class features dealt with the fundamental vocalic/nonvocalic and
consonantal/nonconsonantal distinctions. The distinction sonorant/obstruent was
introduced the former being described as sounds allowing spontaneous voicing.
Syllabic/nonsyllabic - Consonants are described as contoids(consonant-like in
nature) and vowels as vocoids while the same distinction syllabic/nonsyllabic that
differentiated between vowels and glides operates in the case of consonants too.
TOPIC FIVE
20
PHONOLOGICAL PROCESSES
No two languages have exactly the same inventory of phonemes which are realized
by the same set of allophones. No two languages have exactly the same
phonological rules regulating the deployment of their sounds. Phonology is not a
static system in which an established unit remains unchanged in all its
occurrences. Rather, it is a dynamic system in which units change as they come
into contact with other units in the system. We refer to such changes as
phonological processes.
21
FOR EXAMPLE: Some rules make sequences of sounds easier to pronounce.
When we say [kaem bi:] instead of [kaen bi:], we produce two bilabial sounds in a
row (using a single lip gesture) instead of making an alveolar [n] and then a
bilabial [b] (using two different gestures).Some rules make sounds easier to
perceive, as when voiceless stops are aspirated at the beginning of a stressed
syllable.
There are several phonological processes applicable to almost all the languages of
the world. They are discussed in details below.
1) Assimilation
Directionality in Assimilation
In assimilation, we can say whether a sound becomes more like either the sound
that precedes it or the sound that follows it. Progressive assimilation is when a
sound becomes more like the sound that precedes it while Regressive is when a
sound is modified so that it becomes more like the sound that follows it (mostly
22
noted in English voice assimilation). Regressive assimilation is normally
anticipatory.
a) Voicing
The regular plural ending in English written as‘s’ may be pronounced as ‘-s’, ‘-z’
and ‘-Iz’. The choice is not random. The principle that determines the shape of the
suffix is voice assimilation: the suffix must always agree with the preceding sound.
There is the allomorph rule which guides the assimilation process.
[-z] occurs with words ending in voiced consonants or vowels e.g. dogz,
seaz, ribz, lovez, cabz, lidz, tagz etc
[-s] occurs with words ending with voiceless sounds e.g. docks, bits, moths,
giraffes, cats, taps etc.
[IZ] occurs with word ending with [ts], [dz], [S], [Z], [s], [z] e.g. witchiz,
judgiz, rosiz, nausiz, marshiz, busiz,
The same rule applies to the third person singular being [-z, -s, -IZ]. The suffix
also agrees in voicing with the preceding sound. The plural suffix is realized by a
voiced or voiceless alveolar fricative depending on whether the noun ends in a
voiced or voiceless segment. For example, [-z]: see – sees, love – loves, mend –
mends, come – comes; [-s]: paint – paints, hate – hates, talk – talks, wipe – wipes;
[IZ]: teach – teaches, wish – wishes, judge – judges, lose – loses, catch – catches.
The past tense morpheme marker ‘-d’ or ‘-ed’ is realized as [-d, -t, -Id] again
depending on the feature of voicing. For example, [-d]: love – loved, call – called,
rub – rubbed (voiced); [-t]: walk – walked, work – worked (voiceless); [-Id]: land –
landed, sound – sounded, part – parted (ends in either t or d).
Regressive assimilation can further be seen in the nasal prefix in the following
data from Luganda
m-bala ‘I count’
23
m-mala I finish
m-pa I give
n-daga I show
n-sika I pull
ng-gula I buy
ng-kola I work
b) Place of articulation
For example,
Car Keys [ka kIz]
Keep calm [kIp kam]
Give guns [gIv gAnz]
This year, hit you, lead you, praise you, bless you
His shoes [hiz shuz] = [hiz (as in measure) Suz]
Nice shirt [naIs S3t] = [naI S3t]
Miss you [mis ju] = [Mis ju]
Regressive assimilation is also realized when the process of labialization
commences. This is referred to as lip rounding. A speaker starts rounding the lips
before the articulation of the consonant is complete in anticipation of the
neighbouring rounded vowel. For example,
Pool [pwul]
24
Two [twu]
Shoe [Swu]
The nasal of the negative prefix ’IN-‘ assimilates the manner of articulation
features of the liquid so that /n/ becomes /l/ before /l/ commences. This is noted in
words such as
In-legal – il-legal
In-licit – il-licit
In-rational – ir-rational
In-revocable – ir-revocable
However, it should be noted that the nasal shares the place of articulation with a
consonant that follows it. This is a predictable process in Luganda but not
predictable in English. An example is the negative prefix IN- in words such as:
Progressive assimilation is when the phoneme that comes first makes the
following phoneme take its properties. The changes occur during the assimilation
of place, of manner and of voicing in consonants.
Examples
that person, light blue, quite good, bright color, meat pie(p), good night(m), that
side(m) don't be silly, i[k]onclusive, i[g]ratitude, have turned, cause to die,
breathe slowly, garage to let (There is English fricative devoicing)
2. Dissimilation
Whereas assimilation refers to the process in which segments take on the character
of neighboring segments, dissimilation refers to the process in which segments
change to become less like a neighboring segments. On the whole, dissimilation
is much rarer than assimilation. This process ensures that differences between
sounds are enhanced so that sounds become more auditorily distinct and make
speech perception easier.
25
In English, the adjective forming suffix ‘-al’ has two phonetic manifestations ‘-al’
and ‘-ar’. Dissimilation is noted thus electric – electrical, region – regional, orbit
– orbital; angle – angular, circle – circular, single – singular, Fifth, sixth
3. ELISION
This is very simply the omission of certain sounds in certain contexts. The most
important occurrences of this phenomenon regard:
I. Alveolar consonants consonants /t/ and /d/ when sandwiched between two
consonants e.g. The next day, The last car, Hold the dog, Send Frank a card
II. Weak vowels e.g. potato, tomato, perhaps, tonight, police, correct, button,
mutton, chocolate, secretary, articulatory, phonetics
III. Loss of final /v/ in of before consonants: Lots of them, waste of money
IV. Contractions: he’ll, we’ll, will not=won’t, They have=They’ve, They
are=They’re
4. LINKING
5. NEUTRALIZATION
TOPIC SIX
THE SYLLABLE
The syllable has two constituents namely the onset which comes at the beginning
of a syllable and must be a consonant sound and the rhyme which follows the
onset. The rhyme has two components namely the peak/nucleus which is a vowel
sound and the coda which is always a consonant sound. Typically, the nucleus slot
in the rhyme is occupied by a vowel.
N/B: Maximum Onsets Principle (MOP), according to Roach (2000: 77-78), states
that: where two syllables are to be divided, any consonants between them should
be attached to the right -hand syllable, not to the last, as far as possible (p. 78).
Also remember that this means that to recognize a syllable boundary some other
principles are at work in conjunction with MOP.
27
i. The onset of a syllable must be permissible in English
ii. A division is sometimes created such that the strong vowel attracts the
consonant
iii. Two consonants between vowels can be split into two such that the left one
serves as the coda of one syllable and the other, the onset of the other syllable.
Demonstration:
In the case of most words, it should be comparatively easy to point out how many
syllables a given word has eg rat , redeem, humanist, degenerate, electricity and
characterization and say that they contain one, two, three, four, five and six
syllables respectively. Transcribe the examples above
The two main constituents within the syllable are the onset and the rhyme. For
example, in the word tip [tip], the first segment [t] followed by the vowel [I] and
then another segment [p]. The segment [t] together constitutes the onset of the
syllable, while the last two segments together [Ip] constitute the rhyme. The rhyme
may be divided further into two constituents, the nucleus and the coda. In the
example above, the rhyme is [Ip]. Its constituents are [ I ] which is the nucleus
(peak) and [p] which is the coda. This is a closed syllable.
There can also be an open syllable when a syllable does not contain any
consonants in the coda position eg the word tea [ti:]. It does not contain any
28
consonant after the nucleus [i:]. English also has syllables which do not have an
onset but only the nucleus and the coda eg it [It].
TOPIC SEVEN
Tonal languages have morphemes which are at least in part realized by pitch
modulation. Pitch differences can be used to make phonemic contrasts. Pitch
depends on the rate of vibration of the vocal cords. The more taut the vocal cords
are, the faster they vibrate and the higher the pitch. In tone languages, pitch can be
used to distinguish word meaning or convey grammatical distinctions. Non-tonal
languages (stress languages) like English pitch do not have those functions.
Intonation is defined as the rise and fall of the pitch of an utterance (Jolayemi,
1999: 82). It is also described as voice modulation; when you have your voice high
or low. Gimson, (1980: 264) describes intonation as “variation of pitch”. It is
usually said of English that the language sounds musical. One way this is achieved
is the lowering of the pitch towards the end of an utterance from a high level at the
beginning of the utterance. This is referred to as Tune 1
29
Examples are:
i. Let’s go home.
ii. That’s good for you.
iii. The lecture starts right away.
iv. You may call me Mr Thompson.
When you want to lay particular emphasis on certain utterances you want to make
in order to convey some special message, you often use Tune I. this is a speech act
that we make very regularly.
Examples are:
i. I do not hate you.
ii. Rivers flow downhill.
iii. That’s not it.
iv. Corruption won’t help there.
STRESS
Stress means essentially that one phonological element is singled out within
another, longer, phonological element. Sentence-stress involves the picking out
of one word or phrase within the sentence; this word or phrase is usually given
special emphasis of some kind in pronunciation. Stress is primarily a matter of
greater auditory prominence.
30
It is a perceptual phenomenon. An element that is stressed is highlighted so that it
becomes more auditorily salient than the rest of the elements in the string of which
it is part. The main ingredients of stress are pitch, length and loudness.
TYPES OF STRESS
1. Word-level stress
A particular syllable of a word is pronounced in a way that makes it more
prominent than the rest e.g. if you say words like mother, better and pity, you will
notice that the first syllable in all these words is much more salient than the
second. The first syllable is said to be stressed while the second one is unstressed.
In longer words, there is often not just one stressed but a host of unstressed ones. A
word can have a syllable that receives the main or primary stress and then have
other syllables which receives secondary stress. For example, ra/di/a/tor where the
first syllable receives the primary stress and the third syllable receives secondary
stress. It is more prominent than the second and fourth but not as prominent as the
first.
In English, some verbs and nouns can be distinguished by stress. For example,
‘project (n) pro’ject (v), ‘conduct, con’duct; ‘refuse, re’fuse; ‘protest, pro’test
In Kiswahili, there is the use of the penultimate stress unlike in the English
language. For example, piga, pigana, piganisha, tutawapiga, pigapiga
31
3. Sentence Stress Marking
Let me exemplify with “blackboard” and “black board” in these two sentences:
i. The teacher needs the blackboard to write the summary of his
lecture.
ii. The teacher needs the black board to construct the stage for that
scene
By septillion
32