Pamahiin
Pamahiin
Pamahiin
In Partial Fulfillment
Of the Requirements for the Degree
Of Bachelor of Science in Psychology
JANIEVA O. CASTRO
APRIL JOY SALAYSAY
2019
TALE AS OLD AS TIME: A NARRATIVE STUDY OF FILIPINOS
SUPERSTITIOUS BELIEFS IN FUNERALS
JANIEVA CASTRO
APRIL JOY SALAYSAY
An undergraduate thesis manuscript submitted to the faculty of the Department of Arts and
Sciences, Cavite State University – Bacoor Campus, City of Bacoor, Cavite in partial
fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Bachelor of Science in Psychology.
Chapter 1
INTRODUCTION
Superstitions are quite common in the Philippines. Beliefs that are been followed
since the times of our ancestors. These beliefs has been passed down from generations to
generations. It have been handed down from ancestors aiming to prevent danger from
happening or to make a person from doing something in particular. These notions are part of
the Philippine culture, for one derives their beliefs from the influence of what their traditions,
customs and culture have prescribed to explain certain phenomena or just simply to put a
scare to people. Some of these superstitions are practiced mainly because Filipinos believe
there is nothing to lose if they abide with these beliefs. Many people believe that
superstitious beliefs originated during the earliest days of humanity. The earliest
superstitions were created as a way to deal with ignorance and fear of the unknown.
Superstitions are thus, a way of attempting to regain control over events, particularly when
one feels helpless. They are born from casual coincidences and learned behaviours. The
origin of certain superstitions may be centuries old, having been passed down from
generation to generation through observational learning process. Some superstitions are
considered harmless and may even be helpful in encouraging people to achieve their goals,
attributing result to an unrelated cause. In fact, some people depend on luck through
superstitious beliefs.
We, Filipinos have an incredulous number of superstitions. We have one for every
event even for our daily lives. This study is focused mainly on the superstitious beliefs of
Filipinos in Funerals or Burials. Death is a natural occurrence. We are all have our own end,
but for some reasons, Filipinos believe in these funeral superstitious beliefs to avoid death to
happen again or to banish bad luck in the family. During the Pre-Hispanic period the early
Filipinos had already believed in a concept of life after death. This belief in an afterlife
prompted the Filipinos to create burial customs and beliefs to somehow honour the dead
through rituals.
With this, the study has been created to identify the different superstitious beliefs of
Filipinos, to identify how these beliefs pass from generation to generation, specifically Burial
superstitions. This is an approach to clarify our understanding towards superstition in the
local scene and to know its condition in the present time.
This research allows us to clearly distinguish the differences between the concept of
proactive and passive types of superstition and how each type has an effect on the risk-
taking behavior of Thai consumers when interacting with a superstitious object. Exploration
of superstitious beliefs is crucial in consumer behavior because people use them as a
means to cope with uncertainty and alleviate anxiety (Raylu&Oei, 2004). The results
enhance our understanding on how superstitious consumers engage in risk-taking activities
and in which way they can be encouraged to increase or to reduce this interest. In general,
proactive superstitious consumers like to practice superstitious rituals to keep bad forces
away, bring in good forces, and to increase their chance of gaining good luck. Passive
superstitious consumers perceive that they do not have control over their luck, so they use
external forces as a guide in their decision-making process. This research further explains
the consequences of superstitious belief, as a superstitious object can enhance risk-taking
behavior especially for passive superstitiousconsumers. We show that by providing a
magical object (magic pen), passive superstitious consumers increase their risk-taking
behavior. It is possible that the superstitious belief can be transferred through contagion and
impact actual performance (Kramer & Block, 2014). We found that the existence of a
superstitious belief that is perceived to transfer through intermediary objects is more likely
among passive superstitious consumers. The results also show that magical (superstition
activated) objects do not affect proactive superstitious consumers. That could be because
they do not feel they have control over the objects nor have they put in any effort into
acquiring the object themselves. Future studies could examine the difference in situations
where proactive superstitious consumers carry their own lucky charm or have it provided to
them. An alternative explanation for this effect could be attributed to positive mood.
Research has shown that good luck and good mood are highly correlated and lucky people
are happy and optimistic (Duong &Ohtsuka, 2000). When individuals feel lucky, they are
more willing to take financial risks (Jiang et al., 2009). Therefore, receiving the magical
object (pen) may activate the concept of luck, which then elicits positive affect among the
passive superstitious participants who already believe in magic and magical objects.
Therefore, future studies may explore whether positive affect mediates the relationship.
Belief in Superstition
Superstitions are beliefs that are inconsistent with the known laws of nature or with
what is generally considered rational in a society (American Heritage Dictionary 1985). Belief
in superstition dates back thousands of years and continues to the present (Jahoda 1969;
Vyse 1997). Superstitions may be invoked to fend off bad luck, as Michael Jordan did when
he changed the number on his uniform to change his luck after several inferior performances
(USA Today, reported in Darke and Freedman [1997b]). Superstitions are also invoked to
bring about good luck, as deepsea fishermen in New Guinea often do when they perform
elaborate magical rituals to ensure a successful fishing trip (Malinowski 1954). Although
superstitions can be found the world over, superstitious beliefs are most often culturally
specific. For example, Chinese individuals often seek to deflect bad luck by putting up
mirrors in their homes (Simmons and Schindler 2003). The color red and the number 8 are
associated with prosperity and good luck; therefore, the Bank of China opened its doors in
Hong Kong on August 8, 1988—considered to be the luckiest day of the century (Lip 1992).
During Chinese New Year, older relatives give children lucky red envelopes with money
inside. Colors and numbers (e.g., the number 4 and the color black) are also associated with
unlucky consequences (Wiseman and Watt 2004). Some buildings in China have no fourth
floor, and some parents refuse to let their children travel in unlucky taxis on the day of their
college entrance exam (Yardley 2006). Examples of common superstitious beliefs in the
United States include horseshoes and knocking on wood for good luck, and the number 13,
walking under a ladder, or breaking a mirror for bad luck. Although many superstitions are
culturally shared and socially transmitted from generation to generation, others consist of
relatively more idiosyncratic beliefs or rituals. For example, students can often be seen
bringing lucky charms or lucky pens to an exam; an athlete may not change socks during a
tournament; and a job applicant may wear a lucky outfit for a job interview. Each of these
superstitious beliefs and behaviors is associated with desired positive outcomes; that is,
people expect to do well on a subsequent task. Engaging in ritualistic behavior
of this sort is consistent with Darke and Freedman’s (1997b) empirical work on luck. They
demonstrated that people’s associations of luck with one performance create expectations
about luck that extend beyond this single event to other independent and unrelated events.
Research also suggests that people are motivated to rely on superstitious beliefs when their
control over an event is undermined or threatened. Case et al. (2004) conducted a series of
studies investigating the relationship between the use of superstitious strategies and
perceived control. The consistent finding across these studies was that as the likelihood of
failure increased, so did the use of superstitious beliefs. Case et al. concluded that the more
salient the failure is, the more people use superstitious strategies to explain the failed
outcome. However, to date, this research has been limited to studies of personal failure. The
current research therefore starts with a focus on the use of superstitious explanations for
non-personal or inanimate object failure, such as product failures. Morales and Fitzsimons
(2007) recently demonstrated the transference of irrational beliefs onto inanimate objects
(i.e., products). Though their study did not explore superstitious beliefs, Morales and
Fitzsimons showed that irrational beliefs about one product can be contagious and affect
other products. The question remains whether superstitious beliefs are transferred to
inanimate objects in the case of product failures. In addition, we still do not know the degree
to which consumers use superstitious beliefs, either consciously or non-consciously, in their
decision making under uncertainty. Given the importance placed on rationality and
associated norms against relying on superstition in many modern societies (Vyse 1997), the
degree to which the effects of superstition operate non-consciously is an interesting open
question.
Certain athletes also reported feeling that outcomes would “go wrong” without the
ability to perform the superstitious behavior (Schippers& van Lange, 2006, p. 2453). The
researchers showed not only the wide variety of superstitious behaviors among athletes, but
also their desire to carry out their SB. Potential theories or explanations for continued use
and related factors. In addition to the theories that attempt to explain the onset of a SB, there
are other potential theories that explain use and maintenance of an SB (Burke et al., 2006;
Damisch et al., 2010; Keinan, 1994; Schippers& van Lange, 2006). Many theories about the
reasoning behind continued superstition use exist, including, but not limited to: reward
conditioning, external locus of control, and outcome uncertainty (Schippers& van Lange,
2006). Reward conditioning occurs when an individual is conditioned through positive
outcomes to believe that a completely randomunrelated act (the superstitious behavior) is
the reason behind their desired positive outcome (Schippers& van Lange, 2006). External
locus of control refers to the individual’s belief that factors unrelated to their performance
specifically have a role in the outcome (Schippers& van Lange, 2006). These elements have
been found to potentially explain the onset of superstition use; however, they have also been
found when studying individuals’ long-term superstitious behaviors (Burke et al., 2006;
Burhmann&Zaugg, 1981; Schippers& van Lange, 2006). Further, an individual is thought to
be more prone to continued use of superstitious behavior when the outcome is unknown,
levels of anxiety are high, and when the thought of control eludes them (Damisch et al.,
2010). In addition to improvement in cognitive and motor functioning, superstitious behaviors
are thought to decrease perceived stress (Keinan, 1994); however, stress does not have to
be present for individuals to participate in superstitious rituals or beliefs. There may be a
neurological explanation to superstitious behaviors. The Chinese culture is one of many
cultures that widely accept superstitious behaviors/beliefs, particularly regarding numbers
(Rao, Zheng, Zhou, & Li, 2014). The number 16 is recognized within the Chinese culture as
a lucky number (Rao et al., 2014). This foundation of the culturally accepted lucky number
was used to determine what happens neurologically when deciding wedding dates and
prices. Rao et al. (2014) presented 11 female and 10 male participants with the task of
choosing a date for both a friend gathering and a wedding. For each scenario there was an
economically cheaper option on a culturally insignificant date, January 13th, and a more
expensive choice on the culturally lucky date of, January 16th. In addition to the task of
choosing a date for a gathering of friends and a wedding, Rao et al. (2014) also had
participants complete the Fatalistic Superstition Belief Inventory and focused specifically on
the question, “Not choosing a good wedding day will lead to a bad marriage” (p. 769). When
picking a date for the friend gathering, 78% of the participants chose the cheaper option over
the lucky date; in contrast, 81% of the participants chose the more expensive and culturally
lucky option when picking a date for their own wedding (Rao et al., 2014). Furthermore,
there was a significant amount of neural deactivation in the superior frontal and right middle
gyrus of the brain when individuals chose the superstitious date over the cheaper option.
Combining the participant’s neural deactivation with their results on the Fatalistic
Superstition Belief Inventory, researchers found participants who scored high on the
inventory experienced greater deactivation in their brain when making the economical choice
(Rao et al., 2014).
Even though most people don’t believe in superstitions, they still, to some extent,
affect lots of people regardless of today’s modern technology, educational level or ethnicity.
Hence, it is of great importance to build up an appropriate attitude toward superstition though
we may not know all the truth about it. When dealing with superstitions, we don’t have to pay
too much attention or out great emphasis on these so-called ‘signs’ of forthcoming bad
things. If you just inevitably feel tense about what may happen in the near future after seeing
those ‘signs’, just bear in mind that what you believe in is infinitely much more powerful than
superstition itself. However, superstitions are not just that worthless; it makes cultures
around the world more fascinating and attractive after all.
Conceptual Framework of the Study
The conceptual framework was designed to help define the project scope and assist
in navigating patterns and themes.
SUPERSTITIOUS BEHAVIOR
BELIEFS
RESEARCH DESIGN
The researchers will be using a qualitative approach and a narrative analysis in this
study.
The qualitative approach is a process of naturalistic inquiry that seeks in-depth
understanding of social phenomena within their natural setting. It focuses on the "why" rather
than the "what" of social phenomena and relies on the direct experiences of human beings
as meaning-making agents in their everyday lives. Qualitative research worked out under a
combination of observations, interviews, and document reviews. It gives the importance of
looking at variables in the natural setting in which they are found. In the process of
qualitative research interaction between variables is important. Detailed data is gathered
through open ended questions that provided by the researchers.
The data gathering procedure of the study consist of face to face or one on one interview
and document analysis. The researchers choose the face to face interview as this provides
accurate screening, captures verbal and non-verbal cues, keep focusing on interviewee and
track to completion and captures emotion and behaviour (DeFranzo, 2014). In the interview,
the researchers prepared guided questions to ask to the participant.
Procedure
The researchers follow the procedures with a goal to obtain first hand data from the chosen
respondents in order to formulate sound, effect, and useful conclusions and
recommendation for the study.
1. Before starting the data gathering, the study will went through the process of the
Ethical Review Board which will check the readiness and accuracy of the study. They
will also make sure the study will not be harmful for the chosen participants.
2. Recruitment process and its planning. Researchers will gather participants which
were 6 individuals who have a background about the Filipinos superstitious beliefs in
terms of funeral, who lives in the Phillipines since birth.
3. Informed consent. The researchers will prepare an informed consent to the gathered
respondents asking permission to participate in the research. The researchers will
guarantee the confidentiality of any information or data to eb gathered from the
respondents/participants. The researchers will also include in the informed consent
the use of recording device during the interview for transcription purposes as well as
the number of sessions that the interview will consist of.
4. Time and Date schedule. The researchers will ask and appoint a time and date for
the interview with the respondents based on their availability and preference of
schedule and might be composed of two or more sessions.
5. Clarity and the responsibility of the researchers. The researchers will prepare a clear
explanation on the purpose of the study and of the respondents selected. This will
allow any clarifications to be asked by the participants to the researchers regarding
ny other matter about the study.
6. Discussion. On this part of the study, the researchers will ask the respondents the set
of questions provided in order to answer the problem and reach the objective of the
study. Here, the participants will be allowed to have the freedom to express
themselves and relay in detail the answers they had to the questions asked. The
researchers, meanwhile, utilized the recording device for a better and more fluid
transcription of data from respondents that were later examined and presented in the
results of the study.
7. As a form of gratitude, tokens will be handed to the participants gathered.
DATA ANALYSIS
This research will use a narrative analysis that aims to identify the kinds of stories
told by the researched phenomenon and the kinds of stories representing the phenomenon
in culture and society. This is a step by step process. The first step in narrative analysis is to
compile the stories. On the first of several reads through you collection, note and bracket
your responses to the story: what you believe, what you doubt, what touches you (Here’s an
example of what I mean) then analyse the explicit content, the discourse, and the context of
each story focusing on insights and understandings. Consider the latent content that lies
unsaid between the lines Compare and contrast stories for similarities and differences in
content, style, and interpretation. Consider the effects of background variables (ie: history,
geography, gender, age). And then Identify stories or content that illustrates your themes,
insights, and understandings.
ETHICAL CONSIDERATION
The Ethical considerations in this research were very particular since it was used to
build the standards of conduct to everyone who will take part of the study. Before proceeding
with the data gathering, the study will went through first with the Ethical Review Board which
will check the readiness and the accuracy of the study. They will also made sure that the
study will not harm the chosen participants. After gaining the approval of the Board, that’s
the time the researchers will only proceed with the data gathering. One of the ethical
consideration in narrative analysis is Validity Issues. Since narrative research depends
heavily on the data collected from participant stories, the validity of the stories can greatly
impact the outcome. Participants may present a "false story", or purposefully distort the real
story. This results in false data being used for the narrative research. Participants may also
be unable or unwilling to give the complete story, due to trauma or limitations on their
memory. Researchers should be aware of these issues, and can potentially compensate by
coorelating data from multiple sources, fact checking, and careful review of participants.
Another is Ownership. Narratives may involve people or events that the researcher does not
have permission to tell. The issue of ownership can be very complex, and can lead to
delays or unintended consequences of the research. Researchers should try to seek the
permission of not only participants, but of the people that the narrative research may directly
impact.
Additionally, the researcher runs the risk of transposing their own perspective for that
of the particpants. The act of "restorying" can inadvertantly cause the loss of the participants
"voice". Using direct quotes from the participant and careful attention to the settings and
events told can help to aleviate loss of perspective. And the Participant Impacts. Due to the
social nature of many narrative research studies, participants can be negatively impacted by
participation. This can occur either directly by being identified within the study, or indirectly
by the influence a research report may have on their culture or lifestyle. Researchers can
create a more "composite" narrative (if multiple participants are used), or at the least
anonymize the participants.
The researcher must also be aware that the relationship they develop between themselves
and the participants should be reciprocal. Researchers should not be the only one to gain
from the study, and should "give back" in some way.
Guide Questions
The following questions are the guidelines of the researchers to gather information of
the participants in this study;