Dark Side Leadership

Download as docx, pdf, or txt
Download as docx, pdf, or txt
You are on page 1of 319

The Dark Side of Leadership and Its Impact on Followers

Vicki Joy Webster


BA (Hons)

School of Applied Psychology


Griffith University
Queensland

Submitted in fulfillment of the requirements of the degree of Doctor of Philosophy

31 October 2015

i
Abstract

Over the past two decades research has studied the “dark side of leadership” under the

research stream of destructive leadership, variously described as abusive, aversive, derailing,

toxic, and tyrannical leadership. While there has been significant research into the

consequences of destructive leadership, there has been limited investigation of its

antecedents. It has been proposed by a number of researchers, investigating a leader trait

approach to leadership, that “dark side” traits (socially undesirable traits that can have

negative implications for work success and the well-being of employees) exhibit a curvilinear

relationship with key outcomes (e.g., leader and follower performance, follower engagement,

and follower well-being): that is, there exist negative consequences for extremely low or high

“bright side” personality traits (behavioural traits leaders seem to exhibit when they are

performing at their best) and positive consequences for moderate dark side personality traits.

Extreme levels of some bright side (e.g., independence, assertiveness, charisma) and dark

side (e.g., ego-centred, intimidating, manipulative) traits have been identified as antecedents

to exhibiting destructive leadership behaviours. Sustained demonstration of destructive

leadership behaviours are associated with harmful outcomes that lead to serious problems for

employees in the workplace. Yet there is scarce research into the coping strategies followers

employ to cope with destructive leadership.

This research consists of three studies that investigate destructive leadership from

both the leader and follower perspective. This research identifies traits that may predispose

leaders to engage in destructive leadership behaviours, and explores a range of leadership

behaviours that are perceived by subordinates to be harmful to well-being, from derailing

behaviours to more extreme forms of destructive leadership behaviours. The research also

identifies common strategies employed by followers to cope with their leader’s adverse

ii
behaviour; and evaluates the effectiveness of an intervention to increase follower well-being

in the context of destructive leadership.

Study 1 employed a cross-sectional design and a self-report measure to identify

personality traits that predict or inhibit a predisposition to exhibit destructive leadership

behaviours, and explores possible self-regulation traits as mediators, based on trait-activation

theory. To date there has been limited investigation of antecedents to destructive leadership.

It is important to understand the mechanisms by which leaders may activate derailing traits

through failure of self-regulation. This understanding will then inform the design of

leadership development activities that aim to assist leaders to avoid derailment. Analysis of

data from 300 managers (profiled for recruitment or development purposes across a number

of organisations) revealed that high levels of the personality trait independence, typically

viewed as a bright side trait, was significantly related to derailing traits (ego-centred,

intimidating, manipulating, micro-managing and passive-aggressive). These associations

were significantly mediated by levels of leaders’ negative affectivity, which reduced the

effect of self-regulation traits (emotional control, self-awareness, and stress tolerance). The

results are discussed in terms of the theoretical implications for activation of derailing

leadership traits, and practical implications for the selection and development of managers.

Study 2 employed a cross-sectional design and self-report, qualitative responses to an

online survey completed by respondents (N = 76) who perceived they worked for a “toxic

leader”. Data were analysed to confirm destructive leadership behaviours perceived by

respondents as toxic, together with the psychological, emotional, and physical consequences

of perceived destructive leadership behaviours at the individual level through the voices of

affected followers. Respondents’ coping strategies to deal with the effect of destructive

leadership behaviours were analysed against two organising theoretical coping frameworks:

Yagil, Ben-Zur, and Tamir’s (2011) Coping with Abusive Supervision scale; and Skinner,

iii
Edge, Altman and Sherwood’s (2003) family of coping strategies. Implications for coping

theory in the context of destructive leadership are discussed. This research extends our

understanding of which coping strategies might be protective against harm from destructive

leadership, which are ineffective in this context, and which may contribute to escalated leader

engagement in destructive leadership behaviours.

Study 3 presents a quasi-experimental evaluation of a secondary stress prevention

intervention, a four-hour resiliency training session, to improve followers’ well-being in

general and, specifically, when subjected to destructive leadership behaviours. The

intervention was based on four theoretical frameworks that have previously been found to

effectively minimise the negative effects of stress: stress innoculation therapy, acceptance

commitment training, resiliency, and career resilience. Repeated-measures data for

psychological, affective, and physical well-being were collected immediately before the

intervention and after the training session, and then three months post the intervention. Both

intervention process and outcome results were evaluated. Results indicated a positive effect

of the resiliency training intervention on follower well-being, with a significant increase in

affective well-being and a significant decrease in psychological distress three months post the

intervention. No effect was found for physical well-being. A significant increase in

participants’ knowledge, confidence, and motivation to use resiliency techniques was found

immediately post the training session. Additional analyses to test the mediation effects of

participant coping strategies on the association between manager relationship and well-being

found significant effects at Time 1 only. Avoidance coping mediated the association between

poor manager relationship (low manager support and low manager interpersonal justice) and

psychological and affective well-being, such that psychological and emotional distress were

reduced, suggesting these coping mechanisms may be adaptive to destructive leadership, at

least in the short term. This research demonstrates that an individual intervention conducted

iv
to enhance follower resiliency to destructive leadership behaviours can effectively improve

psychological and affective well-being over time. Furthermore, this research is designed to

address commonly cited limitations of intervention research, including lack of empirically-

based interventions, lack of longitudinal research, lack of evaluation of implementation

processes, and failure to address potential mediating effects. Practical implications are

discussed, including recommended changes to research design to improve the efficacy of

interventions aiming to mitigate the harm of destructive leadership to followers.

Finally, theoretical and practical implications of the results from the three studies in

this thesis are discussed. This research contributes to a better understanding of the antecedent

traits and characteristics that predispose a leader to engage in destructive leadership

behaviours, highlighting the role of the trait negative affectivity in suppressing self-regulation

traits. Practical implications for addressing leader derailment focus on organisational

interventions to develop leaders’ resilience and capacity to deal effectively with negative

emotions, such as frustration or fear of failure, to prevent them activating their trait

predisposition and engaging in destructive behaviours. This research also advances

understanding of theoretical coping mechanisms followers engage in when subjected to

destructive leadership behaviours, and confirms previous research propositions that

employees are likely to employ ineffective coping strategies in this situation. Some support

is provided for the utility of existing coping frameworks within the context of followers

responding to destructive leadership. Support is also provided for the utility of secondary

stress prevention interventions (e.g., resiliency training for followers) in improving the well-

being of participants over time, in the context of destructive leadership. Implementation of

primary, secondary, and tertiary stress management interventions is recommended in order to

protect followers from the occupational stressor of destructive leadership. Collaboration

between academics and practitioners is called for in researching this phenomena and

v
influencing organisations to pro-actively address this significant workplace stressor in order

to protect the psychosocial safety and well-being of managers and employees.

Key Words: Abusive supervision, destructive leadership, leader derailment, toxic leadership,

occupational stressor, coping, stress management interventions.

vi
Acknowledgements

Many people supported me throughout the development of this thesis. I am indebted

to my academic supervisors for their assistance with this research. Firstly, I would like to

thank my principal supervisor, Professor Paula Brough, who has provided me with guidance

throughout the process of conducting research, writing journal articles, preparing conference

papers, and writing my thesis. I am also very appreciative of the discussions with Brett

Myors, who assisted with the initial scoping of this research project and the design of the first

two studies. Professor Kathleen Daly, Dr Amanda Biggs and Dr Lorraine Stokes provided

important alternative perspectives and helpful comments that no doubt improved my

academic writing greatly – their input was very much appreciated.

I would like to thank the organisations that participated in this research, and especially

thank the employees who spent time completing surveys/profiles and/or participating in

workshops. I would like to thank Dr Suzie Drummond for her assistance with the PROCESS

bootstrapping methodology employed in Study 1, and Tristan Casey for his assistance with

the analysis of qualitative data in Study 2. I also express appreciation for the invaluable

feedback provided by the anonymous reviewers of the two journal article submissions. Their

detailed feedback and suggestions were invaluable.

This thesis research is the product of many years of hard work that was facilitated by

the support of family and friends. I would particularly like to thank my partner, John, for his

understanding of the curtailment of our social life while I bunkered down in the study in front

of the computer, and for making the no doubt arduous effort to accompany me to Melbourne,

New Zealand, the UK, and Europe when I presented at conferences. My sister, Annie,

assisted with proofing the draft thesis, and university colleagues, Dr Suzie Drummond and Dr

Wendy Muller, shared their experiences and encouraged me to persist through to the end.

vii
Finally, I’d like to thank the many individuals who shared their personal experiences

of destructive leadership, including how they dealt with its harmful effects. Their stories

energised me by confirming the importance of this research. While at times extremely

challenging, it is very rewarding to have completed research that can make a useful

contribution to the growing body of knowledge on destructive leadership and will inform

evidence-based practice.

viii
Table of Contents

Abstract .............................................................................................................................. ii

Acknowledgements ......................................................................................................... vii

List of Tables .................................................................................................................. xiii

List of Figures .................................................................................................................. xv

Abbreviations ................................................................................................................. xvi

Statement of Original Authorship ............................................................................... xvii

Acknowledgement of Papers included in this Thesis ................................................. xvii

Conference Presentations .............................................................................................. xix

Conference Posters......................................................................................................... xix

Chapter 1. The Dark Side of Leadership and its Impact on Followers ...................... 1
Research Context ................................................................................................................... 4

Rationale for Research ........................................................................................................... 8

Research Questions .............................................................................................................. 13

Research Goals and Approach ............................................................................................. 13

Research epistemology .................................................................................................... 14

Research methodologies. ................................................................................................. 14

Structure of Thesis ............................................................................................................... 15

Thesis Scope .................................................................................................................... 18

Chapter Conclusion .............................................................................................................. 18

Chapter 2. Why Leaders Derail to the Dark Side ....................................................... 20


Brief History of Leadership Theory ..................................................................................... 21

Theoretical Review of the Dark Side of Leadership ............................................................ 25

Individual leader traits. .................................................................................................... 27

ix
Followers and Destructive Leaders. ................................................................................. 28

Definitions of Destructive Leadership ................................................................................. 29

Leader derailers or situational strengths? ........................................................................ 31

Abusive supervision. ........................................................................................................ 36

Destructive leadership. ..................................................................................................... 37

Toxic leadership. .............................................................................................................. 42

The dark triad: narcissism, Machiavellianism and psychopathy ..................................... 42

Measures of Dark Side Leadership Traits and Behaviours .................................................. 47

Self-report leadership profiles.......................................................................................... 48

Third-party inventories. ................................................................................................... 50

360 degree feedback. ....................................................................................................... 52

Theoretical Mechanisms for Preventing Leader Derailment ............................................... 53

The role of leader self-regulation. .................................................................................... 53

Individual leader interventions – can leaders change? .................................................... 58

Organisational Responses to Destructive Leadership .......................................................... 59

Challenges in Researching Destructive Leadership Traits and Behaviours ........................ 62

Chapter Conclusion .............................................................................................................. 64

Chapter 3. Follower Responses to Destructive Leadership ....................................... 66


Destructive Leadership as an Occupational Stressor ........................................................... 67

Stressor-strain theory ....................................................................................................... 67

Susceptible followers. ...................................................................................................... 68

Followers Responses to Destructive Leadership ................................................................. 69

Follower typologies. ........................................................................................................ 72

x
Follower Well-being in the Context of Destructive Leadership .......................................... 75

Follower resiliency........................................................................................................... 76

Follower coping ............................................................................................................... 77

Interventions to Maintain Follower Well-being .................................................................. 89

Follower interventions to build psychosocial resilience .................................................. 91

Follower interventions to build career resilience ............................................................. 96

Challenges to implementation of individual follower interventions. ............................... 99

Tertiary interventions to address harm to followers. ..................................................... 102

Chapter Conclusion ............................................................................................................ 104

Chapter 4. Antecedents to derailing leadership traits: The mediating roles of negative


affectivity and self-regulation traits (Study 1)............................................................ 106
Study 1 Method .................................................................................................................. 113

Study 1 Results................................................................................................................... 115

Study 1 Discussion ............................................................................................................. 119

Chapter 5. Fight, flight or freeze: Common responses for follower coping with toxic
leadership (Study 2). ..................................................................................................... 125
Study 2 Method .................................................................................................................. 129

Study 2 Results................................................................................................................... 131

Study 2 Discussion ............................................................................................................. 134

Chapter 6. Intervention: Resilience-Building Training Session to protect Followers’


Well-being (Study 3) ..................................................................................................... 139
Study 3 Method .................................................................................................................. 153

Study 3 Results................................................................................................................... 167

Study 3 Discussion ............................................................................................................. 183

Chapter Conclusion ............................................................................................................ 193


xi
Chapter 7. General Discussion.................................................................................... 195
Theoretical Research Implications ..................................................................................... 196

Antecedent personality traits to destructive leadership.................................................. 197

Behaviours perceived by followers as toxic. ................................................................. 200

Follower strategies for coping with destructive leadership. .......................................... 201

Stress management interventions and follower well-being ........................................... 204

Implications for Organisational Practice ........................................................................... 205

Primary interventions - organisational constraints to destructive leadership ................ 206

Practical implications for leadership development programs. ....................................... 208

Interventions to enhance follower well-being................................................................ 211

Research Strengths of this Thesis ...................................................................................... 212

Research Limitations of this Thesis ................................................................................... 215

Future Directions for Research .......................................................................................... 217

Destructive leadership. ................................................................................................... 218

Susceptible followers. .................................................................................................... 219

Organisational responses to destructive leadership ....................................................... 220

Research Conclusions ........................................................................................................ 222

References ...................................................................................................................... 225

Appendix A Destructive Leadership Definitions and Research Foci ...................... 264

Appendix B Abstracts from papers presented in Chapters 4 & 5 ........................... 267

Appendix C Consequences of Leadership Behaviours Survey (Study 2) ............... 269

Appendix D Results Tables (Study 2)......................................................................... 273

Appendix E Participant Evaluations of Intervention (Study 3) .............................. 291

xii
List of Tables

Table 1 Summary of destructive leadership definitions and focus – 264

Appendix A

Table 2 Degrees of Dark Side Traits using the HDS (Hogan & Hogan, 33

2001)

Table 3 Five features of destructive leadership (Padilla, Hogan, & Kaiser, 39

2007)

Table 4 Measurement tools to identify ‘dark side’ characteristics (self- 51

report and third-party inventories)

Table 5 Two typologies of susceptible followers (Padilla, Hogan, & 71

Kaiser, 2007; Thoroughgood, Padilla et al., 2012)

Table 6 Phases of stress innoculation training (SIT) applicable to the 93

stressor of destructive leadership

Table 7 Study 1 Correlations between the research variables of interest 117

(N = 300)

Table 8 Unstandardized paths and significant indirect effects for the 118

serial mediation models (Study 1)

Table 9 Follower effect on well-being (Study 2) – Appendix D 273

Table 10 Follower coping strategies (Study 2) – Appendix D 276

xiii
Table 11 Leader Behaviours perceived as harmful by Respondents 283

(Study 2) – Appendix D

Table 12 Theoretical bases for intervention (Study 3) 155

Table 13A &B Study 3 Correlations for Full Sample: Experimental and 169

Control Groups at Time 1 (N = 232)

Table 14 Study 3 Correlations of Research Variables for Experimental 171

Group at Time 2 (n = 178)

Table 15 Study 3 Correlations of Research Variables for Experimental 172

Group at Time 3 (n = 54)

Table 16 Study 3 Means and Standard Deviations of Research 173

Variables for Experimental and Control Groups (Full sample

N = 232; Experimental n = 178; Control n = 54)

Table 17 Study 3 Indirect effects for the parallel multiple mediation 180

model for manager support and well-being at Time 1

Table 18 Study 3 Indirect effects for the parallel multiple mediation 181

model for manager interpersonal justice and well-being at

Time 1

xiv
List of Figures

Figure 1. The toxic triangle: elements in three domains related to destructive 7

leadership.

Figure 2. Research model for destructive leadership behaviours and follower 17

coping in the context of perceived destructive leadership.

Figure 3. Trait activation model of destructive leadership. Adapted from van 26

Knippenberg (2012).

Figure 4. Conceptual model of the interaction of leadership style, leader self- 30

regulation, and followership.

Figure 5. Conceptual model of destructive and constructive leadership 38

(Einarsen, Aasland, & Skogstad, 2007).

Figure 6. Conceptual model of follower resiliency to destructive leadership. 78

Adapted from McLarnon & Rothstein (2013).

Figure 7. Hypothesized mediating relationships (Study 1). 112

Figure 8. Structure of Coping with Toxic Leadership, based on Skinner et al.’s 135

(2003) organising structure (Study 2).

Figure 9. Study 3 design. 142

Figure 10. Study 3 Intervention Multiple Parallel Mediation Model. 150

Figure 11. Study 3 Manager Relationship/Well-being Multiple Parallel 151

Mediation Model.

xv
Abbreviations

Abbreviation Definition

ACT Acceptance Commitment Therapy; Acceptance Commitment Training

CBT Cognitive-behavioural Therapy

GPI Global Personality Inventory

HDS Hogan Development Survey

HR Human Resources

LMX Leader-member exchange theory

LVI Leadership Versatility Inventory

SIT Stress innoculation training

SMI Stress Management Intervention

SMT Stress Management Training

xvi
Statement of Original Authorship

This work has not previously been submitted for a degree or diploma at any university. To

the best of my knowledge and belief, the thesis contains no material previously published or

written by another person, except where due reference was made in the thesis itself.

Vicki Joy Webster

Acknowledgement of Papers included in this Thesis

Section 9.1 of the Griffith University Code for the Responsible Conduct of Research

(“Criteria for Authorship”), in accordance with Section 5 of the Australian Code for the

Responsible Conduct of Research, states:

To be named as an author, a researcher must have made a substantial scholarly

contribution to the creative or scholarly work that constitutes the research output,

and be able to take public responsibility for at least that part of the work they

contributed. Attribution of authorship depends to some extent on the discipline and

publisher policies, but in all cases, authorship must be based on substantial

contributions in a combination of one or more of:

 Conception and design of the research project

 Analysis and interpretation of research data

 Drafting or making significant parts of the creative or scholarly work or

critically revising it so as to contribute significantly to the final output.

xvii
Section 9.3 of the Griffith University Code (“Responsibilities of Researchers”), in

accordance with Section 5 of the Australian Code, states:

Researchers are expected to:

 Offer authorship to all people, including research trainees, who meet the

criteria for authorship listed above, but only those people.

 Accept or decline offers of authorship promptly in writing.

 Include in the list of authors only those who have accepted authorship

 Appoint one author to be the executive author to record authorship and manage

correspondence about the work with the publisher and other interested parties.

 Acknowledge all those who have contributed to the research, facilities or

materials but who do not qualify as authors, such as research assistants,

technical staff, and advisors on cultural or community knowledge. Obtain

written consent to name individuals.

Included in this thesis are papers in Chapters 4 and 5 which are co-authored with other

researchers. My contribution to each co-authored paper is outlined at the front of the relevant

chapter. The bibliographic details for the published paper and the status of a second paper

submitted for publication, including all authors, are:

Incorporated as Chapter 4

Webster, V., Brough, P., & Drummond, S. (submitted). Antecedents to derailing leadership

traits: The mediating roles of negative affectivity and self-regulation traits. Group and

Organization Management.

xviii
Incorporated as Chapter 5

Webster, V., Brough, P., & Daly, K. (2016). Fight, flight or freeze: Common responses for

follower coping with toxic leadership. Stress & Health. DOI: 10.1002/smi.2626.

Copyright status: this article has been published online in Wiley Online Library

(wileyonlinelibrary.com) DOI: 10.1002/smi.2626. Authors who wish to reuse their own

article (or an amended version of it) in a new publication of which they are the author, editor

or co-editor, prior permission is not required (with the usual acknowledgements).

Appropriate acknowledgements of those who contributed to the research, but did not qualify

as authors, are included in the published article and in Chapter 5.

Conference Presentations

Webster, V., and Brough, P. (2014). How can followers become resilient to the impact of

destructive leadership? Proceedings of the 28th International Congress of Applied

Psychology. Paris, France. July.

Webster, V., Brough, P., Daly, K., and Myors, B. (2011). Consequences of Toxic leadership

behaviours: A qualitative investigation. Proceedings of the 25th Annual Australian

and New Zealand Academy of Management Conference. Christchurch, New Zealand,

December.

Webster, V., Brough, P., and Drummond, S. (2014). Destructive leadership behaviours:

Mediating roles of negative affectivity and self-regulation. Proceedings of the

Institute of Work Psychology International Conference, Sheffield, UK. June.

Conference Posters

Webster, V., and Myors, B. (2010). The dark side of leadership: Prevalence of potential

leadership derailers in a management population. Proceedings of the 27th

International Congress of Applied Psychology. Melbourne, Australia. July.

xix
Webster, V., Myors, B., and Brough, P. (2011). The dark side of leadership: The

relationship between self-management and the tendency to use derailing leadership

behaviours. Proceedings of the 9th Industrial and Organisational Psychology

Conference. Brisbane, Australia. June

xx
Chapter 1. The Dark Side of Leadership and its Impact on Followers

Imagine. Imagine that as the alarm goes off, you awake with a sense of dread.

Your stomach is churning. Waves of nausea overwhelm you. You feel the beginnings

of a headache. The last thing you want to do is go to work and face him. As soon as

you walk into work, you can hear him yelling abuse at one of your colleagues. Last

week he accepted public praise for the excellent work he had done on a very

important project, knowing full well it was your work. Your colleagues avoid your

eye as you walk by. They treat you as if you have an infectious disease – you are out

of favour and they want nothing to do with you. “Chris, you idiot, get yourself in

here!” Red in the face, eyes bulging. Not a good sign, but you daren’t refuse. You

just don’t know how much longer you can cope with this.

Traditionally, organisational leadership research adopted the approach that all leaders

are inherently good and ethical (Yukl & van Fleet, 1992). The scenario described above,

derived from a collage of responses from recipients of the dark side of leadership, collected

as part of this research, suggests otherwise. Kellerman, a political scientist, also challenged

this view:

In spite of all the work on leadership that assumes it by definition to be good… we

exercise power, authority and influence in ways that do harm. This harm is not

necessarily deliberate. It can be the result of carelessness or neglect. But this does

not make it less injurious and, in some cases, calamitous. (Kellerman, 2004a p. xiii).

As a practitioner in the field of organisational psychology, the researcher has

observed first-hand the negative effects of destructive leaders on well-being, both personally

and when working with clients. Yet, at the time of commencing this thesis, there was little in

the leadership literature to inform evidence-based practice to address this issue in practical

1
terms. The overall aim of this thesis is to provide guidance to practitioners on evidence-

based practices that contribute to addressing the dark side of leadership in organisations from

two perspectives: the leader, who is derailing to the dark side; and the employee, the target of

the dark side leadership behaviours. The purpose of this chapter is to provide an overview of

why the dark side of leadership is a serious issue for organisations and an important area of

research, and outlines the research aims of this thesis, the research questions, and the

approach taken.

While some researchers regard destructive leadership as an oxymoron, asserting

leadership is, by definition, a positive force, other researchers maintain that it is equally

important to understand why people follow destructive leaders (Kellerman, 2004a, 2004b;

Lipman-Blumen, 2005; Padilla, Hogan & Kaiser, 2007; Yukl & van Fleet, 1992). With the

demise of large corporations due to fraudulent transactions and the repercussions of a global

economic crisis still being experienced, there has been a revival in the promotion of values-

based and ethical leadership models, for example, servant leadership (Greenleaf, 1998);

spiritual leadership (Fry, 2003); authentic leadership (George, 2004); and responsible

leadership (Maak & Pless, 2006). Kellerman (2004a, 2004b) pointed out that a focus on the

bright side of leadership resulted in little attention being paid to the followship of “bad”

leaders, whether it be in the political arena, the community, or in business. Influential leaders

who have subsequently been labeled evil, such as Hitler and Stalin, have been extraordinarily

successful in retaining large numbers of followers who adopted their purpose, values, ideas,

and directives. Kellerman argued that to ignore this aspect of leadership was to miss an

opportunity to better understand the nature of leadership and followship, for example, the

nature and effect of followers and following in the leadership process (Uhl-Bien, Riggio,

Lowe, & Carsten, 2014). Alvesson and Spicer (2012) also espoused the benefits of taking a

critical approach to the study of leadership, arguing that many negative consequences implicit

2
in leadership theory and practice are often masked or ignored. They favoured an approach

that fosters investigations of alternative modes and interpretations of leadership, and seeking

to uncover the dark side of leadership is one such critical approach (Alvesson & Spicer,

2012).

Over the past decade scholarly attention has increased in the examination of the dark

side of leadership under the research stream of destructive leadership (Einarsen, Aasland &

Skogstad, 2007; Krasikova, Green, & LeBreton, 2013; Schyns & Hansborough, 2010). This

was expedited by a special edition of Leadership Quarterly in 2007 dedicated to the topic of

destructive leadership. A key obstacle in researching the dark side of leadership is the

emergence in the literature of multiple terms, overlapping definitions, behavioural

dimensions, and categories of destructive leaders (Einarsen et al., 2007; Pelletier, 2010;

Tepper, 2000). This has resulted in diverse leadership research streams that, until relatively

recently, were not cross-referenced (Krasikova et al., 2013; Martinko, Harvey, Brees, &

Mackey, 2013; Schyns & Schilling, 2013; Tepper, 2007). These have been variously

described as incompetent and unethical leadership (Conger, 1990; Kellerman, 2004a);

personalised charismatic leadership (House & Howell, 1992); petty tyranny (Ashforth, 1994);

abusive supervision (Tepper, 2000); aversive leadership (Bligh, Kohles, Pearce, Justin, &

Stovall, 2007; Pearce & Sims, 2002); toxic leadership (Lipman-Blumen, 2005); leader

bullying (Ferris, Zinko, Brouer, Buckley & Harvey, 2007), despotic leadership (De Hoogh &

Den Hartog, 2008), and the dark side of laissez-faire leadership (Skogstad, Einarsen,

Torsheim, Aasland & Hetland, 2007).

The phenomenon of the dark side of leadership has predominantly been approached

from two perspectives. First, the leader-centric approach focused on leaders’ dark side traits

and destructive behaviour (Krasikova et al., 2013; Schyns & Hansborough, 2010), such as

“the systematic and repeated behaviour by a leader, supervisor or manager that violates the

3
legitimate interest of the organisation by undermining and/or sabotaging the organisation's

goals, tasks, resources, and effectiveness and/or the motivation, well-being or job satisfaction

of subordinates” (Einarsen et al. 2007, p.208). Leader-centric approaches have been

criticised for being too one-sided. Interest has grown in how followers enact their own role

in the leadership relationship, with a focus on their followership (e.g., the way they

communicate with the leader, comply with their instructions, and respond to their behaviour;

Barbuto, 2000; Carsten, Uhl-Bien, West, Patera, & McGregor, 2010; Uhl-Bien et al., 2014).

Second, a follower-centric approach, led by the abusive supervision literature (Martinko et

al., 2013; Tepper, 2007), focused on the subjective assessment that subordinates make of a

“sustained display of hostile verbal and nonverbal behaviours, excluding physical contact”

(Tepper 2000, p. 178). Thus, theoretical perspectives on the dark side of leadership have

expanded to include a combination of factors beyond a leader’s role in isolation.

Specifically, research now investigates both the role and interaction of leader and follower

personality characteristics, leader and follower motivations, and how these are activated by

environmental context and opportunity (Coleman, 2006; Furnham, 2007; Hogan & Kaiser,

2005; Judge & LePine, 2007; Krasikova et al., 2013; Lipman-Blumen, 2005; Padilla et al.,

2007; Schmit, Kihm, & Robie, 2000; Schyns & Hansborough, 2010; Spreier, Fontaine, &

Malloy, 2006; Thoroughgood, Padilla , Hunter & Tate, 2012).

Research Context

Based on a relational view of leadership and followship theory, three primary

domains related to destructive leadership have been proposed by Padilla, et al. (2007) and

Thoroughgood, Padilla et al. (2012; see Figure 1). In the first domain, an individual leader

will have a predisposition, through one or more of their personality trait characteristics, to

default to dark side leadership behaviours, in combination with a motivation to achieve their

own agenda, even at the expense of others.

4
In the second domain, followers will comply with destructive leaders’ demands, and

may even support and promote destructive leadership. Padilla et al. (2007) defined two types

of susceptible followers: conformers, who lack a clearly defined self-concept and comply

with destructive leaders out of fear, to minimise personal consequences of non-compliance;

and colluders, who may share the leader’s values and actively participate in the destructive

leader's agenda to seek personal gain through their association with the leader.

Finally, in the third domain, the environment or context allows destructive leadership

behaviours to be tolerated (Coleman, 2006; Judge, Piccolo & Kosalka, 2009; Kellerman,

2004a, 2004b; Lipman-Blumen, 2005; Padilla et al., 2007; Spreier et al., 2006; Trickey &

Hyde, 2009). While not the primary focus of the research in this thesis, an understanding of

destructive leadership would be incomplete if it did not acknowledge the environment within

which dysfunctional leadership occurs. Social constructions of leadership and/or followship

are informed by the context in which the leader/follower dyad interaction takes place (van

Knippenberg, 2012). Situational influences that are purported to provide an ideal

environment for destructive leadership are depicted in Figure 1. They include organisations

that foster low control and high co-operation, and large bureaucracies with high power

distance between leaders and follower. Organisational instability (e.g., periods of radical

change) and perceived threat (e.g., social or economic threats) create environments where

more assertive, autocratic leadership will be tolerated. Cultural values and norms (e.g.,

cultures that endorse the avoidance of uncertainty and collectivism, as opposed to

individualism, or fail to provide an ethical climate) also influence whether destructive

leadership will be tolerated, as will the absence of checks and balances of power and control

that concentrates power in the leader (Ferris et al.,, 2007; Grijalva & Harms, 2014; Mulvey &

Padilla, 2010; O'Boyle & Forsyth, 2012; Padilla et al., 2007; Tepper, 2007). Recommended

organisational interventions to put checks and balances in place to guard against destructive

5
leadership include: organisational controls and sanctions; strong corporate governance;

setting organisational norms and expectations regarding acceptable behaviour through an

ethics code or Code of Conduct; promotion of organisational values; whistleblower policy;

robust performance management and disciplinary policies and processes; leadership

development programs; team building programs to reduce inter-team and intra-team conflict;

and provision of Employee Assistance Programs to counsel and support victims of

destructive leaders (Cotton, 2008; Dewe, 1994; Knox Haly, 2008; Lange, 2008; Padilla et al.,

2007; Thoroughgood, Padilla et al., 2012; Wotruba, Chonko, & Loe, 2001). The three

domains portrayed by the researcher in Figure 1, an expansion on Padilla et al.’s model of the

“toxic triangle,” proposes the interrelationship between destructive leadership (the continuum

from dark side traits and leader derailment through to abusive supervision and the more

extreme form of destructive leadership, the Dark Triad), taxonomies of susceptible followers,

and the contextual environments that allow destructive leadership to flourish in organisations.

There is now a significant body of work outlining conceptual explanations for

destructive leadership, including the consequences for leaders and subordinates, and the role

of organisational context. However, there has been less focus on the antecedents that

predispose leaders to engage in destructive leadership behaviours (e.g., Bardes & Piccolo,

2010). As most leaders derail at some point, it is important to focus on understanding

antecedents to derailment. This can then inform the design of leadership development

programs, which are aimed to assist leaders to avoid derailing.

6
Destructive Leaders

 Dark side traits


 Leader derailment
 Abusive Supervision
 Toxic Leadership
 Dark Triad: narcissism,
Machiavellianism, psychopathy

Susceptible Followers Conducive Environments

 Conformers  Instability or threat


 Colluders  Significant change
 Lost Souls  Cultural norms
 Opportunists  Lack of governance
 Acolytes  Power distance between
 Authoritarians leaders and followers
 Bystanders

Figure 1. The toxic triangle: elements in three domains related to destructive leadership.
(Adapted from Padilla et al., 2007; Thoroughgood, Padilla et al., 2012)

In addition, despite some recent theoretical exploration for how followers may cope

with destructive leadership, there has been little field research to validate these theoretical

frameworks (e.g., May, Wesche, Heinitz, & Kerschreiter, 2014; Yagil, Ben-Zur & Tamir,

2011). As it is not always possible to remove derailing or destructive leaders, it is important

to understand the coping strategies followers employ, that may protect them from the harmful

effects of being the target of destructive leadership behaviours, or, alternatively, may

contribute to further derailment and abuse from the leader.

7
Therefore, the research aims of this thesis are to better understand the antecedents for

leaders engaging in destructive leadership behaviours, and the strategies followers employ to

cope with destructive leadership, so practitioners can employ evidence-based practice to

improve interventions designed to address the negative effects of destructive leadership in

organisational settings. This thesis may inform leadership development programs, to identify

strategies that develop leaders in a way that prevents them engaging in destructive leadership.

It may also inform interventions that are designed to protect followers and/or maintain the

well-being of targets of destructive leadership, and, as a result, provide a less conducive

environment for destructive leadership.

Rationale for Research

Imagine. You are just leaving your doctor’s office with the forms signed so

you can take sick leave from work, and lodge a stress claim against your employer, if

you choose to. You have insomnia, a skin rash that won’t go away, have been

diagnosed with irritable bowel syndrome, and your hair is coming out by the handful.

You vacillate between bursts of anger and irritability and becoming teary at the most

inconvenient and embarrassing times. Your doctor has referred you to a

psychologist. The diagnosis is that all your symptoms have been caused by stress at

work. You feel ashamed that you’ve let your boss get to you to the point it has come

to this. If you lose this job, you don’t know how you are going to pay the mortgage.

Destructive leadership leads to a range of negative consequences for leaders,

individual followers, and organisations, including leader derailment, reduced performance,

reduced well-being, reduced employee satisfaction and commitment, increased absenteeism,

unwanted turnover, and occupational stress claims as a result of psychological distress (Bond,

Tuckey, & Dollard, 2010; Mackie, 2008; Nielsen & Einarsen, 2012; Safe Work Australia,

2011; Schyns & Schilling, 2013; Tepper, 2000, 2007). Negative psychological outcomes

8
reported by followers as a result of destructive leadership include reduced self-esteem,

feelings of threat to security, and distress at unjust treatment, leading to disengagement and

burn out (Pelletier, 2010; Tepper, 2000). In addition, perceptions of abusive treatment by a

leader may lead to retaliatory responses by followers (Tepper, 2007).

It is difficult to estimate the financial costs of destructive leadership, but the resultant

stress, disengagement, withdrawal, and absenteeism costs have been estimated. Substantial

litigation and counselling costs can be incurred to address the effects of hostility, bullying,

discrimination, intimidation, and abuse (Richards & Freeman, 2002; Sutton, 2007).

Organisations are made aware that they have a duty of care to maintain the safety of their

employees (Safe Work Australia, 2011). Organisations typically have codes of conduct or

regulatory guidelines to ensure the conduct of desirable activities, although breaches are often

not counted in official statistics, and enforcing consequences to leaders exhibiting harmful

actions may not be possible. Workers’ Compensation claims focusing on psychosocial

injuries have increased over the past decade (Dollard, Shimazu, Bin Nordin, Brough, &

Tuckey, 2014). Mental stress claims (for example, stressful interpersonal relationships at

work, social or physical isolation, poor relationships with superiors, interpersonal conflict,

lack of social support, bullying and harassment) form a significant proportion of the accepted

serious workers’ compensation claims in Australia. Mental stress claims have been reported

to have increased by 25% from 2001 to 2011 (Safe Work Australia, 2013, 2014), and are the

most expensive form of workers’ compensation claims due to the typically lengthy periods of

absence from work. When stressful situations remain unresolved, they can result in harm

within a short time-frame (e.g., headaches, upset stomach, sleep disorders, and irritability),

while chronic illnesses may develop over a long period of exposure (e.g., anxiety disorder,

depression). Lack of supportive leadership has been reported to be associated with reduced

9
well-being and higher distress in Australian workers (Australian Psychological Society,

2013).

Costs to organisations as a result of the impact of work stressors, such as destructive

leadership, are significant. A Medibank Private commissioned study (Medibank Private,

2008) reported that in 2007 the total cost of work-related stress to the Australian economy

was $14.81 billion; the direct cost to employers in stress-related presenteeism and

absenteeism was $10.11 billion. The proportion of stress claims specifically related to “poor

relationships with superiors” was not reported. The cost of addressing the impact of

workplace bullying and harassment alone has been estimated to be as much as $36 billion per

year in Australia (Richards & Freeman, 2002). Destructive leadership is an example of the

occupational stressor anti-social behaviour, including abusive behaviours beyond bullying

and harassment, and has been shown to result in psychological and emotional harm for

subordinates (Tepper, 2000; 2007).

There are also hidden costs incurred as a result of destructive leadership that are

difficult to calculate, for example costs resulting from high staff turnover (such as re-staffing

and re-training), from the affected employee reciprocating the negative treatment, or from

employees withholding voluntary contributions that might benefit their supervisor or the

organisation (Rafferty & Restubog, 2011). Gallup research reported active disengagement,

caused in part by destructive manager behaviour, costs the US an estimated $450 billion to

$550 billion annually (Gallup, 2013). The cost to US corporations of behaviours associated

with abusive supervision (e.g., the costs of absenteeism, health care, and lost productivity)

has been estimated at $23.8 billion annually (Tepper, Duffy, Henle, & Lambert, 2006),

although this claim has recently been questioned as being based on research specifically

investigating the cost of leader bullying, not abusive supervision (Martinko et al., 2013).

Indirect costs associated with abuse, such as workplace bullying and harassment, include

10
medical costs, premature retirement, and a greater need for social services and welfare

(Samnani & Singh, 2012).

Nevertheless, despite organisations being aware of the personal and financial cost of

destructive leadership, many fail to take steps to ensure the quality of their leadership. They

make personnel selection mistakes, tolerate bad managers, and fail to monitor their leaders’

performance in key areas (Hogan & Kaiser, 2005). Depending on perpetrator power and

intentionality, observers of destructive leadership behaviours are often reluctant to report

such behaviour due to fear of repercussions or anticipated regret about reporting the

wrongdoing, thus perpetuating a climate of silence. Even when it is recognised that a leader

is engaging in unethical or illegal behaviour, the cost of resisting their influence or addressing

their bad behaviour (e.g., legal action, escalated conflict and aggression, retaliatory action)

can be perceived to be greater than the cost of not addressing the consequences of destructive

behaviour (e.g., stress claims from affected followers; Clements & Washbush, 1999; Lipman-

Blumen, 2005; Rate & Sternberg, 2007; Schabracq & Smit, 2007; Shapiro & Von Glinow,

2007). Despite highly funded and well publicised efforts to address physical diseases, such

as cancer, AIDS, diabetes, and heart disease, significantly less effort is put into understanding

and addressing destructive leadership. Yet, the effects of destructive leadership can be

equally harmful to the health and well-being of individuals and organisations and, through

flow on effects, to family well-being and the wider community (Kellerman, 2004a; Martinko

et al., 2013). For these reasons, researching and understanding the nature of destructive

leadership and its impact on followers is an important and growing, although currently under-

developed, research area, with important implications for practice.

Academic discussion and investigations into the processes of destructive leadership

have increased over the past decade. There is now a significant body of work providing

conceptual explanations of destructive leadership, but limited empirical and field research to

11
date into why leaders display destructive behaviours, how followers cope with their

behaviour, and how the toxic consequences of destructive leadership can be ameliorated

(Krasikova et al., 2013; Tepper, 2007). There is currently an incomplete understanding of the

antecedents and causes of destructive leadership behaviours, followers’ responses to such

behaviours, and organisational practices that can prevent harm to employees experiencing

destructive leadership.

The research in this thesis is approached from an applied perspective, hence the

researcher’s determination to use field research and organisational samples, when a

significant share of research in the destructive leadership field uses convenience or student

samples (e.g., Pelletier, 2010; Yagil et al., 2011). This thesis research is designed, firstly, to

address a call to better understand the types of leader characteristics and behaviours that

employees perceive as harmful to their personal and occupational well-being, extending the

descriptors of destructive leadership behaviours as a basis for further investigation and

research (Pelletier, 2010). Secondly, to inform evidence-based practice, there is clearly a

need to build on the research work conducted to date, to verify and extend our theoretical

understanding of the mechanisms that cause leaders to engage in destructive leadership, and

the mechanisms by which followers choose coping strategies in response to perceived

destructive behaviours from their leader. This thesis builds on the extensive coping literature

by testing the veracity of two coping frameworks within the context of employees’ coping

with destructive leadership (Skinner, Edge, Altman & Sherwood, 2003; Yagil, et al., 2011).

Finally, this thesis seeks to address the call for further research to discover effective strategies

for coping with destructive leadership at work (Yagil et al., 2011). As previously discussed,

there is now a significant body of work offering conceptual discussions and theoretical

propositions in the destructive leadership stream of research. However, as a relatively new

stream of research, there remains a significant gap in research that tests these theoretical

12
frameworks. Yet it is important to understand the mechanisms by which leaders derail and

by which target employees are harmed, if researchers are to identify evidence-based solutions

to this issue.

Research Questions

This thesis research is designed to fill the gaps previously discussed by addressing the

following research questions:

What are the antecedent traits that may predispose an individual leader to engage

in destructive leadership behaviours? What traits may inhibit the predisposition of

leaders to employ harmful behaviours?

What are the leader traits and behaviours that followers perceive as harmful to

their well-being? What are the psychological, emotional, and physical outcomes

of destructive leadership traits and behaviours for followers? What coping

strategies do targets of destructive leadership traits and behaviours employ to deal

with those psychological, emotional and physical outcomes? and

What is an effective, evidence-based intervention to enhance well-being and

prevent or reduce harm to followers within the context of destructive leadership?

Research Goals and Approach

The goal of this thesis research is to provide evidence-based data for practitioners, to

inform how they devise strategies to prevent destructive leadership from emerging in

workplaces and to better protect employees from the negative effects of destructive

leadership. Choice of research design is based on the desire to address the research questions

in a way that contributes to informing practical, realistic interventions that can be employed

by practitioners to prevent and/or address destructive leadership in organisations.

13
Research epistemology. Consideration was given to the epistemology that informs

this thesis, including the methodology, and the methods for data collection and analysis

(Creswell, 2009). The research for this thesis is approached from a combination of two

knowledge claim positions: first, pragmatism, investigating a real-world problem (e.g., the

negative impact of destructive leadership behaviours on followers in organisations); and

second, positivism, challenging the traditional notion of our knowledge of leadership, and

utilising empirical observation and measurement to verify existing theoretical frameworks that

explain the causes and outcomes of a real-world problem (e.g., dark side traits, self-regulation

traits, destructive leadership, coping with abusive supervision, and stress management

interventions). Given the pragmatic approach adopted, it is important to the researcher that the

results of the research in this thesis are informative to practitioners and contribute to solutions

to the problem that are applicable in the workplace.

In addition, a combination of three approaches to the research studies in this thesis are

employed: (a) functionalist (identifying correlations between variables associated with

leadership), (b) interpretive (meaning-making process associated with leadership), and (c)

critical (a negative critique of leadership; Alvesson & Spicer, 2012). The research seeks to

identify leader traits correlated with destructive leadership (functionalist and critical), and to

investigate how followers interpret and respond to perceived destructive leadership behaviours

based on their reports of ongoing interactions between both parties (interpretive). The research

then seeks to empirically evaluate an intervention designed to mitigate the negative effects of

destructive leadership behaviours on followers (functionalist and critical).

Research methodologies. Given the goal of this thesis, to identify evidence-based

strategies to prevent or mitigate the damage and costs destructive leadership causes, and given

the limited number of field studies on destructive leadership, the positivistic research approach

chosen focussed on accessing samples of employees within organisations for theory

14
verification to understand the antecedents of destructive leadership and how negative outcomes

for followers might be mitigated. Samples from the “real world” were sought, despite the

difficulty in conducting rigorous research designs in the field. It is acknowledged that this

choice may result in a quasi-experimental methodology, using a non-randomized

organisational sample. Both quantitative and mixed methods methodological approaches were

considered as approaches to address the research questions of interest.

Structure of Thesis

This chapter introduced the argument that destructive leadership is a significant and

costly issue for organisations, and that important gaps remain in our understanding of the

mechanisms by which leaders derail and by which harm is caused to followers by destructive

leadership behaviours. It also introduced the rationale for this thesis research, the research

questions, and the core goals and approach for this thesis.

Chapter 2 reviews the dark side of leadership literature predominantly through a

leader-centric perspective, investigating traits that may predispose leaders to engage in

destructive leadership behaviours, and mediating traits and characteristics that may enhance

or mitigate that predisposition. It reviews existing measures of the dark side of leadership,

and discusses organisational responses when dark side leadership traits and behaviours are

identified. It concludes by discussing the challenges to researching destructive leadership.

Chapter 3 reviews the dark side of leadership literature through a follower-centric

perspective. It considers the impact of destructive leadership on followers as an occupational

stressor, and discusses employees’ responses and coping mechanisms to abusive leadership

behaviours. It concludes by exploring interventions that may assist in maintaining follower

well-being in the context of destructive leadership.

Chapters 4 to 6 present the three research studies designed to address the research

questions. Figure 2 demonstrates the links between the three studies of this thesis: antecedent
15
personality traits may predispose a leader to engage in destructive leadership behaviours,

which are likely to lead to emotional, psychological, and physical harm when destructive

leadership behaviours are perceived by a follower as an occupational stressor. By

participating in a secondary stress prevention intervention (e.g., a resiliency training session),

followers may maintain their well-being by increasing their ability to cope with this stressor,

reducing the potential harm caused.

Chapter 4 outlines the first study, which investigates individual antecedents of

destructive leadership behaviours by assessing the personality traits of independence,

negative affectivity, and self-regulation traits. A research model (Figure 4) is presented

which outlines the hypothesised mediating role of the trait of negative affectivity and the self-

regulation traits of emotional control, self-awareness, and stress tolerance. The results may

provide guidance to practitioners on manager recruitment, selection, and development

processes.

Chapter 5 analyses leader behaviours perceived by followers as harmful, investigating

the consequences of destructive leadership proposed in the literature, and explores the coping

strategies employed by followers to deal with the impact of destructive leadership behaviours

on them. With this study the researcher seeks to build on previous coping frameworks to

advance understanding on how followers cope with the effects of destructive leadership by

examining the fit of coping strategies reported by respondents with two existing theoretical

coping frameworks (Skinner et al., 2003; Yagil et al., 2011), and inform organisational

interventions for maintaining employee engagement, performance, and well-being in the

context of dealing with destructive leadership behaviours.

16
Mediators Secondary Stress
Leader: Prevention Intervention
 Negative affectivity  Education on
 Self-regulation destructive leadership
behaviours
 Self-awareness
 Resilience skills
 Stress tolerance training

Follower Coping Consequences for


Antecedents to followers
Destructive leadership Leadership Follower  Problem Solving/ Direct of perceived
behaviours behaviours perceptions Communication Destructive Leadership
 Leader personality enacted of leadership  Support Seeking  Psychological
traits, i.e. behaviours as  Avoidance/ Escape  Emotional
independence destructive (e.g.  Reframing/ Accommodation  Physical
 Trait activation toxic or abusive)
 Ingratiation  Career
 Leader power
motivation  Information Seeking
 Submission
 Self-Reliance
 Helplessness
 Delegation

Figure 2. Research model for thesis: destructive leadership traits and behaviours and their effect on follower well-being.

17
Chapter 6 describes and evaluates a trial intervention, designed by this researcher to

improve followers’ well-being, through increased resilience. This longitudinal study tests the

effectiveness of a stress management intervention on enhancing well-being, specifically

within the context of destructive leadership. This research was designed to address the call

for evidence-based interventions, underpinned with integrated theoretical frameworks, to

mitigate the stress and negative impact on well-being of destructive leadership behaviours.

Chapter 7 discusses the implications of the findings for the design and implementation

of interventions to address harm caused by the dark side of leadership. The chapter

concludes with the limitations of the thesis studies and relevant directions for future research.

Thesis Scope. Given destructive leadership is a complex and growing area of research

(Schyns & Hansborough, 2010; Tepper, 2007), in order to confine the research this thesis

focuses on the individual (i.e., leadership traits and style) and dyad level (i.e., impact of

relationship between leader and follower) only. This thesis does not research the organisational

and/or team characteristics that allow destructive leadership to flourish and psychological

injury to occur (Cotton & Hart, 2003). It does not explore reasons for the compounding

complication of organisational silence once destructive leadership is identified. Nor does it

examine organisational interventions to address harm after it has been caused by destructive

leadership behaviours. All of these are important and urgent topics for further research.

Chapter Conclusion

This chapter outlined the rationale for this research, the research questions, and the

thesis aims and approach to research. The focus of much of the extant research has been on

the consequences of destructive leadership, with fewer studies focusing on the antecedents,

predictors, and mediators of destructive leadership behaviours, or how followers cope with

these destructive behaviours, the focus of this research. This thesis focuses specifically on

personality traits that may predict or mediate the use of destructive behaviours by

18
organisational leaders with power and status. It investigates the impact of destructive

leadership behaviours on followers’ well-being, follower coping mechanisms within an

organisational context, and interventions to enhance follower well-being.

To research this phenomenon, both leader-centric and follower-centric approaches are

adopted and are described in more detail in Chapters 2 and 3. A review of the literature

focusing predominantly on leader-centric approaches to destructive leadership is discussed in

Chapter 2, and follower-centric and followership approaches are discussed in detail in

Chapter 3.

19
Chapter 2. Why Leaders Derail to the Dark Side

The previous chapter highlighted gaps in the research that this thesis attempts to

address, and established the rationale for why this thesis research is important. The purpose

of this chapter is to provide an overview of the dark side of leadership literature and, more

specifically, to review the literature from the destructive leadership research stream. This

chapter takes a leader-centric perspective, including (a) how the dark side of leadership

theory has been influenced by, and has itself influenced, leadership theory; (b) the various

ways destructive leadership has been conceptualised by theorists; (c) measures of dark side

traits and the concept of derailment (dark side of bright side traits); (d) traits that may prevent

or inhibit leader derailment to the dark side; and (e) methods for researching leaders’ dark

side organisations. Two specific questions in relation to the research model outlined in

Figure 2 (see p. 17) arise: (1) what are the antecedent traits and characteristics that may

predispose an individual leader to engage in destructive leadership behaviours? (2) What

traits may inhibit the predisposition of leaders to employ harmful behaviours? Two

additional topics are reviewed in this chapter in relation to researching this phenomenon:

what measures of destructive leadership are available to researchers, and, once identified, can

leaders with dark side traits change.

There is a growing body of evidence that some leaders behave in a destructive manner

towards subordinates (Martinko et al., 2013; Nielsen & Einarsen, 2012) and towards

organisations (Kellerman, 2004a; Thoroughgood, Tate, Sawyer, & Jacobs, 2012). Few

studies have investigated the prevalence of destructive leadership in contemporary

workplaces based on a taxonomy of destructive behaviours (Einarsen, Skogstad, & Aasland,

2010). One study found up to 25% of their manager sample (N = 6,774) was at risk of at

least one dark side personality trait (De Fruyt, Wille, & Furnham, 2013). Other estimates

suggest up to 70% of employees report their manager is the worst aspect of their job, and

20
abusive treatment by supervisors is one of the most commonly cited causes of stress and

compensation claims (Aasland, Skogstad, Notelaers, Nielsen, & Einarsen, 2010; Einarsen et

al., 2010). Aasland et al. (2010) found up to 60% of all respondents in a sample of 4,500

Norwegian employees reported their immediate supervisor exhibited some kind of consistent

and frequent destructive behaviour (active and passive) during the previous six months, while

only about 40% reported no exposure to such leadership behaviours. If reported prevalence

estimates are correct, it implies that many employees will experience some form of

destructive leadership during their career that may cause harm to their well-being.

The dark side of leadership has been influenced by leadership theory and, in turn, has

itself influenced leadership theory. The first two chapter sections in this chapter review the

history of leadership theory and the dark side of leadership theory. This chapter then

describes definitions, theories, and measures under the destructive leadership literature

stream, as proposed by those studying leadership derailment, abusive supervision, and

destructive leadership. The chapter goes on to discuss organisational responses to destructive

leadership, and concludes with a discussion on the challenges of researching destructive

leadership behaviours in organisations.

Brief History of Leadership Theory

Theoretical conceptualisations of leadership evolved throughout the 20th century (Rost

& Smith, 1992; Uhl-Bien et al., 2014). At the outset, the primary focus was on studying an

individual leader, with traditional leadership definitions highlighting the need for a leader to

inspire confidence, influence, and support people toward goal attainment (Dubrin, 2010).

More recently, the field of leadership has introduced the concept of shared leadership,

focusing not only on the leader, but also on followers, work setting/context, and culture

(Avolio, Walumbwa, & Weber, 2009). The idea of leadership as action taken by great men

(and women) to control organisations and societies was superseded by a group theory of

21
leadership that concentrated on the actions leaders took to engage a group to achieve goals

(Rost & Smith, 1992). During and after World War II, it was purported that certain traits

were required to exercise leadership (e.g., personality variables such as dominance and self-

confidence; Stogdill, 1948). The behavioural movement in the late 1950s discredited trait

theory, finding little association between personal traits and leader effectiveness.

Researchers instead focused on how leaders behaved when they exercised leadership

(Hollander & Julian, 1969; Stogdill, 1948). Both trait and behavioural theories provided

useful lenses to study destructive leadership, as demonstrated “dark side” behaviours,

together with the underlying traits and motivations driving such behaviour, are areas of

interest. These theoretical perspectives are discussed in the context of destructive leadership

later in this chapter. However, these leader-centric theories did not take into account the

dyadic relationship and interaction between leader and follower.

The next phase in the evolution of leadership theory was contingency or situational

leadership theory. This theory purported effective leaders adjusted their behaviours

depending on various factors, such as the skill and motivation of followers (Fiedler, 1967).

Fiedler’s contingency theory took into account individual differences among leaders to

describe their leadership style across different situations, and provided a means to identify

different types of leaders. This theory’s perspective facilitates the view that leader behaviour

that is effective in one situation may be harmful or distressing to followers in another.

Another theory that focused on the development of a two-way relationship between leaders

and followers was the leader-member exchange (LMX) theory of leadership. Subsequent

development of LMX theory suggested that the quality of these LMX relationships influence

subordinates' responsibility, decision influence, access to resources, and performance, and

that organisational success is increased by positive relations between the leader and

subordinate (Graen & Uhl-Bien, 1995). It could also be argued that a good quality LMX

22
relationship is beneficial to the satisfaction and well-being of subordinates. Indeed attention

has been drawn to the possibility that the construct of abusive supervision overlaps with the

concept of low quality LMX relationships. For example, when a leader is unable to select a

contextually appropriate behaviour that will elicit the desired behaviour from an employee,

they are likely to be perceived by the employee as abusive, reducing trust in and satisfaction

with the leader (Ferris et al., 2007; Martinko, Harvey, Sikora, & Douglas, 2011).

In the 1980s, the focus moved to understanding what leaders do to promote

excellence. Leadership was defined as mobilizing resources through building common

motives and purposes so as to engage, influence, and satisfy followers (Burns, 1978).

Leaders were purported to do this by creating a shared mission, shared meaning, and by

demonstrating integrity (Bennis, 2003; Conger & Kanungo, 1987; Schabracq & Smit, 2007).

Concepts of transformational or charismatic leadership and transactional leadership became

popular as the models relating to effective leadership, and for enhancing employee

motivation, task and contextual performance (Bass & Avolio, 1993; Bono & Judge, 2004;

Judge & Piccolo, 2004; Lowe, Kroeck, & Sivasubramaniam, 1996; Wang, Oh, Courtright, &

Colbert, 2011). Transformational leadership characteristics include building trust, inspiring

others, demonstrating integrity, and treating followers as individuals. Transactional

leadership focused on setting expectations and rewards, dealing proactively with difficulties,

and monitoring mistakes. Transformational leadership led to a research focus on values-

based leadership models, including authentic, character-based, ethical, servant, and spiritual

leadership styles (Avolio & Gardner, 2005; Fry, 2003; George, Sims, McLean, & Mayer,

2007; Greenleaf, 1998; Hannah & Avolio, 2010; Sosik & Cameron, 2010; Steger, 2012;

Tanner, Brugger, van Schie, & Lebherz, 2010).

Once attention was drawn to the need to examine the dark side of leadership (Conger,

1990; Kellerman, 2004a), interest returned to the study of individual leader personality traits

23
and motivations in the context of the potential dark side of personality, that is, the dark side

of transformational, charismatic, and authentic leadership styles (Furnham, Trickey, & Hyde.,

2012; House & Howell, 1992; Judge & LePine, 2007; Judge et al., 2009; Khoo & Burch,

2008; Nyberg & Sveningsson, 2014). Interest also returned to the psycho-sociological study

of the interaction between leader personality (e.g., the disposition to think, feel, and act in

certain ways), and the situation in which leadership is enacted (e.g., follower and

organisational context characteristics), particularly in respect to the role of such interaction in

the effectiveness of leader behaviour (Padilla et al., 2007; Uhl-Bien et al., 2014; van

Knippenberg, 2012). A distinction was drawn between an individual’s intrapersonal

leadership (e.g., the link between traits and selected actions) and their interpersonal processes

(e.g., the link between selected actions and outcomes; Dinh & Lord, 2012). It was

hypothesised that different events or situations may trigger different responses from a

particular leader with variable outcomes. Of particular interest was what stimulated leaders

to follow, or not follow, their dispositional tendencies; that is, what led to trait activation.

Trait activation theory purported that trait relevant cues in a particular situation lead to trait

expression and, depending on the effectiveness and appropriateness of the trait expressed, the

effectiveness of the resulting behaviour was determined (Tett & Burnett, 2003; van

Knippenberg, 2012). van Knippenberg (2012) applied Tett & Burnett’s (2003) interactionist

model to leadership: trait expression was a function of

the personality trait x situation trait relevance (trait relevant cues) x situation strength

(the extent to which the situation is clear on what is appropriate and inappropriate

behaviour).

This model is equally applicable to the activation of dark side personality traits when

expressing destructive leadership behaviours (see Figure 3).

24
As interest in the dark side of leadership grew, depending on the theoretical focus,

disparate streams of research resulted in inconsistencies and overlaps of definitions and

conceptual models across the literature. These are discussed in the following chapter

sections.

Theoretical Review of the Dark Side of Leadership

The continuum of dark side of leadership theory outlines a range of negative

characteristics and behaviours, from overused personality traits in inappropriate situations, to

the presence of the Dark Triad of personality: subclinical narcissism, Machiavellianism, and

subclinical psychopathy. For example, at one end of the continuum, attention has been drawn

to a proposed curvilinear relationship between leadership trait characteristics and

performance/leadership effectiveness. This perspective asserts that leadership traits and

behaviours that may be crucial to success in one situation may be unrelated or even

negatively related to success in other contexts; the dark side of bright characteristics and the

bright side of dark characteristics (Ames & Flynn, 2007; Benson & Campbell, 2007; Dinh &

Lord, 2012; Furnham, et al., 2012; House & Howell, 1992; Judge et.al., 2009; Kaiser &

Hogan, 2011; Kaiser, LeBreton, & Hogan, 2015; Khoo & Burch, 2008). As discussed

previously, it has also been acknowledged that context often fosters destructive leadership

behaviours and that susceptible followers and conducive environments can entice leaders to

go astray, by fulfilling their desires (e.g., for power/control) or allowing free expression of

destructive personal traits (e.g., dark triad) and learned negative behaviours in a toxic

environment (Clements & Washbush, 1999; Judge et al., 2009; Kellerman, 2004a; Lipman-

Blumen,2005; Padilla et al., 2007; Thoroughgood, Padilla, et al., 2012).

25
Situation behavioural
Situation trait relevance
appropriateness
 Power motivation
 Crisis management
 Personal goals  Organisational change
 Follower well-being

Leader personality traits Leader behaviour Leader effectiveness


versus destructiveness

Situation strength

 Governance
 Clear code of
behaviour
 Organisational goals
 Performance measures

Figure 3. Trait activation model of destructive leadership. Adapted from van Knippenberg (2012).

26
Those driven by needs for power, prestige, and dominance often seek leadership

positions that require power and control over resources, and the need to influence those at the

highest authority, that is, Chief Executive Officers, boards, and governments. Indeed, some

of the elements of personality disorders, such as narcissistic traits, are strikingly similar to

characteristics sought in organisational leaders (i.e., aggressiveness, alertness, dominance,

enthusiasm, extroversion, independence, risk taking, and self-confidence; Board &

Fritzon,2005; Brown, 1964; Judge et al.,2009). To better understand the individual traits,

characteristics, and motivations that contribute to leaders exhibiting destructive leadership, it

is useful to turn to psychological and psycho-sociological theoretical perspectives.

Individual leader traits. Psychology focuses on personality traits, characteristics,

preferences, motivations, and behaviours to explain features of the dark side of leadership.

Over the past decade, research has returned to the role of traits in both the bright and dark

side of leadership (Judge et al., 2009). The bright side of leadership has been defined as the

ability to build and maintain a high performing team, using personality characteristics based

on the Big Five: higher levels of extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness and

openness, and lower levels of neuroticism, with an absence of dark side traits (Hogan,

Curphy, & Hogan, 1994). When compared with the Big Five, dark side traits tend to be

correlated with lower levels of agreeableness, conscientiousness, and openness (Judge &

LePine, 2007; Judge et al., 2009; Paulhus & Williams, 2002). For some leadership

characteristics, more is not necessarily better; so too with dark side tendencies (Hogan,

Raskin, & Fazzini, 1990). Having a moderate level of dark side tendencies can be beneficial

to leadership success. In fact they may be viewed as situational strengths (Benson &

Campbell, 2007; Furnham et al., 2012; Judge et al., 2009). It is only when trait

characteristics become extreme or certain behaviours are overused that they create problems

(Benson & Campbell, 2007; Hogan & Hogan, 2001; Judge et al., 2009; Najur, Holland, &

27
Van Landuyt, 2004). Examples discussed in the literature include the dark side of charisma

(House & Howell, 1992; Pundt, 2014), the dark side of transformational leadership (Khoo &

Burch, 2008), and the dark side of authentic leadership (Nyberg & Sveningsson, 2014).

At the extreme end of destructive leadership, a dark triad of personality traits has been

proposed by theorists: Machiavellianism, subclinical narcissism, and subclinical psychopathy

(Babiak & Hare, 2007; Boddy, 2015; Clarke, 2005; O'Boyle & Forsyth, 2012; Padilla et al.,

2007; Paulhus & Williams, 2002). Leaders with these characteristics are driven by an

insatiable need for power to achieve their own ends. Some argue that individuals displaying

these traits are unable to be managed and should be exited from the organisation as soon as

possible (Babiak & Hare, 2007; Clarke, 2005), although these recommendations are based on

case studies and anecdotal evidence, rather than empirical research, and so should be read

with a certain amount of caution (Boddy, 2015). Others argue that, due to research and

methodological limitations, not enough is known about psychopaths in the workplace and

making definitive statements regarding their traits, negative attributes and the adverse

consequences of their behaviour are premature (Smith & Lilienfeld, 2013). A more detailed

analysis of the characteristics of this type of destructive leader is discussed later in this

chapter.

Followers and Destructive Leaders. Leader-centric theories of leadership were

challenged by follower-centric and followship theories (May et al., 2014; Uhl-Bien et al.,

2014). This section reviews how these two approaches examine the role of followers in the

phenomenon of destructive leadership. Follower-based approaches adopted the follower as

the primary focus by exploring how follower perceptions and behaviours are related to

organisational outcomes of interest, such as perceived leadership effectiveness (Carsten et al.,

2010; Graen & Uhl-Bien, 1995). One follower-centric approach to leadership, implicit

leadership theory, contended that leadership exists only in the mind of the beholder, usually

28
the follower, based on individual conceptions of prototypic leaders and confirmation of

expectations formed from global impressions of leadership effectiveness (Hollander & Julian,

1969; Junker & van Dick, 2014; Phillips & Lord, 1981). Thus, depending on the prototypic

representation of a leader held by an individual they may (or may not) perceive a particular

behaviour of a leader to be destructive (Martinko et al., 2013).

In contrast, a followership approach examines followers’ perspectives of followship,

rather than followers’ perspectives of leadership (Carsten et al., 2010). A followership

approach explores socially constructed definitions of followship and can be considered a

useful lens to better understand why destructive leaders retain a following. Followers’ need

for safety, security, group membership, and predictability in an uncertain world may make

them susceptible to destructive leadership (Lipman-Blumen, 2005; Padilla et al., 2007;

Thoroughgood, Padilla et al., 2012). Follower silence, compliance, obedience and/or

collusion allows destructive leaders to thrive (see Figure 4). A recent component of the

leader/follower interaction research into destructive leadership is exploration of the extent to

which follower coping responses may encourage or inhibit further abuse (May et al., 2014).

Definitions of Destructive Leadership

As previously discussed, one challenge in researching the dark side of leadership is

the overlap between definitions, conceptualisations and models of destructive leadership.

Although a number of conceptual dark side leadership theoretical frameworks have been

proposed by researchers, there is limited empirical research to demonstrate their validity

(Einarsen et al., 2007; Krasikova et al., 2013; Padilla et al., 2007; Tepper, 2000). In

Appendix A, Table 1 sets out the range of definitions, destructive leadership behaviours, and

categories of destructive leaders emerging in the literature.

29
Constructive Leadership Derailed Leadership Destructive Leadership
Bright Side of Leadership Dark Side of Leadership Dark Side of Leadership
(Values based leadership) (Strengths in Extreme) (Harm to individuals and organisations)

 Authentic Leadership Strengths overused, e.g.,:  Abusive Supervision


 Charismatic Leadership  Extreme self-confidence becomes  Aversive Leadership
 Ethical Leadership arrogance  Dark Side Charismatic
 Responsible Leadership  Extreme assertiveness becomes  Dark Side Transformational
intimidation
 Servant Leadership  Dark Side Authentic
 Extreme competitiveness becomes
 Spiritual Leadership  Laissez-faire Leadership
manipulation
 Transformational Leadership  Machiavellian Leadership
 Extreme affiliation becomes
passive-aggressiveness  Narcissistic Leadership
 Extreme need for control becomes  Organisational Psychopaths/
micromanaging Sociopaths (subclinical)
Negative traits not addressed, e.g.,:  Toxic Leadership
 Low emotional control leads to  Tyrannical Leadership
extreme moods, unpredictability and  Unethical Leadership
volatility

Leader self-regulation Negative affectivity and


poor leader self-regulation
Follower courage, Follower fear, blind
influence and voice obedience, silence, and
compliance

Figure 4. Conceptual model of the interaction of leadership style, leader self-regulation, and followership.
30
The dark side of leadership literature can best be described under three research themes:

leader derailment, abusive supervision, and destructive leadership. This chapter section

reviews the definitions, models, and behavioural inventories within these three research

themes. For the purposes of this thesis, when discussing the dark side of leadership from a

leader-centric perspective the term “destructive leadership” is used. This term includes all

forms of leadership that cause harm to followers, e.g., leader derailment, abusive supervision,

and toxic leadership. Therefore, when analysing the dark side of leadership from a follower-

centric perspective the terms “destructive leadership,” “toxic leadership,” and “abusive

supervision” are used interchangeably, depending on the focus of the research discussed, to

represent the perceived harm caused to followers subjected to destructive leadership.

Leader derailers or situational strengths? Leadership derailment has been defined

as either career derailment (McCall & Lombardo, 1983; Najur et al., 2004) or as an overuse

of strengths (Benson & Campbell, 2007; Kaplan & Kaiser, 2009). It has been estimated that

up to 70% of all leaders will derail through alienating others and losing the loyalty and

commitment of followers (Trickey & Hyde, 2009). Leader career derailment has been

described as a combination of an inability to develop or adapt (e.g., inability to learn from

mistakes or from direct feedback) and a lack of important characteristics or abilities to

succeed (e.g., low levels of personality traits such as agreeableness, inability to build

effective working relations; Benson & Campbell, 2007; Judge & LePine, 2007; Judge et al.,

2009; Leslie & Velsor, 1996; McCall & Lombardo, 1983; Najar, et al., 2004).

Common causes of derailment identified in the literature include: adopting an

insensitive, abrasive, or bullying style; adopting an autocratic, controlling style; displaying

aloofness, arrogance, or self-centered ambition; being emotionally volatile; failing to listen to

a contrary point of view or heed conflicting information; and failing to constructively face an

obvious problem (Conger, 1990; McCall & Lombardo, 1983; Najur et al., 2004). The

31
“strengths in extreme” explanation for leader derailment proposes that there is an optimal

level of personality traits: an excessive level of bright side traits is associated with derailing

leadership behaviours, and having a moderate level of dark side tendencies can be beneficial

to leadership success (Benson & Campbell, 2007; Furnham et al., 2012; Judge et al., 2009;

Kaplan & Kaiser, 2009; Najur et al., 2004; Schmit et al., 2000). This trait approach explains

negative leadership behaviours principally in terms of the occurrence of extreme bright side

or dark side personality traits predisposing the leader to exhibit inappropriate or harmful

behaviours, which in turn lead to leader derailment: “the tendency to use quasi-leadership

tactics or to engage in various behaviours that may prove successful in changing others’

behaviour in the short-term, but ultimately cause the leader to fail or lose support of those

around him or her.” (Previsor, 2000, p.4). Therefore, destructive leadership theorists

proposed that behaviour towards subordinates and the organisation that departs from

constructive leadership behaviour ultimately causes the leader to fail (Einarsen et al., 2007;

Kaiser & Hogan, 2011).

Hogan and Hogan (2001) proposed a taxonomy of personality factors that could

potentially derail a leader and measured these using the Hogan Development Survey (HDS)

within a sample of 10,035 working adults. Table 2 sets out examples provided by Hogan and

Hogan, using HDS scales, showing how high scores in a particular attribute may lead to an

overuse of bright side characteristics, resulting in a loss of the benefit of that attribute. This

can cause a leader to subsequently derail and become ineffective or destructive.

Benson and Campbell (2007) conducted two independent studies (N = 1,306 and N =

290 respectively) investigating the relationship between personality and leadership

performance using Hogan’s HDS and the Global Personality Inventory (GPI).

32
Table 2

Degrees of Dark Side Traits using the HDS (Hogan & Hogan, 2001)

HDS scale Moderate scores High scores

Excitable Consistently generate Waiver between enthusiasm and

enthusiasm intimidation

Skeptical Vigilant to organisational Think people are after their job

politics and changes

Cautious Take time when making Take time making even minor

decisions decisions

Reserved Can make tough calls Create a ‘cold’ culture and are

painfully objective

Leisurely May be ‘diplomatically’ Frequently say one thing but do

(passive aggressive) untruthful at times another

Bold Have firm beliefs Believe that their way is the only

way

Mischievous Test limits but follow rules when Enjoy breaking rules and taking

necessary risks

Imaginative Have lots of good ideas Have lots of good ideas that are

never implemented

Colourful Use charisma to charm others Dominate meetings and

consistently melodramatic

Diligent Can focus on details Cannot see the big picture

33
Dutiful Builds relationships with Lack a spine when dealing with

superiors superiors

They found support for the hypothesis that a curvilinear relationship exists between dark side

personality traits and leadership performance (e.g., a moderate level of dark side

characteristics is related to effective leadership performance). The difference between

leaders who leverage situational strengths and those who derail may lie in the variability to

control impulsivity.

Leaders able to regulate behaviour between impulse and action are able to utilise their

characteristics in situations that are appropriate (Bandura, 1991; Furnham et al., 2012; Judge

et al., 2009; Karoly, 1993; Kupers & Weibler, 2005; Paulhus & Williams, 2002).

Building on this concept of overused strengths, Spreier, Fontaine, and Malloy (2006)

proposed a leadership model that proposed overachievement as contributing to leader

derailment. Spreier et al. proposed that leadership styles result from the interaction of the

need for individual achievement, affiliation, personalised power (where the leader draws

strength from controlling others and making them feel weak), and socialised power (where

the leader’s strength comes from empowering people). Specifically, Spreier et al. proposed

that an extremely strong drive to achieve, to improve personal performance, and to exceed

standards of excellence may cause leaders to micromanage, to pace themselves and others,

and focus on goals and outcomes rather than people. A strong need for affiliation may

motivate leaders to maintain close, friendly relationships, but in the extreme could lead to

passive aggressive behaviours, such as avoiding confrontation or avoiding giving negative

feedback. Spreier et al. also hypothesised how these needs interact with a leader’s preference

for personalised power. Leaders can derail when their excessive need for individual

34
achievement is combined with a need for personal heroics, leading to an overreliance on a

specific leadership style (i.e., a directive or pacesetting style of leadership). This perspective

purports that the more the leader is motivated by personalised power, the more harm they are

likely to cause (McClelland, 1970, 1975; Spreier et al., 2006).

Dysfunctional traits and behaviours tend to be demonstrated under stress, when

success is seen as crucial, when there are time and workload pressures, or when a leader’s

personal and psychological resources are depleted (Benson & Campbell, 2007; Byrne et al.,

2014; Hogan & Hogan, 2001; Najar et al., 2004). For example, while a self-confident leader

can inspire and empower subordinates, an inflated sense of self-confidence may be perceived

as arrogance or hubris, and an exaggerated sense of self-worth makes these leaders defensive

against most forms of critical feedback. Similarly, a leader’s ability to be assertive and take

charge is effective when dealing with a crisis, but extreme levels of control can also be

perceived as intimidating and micro-managing. An extreme preference to build relationships

and offer support can lead to avoidance of conflict and passive-aggressive behaviours (Ames

& Flynn, 2007; Benson & Campbell, 2007; Campbell, Hoffman, Campbell, & Marchisio,

2011; Furnham et al., 2012; Judge et al., 2009). Although leaders may be well intentioned

and generally focused on achieving individual, team, or organisational objectives, if they

have an extreme preference to use these derailing behaviours to achieve their goals, it will

hinder their effectiveness through loss of support of followers and peers over time (Benson &

Campbell, 2007; Judge et al., 2009; Kaplan & Kaiser, 2009). There is some evidence that

managers performing poorly can change derailing behaviours, but they need more intensive

training and coaching than that found in traditional leadership development programs (Hogan

et al., 1994; Sandler, 2012). However, moderate levels of both bright side and dark side

characteristics, if used in an appropriate context, can enhance a leader’s effectiveness.

Indeed, as discussed above, in moderation these preferences may be viewed as situational

35
strengths, especially for leaders with cognitive intelligence, high levels of emotional

intelligence, and political skill (Benson & Campbell, 2007; Burke, 2006; Furnham et al.,

2012; Judge et al., 2009; McCartney & Campbell, 2006).

This idea that leaders need a balance between their dark side and bright side traits

creates a paradox for the concept of authentic leadership: the need to regulate and restrain a

leader’s natural inclinations, such as taking centre stage or being too determined and honest,

may go against the nature of that leader’s “true self” (Nyberg & Sveningsson, 2014).

Effective leaders must, therefore, reconcile the need to be true to themselves with the need to

be aware of how their behaviour will be perceived by their followers. In turn, how their

behaviour is perceived by followers is theorised to be based on the followers’ implicit

prototype of a good/bad leader within the context of social norms (Hollander, 1992; Martinko

et al., 2013; Phillips & Lord, 1981). One definition of destructive leadership that is based on

follower perceptions is abusive supervision.

Abusive supervision. Research on abusive supervision focused on sustained displays

of a supervisor’s hostility (non-physical), as determined by subordinates’ assessments, based

on their observations of a supervisor’s behaviour. Abusive supervision includes behaviours

such as yelling, ridicule, rudeness, breaking promises, invading privacy, lying, and using

silent treatment (Tepper, 2000). Supervisors perpetrate abusive behaviour for a purpose, for

example, to increase performance or reduce mistakes. However, it is not necessary for the

supervisor to intend to cause harm for their behaviour to be included in this category.

While a large volume of research into the dark side of leadership has been conducted

under the term “abusive supervision,” this research has been criticised as being fragmented

and poorly integrated, due to the use of different terminology and measures (Martinko et al.,

2013; Tepper, 2007). Little of the abusive supervision literature has focused on supervisor

traits and characteristics. Tepper (2007) hypothesised that supervisors low in agreeableness

36
(more argumentative, hostile, and conflictive) and high in neuroticism (experiencing greater

anger, frustration, and impulsiveness) are likely to be abusive. However, in the abusive

supervision literature to date there is little research specifically on the personality traits of

supervisors that subordinates perceive as abusive, with the exception of supervisors with

psychopathic traits, who are purported to be particularly abusive (Boddy, 2011a). Studies of

antecedents of abusive supervision found supervisors who are inclined to abuse subordinates

will target their hostility on those who appear to be weak, vulnerable, and unwilling or unable

to defend themselves (Martinko et al., 2013; Tepper, 2007; Tepper et al., 2006). Supervisors

who had themselves experienced unfavourable interpersonal treatment, and who embraced

dominance and control as legitimate forms of leadership, were also more abusive towards

subordinates (Aryee, Chen, Sun, & Debrah, 2007). Abusive supervision was later included

by Einarsen et al. (2007) as one type of destructive leadership.

Destructive leadership. Destructive leadership is demonstrated by a pattern of

behaviour by a leader that sabotages or undermines the achievement of subordinates and/or

organisations, focusing purely on self-interest. In an attempt to integrate the dark side of

leadership literature, Einarsen et al. (2007) developed a model, shown in Figure 5, to describe

the difference between constructive and destructive leadership, based on whether the leader

behaviour benefited individuals and the organisation or caused them harm. The model

focussed on the outcomes of systematic, repeated leadership behaviours, rather than a

leader’s intent. Their model had two basic dimensions: subordinate-oriented and

organisation-oriented behaviours. Anti-subordinate behaviours, such as harassment and

mistreatment, violate the organisation’s interests by sabotaging the motivation and well-being

of subordinates. By contrast, pro-subordinate behaviours foster subordinate motivation and

well-being. The second dimension included anti-organisation behaviours, such as stealing

from the organisation, and pro-organisation behaviours, such as working towards the

37
attainment of organisation goals. Of specific interest to this thesis are the two dimensions

concerned with anti-subordinate behaviour: Derailed Leadership dimension (anti-subordinate

and anti-organisation), such as manipulating subordinates while simultaneously defrauding

the organisation; and the Tyrannical Leadership dimension (anti-subordinate and pro-

organisation), such as humiliating or belittling a subordinate to get a task completed. This

model incorporated leadership derailment and abusive supervision theory into a more unified

theory. However, it failed to take into account the mutual influence of leaders and followers,

and the role of environment in allowing destructive leadership to occur, such as anti-

subordinate and anti-organisation behaviour.

Pro-subordinate
behaviour

Supportive-Disloyal Constructive
Anti-organisation Leadership Leadership Pro-organisation
behaviour behaviour

Derailed Tyrannical
Leadership Leadership

Anti-subordinate
behaviour

Figure 5. Conceptual model of destructive and constructive leadership (Einarsen et al.,

2007).

As discussed in Chapter 1, Padilla et al. (2007) went further than previous leader-

centric models by emphasising that destructive leaders do not act in isolation; they are

influenced by followers and their environment. With this broader focus, Padilla et al.

identified five elements of destructive leadership (Table 3). Similar to Einarsen et al. (2007),

38
they highlighted the characteristics of destructive leaders, their focus on self-interest

(personalised power), and that destructive leaders cause harm to individuals and to

organisations. While both Einarsen and Padilla et al.’s conceptualisations of destructive

leadership have been extensively referenced and researched, an integrated theory of

destructive leadership was yet to be developed (e.g., a unified theoretical framework with a

definition that clarified the boundaries of the construct and distinguished it from related

phenomenon).

Table 3

Five features of destructive leadership (Padilla et al., 2007)

1. Destructive leadership is seldom absolutely or entirely destructive: there are both

good and bad results in most leadership situations.

2. The process of destructive leadership involves dominance, coercion, and

manipulation rather than influence, persuasion, and commitment.

3. The process of destructive leadership has a selfish orientation; it is focused more

on the leader's needs than the needs of the larger social group.

4. The effects of destructive leadership are outcomes that compromise the quality of

life for constituents and detract from the organisation's main purposes.

5. Destructive organisational outcomes are not exclusively the result of destructive

leaders, but are also products of susceptible followers and conducive environments.

Drawing on existing conceptual models of destructive leadership, including those by

Einarsen et al. (2007), Tepper (2000), Lipman-Blumen (2005), and Padilla et al. (2007),

Krasikova et al. (2013) proposed a framework to facilitate further development of a unified

39
theoretical model of destructive leadership. They argued that destructive leadership was a

harmful behaviour imbedded in the process of leading, defining destructive leadership as:

Volitional behaviour by a leader that can harm or intends to harm a leader’s

organisation and/or followers by (a) encouraging followers to pursue goals that

contravene the legitimate interests of the organisation, and/or (b) employing a

leadership style that involves the use of harmful methods of influence with followers,

regardless of justifications for such behaviour. (p. 1310).

By defining destructive leadership as volitional behaviour, Krasikova et al. differentiated

destructive leadership from ineffective or incompetent leadership. Their definition agrees

with Kellerman (2004a), that intent may be present but is not required for a leader’s

behaviour to be destructive. Similar to Einarsen et al. and Padilla et al., they proposed

personal and environmental factors contributed to the enactment of destructive leadership

towards followers and the organisation (e.g., predisposing personality traits, frustration of

achievement need through goal blockage, high leader discretion), and negative or positive

reinforcement mechanisms for deviant behaviour (e.g., the extent to which the organisation

fails to discover or respond with countervailing actions). However, their definition does not

include the interaction or mutual influence of followers on leaders in enabling destructive

leadership.

In their meta-analysis, Schyns and Schilling (2013) attempted to address the problem

of different conceptualisations of destructive leadership by providing an alternative definition

that also excluded ineffective or incompetent leadership:

A process in which over a longer period of time the activities, experiences and/or

relationships of an individual or the members of a group are repeatedly influenced by

their supervisor in a way that is perceived as hostile and/or obstructive (p. 141).

40
Drawing on the work of Thoroughgood et al. (2012), they argued the need to differentiate

between destructive leadership, when leader behaviours are focused towards followers, and

the term destructive leader behaviour, which includes all forms of negative and harmful

behaviour by a leader, which may not be related to their leadership function. Thoroughgood

et al. defined destructive leader behaviour as voluntary acts (verbal or physical, active or

passive) committed by a leader that most people would perceive as harmful towards

followers and/or the organisation.

This researcher is concerned with leader behaviours that are specifically focussed

towards subordinate followers, and so will adopt the term “destructive leadership,”

“destructive leadership behaviours,” or “toxic leadership behaviours” in recognition of the

differentiation made by Schyns and Schilling discussed above. Based on the range of

destructive leadership definitions in the literature and outlined in Table 1, for the purposes of

this thesis destructive leadership is defined by this researcher as:

Systematic, volitional behaviour by a leader over a period of time (verbal and non-

verbal, active or passive), that can inflict or intends to inflict serious and enduring

harm on subordinates by the use of methods of influence that are perceived as hostile

or obstructive by subordinates, and that result in the undermining of the effectiveness,

motivation, and/or well-being of their subordinates, regardless of justifications for

such behaviour.

A number of dark side leadership researchers have now included their research under

the stream of destructive leadership, including: toxic leadership (Lipman-Blumen, 2005;

Pelletier, 2010); narcissistic leadership, Machiavellian leadership, and psychopathic

leadership (Babiak & Hare, 2007; Boddy, 2015; Campbell et al., 2011; Clarke, 2005;

Maccoby, 2004, 2007). These specific types of destructive leadership are discussed in the

following sections.

41
Toxic leadership. Toxic leadership is said to be determined by evaluating the

consequences of a leader’s behaviour, based on followers’ perceptions and attributions

(Lipman-Blumen, 2005; Padilla et al., 2007). Lipman-Blumen (2005) considered leaders

toxic when they inflicted serious and enduring harm on their constituents by using influence

tactics that are extremely hard and/or malicious. That is, the leader is considered toxic when

their actions result in long lasting physical, emotional, or psychological harm to the follower.

Again acknowledging situational influences, Lipman-Blumen noted that a leader may

be toxic in some situations, but effective in others. In Einarsen et al.’s (2007) model, toxic

leadership falls into the tyrannical leadership category, when leaders may harm subordinates

in order to achieve results, but be well regarded by the organisation. Based on Lipman-

Blumen’s definition of toxic leadership, Pelletier (2010) conducted an exploratory study with

200 US employees to identify a typology of toxic leader behavioural characteristics. These

were categorised under eight behavioural dimensions: (a) attacking followers’ self-esteem

(e.g., ridiculing and mocking); (b) divisiveness (e.g., pitting one employee against another);

(c) social exclusion (e.g., excluding individuals from social functions); (d) promoting

inequity (e.g., exhibiting favouritism); (e) abusiveness (e.g., tantrums, yelling); (f)

threatening followers’ security (e.g., using physical acts of aggression); (g) lack of integrity

(e.g., being deceptive); and (h) laissez-faire leadership style (e.g., failing to respond when

employees voiced concerns). These behavioural dimensions have been validated by other

research studies in the destructive leadership and abusive supervision streams (e.g.; Martinko

et al., 2013; Skogstad et al., 2007; Tepper, 2007).

The dark triad: narcissism, Machiavellianism and psychopathy. One of the most

serious categories of destructive leadership is the “dark triad”: narcissism, Machiavellianism,

and subclinical psychopathy (Boddy, 2015; Furnham, Richards & Paulhus, 2013; Paulhus &

Williams, 2002). In Einarsen et al.’s (2007) model these types of destructive leadership fall

42
into the category of derailed leadership, as they may display anti-subordinate behaviours,

such as bullying, humiliation, manipulation, deception or harassment, while simultaneously

performing anti-organisational behaviours, such as fraud and corruption.

Individuals with these dark triad traits may be promoted into leadership roles due to

their ability to present a positive impression. They may even lie about their qualifications

and achievements to enhance their attractiveness as a candidate (Boddy et al., 2015). They

are also likely to be promoted if they have the capability to suppress the damaging behaviours

associated with these syndromes. Once in positions of authority they may be considered

successful if these traits are consistent with the demands of their management/executive role,

as long as they are able to control their impulsivity and anti-social tendencies (O'Boyle &

Forsyth, 2012). However, some support has been found for the role of authority in

decreasing performance and increasing counterproductive work behaviour in those with dark

triad traits (O'Boyle & Forsyth, 2012). Theorists maintained that these types of leaders are

difficult to manage and are not amenable to changing their behaviour, as their primary

motivations are personal power and self-interest (Babiak & Hare, 2007; Boddy, 2015; Clarke,

2005; House & Howell, 1992).

The literature on narcissism examines the bright side of leadership and authority

(charisma and vision), the maladaptive aspects of narcissism (grandiose exhibitionism), and

the dark side of this characteristic (exploitativeness and entitlement; Ackerman et al., 2011;

Campbell et al., 2011; Grijalva & Newman, 2015; House & Howell, 1992; McFarlin &

Sweeney, 2010; Rosenthal & Pittinsky, 2006). Rosenthal and Pittinsky (2006), after

reviewing and critiquing the existing literature, provided the following definition:

“Narcissistic leadership occurs when leaders' actions are principally motivated by their own

egomaniacal needs and beliefs, superseding the needs and interests of the constituents and

institutions they lead.” (p. 629). Narcissistic individuals typically have an inflated view of

43
themselves and their abilities, feel entitled to excessive rewards and recognition, and seek out

roles that provide them with power and influence. Extreme levels of narcissistic self-

importance and arrogance can lead to demonstrations of intense desire to compete, high levels

of distrust (constantly on the lookout for enemies), sensitivity to criticism that threatens self-

image, lack of empathy, and a high level of risk taking, without listening to words of caution

or advice from others. Narcissistic leaders can be abrasive and aggressive with anyone who

seeks to resist or oppose them. While they seek the admiration and adulation of others, they

are at risk of isolating themselves at the very moment of their success, because narcissistic

relationships contain low levels of empathy and emotional intimacy. Narcissistic leaders

typically have numerous shallow relationships that can range from exciting and engaging to

manipulative and exploitative (Campbell et al., 2011; Maccoby, 2004, 2007; Rosenthal &

Pittinsky, 2006).

Machiavellian leaders have a propensity to distrust others, engage in amoral

manipulation, seek to control others, and seek status for themselves (e.g., personalised

power). The concept of Machiavellianism emerged from Richard Christie’s selection of

statements from Nicolo Machiavelli’s original books, a political advisor to the Medici family

in the 1500s (Christie & Geis, 1970). Research indicates while Machiavellian leaders may be

charismatic, they are often unsupportive and inconsiderate of followers. They will engage in

political influencing tactics such as strategic self-disclosure, ingratiation, and intimidation

(Dahling, Whitaker, & Levy, 2009). Machiavellian leaders tend to be self-promoters, who

behave in a cold, manipulative fashion for their own purposes (Paulhus & Williams, 2002).

The conceptual distinction between Machiavellian leaders and organisational psychopaths

has been debated (Boddy, 2015; Furnham et al., 2013). It has been argued that the common

features (e.g., love of money, power and prestige, and a willingness to behave unethically)

44
suggest that there is significant empirical overlap, and theoretically they may even be the

same construct (McHoskey, Worzel & Szyarto, 1998).

The concept of organisational psychopaths in the workplace (also referred to in the

literature as corporate psychopaths or organisational sociopaths) has been made popular by

Babiak and Hare (2007), in their book Snakes in Suits, and by Clarke (2005) in his book

Working with Monsters. These authors provided definitions, examples, and case studies

where the most negative aspects of the narcissistic and Machiavellian attributes have been

demonstrated by organisational leaders. It has been estimated approximately 3-4% of

individuals in higher level positions in organisations exhibit subclinical psychopathy traits

(Babiak, Neumann, & Hare, 2010; Boddy, 2015). While there are competing

conceptualizations of workplace psychopathy, recently it has been conceptualised to consist

of affective and interpersonal traits of psychopathy (e.g., guiltlessness, lack of empathy,

grandiosity, egocentricity, and superficial charm), impulsivity, irresponsibility, and a lack of

behavioural controls (Smith & Lilienfeld, 2013).

Characteristics outlined for subclinical psychopathic types of leaders included

(Babiak & Hare, 2007; Clarke, 2005; Boddy, Miles, Sanyal, & Hartog, 2015):

 Manipulative organisational behaviour

 Unethical behaviour

 Intolerant/easily bored

 Unpredictable behaviours/shallow emotions

 Parasitic behaviour

 Failure to take responsibility for behaviour

 Intimidating/bullying, creating a culture of fear

 Seek increased personal power and control

45
 Claim credit for work they have not done

 Blame others for things that go wrong

 Create conflict between organisation members

 Deceitful and devious

 Charming/superficial

When Paulhus and Williams (2002) examined the characteristics of the dark triad of

personality, narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy, they found the dark triad

revealed one commonality in the Big Five personality traits: a low level of agreeableness.

However, only psychopaths reported minimal anxiety, making them the most treacherous of

the dark triad, even within the normal range of personality found in their sample. Board and

Fritzon (2005) found traits associated with the emotional components of the psychopathic

personality disorder, including manipulative, narcissistic, and compulsive characteristics, in a

sample of 39 UK senior managers. Researchers suggest leaders displaying psychopathic

characteristics lead to a disengaged, disheartened and disillusioned workforce; employees

with higher levels of fear, distress, despair, insecurity, frustration, anger, and lower levels of

well-being (Boddy, 2015). Employees who are perceived by an organisational psychopath as

a threat to the progression of the leader’s career are likely to be targeted, threatened,

undermined, and eventually removed from the organisation (Boddy et al., 2015).

Despite the widely held view that psychopathy is invariably maladaptive, some

researchers have argued that mild expressions of some of its characteristics can be adaptive in

certain settings, such as entrepreneurial activities and in negotiating business dealings. For

example, it has been suggested that psychopathic traits such as fearlessness, grandiosity, and

charm may predispose an individual to maladaptive and/or adaptive behaviours, depending

on situational moderating variables (Smith & Lilienfeld, 2013). However, due to the negative

behaviours of these leaders, their lack of insight, and the fear they instill in those around

46
them, this is an extremely difficult area to research. Some theorists recommend the only way

to manage this type of leader is to remove them from the organisation “like cutting out a

cancer” (Babiak & Hare, 2007; Clarke, 2005).

Measures of Dark Side Leadership Traits and Behaviours

One of the commonly recommended strategies organisations can use to protect

themselves is to screen out destructive leaders during selection, promotion, and/or succession

management. Assessing candidates via multiple measures may guard against a poor

selection. As destructive leaders are likely to be charming and self-enhancing at interviews, a

combination of interviews, self-report inventories (with guards against faking), simulations,

and/or 360 degree feedback mechanisms are likely to be more effective in identifying

potentially harmful characteristics (Furnham, 2007; Judge & LePine, 2007; McFarlin &

Sweeney, 2010; Schmit et al., 2000; Thoroughgood, Padilla et al., 2012; Trickey & Hyde,

2009). However, caution has been advised when screening for maladaptive personality traits

using personality assessment for selection or development in a workplace environment

(Christiansen, Quirk, Robie, & Oswald, 2014; Guenole, 2014).

Assessment using clinical personality assessments for purposes other than that for

which they were designed is not recommended as it is likely to lead to ethical and legal

implications. While conducting selection screening using personality profiling is well

accepted practice in Australia, caution is called for in its application. For example, if the

assessment result implies psychological disability (e.g., personality disorder), it may be

deemed a medical examination by the courts in some countries and in such a case it is only

considered acceptable to conduct such profiling after a conditional job offer (not as part of

the screening process). Privacy considerations for applicants must also be taken into account,

particularly if a poor personality profile results, due to the possibility that such personality

profiles have the potential to offend recipients (Christiansen et al., 2014). While profiles

47
based on DSM-IV personality disorders may be appropriate as research tools for investigating

destructive leadership in experimental settings, when using assessment to predict specific

dysfunctional work behaviours, it may be safer for practitioners to refrain from using scales

or composites named after an existing clinical diagnosis, such as those outlined in the DSM-

IV (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). Researchers and practitioners may be better

advised to select inventories with item content that refers directly to behaviour at work

(Christiansen et al., 2014). There are a limited number of inventories designed to specifically

measure dark side traits and behaviours in the workplace, and a significant proportion of

them are based on the DSM-IV.

Self-report leadership profiles. A review of the literature identified five self-report

measures of the characteristics of the dark side of leadership (leadership derailers and

destructive leadership), designed to be used within a normal population:

 The Global Personality Inventory (GPI; Schmit et al., 2000) is a measure of

leadership style preferences and personality characteristics, and includes a composite

scale termed “leadership debilitators.” This composite scale measures five

characteristics that may predispose leaders to achieve objectives in the short term, but

over time may cause them to lose the support of others: ego-centred, intimidating,

manipulative, micromanaging, and passive-aggressive. This inventory was developed

using a multi-cultural sample of the normal population and is not linked to

characteristics of clinical personality disorders (this inventory was employed as a

measure in Study 1).

 The Hogan Development Survey (HDS; Hogan & Hogan, 2001) measures the dark

side of leadership, based on characteristics of clinical personality disorders outlined in

the DSM-IV: excitable, skeptical, cautious, reserved, leisurely, bold, mischievous,

colourful, imaginative, diligent, and dutiful.

48
 Overcoming the Dark Side of Leadership – Inventory of Traits (McIntosh & Rima,

2007) is a self-report inventory to assist individuals to identify whether they exhibit

any compulsive, narcisstic, paranoid, codependent, or passive-aggressive traits.

 The Dark Triad Dirty Dozen consists of a 12-item measure of the triad of narcissism,

Machiavellianism and psychopathy (Jonason & Webster, 2010).

 Finally, Hay Group have recently developed the Talent Q Dimensions personality

profile which includes leadership derailers, also based on characteristics of clinical

personality disorders outlined in the DSM-IV: hyper-sensitivity, eccentricity,

exhibitionism, over-dependence, isolation, iconoclasm, over-confidence, and micro-

management (TalentQ, 2013).

By comparison, most measures of psychopathy have been developed and tested in

clinical populations (e.g., Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory [MMPQ],

Psychopathic Deviate [Pd] scale; the California Psychological Inventory [CPI], Socialization

[So] scale; Self-Report Psychopathy Scale – Revised [SRP-R]). These clinical measures have

often been developed on delinquent populations and appear to assess antisocial and criminal

behaviours, rather than the core personality features of psychopathy (Lilienfield & Andrews,

1996). These tools have limited utility for the study of organisational psychopaths, given that

it can be assumed most managers and executives can function successfully, and would not

have achieved their positions and status if they had demonstrated antisocial behaviours,

specifically towards their managers and peers. The Psychopathic Personality Inventory (PPI)

assesses major personality traits of psychopathy and has been tested in a noncriminal

population, although the undergraduate samples used raises the question of generalisability to

management populations (Lilienfield & Andrews, 1996). The eight PPI subscales include

Machiavellian egocentricity, social potency, cold-heartedness, carefree non-planfulness,

fearlessness, blame externalisations, impulsive nonconformity, and stress immunity.

49
This thesis focuses on the prevalence of destructive leadership traits within a normal

organisational population. Yet most dark side inventories continue to link their scales to the

DSM-IV criteria for personality disorders. This presents a potential risk: utilising clinically-

based measures to identify destructive leaders, even when designed for normal populations,

may allow the perpetrators to claim a lack of responsibility for their behaviours, explaining

their behaviour away as characteristics of an innate personality disorder (Babiak & Hare,

2007; Clarke, 2005). In addition, as discussed above, if aspects of a personality disorder are

implied by an assessment carried out as part of a recruitment and selection process, the

assessment may be deemed a medical examination by the courts, preventing it being used as

part of the applicant screening process prior to selection (Christiansen et al., 2014).

Therefore, the use of a valid assessment, which is developed using a normal population and is

not based on DSM-IV/DSM-V criteria, may be a more appropriate measure.

Third-party inventories. There are a number of third-party inventories designed for

individuals to rate their observation of harmful behaviours by their manager. Tepper

developed 15 abusive supervision items to be used to survey followers’ observations of their

supervisor’s behaviour (Tepper, 2000). Babiak and Hare (2007), Clarke (2005), and Lubit

(2004) provided a set of criteria to screen candidates or identify existing destructive leaders in

organisations. Crowley and Elster (2009) provided a checklist of 20 behaviours for

followers’ reference to detect managerial behaviours that are likely to frustrate them.

Table 4 compares four leader self-report inventories (e.g., GPI, HDS, McIntosh &

Rima, and Dimensions) with two third-party typologies designed to be completed by

followers/victims, checking the dark side traits they purport to identify or measure, and

showing the disparity between measures, in part due to the origins of the inventories.

50
Table 4

Comparison of measurement tools to identify dark side characteristics (self-report and third-

party inventories)

Self-Report Third-Party

Schmit et Hogan TalentQ McIntosh Jonason Tepper Lubit Babiak


al. (GPI) (HDS) (Dimensions) & Rima & (Abusive &
Webster Supervision Hare;
Scale) Clarke
Subclinical  
Psychopathy
Ego Centred;       
Narcissistic
Intimidating     

Machiavellian;    
Manipulative
Compulsive;      
Micro-managing
Passive-Aggressive     

Unethical     

Risk Taking    

Excitement Seeking   

Emotional Control  

Paranoid    

Codependent;   
Dependent
Schizoid  

The Hogan, McIntosh and Rima, Talent Q, Babiak and Hare/Clarke, and Jonason and

Webster inventories have all been based on DSM-IV personality disorder criteria. While

third-party diagnostic inventories may be of assistance to followers in determining whether

51
they work for a toxic manager, for leader development multi-source feedback measures are

more commonly employed.

360 degree feedback. A 360 degree feedback mechanism that includes self-report,

manager, peer, and direct report observations could be an effective way to capture the

combination or pattern of effects of destructive leadership by assessing the disparity between

self and raters’ responses (Furnham, 2007; Gentry et al., 2007). Using a self-report measure

alone to diagnose dark side characteristics of leadership has been criticised, due to the

likelihood of target participants manipulating the tool, or providing self-inflated ratings

(Babiak & Hare, 2007; Clarke, 2005; Gentry, Hannum, Ekelund, & de Jong, 2007).

However, conducting 360 degree feedback assessments on destructive managers who exhibit

arrogant, intimidating, and manipulative behaviour may result in raters choosing not to

participate, or choosing not to be truthful in their feedback, for fear of reprisals.

There are only three 360 degree tools that purport to measure dark side constructs: (a)

Benchmarks® (Centre for Creative Leadership); (b) the Leadership Versatility Index® (LVI;

Kaiser et al., 2015; Kaplan & Kaiser, 2008); and (c) CLS360 (Redeker, de Vries, Rouckhout,

Vermeren & de Fruyt, 2014). Currently there is no 360 degree feedback tool with

comprehensive items that have been designed to cover the range of dark side leadership

behaviours and characteristics discussed in the literature: derailed leadership, abusive

supervision, and destructive leadership. Therefore, a 360 degree measure was not utilised in

this thesis research.

Measuring leadership traits and behaviours to identify potentially derailing

characteristics can be useful for selection purposes, and for raising leaders’ awareness of

potential derailers, but may have limited utility in assisting leaders to change their behaviour.

We turn now to a review of the theoretical mechanisms by which leaders may avoid

derailing.

52
Theoretical Mechanisms for Preventing Leader Derailment

Given the reported prevalence of destructive leadership and its negative impact on

followers, it is important for researchers and practitioners to understand whether ‘dark side’

leaders can inhibit or change their destructive behaviour.

The role of leader self-regulation. Impaired leader self-regulation has been

associated with enacting destructive leadership behaviours (Kaiser et al., 2015; Krasikova et

al., 2013). The literature consistently identifies impulsivity as one of the key characteristics

of destructive leader behaviour, often accompanied by an inability to control the high need

for power, achievement, money, and/or success (Babiak & Hare, 2007; Jones & Paulhus,

2011; Kellerman, 2004a; Paulhus & Williams, 2002; Vazire & Funder, 2006). The role of

intra-personal self-regulatory processes in controlling impulsivity has been identified as an

important characteristic in achieving personal goals (Bandura, 1991; Karoly, 1993). It is

argued that the capacity to engage in intrapersonal self-regulatory mechanisms (e.g., direct

purposeful actions from within to override, alter, or inhibit dysfunctional responses or

behaviours) enables better goal selection, goal co-ordination, and goal achievement (Bandura,

1991; Heatherton & Baumeister, 1996; Karoly, 1993). However, an emerging line of

research discussed how goals can also have negative consequences. Goal difficulty and the

extent to which rewards are contingent upon goal attainment contribute to destructive

leadership behaviours through their effect on levels of stress. For example, increased

negative affectivity can be associated with the anticipation of impending adversities or

anticipation of an inability to attract rewards due to failure to perform through goal

achievement (Bardes & Piccolo, 2010).

Krasikova et al. (2013) proposed that perceived goal blockage was a predictor of the

choice to engage in destructive leadership, especially in leaders with a high need for

individual achievement, such as those motivated by personalised power, or leaders with low

53
self-esteem who rely primarily on their individual achievements to boost their self-worth

(Crocker, 2002). Krasikova et al. argued that leaders with self-regulation impairment are

more likely to react to goal blockage by choosing to engage in destructive leadership.

Leaders with high negative affectivity have a negativity bias when interpreting events and are

more likely to perceive their goals as being blocked by the organisation or their followers.

Failure of self-regulatory processes may also result from a depletion of resources to deal with

work related stressors, that is, role overload, organisational constraints, or interpersonal

conflict (Byrne et al., 2014; Wang, Sinclair, & Deese, 2010).

Self-regulation (related terms include self-management, self-leadership, self-

influence, and self-control; Manz, 1986), as a mechanism for self-goal setting, self-

observation, self-reward, self-punishment, and self-control of behaviours, has been put

forward as an important variable when dealing with the frustration of goal achievement

(Karoly, 1993). Leaders may develop sensitivities due to a set of unconscious emotionally

charged beliefs and expectations, generalized from past experience, that serve to protect them

from repeating a painful experience. Such leaders are more likely to interpret environmental

cues negatively when fatigued or stressed, and to react intensely and compulsively as a form

of self-protection (Kaiser & Kaplan, 2006). Alternatively, resilient, emotionally stable

leaders are less likely to experience intense negative emotions to stressors. Therefore,

personal characteristics such as optimism, emotional control, and stress tolerance are likely to

predict better ability to engage in self-regulation processes when under stress (Fowlie &

Wood, 2009; Kaiser et al., 2015; King & Rothstein, 2010; Wang et al., 2010). Self-

regulatory mechanisms in human motivation and action were considered essential for self-

observation and self-monitoring (e.g., impression management) to ascertain the determinants

and effects of one’s behaviour. Self-regulatory mechanisms include monitoring self-talk,

making cognitive choices, delaying gratification of impulses, and tolerating frustration

54
(Bandura, 1991). Such mechanisms enabled a leader to demonstrate positive performance by

managing negative emotions and enhancing cognitions, emotions, behaviours, and moral

agency (Bandura, 1991).

Multiple definitions, operationalisations, and models of the term self-regulation have

arisen from a range of sources, developed across disciplines (e.g., including the domains of

personality, motivation/emotion, and social psychology; Karoly, 1993). Depending on the

originating discipline, a range of self-regulation processes have been hypothesised: volitional

processes, reinforcement processes, biological processes, cultural processes, and social-

cognitive processes (Heatherton & Baumeister, 1996). In general, self-regulation refers to

the process by which individuals initiate, adjust, terminate, or otherwise alter actions to

promote attainment of personal goals (Heatherton & Baumeister, 1996). Karoly offered the

following definition as a conceptual roadmap for future study:

Self-regulation refers to those processes, internal and/or transactional, that enable an

individual to guide his/her goal-directed activities over time and across changing

circumstances (contexts). Regulation implies modulation of thought, affect,

behaviour, or attention via deliberate or automated use of specific mechanisms and

supportive metaskills. (p. 25).

Multiple explanations for how self-regulation is enacted have been proposed by

researchers, incorporating personal characteristics (e.g., self-efficacy, self-esteem, and self-

awareness; Karoly, 1993); skills (e.g., political skill; Whitman, Halbesleben, & Shanine,

2013); and behaviours (e.g., self-talk, self-observation, self-monitoring, self-recording;

Karoly, 1993; Rogelberg et al., 2013). Individual differences in goal-directed behaviour may

depend on an individual’s regulatory focus: self-regulation with a promotion focus to realise

positive outcomes (e.g., accomplishment of aspirations) versus self-regulation with a

prevention focus in order to avoid negative outcomes (e.g., threat to well-being or loss of

55
reputation; Higgins, 1997; van Knippenberg, 2012). A related theory, self-discrepancy

theory, proposed that individuals take action to avoid negative emotions or to experience

positive emotions, depending on the discrepancy between anticipated outcomes and actual

realization of aspirations (Higgins, 1987, 1997, 1999).

Social cognitive theory of self-regulation proposed that through actions such as

thought self-regulation and the exercise of forethought, a leader can develop the agency and

motivation to think and behave in ways that actively create positive future experiences

(Bandura, 1991, 2001). This theory of self-regulation included the ability to form a moral

judgment of the rightness or wrongness of conduct as evaluated against personal standards

and situational circumstances. For example, through self-reflection on experiences, a leader

can gain knowledge on how his or her thoughts and corresponding behaviours lead to positive

or negative experiences.

In addition to intrapersonal self-regulation mechanisms, self-regulation has also been

defined as a social or interpersonal process. A leader uses dual processes of adaptive self-

regulation to plan their behaviour, choosing behaviour based on both their internal standards

and attitudes, as well as external social structures and norms (Tsui & Ashford, 1994; Wang et

al., 2010). Given these propositions, it is proposed in this research thesis that the ability to

engage self-regulatory processes, through activating self-regulatory traits, is likely to mediate

the relationship between a strong need for power and personal goal achievement and the

predisposition to use constructive or destructive leadership behaviours when facing

frustration or perceived failure. The difference between effective leaders, who use their dark

side tendencies as strengths in specific situations, compared to derailed leaders, may partially

reside in the leaders’ ability to develop self-insight (e.g., awareness of their own conscious

and unconscious motivations), which can set in motion a process of corrective change.

Effectiveness is also likely to reside in a leader’s ability to regulate their impulsive behaviour

56
and need for personal power, even in competitive or stressful situations, so that a leader’s

strengths are utilised only in appropriate contexts and goals are achieved in a way that does

not cause harm (Goleman, 2004; Hannah, Woolfolk, & Lord, 2009; Judge et al., 2009;

McClelland, 1970, 1975; Paulhus & Williams, 2002; Spreier et al., 2006).

Despite the significant body of self-regulation theory, little is understood of the role of

self-regulation in inhibiting the predisposition to destructive leadership behaviours.

O’Connor, Mumford, Clifton & Gessner (1995) discussed the role of self-regulation in

preventing destructive behaviours in a sample of historical charismatic leaders. O’Connor et

al. defined self-regulation as “the leader’s ability to monitor thoughts and actions in the

presence of others; the degree to which the leader was honest about his or her negative

attributes.” (p. 539). O’Connor et al. found weak support that self-regulation acts as an

inhibitory influence upon charismatic leaders’ willingness to use others for personal gain, or

their willingness to engage in destructive acts. They hypothesised that the ability to self-

regulate appeared to be a skill necessary to become a high-level leader and was of little use in

differentiating between those motivated by personalised power and those motivated by

socialised power. However, there are additional self-regulatory characteristics and

behaviours, beyond the ability to control moods and impulses (e.g., emotional self-control),

that researchers have suggested can develop positive forms of leadership. These include low

negative affectivity; self-awareness (e.g., of motivations, values, goals, strengths and

limitations, and their effects on others); the ability to tolerate and endure typically stressful

situations without undue physical or emotional reaction; self-confidence; optimism; openness

to change; and adaptability (Avolio & Gardner, 2005; Bandura, 1991; Fowlie & Wood, 2009;

Goleman, 2004; Hannah & Avolio, 2010; Karoly, 1993; Krasikova et al., 2013; Kupers &

Weibler, 2005; Ruderman, Hannum, Leslie, & Steed, 2001; Schmit et al., 2000). Leaders

57
with these personal characteristics are more likely to be able to self-regulate their behaviour

when faced with stressful, adverse, uncertain, or unpredictable situations.

Further investigation is required to confirm the personality traits that enable or hinder

effective intrapersonal self-regulation in relation to engagement in destructive leadership

behaviours. Once identified, leadership development interventions can focus on building

self-awareness of attributes that contribute to self-regulation competency, so that leaders have

the agency to choose constructive leadership behaviours, even when fatigued or frustrated.

Individual leader interventions – can leaders change? There are a range of

interventions proposed in the leadership research to develop individual leaders by increasing

self-awareness and assisting them to develop self-regulatory processes to address the negative

aspects of their leadership (Harms, Spain, & Hannah, 2011; Nelson & Hogan, 2009).

Behavioural coaching to increase self-awareness of the nature and triggers of harmful

behaviours may enable toxic leaders to change their behaviour (Moran, 2011; Pelletier, 2010;

Trickey & Hyde, 2009). For example, self-awareness may be increased through coaching

based on leadership profiling of personality characteristics that informs leadership style and

motivation, or through 360 degree feedback on leadership behaviours (Gentry et al., 2007;

Hartley & Hinksman, 2003; Nelson & Hogan, 2009). Programs introducing leaders to

Acceptance Commitment Training (e.g., strategies for dealing with aversive feelings and

moods, reframing unhelpful thoughts, clarifying values, and thoughtful planning of actions)

and mindfulness techniques may assist individuals to improve their resiliency in order to

respond effectively to stressful situations. Such strategies assist the leader to avoid reacting

or being caught up in emotional and cognitive obstacles that distract attention away from

their goals (Atkins, 2008; Moran, 2011). However, practices to develop leaders, such as

managerial training, multi-source feedback, executive coaching, formal or informal

mentoring, networking and action learning, have received little hard evaluation evidence of

58
their effectiveness (Avolio et al., 2009; Day, 2000; Harms et al., 2011; Moran, 2011;

Nieminen, Smerek, Kotrba, & Denison, 2013; Powell & Yalcin, 2010).

There is little empirical research to date into the development of leaders with dark

side characteristics to inform the best approach. Theorists recommend coaches be sensitive

to the presence of traits that may cause derailment at work or derailment of the development

process itself (Harms et al., 2011; Nelson & Hogan, 2009). Even with professional

assistance, leaders’ efforts at self-management may not be successful (Karoly, 1993). The

consensus appears to be that leaders with high levels of subclinical traits are less likely to

participate in or learn from development activities (Harms et al., 2011). Therefore, while

leadership development interventions can be useful in addressing leadership derailers, to

assist leaders to increase their self-awareness, self-regulation, their repertoire of behaviours,

and where and when to use their strengths (Sandler, 2012), it has been argued that these

interventions have limited success when leaders exhibit subclinical psychopathy (Babiak &

Hare, 2007; Boddy, 2015). In fact, leaders with narcissistic and Machiavellian characteristics

are likely to utilise the skills and techniques obtained in leadership development and

executive coaching sessions to manipulate others even more effectively to achieve their own

ends (Babiak & Hare, 2007; Campbell et al., 2011; Clarke, 2005; Nelson & Hogan, 2009).

Organisational Responses to Destructive Leadership

The literature suggests that when destructive leaders are identified or reported, and it

is clear that they are unable or unwilling to change their behaviour, there is often a reluctance

to resist or remove them; for example, if the harmful behaviour is justified in the achievement

of organisationally sanctioned goals or it occurs in order to meet some greater purpose

(Krasikova et al., 2013; Rate & Sternberg, 2007). For an organisation to respond to

destructive leadership, it must be able to recognise when it is occurring and be motivated to

take action once it is discovered (Krasikova et al., 2013). Using potentially destructive

59
tactics when in a position of authority (e.g., high levels of assertiveness, self-promotion, and

politicking) leaders can initially enhance their power and give the perception of high levels of

performance (O'Boyle & Forsyth, 2012). However, as discussed previously, over time these

behaviours may become destructive to the leader themselves, as well as to others, as their

behaviour eventually sabotages relationships and their own career, for example, by their

being perceived as intimidating and manipulative (Carson et al., 2012; Clarke, 2005; Ferris et

al., 2007; House & Howell, 1992; Padilla et al., 2007; Tepper, 2007; Trickey & Hyde, 2009).

Leaders who maintain tight control, and who have achieved significant power and managerial

discretion, are often associated with followers who are not empowered to act or to question

authority. Typically, organisational environments with this sort of culture are devoid of

meaningful checks and balances and, therefore, fail to provide oversight and scrutiny of their

leaders (Mulvey & Padilla, 2010).

Despite the availability of assessment tools to screen out managerial applicants with

destructive leadership styles, managers who are known to be “toxic” may be promoted or

retained by organisations because of the perceived high costs associated with their removal.

For example, such leaders may have powerful allies, hold key relationships, bring in

significant revenue, and/or are likely to become litigious if challenged (Boddy, 2015; Clarke,

2005; Kellerman, 2004; Judge & LePine, 2007; Lipman-Blumen, 2005; Padilla et al., 2007).

Individuals who express concerns about leaders publicly may be marginalized, made to feel

irrelevant, or even fired (Perlow & Williams, 2003). As a result, they may choose to suffer in

silence, with harmful consequences to themselves, and substantial costs to the organisation.

As has been discussed, failure to take action to prevent and/or address destructive leadership

leads to negative consequences for the leaders themselves, their followers, peers, teams and

the organisation. Due to the complexity of the dynamic relationship between leaders and

followers, and the multiple factors required to allow destructive leadership to flourish,

60
organisations struggle to respond effectively to destructive leadership, and there is little

evidence-based practice to guide them.

While there is limited research on how organisations should respond to the

predicament of destructive leadership, there is considerable stress management intervention

research that may inform approaches to dealing with destructive leadership as a significant

source of occupational stress. The stress management intervention literature describes three

levels of interventions: primary prevention interventions that attempt to eliminate the sources

of stress in the organisational environment (e.g., codes of conduct, governance frameworks,

recruitment and selection screening, targeted leadership development activities, and

performance management processes); secondary prevention interventions that focus on

improving individuals’ stress responses and coping (e.g., manager and employee training

programs to increase resilience, coping skills, and well-being); and tertiary prevention

interventions that attempt to repair the damage caused by the stressors and assist in the

recovery process (e.g., employee assistance programs, counselling services, conflict

resolution and mediation processes; Berridge, Cooper, & Highley, 1992; Dewe, 1994;

Hargrove, Quick, Nelson, & Quick, 2011; Probst, 2013).

While the most effective organisational response to the phenomenon of destructive

leadership may be to employ all three levels of stress prevention intervention, the literature

suggests that primary prevention is the most effective in addressing the root cause of this

particular stressor, with removal of a destructive leader enhancing follower well-being

(Probst, 2013). Potential liability for failure to provide a safe workplace, the cost of reduced

productivity, absenteeism, disengaged presenteeism, and unwanted turnover provide

compelling reasons for the use of primary prevention tactics. Primary prevention tactics may

include disciplinary action or termination of destructive leaders upon evidence of frequent

and ongoing demonstration of destructive leadership behaviours (Lipman-Blumen, 2005;

61
Safe Work Australia, 2013, 2014). In circumstances where the leader is unable to change

their destructive behaviours, or the organisation chooses not to act to remove the leader,

conducting secondary prevention interventions, such as providing followers with the

knowledge and tools to better identify and cope with destructive leadership, may be a useful

stress prevention strategy.

Challenges in Researching Destructive Leadership Traits and Behaviours

Obtaining access to destructive leaders for research purposes is difficult, making the

research of destructive leadership traits and behaviours challenging. Most leaders do not self-

identify as destructive, and organisations are generally unwilling to admit that they employ

leaders who display destructive behaviours. If access is obtained, ego-centred and

Machiavellian leaders are likely to try to manipulate the research processes in order to present

themselves in the best possible light, especially if the results of the study are going to be

communicated to their own manager or executive.

As a result, archival data that had been collected for other reasons, such as organisational

recruitment or manager development, is often employed (e.g., Benson & Campbell, 2007;

Gaddis & Foster, 2015; Hogan & Hogan, 2001; Trickey & Hyde, 2009). This approach has

limitations because the data collection instrument has not been designed for research purposes

and, as a result, much demographic or contextual information is not available. In addition, it

provides cross-sectional data, preventing any analysis of causal relationships, which is

necessary to effectively establish antecedents and consequences of destructive leadership.

However, when data are collected from participants across organisations and industries, there

can be greater generalisability of findings, compared to data from one organisation/sector, or

from student samples, such as MBA programs (e.g., Carson et al., 2012; Dahling et al., 2009;

Fowlie & Wood, 2009; Najur et al., 2004).

62
An alternative approach is to recruit followers of destructive leaders to describe the

behaviours they experience that cause them harm (Martinko et al., 2013; Pelletier, 2010). In

this instance, it is also difficult to get access to data within organisations because employees

are often too fearful to participate and/or to be honest about their experiences. As a result,

convenience or snowball sampling is often employed to recruit participants external to any

specific organisation, such as recruitment through self-selection if participants feel they meet

the criteria of working for a toxic leader (e.g., Blase & Blase, 2002; Pelletier, 2010). Data

collection is often obtained via cross-sectional surveys (Benson & Campbell, 2007; Hogan &

Hogan, 2001; Pelletier, 2010). Few studies interview leaders directly (e.g., Board & Fritzon,

2005; Leslie & Velsor, 1996). Some studies conduct research in relation to managers who have

left the organisation, but this approach is rare and has the potential for retrospective bias

(Carson et al., 2012; Leslie & Velsor, 1996).

The desire for access to data that provides generalisable results must be balanced against

the call for longitudinal data that investigates antecedents, causes, and consequences of

destructive leadership over time. Additionally, determining from participant responses if a

leader’s behaviour is volitionally harmful is a sensitive research topic, with many inherent

problems (Krasikova et al., 2013). If the data are collected from the leader, which provides

just one view of their behaviour, the problem of impression management must be addressed.

Yet data obtained from subordinates, peers, or supervisors would require inferences about

intentions (an internal state of the leader), which the respondents are unlikely to be privy to.

Moreover, acting on such inferences could have potentially serious implications for the leader

(i.e., career derailment) and the organisation, if the inferences are unfounded (Krasikova et al.,

2013). Despite the difficulties in researching destructive leadership, the consequent personal

and financial costs to individuals and organisations make investigation of this significant

psychosocial risk imperative (Boddy, 2015).

63
Chapter Conclusion

This chapter presented a broad overview of the dark side of leadership literature,

including the psychological and psycho-sociological theories and explanations for the

presence of destructive leadership traits and behaviours. Research challenges of overlapping

definitions, multiple conceptual frameworks, inconsistent terminology, the range of foci of

researchers, and the diversity of measures of this construct were discussed (Krasikova et al.,

2013; Schyns & Schilling, 2013; Tepper, 2007).

Despite reviews of the study of destructive leadership confirming that destructive

leadership is prevalent, studies that test theoretical mechanisms that predispose leaders to

engage in destructive leadership, conducted within an organisational setting, remain scarce.

Meta-analyses of the current literature have identified theoretical propositions for traits that

predispose leaders to choose harmful behaviours, and potential mediators of trait activation.

However, empirical support for these conceptual frameworks is limited. There are few

replication studies from samples outside of the US, and many researchers use student

samples, rather than organisational samples, which limits the generalisability and utility of

the results. Nor is it understood whether destructive leaders can change to a more

constructive leadership style and, if so, what leadership development interventions may be

effective in facilitating behavioural change.

This thesis is concerned with destructive leadership behaviours that are specifically

focussed towards subordinates. The first study seeks to further our understanding of the

antecedents of destructive leadership behaviours by investigating the roles of negative

affectivity and self-regulation traits in mediating the relationship between high levels of

independence (a focus on personal goal achievement) and a predisposition to demonstrate

derailing leadership behaviours. This study is discussed in Chapter 4.

64
A second key area of interest in the study of the dark side of leadership is the severity

of its effect on individual followers (Schyns & Schilling, 2013). A number of empirical

studies in the destructive leadership and abusive supervision literatures have confirmed that

there are likely to be serious negative effects on psychological, emotional and physical well-

being for subordinates subjected to destructive leadership behaviours (Einarsen et al., 2010;

Martinko et al., 2013; Tepper, 2007). If destructive leaders cannot change their ways, or

organisations are unwilling to remove them, an alternative approach to address the issue of

destructive leadership and the risk of harm to employees is proposed in this thesis: to

investigate interventions that assist followers to maintain their well-being by becoming more

resilient to the effects of destructive leadership. In other words, assist organisations to create

environments that are less conducive to destructive leadership, and more protective of

followers’ well-being, through enhancing follower resilience. The next chapter examines the

role of followers, the impact destructive leadership has on them, and their response to it.

65
Chapter 3. Follower Responses to Destructive Leadership

The previous chapter reviewed the destructive leadership literature predominantly

from a leader-centric perspective. The purpose of this chapter is to review the literature on

the destructive leadership research stream from a follower-centric perspective. When

studying the consequences of destructive leadership behaviours, a range of negative effects

on well-being have been identified, particularly at the individual employee level, which can

also impact negatively on performance (Fowlie & Wood, 2009; Richards & Freeman, 2002;

Schyns & Schilling, 2013; Sutton, 2007; Tepper, 2000, 2007). A review of the followership

literature, with a specific focus on followship of destructive leaders, will be provided in this

chapter.

In relation to the research model in Figure 2 (see p. 17), this review explores the

leadership behaviours that followers perceive as destructive, follower coping strategies, and

consequences for followers subjected to destructive leadership. In addition, this chapter

investigates interventions to mitigate harm caused to followers by destructive leadership

behaviours. Research questions of interest generated by this phenomenon are:

(a) What are the traits and behaviours that followers perceive as harmful to their well-

being?

(b) What are the psychological, emotional, and physical outcomes of destructive

leadership traits and behaviours for followers?

(c) How do targets of destructive leadership traits and behaviours cope with those

psychological, emotional and physical outcomes?

(d) What is an effective, evidence-based intervention to enhance well-being and

prevent harm to followers within the context of destructive leadership?

66
This chapter reviews theoretical explanations for how followers identify, respond to and cope

with destructive leadership behaviours, and stress management interventions that may

increase follower well-being through enhanced resilience to destructive leadership.

Destructive Leadership as an Occupational Stressor

Harmful consequences to followers subjected to destructive leadership include

psychological distress (e.g., reduced self-esteem, anxiety, depression, difficulty

concentrating); emotional harm (e.g., rumination, anger, fear); physical health problems (e.g.,

chronic fatigue, hair loss, insomnia, low energy); and a number of career impacts (e.g.,

reduced effectiveness, disengagement, burnout, reduced job, work and life satisfaction, work

and family conflict, and reduced discretionary effort; Martinko et al., 2013; Schyns &

Schilling, 2013). Extremely abusive behaviours, such as yelling at and berating a subordinate

in public, may create an acute stress process within a short duration (e.g., tears or retaliation).

However, definitions of destructive leadership styles include the criteria that the destructive

behaviours must be systematic, repeated, and sustained. Therefore, such behaviour would be

characterised in the stress and coping literature as producing chronic stress processes over a

period of months or years (Edwards, 1992). Exposure to even one chronic stressor that is

perceived by the individual as threatening can be damaging to well-being; however, followers

are likely to be subjected to multiple psychosocial stressors when experiencing destructive

leadership behaviours.

Stressor-strain theory. Stressor-strain theory suggests that destructive leadership

behaviours are likely to cause strain in recipients (e.g., negative psychological, emotional,

and physical responses) when they are perceived as a stressor (Beehr, 1998). The negative

effects of destructive leadership on well-being may be explained as exposure to a number of

stressors identified in the occupational stress literature. These stressors include anti-social

behaviours (e.g., ridicule, bullying, harassment, sabotage of work, verbal aggression),

67
interpersonal conflict, lack of social support, and loss of job/career control, as a result of a

destructive managerial style (O'Driscoll & Brough, 2010; Sparks, Faragher, & Cooper, 2001).

Responses to stressors vary depending on individual differences, and the way such

differences influence an individual’s appraisal of the stressor and their choice of coping

strategies. For example, behaviour by a leader may be appraised as a significant stressor by

one subordinate, and less of a stressor by another. Therefore, variables proposed in the

literature to moderate the stressor-strain relationship include the individual characteristics and

personal resources of the follower.

One perspective on the stressor-strain theory, the challenge stressor-hindrance

stressor framework, proposed an individual’s appraisal of and reaction to a stressor may be

determined by whether they view the stressor as a challenge (the individual perceives they

can master the situation) or as a hindrance (the individual perceives the stressor as a threat to

their well-being and one that they may not be able to control; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984;

LePine, Podsakoff, & LePine, 2005). Exposure to destructive leadership behaviours is likely

to be perceived by most followers as a hindrance stressor (e.g., a threat), which is negatively

associated with well-being, motivation, and performance. However, individual variation

within followers (e.g., follower characteristics, coping responses, motivations, needs and

fears, sense of entitlement) and their environments (e.g., level of social and organisational

support available) ultimately determines levels of perceived stress (DeFrank & Cooper, 1987;

Harvey, Harris, Gillis, & Martinko, 2014; May et al., 2014; Thoroughgood, Padilla et al.,

2012).

Susceptible followers. The literature suggests some followers are more susceptible

to destructive leadership and the consequent harm than others (May et al., 2014; Padilla et al.,

2007; Thoroughgood, Padilla et al., 2012). Followers seek group benefits, to be provided

with a group goal, and the instruments of goal achievement (Kellerman, 2008). Under the

68
leadership of destructive leaders, inter and intra team conflict often escalates, increasing

stress, reducing social support from colleagues, and negatively impacting on employees’

performance and goal achievement (Babiak & Hare, 2007; Clarke, 2005; Boddy, 2015;

Kellerman, 2004a; May et al., 2014). The type of interpersonal interaction between leader

and follower may also determine whether further perceived mistreatment will be handed out

to the follower (May et al., 2014). Social adaptability skills of followers have been proposed

by researchers as a mediator between perceived abusive supervision and its consequences for

subordinates; that is, followers with the ability to adjust their cognitions and behaviours in

response to the perceived threat of abusive supervision will be less strongly affected by it

(Mackey, Parker Ellen III, Hochwarter, & Ferris, 2013).

Despite negative outcomes for followers, it is often reported that limited action is

taken to address offending leaders’ behaviour and, as a result, employees either leave, or

remain with the organisation, but in a reduced capacity, that eventually leads to significant

harm to their well-being (Sutton, 2007). Further research into the coping strategies followers

employ to deal with destructive leadership behaviours has been called for (May et al., 2014;

Pelletier, 2010; Yagil et al., 2011).

Followers Responses to Destructive Leadership

Followship research studies the behaviours of individuals acting in relation to a leader

based on their own schema of good followership, and provide possible explanations for why

people comply with destructive leaders. A number of reasons have been hypothesised for

why people conform with destructive leaders’ demands and fail to challenge their negative

behaviours (Kellerman, 2004a; Lipman-Blumen, 2005; Padilla et al., 2007; Thoroughgood,

Padilla et al., 2012). Despite arguments that followers, with no apparent power or authority,

can influence leaders, often individuals choose not to act against toxic leaders, or to report

them (Chaleff, 2009; Kellerman, 2008). This failure to act can be due to a range of factors

69
(Babiak & Hare, 2007; Clarke, 2005; Kellerman, 2004a; Lipman-Blumen, 2005; Padilla et

al., 2007; Rate & Sternberg, 2007). It may be a specific skill or power the leader has, such as

expert knowledge or holding powerful relationships. Lack of knowledge of the

characteristics of destructive leadership can lead followers to believe it is themselves, and not

the leader, who are at fault. They may feel that it is something about their personality or

something that they have done that has caused them to be treated in such a way. Failure to

recognise their leader’s behaviour as toxic, reluctance to face conflict and the unpleasantness

of addressing these behaviours with the leader, or a fear of retribution, can lead to a failure of

courage in acting (Chaleff, 2009).

The destructive leadership literature identifies two primary motivations for

compliance with toxic leaders (Lipman-Blumen, 2005; Thoroughgood, Padilla et al., 2012).

Compliance may be due to the follower’s self-evaluation (e.g., low self-confidence, low self-

esteem, and/or low self-efficacy, driven by fear as a result of a need for safety, security,

group membership, and/or predictability in an uncertain world). Followers driven by these

motivations have been described as conformers. Alternatively, followers may identify and

align themselves with the leaders’ values and beliefs, or they may follow because they benefit

from their destructive activities. Followers driven by these motivations have been described

as colluders.

Thoroughgood et al. (2012) extended Padilla et al.’s (2007) typology to explain

followship of destructive leaders. Table 5 provides a comparison of these two key

typologies. Thoroughgood et al. (2012) drew on Barbuto’s theory of follower compliance in

an attempt to understand the psychological processes that motivate followers to comply with

destructive leaders’ demands. Barbuto (2000) proposed three conditions required to achieve

follower compliance: (a) the leader's perceived power bases (e.g., legitimate, referent,

coercive, expert, and reward power; French & Raven, 1959); (b) the follower's sources of

70
motivation; and (c) the follower's resistance level. Compliance is likely to be greater when

the influence trigger employed by the leader (i.e., power base, goals, or values) is aligned to

the follower’s motivation (i.e., self-concept and desired outcomes), and the follower’s

resistance to the influence attempt is low.

Table 5

Two typologies of susceptible followers

Padilla et al. (2007) Thoroughgood et al. (2012)

Conformers Lost Souls

Comply out of fear Low self-concept and strong identification with the

leader (susceptible to referent power)

Bystanders

Passive, motivated primarily by fear to minimise costs

and punishment (susceptible to coercive power)

Authoritarians

Obey because of rank and status of leader (susceptible

to legitimate power)

Colluders Opportunists

Participate in leader’s agenda Form alliance with leader as a vehicle for personal

gain (susceptible to reward power)

Acolytes

Share congruent values and goals with the leader

(susceptible to expert power)

71
Moderate support has been found for Barbuto’s proposed relationship between leaders’

management styles or behaviours and targets’ motivation or resistance (Barbuto, Scholl,

Hickox III, & Boulmetis, 2001; Barbuto & Xu, 2006). Thoroughgood, Padill et al.’s (2012)

model has been critiqued as leader-centric, focusing on followers’ compliance with the

destructive agenda set by the leader and/or followers’ resistance to the leaders’ destructive

instrumental influence attempts (May et al., 2014).

May et al.’s (2014) leader-follower interaction process suggested an alternative view;

that follower responses to perceived mistreatment by their leader may either trigger or

prevent further interpersonal mistreatment. If followers’ behaviour is interpreted by the

leader as either submissive (conformers) or aggressive and retaliatory, the leader will

maintain or increase their destructive behaviours (May et al., 2014). Followers who are

perceived by the leader to cope in a constructive manner (such as colluders) are likely to be

the recipient of constructive leadership behaviours. May et al. argued that using this

perspective enables a better understanding of how followers cope with destructive leadership.

A number of follower typologies have been proposed by theorists in an attempt to better

explain follower responses to destructive leadership.

Follower typologies. Jean Lipman-Blumen (2005), in her discussion of why

followers willingly obey toxic leaders, suggested three general categories of followers who

enabled and supported bad leaders: (a) benign followers, gullible followers, unquestioning of

what the toxic leader is saying or requesting, and following for pragmatic reasons such as

keeping their job (conformers); (b) the leader’s entourage, committed to the leader’s agenda

(colluders); and (c) malevolent followers, driven by greed, envy, or competitiveness, who

work against the leader and may wish to depose the leader in order to become the leader

themselves.

72
Similar to Lipman-Blumen’s malevolent followers, Kellerman’s (2008) typology of

followers of bad leaders included an additional contribution in the leader-follower interaction

process: followers who may, over time, become resisters of destructive leaders, working to

overthrow or remove them. Kellerman’s first two types fall into the category of submissive

conformers: isolates, who are detached and strengthen leader’s position by not responding to

the leader; and bystanders, who observe but do not participate. Participants, who are

engaged and trying to have an impact, may be perceived as responding constructively.

However, activists, who work hard, either on behalf of their leader or to undermine them; and

diehards, who are prepared to die for their cause, and are either deeply devoted to their

leaders or ready to remove them. Activists and diehards may be perceived by the leader as

constructive if supporting the leader’s agenda, and aggressive or retaliatory if they are

perceived to be working against their leader, thus perpetuating abuse by the leader

(Kellerman, 2008; May et al., 2014).

Carsten et al. (2010) investigated how individuals’ schemas of followership informed

the way they responded to and complied with their leader, on a continuum from passive to

proactive followership. They found passive followers held hierarchical views of

followership, regarding it as deference, subordination, and obedience, being less willing to

speak out or take risks (e.g., likely to comply with a destructive leader holding a high level

position within the organisational structure). Active followers saw their role as a partner in

delivering leadership outcomes and felt able to voice their opinions in a positive way. Active

followers were described as emphasising the importance of supporting their leader’s

decisions, even if they didn’t agree with them (e.g., not speaking up when observing negative

behaviour by a leader). Proactive followers were less concerned with obedience, preferring

to take initiative and ownership for outcomes. Proactive followers were described as being

73
more likely to voice their concerns and offer solutions to problems before being asked to do

so (e.g., confront their leader on their negative attitude or behaviour; Carsten et al., 2010).

In a similar typology to Carsten et al., Chaleff (2009) proposed four followership

styles to address the power dynamics between leaders and followers, based on the level of

support and challenge offered to the leader by the follower: partner (high support, high

challenge); implementer (high support, low challenge); individualist (low support, high

challenge); and resource (low support, low challenge). These follower taxonomies provide a

possible explanation for the individual differences in responses to destructive leadership.

Finally, Crowley and Elster (2009) incorporated a leader-follower interaction process

when developing 10 follower types based on follower expectations of leaders, and their own

needs and fears. They developed an inventory for 20 “bad boss” behaviours, advised typical

follower responses to perceived mistreatment for each follower typology, such as sulking,

self-doubt, and retaliation, then suggested more adaptive responses, such as detecting

mistreatment, detaching, and depersonalising (Crowley & Elster, 2009). While their

typology was based on their own work experience rather than on a theoretical framework,

their work does provide a self-help tool to assist followers to understand their reactions when

dealing with destructive leadership behaviours.

Follower typologies highlight follower characteristics and make distinctions between

types of followers. However, discussion on theoretical explanations for how these different

types of followers influence the interaction between leader and follower is still in its early

stages (Oc & Bashshur, 2013; Uhl-Bien et al., 2014). The typologies for followers of

destructive leaders discussed above are descriptive and help us to theorise how followers may

react and behave towards a toxic leader; for example, avoid, support, challenge, or overthrow

the leader. However, individuals may self-identify in more than one follower type, or their

follower type may change over time depending on the context (Kellerman, 2008).

74
Perhaps due to the relatively recent growth in conceptualisations of followership and

follower typologies in the context of destructive leadership, no empirical research validating

these theories and typologies was found. Further empirical research is required to validate

theoretical follower typologies in field studies to inform future research, and to identify how

understanding follower types can assist individuals to cope better with destructive leadership.

As no unified, validated measure of follower types has been designed, such typologies

provide limited assistance in researching leader-follower interactions in the context of

destructive leadership. In practical terms, descriptions of follower typologies may assist in

developing some self-awareness by followers into their needs and wants from a leader and

their responses to leader behaviours, but have limited utility in assisting individuals to

develop personal initiative or resilience when dealing with the adversity caused by

destructive leadership behaviours (Thoroughgood, Padilla et al., 2012). In addition, many of

the follower typology labels presented in the literature (e.g., colluder, malevolent, isolate,

bystander) are not useful terms for practitioners to employ in their endeavours to support and

assist followers to maintain well-being in the face of destructive leadership.

Follower Well-being in the Context of Destructive Leadership

As discussed previously, theoretical frameworks and research relevant to addressing

the negative impacts of destructive leadership can be found in the occupational stress

management literature. In order to determine ways of reducing the harmful effects of

destructive leadership and enhancing follower well-being, theories that inform the design of

interventions to build follower coping skills and resilience to the occupational stressor of

destructive leadership will now be reviewed. There are many similarities between the

resilience and coping literatures, even though they appear to have evolved along parallel yet

separate paths (Harland, Harrison, Jones, & Reiter-Palmon, 2005). Resilience research has

largely been conducted by developmental psychologists, predicting the development of

75
resilience in children and adolescents, whereas coping research has mainly focused on adult

populations. Yet both literatures have investigated the individual factors (e.g., personality

traits such as optimism) and situational factors (e.g., social support) influencing resilience

through effective coping with stressful experiences (Harland et al., 2005; Seligman, 1992).

Both resilience and coping literatures have also focused on assessing the effectiveness of

strategies used by individuals when faced with challenges. It has been argued that coping

strategies employed by individuals to address and deal with an adverse experience are the

very processes by which the outcome of resilience is achieved (Harland et al., 2005). In the

following sections, resiliency and coping theories are discussed in relation to their relevance

to maintaining follower well-being by building follower resilience to destructive leadership.

Follower resiliency. Resiliency is the process by which well-being is restored and

includes the characteristics that influence the return to a state of well-being following an

adverse event (McLarnon & Rothstein, 2013). The study of resiliency in the workplace is a

relatively new area of research (Luthans, Youssef, & Avolio, 2007; McLarnon & Rothstein,

2013). Resiliency goes beyond the definition of resilience, which has been defined as an

outcome: “the capacity to rebound or bounce back from adversity, conflict or failure”

(Luthans et al., 2007, p.18). Resiliency includes what a person learns when dealing with

adversity and the processes that lead to personal choices, career recovery, and personal

growth (King & Rothstein, 2010). Specifically, resiliency has been conceptualised as

involving cognitive, affective, and behavioural self-regulatory processes to assist recovery

and restoration of optimal functioning after a significant adverse workplace experience (King

& Rothstein, 2010). It is purported that resiliency processes involve three domains: (a) self-

regulation of beliefs or cognitions; (b) self-regulation of emotions; and (c) engagement of

behavioural strategies that provide a sense of personal control and personal self-efficacy

(McLarnon & Rothstein, 2013). If successful, these adaptive responses will result in

76
outcomes that demonstrate resilience has occurred (e.g., psychological, emotional, and

physical well-being). However, outcomes may differ depending on the situational context

and the adversity experienced.

McLarnon et al. (2013) developed a conceptual model of resiliency adapted from the

framework proposed by King and Rothstein (2010). As no conceptual model for resiliency in

the context of destructive leadership was found, McLarnon et al.’s model has been further

adapted by this researcher in the context of destructive leadership (see Figure 6). Followers

exhibit an initial response to the traumatic work experience, in this instance, destructive

leadership behaviours. Personal characteristics (e.g., follower type, self-efficacy, self-

discipline) will affect the initial response (e.g., negative cognitions and emotions, perceived

stress). The relationship between the follower’s initial response to perceiving toxic

leadership behaviours, and the cognitive, affective, and behavioural self-regulatory processes

they employ in response, will be mediated by their personal characteristics, as well as

situational factors, such as supports and resources (e.g., social support from colleagues,

friends and family, organisational support). Achieving positive life and work outcomes will

depend on the effectiveness of their self-regulatory processes (e.g., their ability to understand

and control ineffective thoughts and thinking patterns, negative emotions, and counter-

productive behaviours). In sum, the ability of a follower to return to a state of well-being is

determined by their level of resiliency, and follower coping skills will determine their

resiliency in recovering from the experience of destructive leadership.

Follower coping. With destructive leadership shown to be related to occupational

stress, it follows that targets of destructive leadership may employ a range of coping

strategies when attempting to deal with this particular occupational stressor (Nandkeolyar,

Shaffer, Li, Ekkirala, & Bagger, 2014; Schyns & Schilling, 2013).

77
Personal Characteristics
Follower Type, i.e.
conformer, colluder
Perception of personal
control

Destructive Leadership Initial Responses Self-Regulatory Outcomes


Behaviour Processes  Psychological,
Major threat to follower self- Interpretation of experience  Cognitive, affective, emotional, and
esteem, identity, fundamental and resulting disequilibrium, and behavioural physical well-being
values and/or beliefs e.g., negative cognitions and strategies  Life and career
emotions, perceived stress  Coping skills satisfaction

Supports and Resources


 Peer support
 Social support
 Organisational support

Figure 6. Conceptual model of follower resiliency to destructive leadership. Adapted from McLarnon & Rothstein (2013).

78
Destructive leadership behaviours form part of the psychosocial occupational stressor anti-

social behaviours, which have been found to lead to anxiety, fear, depression, and avoidance

behaviours in affected individuals (O'Driscoll & Brough, 2010; Yagil et al., 2011). In the

occupational stress and coping literature, occupational stress is hypothesised to affect both

well-being (e.g., the psychological, emotional, and physical health of an employee) and

coping responses (e.g. efforts to prevent or reduce the negative effects of stress on well-

being; Edwards, 1992; Goh, Sawang, & Oei, 2010). Upon perceiving a threat, initially a

fight-flight-freeze stress response can be triggered; a physiological reaction that occurs in

response to a perceived threat. The fight or flight response is activated when an individual

believes there is a chance they can survive the threat. The freeze response, however, is

activated when an individual’s coping capacity is overwhelmed, when they believe that there

is no hope of dealing effectively with the stressor (Cannon, 1929).

How coping responses fit within the psychological stress process to preserve well-

being is still being discussed. There has been some debate in the literature over the definition

of coping, including whether coping should be defined as a conscious process, a cognitive

behavioural (transactional) process, or as a form of adaptation (Dewe, O'Driscoll, & Cooper,

2010). The transactional model of stress coping defines coping as a conscious, intentional,

goal-directed response to the specific demands of a stressor (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984).

More recently coping has been defined as “. . . conscious, volitional attempts to regulate the

environment or one’s reaction to the environment under stressful conditions” (Connor-Smith

& Flaschsbart, 2007, p. 1080). There has been considerable research in the occupational

stress and coping literature on how employees deal with a range of stressors within the work

environment, but few studies have focused on understanding how followers cope with the

stress produced specifically by destructive leadership behaviours and which coping responses

might preserve well-being (e.g., May et al., 2014; Yagil et al., 2011). When researching how

79
individuals manage work stress, there are a number of types of coping proposed in the

literature, and many approaches to categorizing coping strategies (Dewe et al., 2010; Skinner

et al., 2003).

One of the most influential models of stress and coping was proposed by Lazarus and

Folkman (1984), who considered individual responses to stress to be shaped primarily by

individuals’ appraisal and coping responses. The first major component of their transactional

model is appraisal of a stressor. Two forms of stressor appraisal were specified in the model:

primary appraisal and secondary appraisal. Primary appraisal is triggered in response to

exposure to a potentially stressful event that involves an assessment by the individual as to

whether the event or person poses a potential threat to well-being, and the degree of that

threat. If the stressful event is appraised as a threat, secondary appraisal occurs to determine

the individual’s perceived ability to cope with and manage the threat, including an evaluation

of the resources available to them to deal with the threat, such as potential courses of action

they might be able to take. Such cognitive appraisals determine whether the individual views

the stressor as a challenge, which they feel able to confront with some level of confidence

and which may enhance their well-being, or as a threat to their psychological, emotional, or

physical well-being. How an individual appraises a threat is likely to determine the coping

behaviours or strategies they employ to deal with the potential stressor. The second major

component of the transactional model is coping responses, defined as a complex sequence of

behaviours that include action impulses and psycho-physiological reactions. Lazarus and

Folkman (1984) proposed two higher order coping responses initiated after primary and

secondary appraisals: problem-focused coping (e.g., attempting to remedy or change a

threatening or harmful situation); and emotion-focused coping (e.g., ventilating one’s

emotional responses to stressors or removing oneself from exposure to stressors). Therefore,

Lazarus and Folkman’s transactional model describes a linear process, whereby primary

80
appraisal, secondary appraisal, and coping responses mediate the relationship between

stressor (e.g., destructive leadership behaviours) and the individual’s stress outcomes (e.g.,

psychological and emotional distress).

Goh, Saway, and Oei (2010) revised Lazarus and Folkman’s proposed linear

transactional process of appraisal, stress, and coping. Goh et al. incorporated stress outcomes

(e.g., psycho-physiological distress resulting from the interaction between the stressor and the

individual’s perception of control over the stressor) into the process, as an intermediary

variable, in a more dynamic representation of the relationship between stress and coping.

Goh et al. proposed their model accounted for how individual affectivity, that is, the

influence of strong emotions, may determine both the choice of coping strategy and stress

outcomes over time. Goh et al. found support for their proposal that, while both primary and

secondary appraisals influence choice of coping strategy and stress outcomes, stress

outcomes themselves may have a direct or indirect effect on subsequent stress outcomes over

time, via the coping strategies employed. For example, if the coping strategies employed are

unable to resolve the stressor, resulting in a negative stress outcome, this itself is likely to

lead to even more negative outcomes over time, such as severe anxiety or depression (Goh et

al., 2010). However, Goh et al. did not delve into appraisals and coping strategies for

specific occupational stressors.

Other coping researchers have developed alternative coping categorisations to that

proposed by Lazarus and Folkman, (e.g., cybernetic coping and active/avoidance coping).

Cybernetic coping theory viewed stress as an important discrepancy between an individual’s

perceived state and their desired state, and conceptualized coping as attempts to reduce or

eliminate the negative effects of stress on well-being, outlining five forms of coping: change

the situation (take steps to bring the situation into line with their desired state); symptom

reduction (taking direct steps to improve well-being), avoidance (repressing uncomfortable

81
or distressing emotions and thoughts about the stressor), accommodation (adjusting

individual desires so they are better aligned with the perceived situation), and/or devaluation

(minimising the importance of the discrepancy between the desired situation and reality;

Edwards & Baglioni, 1993).

Another perspective compares active coping (behaviour directed toward the stressor),

a variation of problem-focused coping, with avoidance coping (behaviour directed away from

the stressor), a form of emotion-focused coping (Connor-Smith & Flaschsbart, 2007; Suls &

Fletcher, 1985). A classification proposed along similar lines to active/avoidance coping is

the approach coping and avoidance coping categorisation (Carver & Connor-Smith, 2010;

Roth & Cohen, 1986). Approach coping strategies aim to reduce, eliminate, or manage the

internal or external demands of a stressor. As with previous definitions, avoidance coping

referred to disengaged coping, where the aim is to ignore, avoid, or withdraw from the

stressor or its emotional consequences (Nes & Segerstrom, 2006; Roth & Cohen, 1986; Suls

& Fletcher, 1985).

Nes and Segerstrom (2006) combined the approach/avoidance coping classification

with the problem-focused/emotion-focused classification to propose four types of coping

behaviour: problem-focused approach coping (e.g., planning, active coping, seeking

instrumental support); emotion-focused approach coping (e.g., cognitive restructuring,

seeking emotional support, acceptance); problem-focused avoidance coping (e.g., avoiding

the problem, behavioural disengagement); and emotion-focused avoidance coping (e.g.,

denial, mental disengagement, wishful thinking, social withdrawal). The coping

classifications reviewed above have been discussed in the abusive supervision literature in the

context of destructive leadership behaviours in the workplace (Aquino & Thau, 2009; May et

al., 2014; Nandkeolyar et al., 2014; Tepper, Moss, Lockhart, & Carr, 2007; Yagil et al.,

2011).

82
May et al.’s (2014) conceptual paper on follower coping with destructive leadership

adopted Nes and Segerstrom’s approach, combining Lazarus and Folkman’s problem-focused

versus emotion-focused strategies, with Roth and Cohen’s approach versus avoidance coping

strategies (e.g., whether behaviour is directed toward or away from the destructive leader).

May et al. proposed that followers of destructive leadership were likely to engage in multiple

problem-focused and emotion-focused coping strategies, and these strategies would vary in

the level to which they confronted the leader (e.g., degree of assertiveness and aggressiveness

of the coping behaviour). Problem-focused approach coping may include strategies such as

challenging the leader’s behaviour, employing upward influencing tactics such as

ingratiation, or seeking instrumental support. Emotion-focused approach coping may include

strategies such as cognitive restructuring (reframing) and seeking emotional support.

Examples of problem-focused avoidance coping included avoiding all contact with the leader.

Examples of emotion-focused avoidance coping included denial of the situation and wishful

thinking (May et al., 2014).

May et al. proposed perceptions by leaders of submissive and/or aggressive reactions

from followers would lead to leaders engaging in further destructive leadership behaviour.

May et al. assumed the level of follower confrontativeness would influence how leaders

perceived the follower was coping with their leadership, and would determine their behaviour

in response to the follower. There are three limitations with this approach: (a) the leader

must be able to observe the follower coping strategy to be able to perceive it as submissive,

aggressive, or constructive, and emotion-focused strategies (e.g., cognitive restructuring) in

particular are unobservable; (b) the leader’s perception of the follower’s coping behaviour

may not be accurate; and (c) the leader must have the resources and inclination to follow up

with constructive behaviour when responding to follower coping attempts (May et al., 2014).

In addition, one of the few research studies into coping with abusive supervision and well-

83
being found employees were more likely to use avoidance-focused (non-assertive, non-

confrontational) coping strategies. Yagil et al. (2011), in their study on follower coping

responses to abusive supervision in two employee samples, found that while problem-focused

coping was associated with positive affect, most of their participants chose emotion-focused

coping strategies and, in particular, avoidance-focused coping responses. Yagil et al.’s

finding corresponds with research that reported followers withdraw and/or leave in the face

of abuse delivered by organisational psychopaths (Boddy, 2011b). These findings support

the proposition that if an individual feels they cannot avoid the stressor, or their attempts to

cope with the stressor do not work, this may lead to “learned helplessness,” when the

individual gives up, resulting in them experiencing higher levels of stress outcomes and

longer-term harm after unsuccessful coping over time (Goh et al., 2010; Seligman, 1992).

Yagil et al. concluded that individual employees do not know how to cope effectively with

abusive supervision.

It is generally maintained in the literature that problem-focused coping is more

effective in perceived controllable situations, whereas emotion-focused coping may be better

in situations where the individual has limited or little control (e.g., Dewe et al., 2010;

O'Driscoll, Brough, & Kalliath, 2009). There is some support in the literature for this

proposition. One study with a sample of 109 college students found that individuals who are

capable of deploying both problem-focused and emotion-focused coping strategies to fit the

demands of the stressor experience experienced lower levels of distress; that is when they

employed problem-focused for controllable stressors (taking an exam) and emotion-focused

for uncontrollable stressors (the terminal illness of a friend; Roussi, Miller, & Shoda, 2000).

A longitudinal study with a community sample of 72 adults found appraised control over the

stressor significantly predicted the type of coping, such that greater control was associated

more with problem-focused coping and less with emotion-focused coping (Zakowski, Hall,

84
Klein, & Baum, 2001). Therefore, if the situation is perceived as uncontrollable or previous

coping efforts have failed, as is likely in the case of destructive leadership, emotion-focused

attempts to minimise distress are likely to be employed. Two studies researching coping with

abusive supervision found respondents dealing with abuse from their manager reported using

emotion-focused coping, specifically avoidance coping (Nandkeolyar et al., 2014; Yagil et

al., 2011).

One of the emotion-focused coping strategies, seeking social support, including

emotional and instrumental support, have been posited as an important coping resource to

alleviate stress and strain and promote well-being at work (Carver, Scheier, & Weintraub,

1989; O'Driscoll & Brough, 2010; Viswesvaran, Sanchez, & Fisher, 1999). There has been

considerable research on the role of social support in mitigating the negative impact of

stressors, but the mechanism by which it reduces strain and enhances well-being is not well-

understood (Dewe et al., 2010). There have been a number of mechanisms proposed in the

literature, for example: that social support can have a direct effect on reducing strain; that

those who have access to social support are less likely to experience strain and are likely to

have greater well-being than those who do not; that social support may have a buffering

effect by protecting individuals from potentially harmful consequences, moderating the

stressor-strain relationship; or it may be a mediating variable, that the stressor induces the

affected individual to seek support (Dewe et al., 2010; Viswesvaran et al., 1999).

Irrespective of which theoretical mechanism is preferred, there is considerable evidence for

the beneficial effects of social support in relation to work stress, though limited evidence of

its role in the relationship between destructive leadership and strain (Martinko et al., 2011).

As an interpersonal stressor, destructive leadership threatens the abused subordinate's social

support, potentially reducing their external resources. Strategies for gaining social support in

the workplace, such as from effective ingratiation tactics, may increase the individual’s

85
perceived control, and this feeling of control (e.g., the perceived ability to cope with

stressors) has been argued to improve individuals' coping abilities (Harvey, Stoner,

Hochwarter, & Kacmar, 2007). Alternatively, it may be that peer support provides a

buffering effect in the face of a common enemy, the abusive manager. Seeking social support

in order to discuss experiences with sympathetic colleagues, or to express anger or frustration

may provide the emotional support necessary to cope with the abuse and adverse work

conditions created by their manager (van Emmerik, Euwema, & Bakker, 2007). Furthermore,

cross-domain buffering theory proposed that social support in one domain (e.g., that of

coworkers) may buffer the negative effects in another domain (e.g., abusive supervisor;

Duffy, Ganster, & Pagon, 2002).

The role played by social support may depend on a number of factors, such as the

situation, the type of social support being provided, and the stressor being confronted (Dewe

et al., 2010). For example, when dealing with destructive leadership behaviours, social

support offered by colleagues may exacerbate the negative impact of this stressor if it

confirms the aversive nature of the stressor. Social support may also encourage the

individual to ruminate on the negative aspects of their experience, particularly if the

individual is susceptible to emotional contagion and easily influenced by the emotions of

others (Wu & Hu, 2009). There is some evidence to suggest that negative interactions (e.g.,

causing feelings of resentment or shame, making critical remarks, failing to provide the

promised help, social undermining) have been found to negate the benefits of social support

on psychological well-being (Lincoln, 2000). Coworkers are likely to also be engaging in

self-protective behaviours against the abuse of their common manager, which could lead to a

lack of positive support for victimized colleagues. Indeed, employees report lowered levels

of friendliness and communication under the abusive leadership of organisational

psychopaths (Boddy, 2011b).

86
More investigation into the role of social support in mitigating the effects of

destructive leadership is needed. One study found the impact of perceived abusive

supervision reduced when peer support was high (Hobman, Restubog, Bordia, & Tang, 2009)

and, conversely, another study found the impact of perceived abusive supervision on

emotional exhaustion was stronger when perceived coworker support was high (Wu & Hu,

2009). One explanation offered for this finding was the reverse buffering effect, that support

from coworkers may remind individuals of the negative aspects of the work environment and

aggravate their discomfort. Therefore, it may be that a reduction in stress is more likely to be

experienced when victims are provided with social support from outside the organisation

(e.g., emotional support from friends and family, or instrumental and informational support,

such as guidance and advice from a trusted advisor external to the workplace). In support of

this proposition, cross-domain buffering theory suggests that family sources of social support

can buffer workplace stress by engendering feelings of belonging and solidarity. This

buffering results in improved mood and perceptions of the workplace as a less aversive place.

In summary, supportive relationships in one domain, the family, may compensate for less

desirable relationships at work (Duffy et al., 2002).

Further research is required to investigate functional versus dysfunctional coping

strategies employed by followers to deal with the stressor of destructive leadership. Yagil et

al.’s (2011) Coping with Abusive Supervision Scale, based on the cognitive model of stress

and coping (Harvey et al., 2007; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984), measured five coping strategies:

ingratiation, direct communication, avoidance of contact, support-seeking, and reframing.

However, Yagil et al.’s scale focused on positive coping strategies when responding to

destructive leadership behaviours and does not advance our understanding of commonly

used, less effective coping strategies that may increase the severity of outcomes over time.

While May et al. (2014) did include both functional (e.g., ingratiation, negotiating better

87
treatment from the leader) and dysfunctional responses (e.g., retaliation by verbally attacking

or ridiculing the supervisor and/or their co-workers), they acknowledged that the possible

strategies they posited to cope with destructive leadership were not exhaustive.

Finding a consistent taxonomy of coping behaviours has remained elusive (Brough,

O'Driscoll, & Kalliath, 2005). Skinner et al. (2003) argued that existing coping models failed

to explain the full range of coping responses. Skinner et al.’s meta-analysis of the coping

literature proposed a theoretical structure of 12 families of coping behaviours. These 12

coping categories included: problem solving, information seeking, opposition, negotiating,

and self-reliance (problem-focused coping); and seeking support, delegation,

accommodation/distracting, submission, helplessness, escape, and isolation/ withdrawal

(emotion-focused coping). Skinner et al.’s categories of coping behaviours provide a useful

and comprehensive framework for analysing a fuller range of coping responses to destructive

leadership. It has been utilised in subsequent coping research (e.g., Koole, 2009).

Given the ongoing discussion in the literature as to which coping theory/taxonomy

best explains coping behaviours, and the limited field research into coping specifically with

destructive leadership, an exploratory approach was adopted in this research. Qualitative

questions were employed to investigate the behaviours employees found harmful, the coping

strategies they chose in response to the perceived threat of exposure to destructive leadership,

and how they coped with the resultant stress outcomes, such as emotional, psychological, and

physical harm. The subsequent data were analysed through the lenses of Yagil et al. and

Skinner et al.’s coping frameworks reviewed above. This study is discussed in Chapter 5.

In the context of destructive leadership, it is difficult to employ another form of

coping, proactive coping (Aspinwall & Taylor, 1997). Proactive coping describes efforts

undertaken in advance of exposure to the stressor to prevent or modify its impact. Proactive

coping distinguishes itself from the reactive forms of coping described above, which are

88
enacted after the person has encountered the stressor. While it may not be possible to prevent

this stressor from occurring in the first place, it may be possible to modify the potential

impact of destructive leadership on followers. The next section reviews approaches to

follower interventions that will assist followers to acquire the resources and skills to tackle

destructive leadership behaviours when they arise in a way that is more likely to preserve

their well-being.

Interventions to Maintain Follower Well-being

While experiencing positive, supportive relationships with direct managers can

significantly offset workplace stressors and can reduce overall stress experiences, conversely,

experiencing aversive relationships and negative interactions with supervisors is a significant

cause of occupational stress (O'Driscoll & Brough, 2010; Sparks et al., 2001). If Kellerman

(2004a and 2004b) and Krasikova et al. (2013) are correct in their propositions that all leaders

are likely at some time to cause negative interactions with followers that will create stress, it

is argued in this thesis that more than leadership development programs are required. It

would be effective to also employ interventions that protect followers from potential harm

caused by destructive leadership behaviours. Rather than focusing purely on trying to change

the behaviour of the offending leader, whether intentional or not, a more effective approach

would be to improve the personal resiliency of followers to destructive leadership (Clements

& Washbush, 1999; Cotton, 2008; Hollander, 1992; Kellerman, 2008; Lipman-Blumen, 2005;

Padilla et al., 2007). However, there is scarce empirical research investigating the most

appropriate psychological interventions to assist susceptible followers in coping with the

specific occupational stressor of experiencing destructive leadership behaviours (e.g.,

Pelletier, 2010).

A review of the stress management intervention (SMI) literature for dealing with

occupational stress was conducted in order to inform practice within the context of

89
destructive leadership (Webster & Brough, 2015). DeFrank and Cooper (1987) proposed a

classification system focused on the target of the interventions, highlighting the need for

multiple approaches to stress reduction: interventions directed at individuals (equipping

individuals with skills to manage stress, e.g., coping skills); the individual/organisational

interface (interactions between individuals and their workplace, e.g., level of organisational

and social support); and organisations (the organisational context, e.g., code of conduct,

counselling services). When designing interventions, the foci identified by DeFrank and

Cooper are often embedded in a tripartite framework that describes three levels of SMIs:

primary prevention organisational interventions to eliminate stressors from the work

environment and prevent stress (e.g., sanction or remove the destructive leader; put policies,

procedures, and practices in place that promote positive values and protect the well-being of

employees); secondary prevention interventions, detecting stress and providing stress

management skills (e.g., resilience and coping training for individuals to deal with the

stressor of destructive leadership); and tertiary prevention interventions to rehabilitate

stressed workers (e.g., providing safe reporting mechanisms, counselling, mediation,

workplace conferencing; Bond et al., 2010; Dewe, 1994; Murphy, 1988). To date, much of

the SMI research has focussed predominantly on secondary prevention stress management

interventions for individuals, with cognitive-behavioural programs producing larger effect

sizes in reducing distress, at least in the short term (Murphy, 1996; Richardson & Rothstein,

2008; van der Klink, Blonk, Schene, & van Dijk, 2001), with limited evidence of individual

interventions producing long term-effects (Giga, Noblet, Faragher, & Cooper, 2003;

Lamontagne, Keegel, Louie, Ostry, & Landsbergis, 2007). To maximise their effectiveness,

secondary prevention interventions are best held in organisations with strong governance

frameworks and explicit organisational support for well-being programs (primary

90
prevention), as well as effective mechanisms to support staff after a stressful interaction

(tertiary interventions; Brough, Brown, & Biggs, in press).

One of the criticisms of SMI is the failure of intervention design to be theory-based

(Dewe, 1994). In this thesis four theoretical approaches are proposed to underpin an effective

secondary prevention stress management intervention, designed to improve well-being

through increased resiliency and coping skills in employees experiencing occupational stress

as a result of the effects of destructive leadership (Webster & Brough, 2015). These four

approaches include: (a) stress inoculation training (SIT; Saunders, Driskell, Johnston, &

Salas, 1996); (b) positive psychology techniques (Seligman, 1992; Seligman &

Csikszentmihalyi, 2000); (c) Acceptance Commitment Therapy/Training techniques (ACT;

Bond & Bunce, 2000; Burton, Pakenham, & Brown, 2010); and (d) career management

strategies to increase perceptions of personal control and enhance career resilience (Vuori,

Toppinen-Tanner, & Mutanen, 2012). These four theoretical approaches are discussed in

detail next.

Follower interventions to build psychosocial resilience. Three interventions, based

on cognitive behavioural principles, relevant to addressing the stressor of destructive

leadership, are: SIT, positive psychology, and ACT. To date, the concept of SIT has been

utilised in organisations to mitigate the effects of psychological and emotional distress as a

result of stress, burnout, and/or physical injury (Foley & Stone, 1988; Meichenbaum, 1996;

Saunders et al., 1996; Van der Hek & Plomp, 1997). SIT emerged from an attempt to

integrate research on the role of cognitive and affective factors in coping processes with the

emerging technique of cognitive behaviour modification. SIT has been implemented to help

individuals cope with the aftermath of exposure to stressful events and on a preventative basis

to “inoculate” them to future and longer term stressors (Cotton, 2008; Meichenbaum, 1996).

91
It is proposed by this researcher that the same principles used in SIT may be useful in

building employees’ awareness of the potential harm resulting from destructive leadership

behaviours. For example, how employees recognise early in the relationship when they may

potentially fall victim to such behaviours, and what techniques they can employ to develop

resiliency to prevent or minimise psychological, emotional, and/or physical harm. In essence,

an intervention to inoculate them to the effects of destructive leadership. Meichenbaum

(1996) recommends three overlapping phases of SIT strategies, described in Table 6, which

this researcher proposes can be applied to an organisational setting when dealing with the

effects of destructive leadership. SIT techniques include increasing knowledge and

awareness of stress triggers, providing reading materials, problem articulation skills,

describing emotions likely to be experienced on the job, focusing on the transient nature of

unpleasant situations, conflict resolution, and self-regulatory training (Meichenbaum, 1996;

Van der Hek & Plomp, 1997; Waung, 1995).

Based on a meta-analysis of 37 SIT studies, Saunders et al. (1996) concluded that SIT

is likely to be effective for non-clinical groups with “normal” levels of anxiety, such as

targets of destructive leadership. In this context, SIT may be effective in reducing state

anxiety and enhancing performance under the stress of working for an abusive supervisor. Of

the 37 studies reviewed, three were conducted in an occupational setting. All three studied

teacher stress and found support for the positive effect of SIT in reducing teacher stress and

anxiety resulting from multiple stressors, including relationships with supervisors (Saunders

et al., 1996).

92
Table 6

Phases of stress innoculation training (SIT) applicable to destructive leadership

Phases of stress innoculation training (SIT)

Conceptualisation  Establish a collaborative relationship between employees and the

phase trainer

 Educate employees about the nature and impact of the stress (i.e.,

how to recognise specific destructive leadership behaviours)

 Encourage employees to identify and view the perceived threat as a

problem to be solved

 Help them to distinguish between which elements of the problem

they may be able to change and those which they cannot (which may

be especially useful with destructive leadership)

 Teach employees to set specific short, medium, and long term

coping and resilience-building goals

Skill acquisition  Education (i.e., follower style)

phase:  Skills practice (i.e., emotional self-regulation, cognitive

restructuring, problem solving, using social support systems,

acceptance of uncomfortable emotions)

Application and  Help employees to identify high risk situations and warning signs to

follow-through prevent harm (i.e., to identify destructive leadership behaviours

phase: when they occur and plan mitigation strategies)

 Encourage employees to help others with similar problems (social

support)

93
Results of a study investigating the efficacy of SIT in reducing stress in student

nurses suggested SIT may be a promising approach, given it reduced anxiety and negative

affectivity in the experimental group, but interpretation of the results was limited by the small

sample (Foley & Stone, 1988). SIT was found to effectively reduce occupational stress in a

sample of 60 registered acute-care nurses over a four-month period (West, Horan, & Games,

1984). Flaxman and Bond (2010b) found that SIT, delivered as part of a stress management

program in two large local government organisations, was effective in reducing psychological

distress across a three-month assessment period. While SIT appears to offer a useful

component to stress management interventions, due to the limited evidence of the efficacy of

SIT for non-clinical, workplace populations, features of the SIT approach were included in

the intervention, and additional cognitive behavioural based approaches were investigated to

complement SIT techniques.

Traditional stress management interventions, such as teaching employees coping

skills, have been criticised as having limited enduring effect on employee well-being and

performance (Cotton & Hart, 2003; Ivancevich et al., 1990). Stress management training

(SMT) has been shown to have been moderately effective in improving employees’

psychological health (Flaxman & Bond, 2010a). SMT has typically been based on principles

of cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT). More recently, the focus has moved to interventions

to increase psychosocial resilience based on two areas of psychology that have emerged

within the CBT movement: positive psychology (Seligman, 1992, 2002) and ACT (Burton et

al., 2010). Resilience-building programs have been reported as showing some efficacy (e.g.,

Pipe et al., 2012), with resilience in this context being defined as “positive adaptation and

normal functioning following exposure to a significant threat” (Zellars, Justice, & Beck,

2011, p. 6).

94
Positive psychology, founded by Martin Seligman (Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi,

2000), is primarily concerned with using psychological theory, research, and intervention

techniques to understand the positive and adaptive aspects of human behaviour, especially in

relation to reducing anxiety and depression resulting from stress. Positive psychology

strategies are designed to help individuals change negative styles of thinking in order to

change their feelings, even in situations where they experience negative emotions and/or

helplessness. Tactics include reviewing life satisfaction, identifying personal values and

signature strengths, and using mindfulness techniques to stay in the moment, rather than

ruminating about the past or worrying about the future (Brown, Ryan, & Creswell, 2007;

Burton et al., 2010; Gander, Proyer, Ruch, & Wyss, 2012; Pipe et al., 2009; Pipe et al., 2012).

A review of 15 studies examining the effects of positive psychology interventions in

organisations revealed these interventions seem to enhance employee well-being and tend to

decrease stress and anxiety (Meyers, van Woerkom, & Bakker, 2013).

ACT, also a cognitive behavioural and mindfulness-based approach, uses a range of

techniques designed to increase psychological flexibility and facilitate values based action

(Hayes, Luoma, Bond, Masuda, & Lillis, 2006). ACT promotes six core processes to

increase psychological flexibility and resilience: (a) choosing acceptance rather than avoiding

negative emotions; (b) cognitive defusion, changing the interaction with thoughts to defuse

the believability of unhelpful thoughts; (c) self-observation, being aware of one’s thoughts

and experiences without attachment to them; (d) being present or mindful so an individual

can experience the world more directly, in the moment, rather than ruminating about the past

or worrying about the future; (e) using personal values to drive purposeful action; and (f)

committed action aligned to personal values (Harris, 2009; Hayes et al., 2006). Techniques to

provide affected followers with the opportunity to choose a more constructive response to

abusive supervision may include: practising mindfulness exercises to increase present

95
moment awareness and reduce the struggle with undesirable and aversive thoughts and

emotions; completing a values inventory; and completing an action plan to facilitate

clarification of goals and actions. Research suggests that including ACT techniques in

workplace stress management training interventions is suitable for addressing work-related

stress (Bond & Bunce, 2000; Brinkborg, Michanek, Hesser, & Berglund, 2011; McConachie,

McKenzie, Morris, & Walley, 2014; Öst, 2014; Stafford-Brown & Pakenham, 2012).

Furthermore, it is found to be particularly beneficial for participants experiencing

psychological distress (Flaxman & Bond, 2010b), with one study demonstrating that both SIT

and ACT have been found to be equally effective in reducing psychological distress at work

(Flaxman & Bond, 2010a).

No research has investigated the efficacy of using SIT, positive psychology, and/or

ACT, either individually or combined, to reduce stress caused by experiencing destructive

leadership. This researcher proposes that adopting a combination of these processes in an

intervention will better enable individuals to cope effectively with destructive leadership by

teaching them to identify destructive leadership behaviours early and to address such

behaviours assertively, before they can cause harm. If the toxic behaviour was ongoing,

psychosocial strategies (e.g., reaffirming individuals’ personal values, and providing

participants with a framework within which to choose a response) may enable individuals to

cope with the psychological and emotional distress caused by this stressor (Bond & Bunce,

2000), although limited research into stress management training on physiological symptoms

has been conducted (Richardson & Rothstein, 2008).

Follower interventions to build career resilience. Interventions based on cognitive

behavioural principles have been found to be more effective when combined with techniques

to increase individuals’ personal resources (Richardson & Rothstein, 2008). Engaging

individuals in their own career development has been identified as one strategy to increase

96
personal resources, subsequently reducing workplace stressors (Cooper & Cartwright, 1997).

A review of interventions based on career resilience theory will now be discussed. In

contrast to the literature on career success and satisfaction, less attention has been given to

what happens when careers go badly (Kidd, 2008). Kidd (2008) found in a study of 89 UK

workers that the most common negative career experience was interpersonal difficulties with

their manager, such as working for an aggressive, undermining, or unsupportive boss. These

negative career experiences lead to many negative emotions, including unhappiness,

dejection, anxiety, frustration, and feelings of worthlessness (Kidd, 2008). Given the effect

destructive leadership has on followers’ careers and job satisfaction, career management

skills are likely to enhance followers’ personal resources and, as a result, their sense of

control. London (1997) suggested individuals high in career resilience, defined as an

individual’s “resistance to career disruption in a less than optimal environment” (London,

1983, p. 621), are better able to cope with the strong emotions generated by a negative work

situation, such as manager abuse, interpersonal conflict, or unfair treatment. London (1997)

proposed that lack of support in building career resilience can result in dysfunctional or

destructive coping strategies when confronted with career barriers.

Destructive leaders have been shown to create unpredictable work environments,

leading to a negative effect on individuals’ well-being, interpersonal relationships, and

careers, for example, through creating conflict, harming a follower’s reputation, or by taking

credit for their work (Boddy et al., 2015; Webster, Brough, & Daly, 2016). Career resilience

interventions focus on encouraging individuals to take responsibility for their own career

development in an unpredictable work environment. Career resilience can be enhanced by

training interventions that help individuals interpret career barriers accurately, deal with

strong, negative emotions, and formulate coping strategies that remove the barriers, by

reframing their ideas, repositioning their energies, and/or changing career direction (London,

97
1997). Career management skills training may assist individuals with their sensemaking (the

ongoing process through which individuals attempt to make sense of the circumstances in

which they find themselves and determine how the situations affect them) when facing

adverse career experiences, by enabling them to evaluate their choices and choose an

appropriate response (Zellars et al., 2011).

A group training intervention to enhance individuals’ career management skills has

been shown to improve career resilience and reduce mental distress symptoms (e.g., Vuori et

al., 2012). Resiliency is defined as more than self-management of one’s thoughts, feelings,

and actions in response to traumatic workplace experiences. Building resiliency assists

individuals to be able to restore their sense of well-being following an adverse event

(McLarnon & Rothstein, 2013). Therefore, including components to enable followers to

improve their career management skills (e.g., identifying their strengths, finding the means to

attain career-related resources, solve interpersonal conflicts with their supervisor/manager,

and learning concrete means for managing their own career), as well as inoculating them

against career barriers (e.g., providing them with the ability to anticipate setbacks and giving

them the skills to cope or to adjust to their work environment), are likely to be essential

techniques when dealing with the stressor of destructive leadership (McLarnon & Rothstein,

2013; Vuori et al., 2012). Without career management skills, followers are likely to

“unwillingly stay” in their job, which may lead to longer term harm to their well-being

(Webster, Brough & Daly, 2016). While most SMT programs include cognitive-behavioural

techniques to build personal resilience, with a goal setting component, there are few research

studies into the efficacy of training programs to improve career resilience and career well-

being. The author could find no studies evaluating the impact of career resilience training on

individuals’ ability to cope with the stressor of destructive leadership.

98
Due to the psychological, emotional, physical, and career harm reported as a result of

being subjected to destructive leadership behaviours, it is proposed in this thesis that an

intervention that draws on a combination of stress management training techniques, based on

theoretical foundations of CBT, SIT, positive psychology, ACT, and career resilience, is

likely to be useful in improving follower well-being by enabling them to become more

resilient to destructive leadership behaviours. Such an intervention formed the basis of the

research study discussed in Chapter 6.

Challenges to implementation of individual follower interventions. It is difficult

to find a compromise between administering an effective intervention that employs

appropriate theoretical components with an optimal research design (e.g., adheres to a

randomised procedure when enrolling organisational employees to participate in a

longitudinal study), and one that facilitates active participation in an intervention that fulfils

organisational requirements and is sponsored by the executive (Nielsen, Taris, & Cox, 2010).

Individual interventions still dominate the stress management intervention literature, and

many such interventions include group training sessions (Richardson & Rothstein, 2008).

However, interventions with a preventative focus have had limited evaluation of outcomes

and any reported effects of the intervention on outcomes have been typically small (Murphy,

1996). This may be due to the challenges in conducting SMI research in organisations (Biggs

& Brough, 2015a; Brough & Biggs, 2015; Nielsen, 2000; Wickstrom, 2000). Implementing

the data collection activities in the workplace is often inconvenient and there can be many

constraints to address, for example, accommodating shift workers, or releasing staff from

operational duties for the intervention. Higher participation rates can often be gained by

holding the intervention on site; however, this can lead to interruptions when employees are

called away from sessions for work reasons.

99
Support from executive and local managers (champions) has been found to be crucial

to effective participation and implementation of interventions (Biggs & Brough, 2015a; Giga

et al., 2003). However, executive organisational priorities can change over the months of a

longitudinal study, and competing projects can be undertaken, altering the level of support

provided to the study. Commitment from employees is required for successful data

collection, especially for longitudinal designs, when there are often varying interests across

different parties. It can be difficult to find a control group for comparison, especially if

relying on third-party managers to recruit participants. “Waiting” control group participants

may feel data collection is too demanding when they are not participating in the intervention,

even if they will receive the program at a later date (Nielsen, 2000). Alternatively, it may not

be ethical to keep the control group waiting for the intervention long enough to carry out

longitudinal analyses (Pipe et al., 2009). These factors often negatively affect the ability to

include a control group and on adequacy of sample sizes.

Employees are not passive recipients to interventions. Therefore, the role of process

factors in intervention research has been highlighted (Biggs & Brough, 2015b). Lack of

support for the intervention, employee readiness to change, employee perceptions of the

value and relevance of the intervention, level of manager support, and adequacy of

managerial follow-up are all factors that influence implementation and participant outcomes

such as well-being (Dewe et al., 2010).

To address the potential barriers discussed above, interventions must be designed to

be practical, highly relevant to the workplace, and as interactive as possible. Teams with

high levels of manager support are more likely to be encouraged to implement the

intervention back in the workplace and this can mediate the effect of the intervention.

However, this may be difficult to influence if the manager’s behaviour is the stressor. In

addition to level of manager support, other external factors that have been identified as

100
uncontrollable are the need for executive sponsorship to implement an organisation-wide

intervention, the executive team choosing the departments/teams they wish to participate in

the intervention, managers selecting employees for participation, and the readiness of the

individuals selected to use the skills learned (Pipe et al., 2012; Wickstrom, 2000). These

challenges were considered by the researcher in relation to the design of an intervention to

enhance resiliency and coping skills in the context of destructive leadership.

Despite these implementation challenges, organisational/field research may be able to

identify and test potential solutions to coping with destructive leadership that are not possible

to examine in traditional experimental research. To determine whether this has been

achieved, the effectiveness of interventions to assist followers to manage the stressor of

destructive leadership must be evaluated. Due to the difficulty of controlling the influence of

external factors to the intervention process itself, it has been recommended that an evaluation

be made of the extent to which the intervention was implemented as intended, including

recording participation in the intervention (Biggs & Brough, 2015b; Nielsen & Abildgaard,

2013). Furthermore, SMI research has been criticised for the paucity of studies that conduct

follow-up assessment of the effectiveness of the interventions and, when follow-up

assessment is included, the variable range of outcome assessments employed, which prevents

easy comparison of intervention effectiveness (Lamontagne et al., 2007; van der Klink et al.,

2001). Finally, attention has been drawn to the importance of evaluating both micro

processes (e.g., increased participant resilience and well-being) and macro processes (e.g.,

participant attendance and participant appraisal of quality of intervention), when evaluating

the implementation of an individual-level, secondary prevention stress management

intervention (Biggs & Brough, 2015b). The specific evaluation factors, micro, and macro

processes discussed above were incorporated into the design of the follower intervention

discussed in Chapter 6.

101
Tertiary interventions to address harm to followers. It is acknowledged that while

interventions to build resilience to stressors may assist in preventing future harm, a number of

employees are likely to have already been targeted by destructive leadership practices during

the course of their career, with significant negative impact on their psychological, emotional,

and physical health (Lipman-Blumen, 2005). The efficacy of tertiary stress management

interventions is not the subject of this research thesis, but for the sake of completeness it is

included in this literature review. In addition to a consideration of primary and secondary

prevention interventions, tertiary interventions, such as providing employee assistance

programs and counselling support, may be useful in addressing the effects of harm caused by

the workplace stressor, destructive leadership. However, the evidence suggests their success

may be short lived (Berridge et al., 1992; Cooper & Cartwright, 1997; Dewe, 1994;

Ivancevich et al., 1990). A review of the literature suggests little research has been

conducted into how organisations address the negative impacts of destructive leadership

“after the fact” (Kellerman, 2004a; Lipman-Blumen, 2005). Mediation that involves bringing

the perpetrator and victim together as accused and accuser may not be appropriate or helpful

in the context of destructive leadership. One approach that may effectively address the

significant harm caused by exposure to destructive leaders is workplace conferencing, based

on a restorative justice approach. This approach has been employed to address the effects of

leader bullying (Thorsborne, 1999).

Principles of restorative justice have been applied to workplace dilemmas through

workplace conferencing, for example, when dealing with conflict or broken relationships in a

workplace context, addressing internal staff complaints and grievances, and/or inappropriate

behaviours from employees and managers as they arise (Davey, 2007; Moore, 1996;

Thorsborne, 1999; Wachtel, 1999). Restorative practices offer those who have been harmed

the opportunity to have the harm acknowledged and amends made. Sometimes this approach

102
is referred to as “transformational justice,” when it can do more than restore the person to the

way they were before, but goes on to create a healing and transformational experience for all

parties (Liebmann, 2007; Moore, 1996). With the formation in 1995 of Transformative

Justice Australia, workplace conferences have been conducted to deal with inadequate and

abusive management in organisations, including abusive supervision at a television station

and at a coal mine (Moore, 1996).

In addition, in the US, Fitzgerald (2006) pioneered the use of corporate circles to

build trust in organisations by addressing conflict, resolving disputes, and rebuilding

damaged workplace relations. Corporate circles, like workplace conferences, bring together

those people who are affected by abuse or bullying. Circles focus on providing a safe

environment, where participants sit in a circle and feel safe to share their perceptions about

what has been happening in the workplace, to express even negative feelings, and explain

how they have been impacted (e.g., by interpersonal conflict, group conflict). The group

discusses how best to repair any harm, and how to prevent future conflict (Fitzgerald, 2006).

There are some risks associated with using interventions based on restorative justice

principles in the context of destructive leadership. One key principle of restorative justice is

that the offender makes admission of the offending and takes responsibility for their actions

(Liebmann, 2007; Moore, 1996; Wallis & Tudor, 2008). If this does not happen, as is likely

with destructive leaders, the victim can feel abused again and further harmed. Where it is

deemed by the facilitator that there is a risk the offender will not admit offending, accept

accountability for their behaviour, or express remorse for their actions, two options are open:

indirect mediation between the parties, without a face-to-face meeting; or a workplace circle

between executive management and the victim (excluding the offending destructive leader),

with the organisational community accepting responsibility for the harm the organisation has

caused through the behaviour of the destructive leader and exploring ways to prevent it

103
happening again (Wallis & Tudor, 2008). This thesis focuses on strategies to prevent harm to

followers, rather than investigating post harm interventions, such as workplace conferencing

or corporate circles. However, restorative justice interventions present an alternative,

complementary approach to stress prevention interventions that warrants further research in

the context of destructive leadership (Webster & Brough, 2015).

Chapter Conclusion

This chapter discussed the role of followers in destructive leadership, including

factors that might make them susceptible to abuse. It provided an overview of theory and

research on follower responses to destructive leadership and how targets may choose to cope

with the psychological, emotional, and physical effects of destructive leadership behaviours.

Much is still to be learned about the interaction within the follower and destructive leader

dyad, with few field studies having investigated follower coping strategies in the wake of

destructive leadership. Conceptual papers propose that conformers are more likely to use

passive, submissive, or avoidant coping strategies, while colluders may be perceived by

destructive leaders as using constructive, assertive responses to their leadership. However, if

colluders over time decide to undermine or overthrow the destructive leader, they may be

perceived by the leader as exhibiting aggressive responses, and the leader is then likely to feel

justified in retaliating. Irrespective of the follower’s coping response, if the leader does not

have the resources to choose a constructive response, they will continue to engage in

destructive leadership behaviours, and harm is likely to be caused to followers’ well-being.

Possible interventions to reduce harm to followers were discussed, including an intervention

to build follower resilience to destructive leadership behaviours, based on principles from

multiple theoretical frameworks: CBT, SIT, positive psychology, ACT, and career resilience.

This chapter concludes the literature review of the theoretical frameworks relevant to

antecedents to engagement by leaders in destructive leadership behaviours, follower

104
responses to destructive leadership, and theories that inform strategies to mitigate the impact

of destructive leadership on followers. These theoretical typologies and frameworks guide

our understanding of the causes and effects of destructive leadership, and followers’

responses to it, but there is a dearth of field studies that have tested their efficacy and validity.

The purpose of this thesis is to address the research questions (refer p. 13) by

undertaking research that contributes to practical, realistic interventions that practitioners can

employ that will assist in addressing the issue of destructive leadership in organisations. The

next three chapters describe each of three studies conducted as part of this thesis: Study 1, a

quantitative, cross-sectional analysis of archival data, investigating the traits that may

predispose or inhibit leader engagement in destructive leadership behaviours; Study 2, a

cross-sectional, mixed methods analysis of the coping strategies followers report after

experiencing leadership behaviours they perceive as toxic; and Study 3, a longitudinal, quasi-

experimental study, which trials and evaluates an intervention designed to assist followers

employed in a healthcare organisation to maintain well-being through building resiliency,

within the context of destructive leadership.

105
Chapter 4. Antecedents to derailing leadership traits: The mediating roles of negative

affectivity and self-regulation traits (Study 1)

Statement of contribution to co-authored published paper

This chapter includes a co-authored paper. The status details of the

co-authored paper, including all authors, are:

Webster, V., Brough, P., & Drummond, S. (submitted). Antecedents to derailing leadership

traits: The mediating roles of negative affectivity and self-regulation traits. Submitted to

Group and Organization Management.

My contribution to the paper involved the design of the study, the collection and

provision of data, providing direction on the data analysis, and as primary author in writing the

journal article.

A copy of the abstract is provided in Appendix B.

(Signed) (Date) 26th October 2015

Vicki Webster (Corresponding Author)

(Countersigned) (Date) 21st October 2015


Supervisor: Paula Brough

(Countersigned) (Date) 31st March 2015

Co-Author: Suzanne Drummond

106
In spite of all the work on leadership that assumes it by definition to be good… we

exercise power, authority and influence in ways that do harm. This harm is not

necessarily deliberate. It can be the result of carelessness or neglect. But this does

not make it less injurious and, in some cases, calamitous. (Kellerman, 2004 p. xiii).

The past three decades has produced an increase in research assessing the “dark side”

of leadership performance under the research stream of destructive leadership; variously

described as abusive, derailing, toxic, and tyrannical leadership (e.g., Einarsen, et al., 2010;

Hogan & Hogan, 2001; Martinko et al., 2013). Most research within the destructive

leadership field has focused on the outcomes of destructive leadership (e.g., Tepper, 2007;

Webster et al., 2016), while the antecedents of destructive leadership have received

considerably less attention. It is clear, for example, that destructive leadership behaviours are

often employed by ‘toxic’ leaders to resolve goal blockages that thwart their own

achievement aspirations (e.g., Bardes & Piccolo, 2010). Leaders with a high need for

independence and personal achievement are especially likely to perceive non-attainment of

goals as a significant failure, and anticipated failure commonly produces negative emotions

that can lead to engagement in destructive behaviours (McClelland & Boyatzis, 1982).

The research reported in this article extends current discussions examining specific

personality traits of leaders associated with negative leadership performance. In these

discussions maladaptive individual differences have been conceptualised as extreme variants

of adaptive or “bright side” traits. Theoretical explanations of the “dark side of leadership”

describe the common negative personality characteristics exhibited by destructive leaders

(Judge et al., 2009), including for example, the ‘Dark Triad’ of personality traits consisting of

narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy (Boddy, 2015). Indeed, some elements of

personality disorders, such as narcissistic traits, are strikingly similar to characteristics

actively sought in organisational leaders (i.e., aggressiveness, alertness, dominance,

107
enthusiasm, extroversion, independence, risk taking, and self-confidence; Board & Fritzon,

2005). Dark side leadership personality traits are often assessed with the Big Five personality

test (Costa & McCrae, 1992) and specifically consist of low levels of agreeableness,

conscientiousness, and openness (Judge & LePine, 2007). Negative affectivity, the tendency

of an individual to differentially dwell on the negative aspects of themselves and their life, to

experience negative emotions such as anxiety, fear, and anger, and to react impulsively to

such emotions, has also been positively associated with destructive leadership performance

(Schyns, & Schilling, 2013; Tepper, 2007; Watson & Clark, 1984). Individuals with high

levels of the trait negative affectivity are particularly sensitive to the minor failures and

frustrations of daily life. In contrast, the ‘bright side’ leadership personality is defined as the

ability to build and maintain a high performing team and is characterized by the Big Five

personality traits of high levels of extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness and

openness, and low levels of neuroticism (Hogan et al., 1994).

Recent research attention has focused on the proposed curvilinear relationship

between leadership trait characteristics and performance/leadership effectiveness. The key

argument here is that leadership traits and behaviours that may be crucial to success in one

situation may be unrelated or even negatively related to success in other contexts; i.e., the

dark side of bright characteristics and the bright side of dark characteristics (e.g., Furnham,

Trickey, & Hyde, 2012; Kaiser et al., 2015). More specifically, it has been suggested that a

leader’s personality traits exhibit a curvilinear relationship with their followers’ levels of

engagement, well-being and performance. That is, there are negative consequences for both

low levels and high levels of bright side personality traits and positive consequences for

moderate levels of dark side personality traits (e.g., Judge, et al., 2009). For example, high

levels of assertiveness, a person’s tendency to pursue and speak out for their own interests,

may bring short term goal achievement, but is also likely to have an adverse effect on

108
relationships over time (Ames & Flynn, 2007). One aspect which has received surprisingly

scant attention within these discussions is the ability of leaders to self-regulate when they

have high levels of these ‘bright side’ and ‘dark side’ traits. We argue here that self-

regulation is a crucial element for both the prediction of leader derailment and the

management of destructive leadership performance.

The Role of Self-Regulation in the “dark side” of Leadership

Self-regulatory processes, such as emotional control (also referred to as emotion

management or emotion regulation; Lawrence, Troth, Jordan, & Collins, 2011) have been

theorized to result in functional or dysfunctional outcomes, depending on an individual’s

ability to regulate negative emotions in response to organisational stressors (Jordan &

Lindebaum, 2015; Ruderman, et al., 2001). Low levels of stress tolerance (i.e., the inability

to tolerate stressful situations without undue physical or emotional reactions) and low levels

of self-awareness (i.e., lack of ability to read one’s emotions and motivations and understand

their effects on others) have both been associated with self-regulation impairment (Wang, et

al., 2010). For example, leaders low in stress tolerance are more likely to have intense

negative reactions to stressors, such as goal blockage, leading to impulsive, ineffective

responses. Furnham et al. (2012) demonstrated in a sample of British working adults that the

trait of stress tolerance was significantly negatively related to “dark side” traits. In addition,

leaders low in self-awareness are unlikely to be aware of their limitations, which may lead

them to take on unachievable goals or prevent them delivering the outcomes they desire.

They are also more likely to interpret constructive feedback as a threat or a sign of failure and

become angry or dismissive, resorting to destructive behaviours (Goleman, 2004). In a test

of this specific association, Harms, et al. (2011) demonstrated that self-awareness training

was effective in mitigating the negative effects of destructive personality traits within a

sample of military cadets. However, empirical research testing these theories in the specific

109
context of organisational leadership is surprisingly scarce and this is one limitation which is

directly addressed by the current research.

We suggest that an individual’s ability to self-regulate their behaviours in the

achievement of goals, is an important characteristic of successful leaders. Leaders with high

levels of independence and negative affectivity, and with self-regulation impairment are more

likely to react to goal blockage by choosing to engage in destructive leadership behaviours.

This study therefore investigates the (indirect) effect that negative affectivity and three

common self-regulation traits (self-awareness, emotional control, and stress tolerance) have

upon the likelihood of leaders engaging in destructive behaviours.

Leader independence

A leader’s desire for independence has typically been described as one of the “bright

side” personality traits, particularly in Western society, where hierarchical power with a focus

on individual achievement is rewarded (Fletcher & Kaufer, 2002). In this study the trait of

independence is defined as a desire for autonomy and decision-making, without input from

others (Schmit, et al., 2000). Independence has been incorporated under the Big Five factors

as being related to openness to experience (Schmit et al., 2000) and emotional stability

(Hogan et al., 1994), and is commonly associated with effective leadership performance (e.g.,

Judge, Bono, Illies, & Gerhadt, 2002).

It is paradoxical that, as leaders emerge based on their independence and personal

achievements, they are promoted into positions that require them to be interdependent, to

influence and work through others, so that to be effective they can no longer rely solely on

their own skills and efforts (Fletcher & Kaufer, 2002). A shift in focus in the literature to

models of shared leadership has emphasised the importance of interdependence. The trait of

interdependence is defined as a person’s preference to work toward the goals of the group,

rather than individual goals, in order to effectively achieve team and organisational goals

110
(Fletcher & Kaufer, 2002; Schmit et al., 2000). There is also an established link between

personality traits and motivation, that is, that individuals high in independence are likely to be

motivated by personalised power, such as the achievement of personal goals (McClelland &

Boyatzis, 1982), as compared to individuals high in interdependence, who are more likely to

be movitated by socialised power, achieving their goals through others (McClelland, 1970).

Leaders with a strong need for independence are likely to ignore others’ points of view or

requests, and become irritated or argumentative if others persist (Hogan & Hogan, 2001). If

the leader remains wedded to their own agenda and fails to listen to others they become

susceptible to derailment (Hogan et al., 1994). Thus leaders with high levels of independence

may be predisposed to exhibit ‘dark side’ behaviours because they are more likely to be

motivated to use any means at their disposal to achieve their personal goals, and to prioritise

their personal goals over the needs of the group (Hogan et al., 1994; Krasikova, et al., 2013).

However, research to test the hypothesised direct relationship between high levels of

independence and the predisposition to exhibit derailing behaviours is surprisingly scarce.

Similarly, the impact of the indirect role of negative affectivity and/or self-regulation traits on

the negative effects of high levels of “bright side” traits (e.g., assertiveness; Ames & Flynn,

2007) is also scarce.

In the current research it is specifically predicted:

Hypothesis 1: High levels of independence will be positively associated with

leadership derailment, and this association will be increased by negative affectivity (H1a), but

reduced (mediated) by self-regulation traits (H1b).

The hypothesized relationships between independence and leadership derailing traits

are illustrated on Figure 7.

111
Self-Regulation Traits
Negative Affectivity d
 Emotional Control b
 Self-Awareness

a
Leadership Derailing

c’ Traits
Independence
 Ego-Centred
 Intimidating
 Manipulating
Figure 7. Hypothesized main effect and mediating relationships.

The Role of Negative Affectivity in Destructive Leadership

As noted above, a pertinent area of recent research in the destructive leadership field

is the impact that the trait negative affectivity has upon the levels of psychological stress

experienced by leaders and consequentially, their good/bad performance outcomes.

Individuals with high levels of negative affectivity are typically viewed as more independent,

are more likely to perceive failure as a stressor, and in stress situations tend to exhibit a

decreased need for affiliation (Watson & Clark, 1984). Krasikova et al. (2013) proposed that

negative affectivity (e.g., anxiety and worry) is an important antecedent to a range of

destructive leadership behaviours. Krasikova et al. (2013) suggested that if a leader perceives

the progress from their current state towards their desired goal is too slow, levels of negative

affectivity increase, stimulating actions to reduce this goal attainment discrepancy. In this

context, increased negative affectivity is associated with either the anticipation of impending

adversities, or the anticipation of an inability to attract rewards, resulting in an increased

predisposition to engage in destructive behaviours.


112
Negative affectivity is commonly defined as reducing an individual’s ability to self-

regulate their behaviours and reactions (Magno, 2010). Leaders with high levels of negative

affectivity are likely to experience greater impulsivity, a key characteristic of destructive

leadership performance (Schyns & Schilling, 2013). Conversely, leaders with low levels of

negative affectivity are less likely to have intense negative reactions to stressors, and are

more likely to engage in self-regulation processes when under stress or when fatigued. These

hypothesized relationships between negative affectivity, self-regulation and leadership

derailing outcomes are illustrated on Figure 1. Specifically, it is predicted:

Hypothesis 2: Negative affectivity will be negatively associated with each self-

regulation trait (self-awareness, stress tolerance, and emotional control) and this association

will mediate the effect of the self-regulation traits on the relationship between independence

and derailing leadership outcomes.

Study 1 Method

Participants and Procedure

Archival data consisting of leadership profiles collected from N = 300 organisational

managers during processes for candidate selection, promotion, and leadership development

was utilized by this research. Data were collected from individuals across a heterogeneous

sample of Australian industries and occupations, with the most common sectors consisting of

commercial, not for profit, and government owned corporations (n = 210 respondents; 70%).

Public sector organisations consisted of federal, state, and local government departments and

agencies (n = 90 respondents; 30%). The data were collected over two years (2007 to 2008)

via a self-report online survey. Limited demographic details of the respondents were made

available to the researchers, limiting the level of analysis that could be conducted, such as

controlling for tenure or age. Over half of the respondents were male (n = 192; 64%) and

113
most were Australian (n = 289; 96%), with 11 participants (4%) from New Zealand, the UK,

South Africa and Asia.

Materials

Global Personality Inventory (GPI). Eleven subscales (84 items) of the Global

Personality Inventory (GPI; Schmit, et al., 2000) were included in this study. The GPI

describes personality traits based on the traditional Big Five model of personality, and also

assesses a composite “dark side” scale Derailing Leadership. Independence (8 items; a =

.67) was measured as the independent variable, e.g., “I like the freedom to decide how things

are going to be done.” The five facet scales that make up the Derailing Leadership composite

were used as the dependent variables: ego-centred (7 items; a = .64), e.g., “I am continually

forced to work with simple-minded people”; manipulating (10 items; a = .73), e.g., “There

are people who deserve to be used for my own purposes”; intimidating (7 items; a = .56) e.g.,

“People are afraid to question or contradict me”; micro-managing (7 items; a = .53), e.g.,

“Things get done more efficiently when I do them myself”; and passive-aggressive (7 items;

a = .60), e.g., “I find that it is just as effective to block an idea by doing nothing to support it

as it is to publicly disagree with the idea.” The mediating variables consisted of: negative

affectivity (7 items; a = .58), e.g., “I am dissatisfied with what I have achieved so far”; and

three self-regulation subscales: emotional control (7 items; a = .75), e.g., “Even when I am

upset it is easy for me to control my emotions”; stress tolerance (8 items; a = .79), e.g., “I

worry about things that I know I should not worry about”; and self-awareness (9 items; a =

.76), e.g., “I know what motivates me”

Given the purpose of this data collection, the desire to impress may have influenced

respondents’ answers. The impressing facet scale (7 items; a = .48), e.g., “It is always best to

keep important people happy”, was therefore also included to control for any bias in the

analysis (Schmit, et al., 2000). The respondents indicated their level of agreement with each

114
of these GPI items on a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (strongly agree) to 5

(strongly disagree; Previsor, 2001). Construct validity has been demonstrated by the scale

developers as well as in other leadership research (e.g., Benson & Campbell, 2007; Schmit, et

al., 2000).

Data Analysis

The tests of mediation were undertaken via bootstrapping using the PROCESS macro

for SPSS (Hayes, 2012). Following recent advances in the literature regarding the testing of

indirect effects (Hayes, 2013), a significant indirect effect is deemed to be present when the

confidence intervals for the indirect effect do not contain zero. For each of the bootstrapped

analyzes, 95% bias corrected confidence intervals and 5,000 bootstrap resamples were

specified. Serial mediation was tested with negative affectivity in the M1 position and the

self-regulation variables added as mediators in the M2 position, between independence and

each of the five derailing traits, with the control variable, impressing, included as a covariate.

As this serial model only enables one mediator in the second position to be tested at one time

(e.g., IV  M1  M2  DV), three separate analyzes were conducted for each DV,

substituting one of the three self-regulation variables in the M2 position each time.

Study 1 Results

Descriptive Results

The majority of respondents (n = 185; 62%) reported a strong predisposition for

exhibiting derailing leadership behaviours, reporting elevated levels of Derailing Leadership

traits (70% or higher compared to the norm of Australian managers), e.g., ego-centred (n =

58; 19%), intimidating (n = 86; 29%), manipulative (n = 87; 29%), micro-managing (n = 83;

28%), and passive-aggressive (n = 59; 20%). The means, standard deviations, and

correlations between the research variables are displayed in Table 7. As expected,

115
independence was positively related to the derailing leadership traits (Hypothesis 1).

Independence was positively related to negative affectivity (H1a) and negatively related to

the self-regulation variables (H1b). Also as expected, negative affectivity was negatively

associated with each self-regulation trait (Hypothesis 2). The self-regulation traits were

negatively associated with four of the derailing leadership traits. Interestingly, ego-centred

was not significantly correlated with any of the self-regulation traits, but was positively

correlated with negative affectivity. The significant associations between impressing and all

dependent variables (except intimidating) justified its inclusion as a covariate (control

variable) in the bootstrap analyzes.

The Mediating Role of Negative Affectivity and Self-Regulation Traits

In the serial mediation analysis, independence was found to have a significant direct

effect on the derailing leadership traits: ego-centred (c’ = .51, p < .001), intimidating (c’ =

.34, p < .001), micromanaging (c’ = .32, p < .001), passive-aggressive (c’ = .20, p < .01), and

manipulating (c’ = .30, p < .001).

A summary of the significant results for the serial mediation analyses are presented

for each of the dependent variables in Table 8 (full results are available on request). The

traits of self-awareness and stress tolerance were found to significantly mediate the

relationship between independence and some of the leadership derailing traits. Emotional

control was not found to mediate these relationships. No indirect pathways predicted ego-

centredness. Significant serial indirect effects were observed for self-awareness with three

dependent variables: intimidating, micromanaging, and passive-aggressiveness, as well as for

stress tolerance with micromanaging and manipulating. As serial mediation posits a causal

link between two (or more) mediators, it is necessary to rule out whether their relationship

may be due to their shared common cause (i.e., the independent variable; Hayes, 2013).

116
Table 7
Correlations between the research variables of interest (N = 300)

M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

1. Impressing (control) 3.04 1.06

2. Independence (IV) 2.66 1.08 .10

Mediation Variables

3. Emotional Control 3.52 1.11 .27*** -.17**

4. Self-Awareness 3.29 1.02 .19** -.17** .42***

5. Stress Tolerance 3.45 1.15 .11 -.18** .54*** .40***

6. Negative Affectivity 2.57 1.22 -.09 .22*** -.37*** -.45*** -.37***

Dependent Variables

7. Ego-Centred 2.58 1.30 .22*** .46*** -.05 -.04 -.05 .17**

8. Intimidating 3.03 1.20 .04 .36*** -.12* -.23*** -.12* .30*** .37***

9. Manipulating 2.96 1.25 .15* .34*** -.17** -.21*** -.23*** .23*** .45*** .43***

10. Micromanaging 3.05 1.09 .13* .41*** -.27*** -.28*** -.34*** .26*** .38*** .29*** .41***

11. Passive-aggressive 2.86 1.11 .16** .29*** -.15** -.29*** -.21*** .26*** .23*** .25*** .47*** .26***

Note. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

117
Table 8

Unstandardized paths and significant indirect effects for the serial mediation models

IV Mediator 1 Mediator 2 DV a d b c’ ab SE CI LL, UL


ab
Independence Negative Affectivity Self-Awareness Intimidating .167** -.242*** -.143 .335*** .006 .004 .001, .017

Independence Negative Affectivity Self-Awareness Micromanaging .167** -.242*** -.139* .318*** .006 .003 .001, .016

Independence Negative Affectivity Self-Awareness Passive-aggressive .167** -.242*** -.229** .197** .009 .005 .003, .023

Independence Negative Affectivity Stress Tolerance Micromanaging .142* -.132* -.169** .318*** .003 .002 .001, .011

Independence Negative Affectivity Stress Tolerance Manipulating .142* -.132* -.123 .301*** .002 .002 .0001, .009

Note. CI = 95% bias corrected confidence interval. a = IV  Mediator 1; d = Mediator 1  Mediator 2; b = Mediator 2  DV; c’ = direct

effect IV  DV; ab = indirect effect. All indirect effects are significant.

*
p<.05; **p<.01; ***p<.001.

118
Therefore, prior to analyzing the serial mediation models, the partial correlations between

negative affectivity and the two significant self-regulation variables were examined after

controlling for independence. The results revealed negative affectivity remained significantly

correlated with each of the two self-regulation variables after controlling for their shared

predictor: self-awareness r = -.43, p < .001; and stress tolerance r = -.35, p < .001. All of the

indirect effects were positive, such that independence increased derailing leadership traits

through a process of increasing negative affectivity which decreased self-regulation traits

(self-awareness and stress tolerance). No significant effects were found for emotional

control. These results provide partial support for the hypothesised relationships (Hypothesis

2).

As this study is cross-sectional in nature, the serial mediating variables were also

tested in reverse order to provide further support for our hypothesized direction of

relationships (Magno, 2010). Negative affectivity and each of the self-regulation variables

were tested in reverse order, such that independence  self-regulation  negative affectivity

 derailing behaviours. Impressing was again included as a control covariate. The results

revealed no significant relationships, thereby providing support for the direction of

relationships tested in the Hypothesis.

Study 1 Discussion

This research explored the role that negative affectivity and self-regulatory traits play

in mediating the relationship between independence and a predisposition to engage in specific

derailing leadership behaviours. Our results confirmed the complexity of the relationships.

The hypothesised main effect between independence and the derailing leadership traits was

confirmed (Hypothesis 1), while partial support was produced for the mediation hypothesis

(Hypothesis 2). Self-awareness and stress tolerance did significantly mediate the relationship

between independence and some of the derailing behaviours, after negative affectivity was

119
included as a mediator. However, emotional control was not found to be a significant

mediator between independence and derailing leadership traits. No effects were found for

leaders with a predisposition to be ego-centred.

For leaders reporting high levels of independence and negative affectivity, a

predisposition to be intimidating, micro-managing, or passive-aggressive was increased by

their reduced (suppressed) level of self-awareness. One explanation for this pattern of results

may be that negative affectivity makes factors that lead to intimidating, micro-managing, or

passive-aggressive behaviours more salient (i.e., fear of conflict, fear of loss of control),

reducing the individuals’ ability to have insight into the ineffectiveness of these responses.

Another explanation is that as the discrepancy between the current and desired future state

broadens, and as leaders’ anxiety about perceived blockage to achievement of their personal

goals increases, their ability for self-reflection is impaired. Self-awareness did not

significantly mediate the hypothesised path from independence to manipulation. It could be

that leaders reporting a strong preference to engage in manipulative behaviours have a good

level of self-awareness into their own motivations, actions, and the outcomes they desire, so

that negative affectivity does not suppress their self-awareness or their focus on achieving

their agenda.

In relation to the mediator of stress tolerance, the results are more difficult to

interpret. For leaders reporting a strong preference to be independent and to be micro-

managing or manipulating, the more negative affectivity they reported the less predisposed

they were to maintain stress tolerance and to regulate their behaviour. However, for leaders

reporting a strong preference for passive-aggressive or intimidating behaviours, neither

negative affectivity nor stress tolerance mediated the relationship between independence and

these potentially derailing behaviours. It may be that in stressful situations passive-

aggressive leaders maintain positive affect by avoiding all engagement or confrontation with

129
the stressor, thus negating any mediation effects. For those with a preference to be

intimidating, they may maintain positive affect by using intimidation to overcome any

resistance or “bulldoze” their way through any obstacles encountered, and this enables them

to achieve their outcomes even in stressful situations, thereby negating any mediation effects.

Despite these mixed results, our findings expand Krasikova et al.’s (2013) work by

demonstrating that when self-regulation traits were included in the serial mediation models,

negative affectivity was influential in increasing the effects of four of the derailing traits

(intimidation, passive-aggressiveness, micromanaging, and manipulating) through reducing

the effects of the self-regulation mediating traits of self-awareness and stress tolerance.

It was hypothesised that emotional control would reduce engagement in derailing

leadership behaviours, e.g., that regulation of negative emotions in response to workplace

stressors would result in functional outcomes (Jordan & Lindebaum, 2015; Lawrence et al.,

2011). Contrary to expectations, this association was not found to be significant. In this

study only one component of emotional regulation (suppression regulation) was analyzed. If

enacting emotional control failed to decrease the intensity of the emotion being experienced

(i.e., anger or frustration), then attempting to suppress such emotion may actually amplify the

experience and increase the likelihood of an impulsive, dysfunctional response.

Negative affectivity and the self-regulation traits were not found to mediate the

relationship between independence and a predisposition for ego-centredness. These findings

may be explained by the narcissism literature. Leaders showing narcissistic traits are

typically highly self-confident, with an inflated view of their own abilities, and are unlikely to

consider others’ viewpoints or to engage in self-reflection (Padilla, et al., 2007; Vazire &

Funder, 2006). In their efforts to manage the impressions of others and achieve their own

self-interests such leaders are likely to employ the tactics required to achieve their goals,

believing that they are justified in their behaviours towards others, irrespective of the

121
circumstances (House & Howell, 1992). As a result, they are unlikely to develop insight into

the impact of their behaviour, nor are they likely to see the need to regulate or change their

behaviour, even in stressful situations.

An alternative explanation, posited by Campbell, et al. (2011), is that leaders with

narcissistic traits employ different self-regulatory mechanisms. This explanation is supported

by the descriptive statistics in this study: ego-centredness was not significantly correlated

with any of the self-regulation variables. Primary motivators of narcissistic behaviours

include self-enhancement, a sense of entitlement, and a need for power. Narcissists are more

focused on approaching success or sensation seeking, than avoiding failure or minimizing

anxiety. Therefore, they will be less amenable to self-regulatory strategies that present a

different view of their capabilities to their own inflated view (building self-awareness), or to

dealing with stress-induced anxiety (stress-tolerance). Leaders with high levels of ego-

centredness are more likely to use narcissistic self-regulatory strategies, such as bragging,

seeking fame, and using self-serving biases to build pride, esteem, and excitement when their

goals are met, and expressing anger and aggression when their goals are thwarted. These

research findings suggest sub-clinical narcissism should continue to be researched

independently, as a sub-category of destructive leadership, to validate theoretical antecedents

and self-regulatory processes that specifically predispose leaders to engage in ego-centred,

narcissistic behaviours.

Practical Implications

Given the strong focus on achievement of results in most organisations, this study

highlights the importance of selecting and developing leaders who have, not only a need for

independence and individual achievement, but also the self-regulation traits to be successful,

without overusing their strengths in a way that may derail them. Based on these findings it is

recommended that organisations continue to include personality assessment as an objective

122
measure to screen out individuals reporting extremely high levels of ego-centredness or

negative affectivity. Such assessment can also advise whether an individual reporting higher

levels of independence, manipulation, intimidation, micromanaging, and/or passive-

aggressiveness has the self-awareness and stress tolerance required to effectively manage the

effects of such tendencies (e.g., Judge & LePine, 2007). Doing so can minimize the risk of

employing or promoting a leader who may resort to destructive behaviours under pressure.

Limitations and Future Research

As with all studies utilizing cross-sectional data, true causality among variables

cannot be established. We are, however, confident in the direction of relationships tested in

this research because, not only are the hypotheses consistent with theory, but non-significant

results were produced when the serial mediating variables were reversed. However, we

acknowledge that longitudinal research designs tracking the impact of these personality traits

and mediators is necessary to empirically demonstrate causality.

A second research limitation concerns the reliance solely on self-report measures,

which presents a potential issue of common method bias (Podsakoff, MacKenzie, &

Podsakoff, 2012). However, a review of misconceptions regarding common method bias has

challenged the view that relationships between self-reported variables are necessarily and

routinely upwardly biased (Conway & Lance, 2010). Self-reports are deemed appropriate for

analysing private events. In this case the constructs of interest related to innate dispositions

(e.g., leader personality traits), and as a result self-report measures were considered

appropriate to generate accurate representations of respondents’ inner thoughts, affect, and

motivations.

Conclusions

The results presented here extend current knowledge of the antecedents of five

specific destructive leadership traits. Leaders with both dispositional negative affectivity and

123
a strong need for independence are more likely to be predisposed to derailing behaviours

through reduced self-regulatory processes. That is, they have the potential to use derailing

behaviours to ensure achievement of their personal goals. These relationships were not

evident however, for ego-centred traits, suggesting these individuals will be particularly

resistant to feedback, and are unlikely to be able to modify their reactions when achievement

of their goals is challenged. Further research in this field, especially the inclusion of

longitudinal research designs, is recommended to increase our understanding of the

antecedent traits or combination of traits that predict leader derailment over time.

124
Chapter 5. Fight, flight or freeze: Common responses for follower coping with toxic

leadership (Study 2).

This chapter includes a co-authored paper. The bibliographic details of the

co-authored paper, including all authors, are:

Webster, V., Brough, P., & Daly, K. (2016). Fight, flight or freeze: Common responses for

follower coping with toxic leadership. Stress & Health. DOI: 10.1002/smi.2626.

My contribution to the paper involved the design of the study and qualitative survey,

the collection and provision of data, providing direction on the preliminary analysis and

categorisation of the data, data analysis, and as primary author in writing the journal article.

A copy of the abstract is provided in Appendix B.

(Signed) (Date) 26th October 2015

Vicki Webster (Corresponding Author)

(Countersigned) (Date) 21st October 2015


Supervisor: Paula Brough

(Countersigned) (Date) 1st October 2015

Supervisor: Kathleen Daly

Acknowledgement

The authors would like to acknowledge the contribution of Brett Myors in designing this

study and Tristan Casey for his assistance with the data analysis.

125
The considerable coping literature describes how employees manage a variety of

workplace stressors, but few studies have focused on understanding how followers cope with

the specific stressor of toxic leadership behaviour. Toxic leadership, also described as

abusive supervision and destructive leadership (Krasikova et al., 2013; Lipman-Blumen,

2005; Tepper, 2007), describes leaders who engage in a consistent range of negative

behaviours that, if systematic and repeated, cause psychological harm for their followers

(Pelletier, 2010). Toxic behaviours include, but are not limited to, intimidating, bullying,

manipulating (Machiavellianism), micromanaging, arrogance (narcissism), and engaging in

abusive or unethical behaviour. For affected followers toxic leadership behaviours are

associated with psychological distress (e.g., anxiety and depression); emotional harm (e.g.,

emotional exhaustion, fear, and social isolation); and physical health problems (e.g., chronic

fatigue and insomnia; Einarsen et al., 2010; Kelloway & Barling, 2010; Tepper, 2007). Field

studies investigating the coping strategies of affected employees are limited, despite repeated

calls for this research (Martinko et al., 2013; Pelletier, 2010; Yagil et al., 2011). The current

research directly responds to these calls by assessing the specific coping strategies adopted by

employees who experience toxic leadership.

Coping Frameworks

Little consensus can be found among coping researchers on how to conceptualize,

classify and measure coping behaviours (Skinner et al., 2003). At the highest level coping

has been referred to as an adaptive process, sometimes unconscious and involuntary, that

intervenes between a stressor and its psychological, emotional and physiological outcomes,

for example the fight, flight or freeze response to stress (Skinner et al. 2003; Vaillant, 1977).

At the lowest level coping responses are referred to as numerous real-time actions individuals

take to deal with stressful situations (Skinner et al., 2003). Researchers are challenged to

construct an organising coping structure that spans the conceptual space between higher level

126
adaptive processes and specific coping instances. One example of such an organising coping

structure is Skinner et al.’s (2003) four level hierarchical coping structure, consisting of:

adaptive processes, families of coping, ways of coping, and coping instances. Based on their

meta-analysis of existing coping literature, Skinner et al. proposed a theoretical structure of

12 families of coping as an intermediate level between adaptive processes and ways of

coping. Their structure denotes that lower order categories are nested within the higher order

coping categories, rather than as a delineation between good and bad, healthy and harmful, or

mature and immature ways of coping (Valliant, 1977). Skinner et al.’s typology has been

independently validated (e.g., Koole, 2009) and provides a useful organizing structure for

analysing coping responses to destructive leadership.

To the best of our knowledge, there is only one coping scale specifically focussed on

coping instances of followers in response to toxic leadership. Yagil et al. (2011) utilised

Lazarus and Folkman’s coping model to develop a specific coping measure for dealing with

abusive supervision at work. Yagil et al.’s scale consisted of five coping strategies:

ingratiation, direct communication, avoidance of contact, support-seeking, and reframing

(Harvey et al., 2007; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). Yagil et al.’s study suggested that as

employees experienced high levels of abuse they tended to disengage with the leader and use

avoidance tactics (i.e., emotion-focused coping; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984), which was

related to high negative affect. Seeking emotional support was also related to increased

negative affect. Although associated with positive affect, few respondents reported using

problem solving coping responses (i.e., problem-focused coping; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984).

Yagil et al. concluded that followers do not have solutions on how to cope effectively with

abusive supervision, and followers’ reliance on support seeking or avoidance responses

appeared to prevent them utilising problem-focused coping strategies. This study emphasised

127
the importance of better understanding the coping strategies followers commonly employ in

response to this stressor in order to be able to assist them to access effective coping strategies.

Similarly, Skinner et al. argued that some coping instances fall outside the higher

order categorisation of problem-focused and emotion-focused coping. Four of Skinner et

al.’s coping families demonstrate apparent construct overlap with Yagil et al.’s (2011)

theoretical framework: problem solving (direct communication); support seeking (support

seeking), escape (avoidance of contact) and accommodation (reframing). Skinner et al.’s

additional coping families consist of: information seeking, self-reliance, negotiation, as well

as perceived maladaptive coping responses of helplessness, delegation, isolation, submission,

and opposition. Skinner et al. suggested such responses may be viewed as an indicator of a

stressor the individual cannot handle and, depending on the circumstances and the personal

resources available to an individual, such coping responses may be regarded as locally

adaptive.

In consideration of the above, the current research included both Yagil et al.’s (2011)

coping scale and Skinner et al.’s (2003) coping structure to categorise the coping responses

reported by followers exposed to toxic leadership. This study, therefore, aims to understand

the coping strategies employed by individuals who perceive their leader’s behaviour to be

harmful. This research adopted a mixed method approach via the use of open ended survey

questions and data analyses based on two theoretical coping models. The key aim of this

study is to understand how well these theoretical coping frameworks explain the coping

responses adopted by employees adversely affected by toxic leader behaviours. The results

of this study have practical implications for how organisations can facilitate the use of

effective coping strategies when they respond to victims of abusive leader conduct.

128
Study 2 Method

Participants and Procedure

A purposive sample of individuals who perceived they had worked or were working

for a toxic leader was invited to participate in an electronic survey. Given that victims of

destructive leadership are often missed in organisational research, as they are too fearful to

participate or have left the organisation (Lipman-Blumen, 2005), this study sought volunteers

across multiple organisations. This sampling method also controlled for the effect of other

potential confounds, such as specific organisational cultures and work contexts (Padilla et al.,

2007; Tepper, 2007). The survey hyperlink was advertised by seven professional human

resources, psychology, and university associations across Australia and New Zealand to their

members. Of the self-selected respondents, N = 76 participants reported behaviours

consistent with toxic leader behaviour typologies and completed the survey. The majority of

these respondents were female (n = 54; 70%) and predominantly Australian 22 (n = 73;

96%). Most respondents were aged between 30 years and 49 years (n = 46; 70%). Eighteen

respondents (24%) held undergraduate qualifications and 44 respondents (58%) had post-

graduate qualifications. Forty-two respondents (55%) were employed in the public sector, 24

respondents (32%) were employed in the private sector, and 10 respondents (13%) were

employed in the not- for-profit sector. The main industries represented were education (n =

21; 28%), healthcare (n = 14; 18%), professional services (n = 9; 9%), and financial

services/accounting (n = 8; 11%). Job tenure ranged from less than five years to over 16

years, with a modal tenure of less than five years (n = 44; 58%). Forty-two respondents

(55%) no longer worked for the organisation where the reported incidents happened.

Survey Questions

In order to confirm the behaviours that followers perceived to be harmful, and to

avoid influencing their responses, no formal definition of toxic leadership was provided to

129
respondents. The survey consisted of six open-ended qualitative questions focused on three

areas of interest: (1) specific behaviours that toxic leaders demonstrate (e.g., “What specific

behaviours did they [the leader] demonstrate?”); (2) the impact of that behaviour on the

respondent (e.g., “What impact did the incident/s have on you personally?”); (3) the coping

strategies followers employed to cope with toxic leadership behaviours (e.g., “How have you

coped, both at the time of the incident/s and since?”). Participants were invited to respond

via free text to each question.

Data Analysis

A mixed method analysis of the qualitative data was undertaken to identify the key

coping strategies employed (Creswell, 2009). Specifically, directed content analysis was

employed based on the premise that an existing theory of research was drawn on to inform

(1) the coding schedule (2) the relationships between variables, and/or (3) the nature of

relevant variables (Hsieh & Shannon, 2005; Potter & Levine-Donnerstein, 1999). First, the

data were coded by an independent data analyst using a cycle method of initial coding

(Saldana, 2009). The data analyst reviewed the themes in the data, after being briefed on the

nature of the research, without having any preconceived notions from the guiding literature.

Coding took place at the sentence level, taking into account the context of the paragraph

surrounding each sentence. First cycle coding reduced the raw data to an initial set of 402

codes, based on the three key survey topics.

A second review of the data was conducted using a higher-order coding schedule

based on destructive leadership theory and the three selected theoretical coping models. The

initial codes were refined by eliminating redundant instances (respondent comments that fell

outside the research question and theoretical typologies). A total of 255 unique codes were

retained. Computer-Assisted Qualitative Data Analysis Software (CAQDAS) via NVIVO 8,

was employed to analyse this data. The 255 codes were aggregated into three subcategories

130
of perceived toxic leadership, consequences of toxic leadership, and theoretical coping

strategies. Frequencies of responses in each subcategory were employed to identify common

themes emerging from the data. The resulting themes which were common to both the data

analyst and the researcher are reported here. Coping themes were categorised by the

researcher based on the definitions provided by the theorists (Skinner et al., 2003; Yagil et

al., 2011).

Study 2 Results

Followers’ Perceptions of Toxic Leader Behaviours

Behaviours perceived by respondents as harmful were consistent with theoretical

typologies described within the destructive leadership literature. The six behaviours most

frequently reported consisted of manipulating behaviours (n = 56; 74%): such as creating

conflict - “she had her favourites,” “pitted subordinates against each other;” use of deception

- “lying,” “presented my presentations to the board, replaced my name with his;” and

professional misconduct - “he started undermining my authority,” “had inappropriate

personal relationships with several female staff members below his level.” Second,

intimidating and bullying behaviours (n = 49; 64%): such as an autocratic management style

- “my way or the highway,” “bullying, overbearing and control freak”; belittling staff –

“swearing and using intimidating language”; and targeting dissenters - “would target people

who for some reason had fallen out of favour,” “if an employee spoke up against the manager

he would target them and systematically try to get rid of them.” Third, abusive or

emotionally volatile behaviours (n = 20; 26%): “abuse, tantrums and threatening behaviour,”

“had very erratic behaviour towards staff - peaks and lows of mood.” Fourth, narcissistic

behaviours (n = 20; 26%): such as an arrogant demeanour - “constantly seek and need

praise,” “lapses into numerous, time consuming, self-praising anecdotes”; and no regard for

the needs of others – “He has to win at all costs.” Fifth, micromanaging behaviours (n = 18;

131
24%): “treated workplace interactions as a fault-finding exercise,” “had to know what I was

doing every minute of the day.” Finally, passive aggressive behaviours (n = 17; 22%): “the

leader agreed to take action and then later negated on this,” “if people persisted in raising

issues, he would ignore them from that point on – no greeting.”

Impact on Follower Well-being

A variety of distressing consequences were reported as negatively impacting on

respondents’ well-being, including physical health complications, emotional harm, and

psychological distress (refer Table 9 in Appendix D). Psychological effects included self-

doubt (n = 22; 29%): “I lost confidence in myself.”; feeling highly stressed (n = 17; 22%):

“…eventually I was unable to function in the role … from the stress.”; anxious (n = 13; 17%)

and depressed (n = 10; 13%): “anxiety, loss of professional and personal esteem…”

Emotional effects included mistrust (n = 17; 22%): “The incident created feelings of

mistrust.”; anger (n = 15; 20%): “I was very angry and disappointed.”; and fear (n = 10;

13%): “I felt vulnerable and unable to stand up for myself.” Twenty-two respondents (29%)

reported a range of physical symptoms, including health problems such as colds, gastric

upsets, hair loss, skin rashes, headaches and insomnia. While the literature conceptualizes

destructive leadership as regularly exhibiting a number of harmful behaviours, for 17 (22%)

respondents, significant harm was reported with just one perceived toxic behaviour.

Follower Coping Strategies

The most frequently reported ways of coping when dealing with toxic leadership

were: seeking social support; leaving the organisation or taking leave; ruminating; and

challenging the leader. An analysis of the reported coping instances demonstrated strong

support for four of Yagil et al.’s (2011) five coping scales, as shown in Table 10 (Appendix

D). Only two respondents (3%) reported the use of the ingratiation coping response: “I

attempted to challenge him. That did not work at all, so then I tried flattery and wheedling.

132
That was semi-successful.” “I just worked harder, longer, and tried to please.” As can be

seen from Table 10, respondents’ coping instances supported nine of Skinner et al.’s (2003)

12 coping families for dealing with toxic leadership. Three of Skinner et al.’s coping families

not represented by the data were: Negotiating with the leader to reach a compromise; social

withdrawal; and using aggression. However, Skinner et al. provided five additional coping

categories over and above Yagil et al.’s scale, extending our understanding of responses to

this specific stressor by including perceived maladaptive coping responses (e.g., ruminating)

and adaptive coping responses (e.g., seeking social and instrumental support).

Referencing both Yagil et al. and Skinner et al.’s coping frameworks, commonly

reported direct communication/problem-solving coping strategies included: assertively

challenging or confronting the leader (n = 17; 22%): “Tried to name poor behaviours if I

actually witnessed them.”; formal complaint (n = 11; 14%) and documenting incidents (n = 9;

12%): “I raised the issues as a grievance through a formal process.” In addition to seeking

social support, frequently reported support seeking coping strategies included: seeking social

support from a mentor or colleague (n = 26; 34%) or family and friends (n = 20; 26%):

“Discussed the incident with family, friends and trusted colleagues.” Frequently reported

avoidance of contact/escape strategies included: leaving the situation by leaving the

organisation (n = 34; 45%) “…while she was on leave I did my hand over and left.”; taking

leave (n = 16; 21%): “Sick leave and recreation leave were my biggest defence…”; bypassing

the leader (n = 13; 17%) or ignoring them (n = 7; 10%): “I tried to avoid and work around

him as much as possible.” Frequently reported reframing/accommodation coping

mechanisms utilised cognitive restructuring strategies (n = 13; 17%): “I tried… to reflect on

the fact that it wasn’t personal – it wasn’t about me.” Nine respondents (12%) reported

trying problem-solving approaches “at first,” but when these failed to work, they resorted to

avoidance-based strategies. Coping strategies explained by Skinner et al.’s model only were:

133
experiencing submission, i.e., rumination (n = 23; 30%): “To this day I still feel angry for the

unfair treatment that I received.”; self-reliance by focusing on work (n = 10; 13%): “My

response was to work harder.”; seeking information and professional support, such as from a

counsellor (n = 6; 8%) or General Practitioner (n = 6; 8%): “Get counselling to try to

survive.”, and experiencing delegation, i.e., feelings of shame (n = 4; 5%); and helplessness

(n = 4; 5%).

The 10 families of coping with toxic leadership that were described by these results

were directly compared employing Skinner et al.’s (2003) coping structure. This comparison

is illustrated in Figure 8. Clear similarities between Yagil et al.’s and Skinner et al.’s coping

families and the coping responses reported in this research are demonstrated. These results

illustrate that neither Yagil et al.’s framework nor Skinner et al.’s framework alone accounts

for the comprehensive range of coping responses exhibited by employees experiencing toxic

leadership. Instead the coping responses are better explained by a combination of both

frameworks.

Study 2 Discussion

This research examined coping responses reported by followers to reduce the

psychological, emotional, and physical impact of their experiences of toxic leadership. The

toxic leadership behaviours described as distressing were consistent with other reported

observations (e.g., Krasikova et al., 2013; Pelletier, 2010). This study provides a unique

contribution to the literature by identifying both adaptive and perceived maladaptive

strategies that followers employed to cope specifically with leadership behaviours they

perceive to be toxic.

134
1. Adaptive Processes

2. Families of Coping for Toxic Leadership

Problem-solving/ Support Seeking Escape/ Accommodation/ Information Submission Self-reliance Helplessness Delegation Ingratiation

Direct Communication Avoidance of Contact Reframing Seeking


3. Ways of Coping with Toxic Leadership

Confronting the Mentor or Taking leave Cognitive Professional Rumination Emotional Feelings of Feelings of Flattery
Leader colleague restructuring advice regulation helplessness shame
Resigning/retiring , doubt and Wheedling
insecurity
Managing Up Family and Self-education Behavioural Self-blame
friends Taking redundancy regulation
Instrumental Action Reading
Seeking transfer

Undertaking health Bypassing the leader


and well-being
activities

4. Coping Instances for Toxic Leadership

(see examples in Table 10)

Figure 8. Structure of Coping with Toxic Leadership, based on Skinner et al.’s (2003) organising structure (Study 2).

135
Key reported coping strategies using Skinner et al.’s (2003) framework, that Yagil et

al. (2011) didn’t capture in their abusive supervision measure, were problem-solving

strategies such as instrumental action (e.g., making a formal complaint, seeking mediation,

whistle blowing), or information seeking (e.g., seeking professional advice); and emotion-

focused strategies such as self-reliance (e.g., working harder), submission (e.g., ruminating),

helplessness, and delegation (e.g., feelings of shame, self-blame, disgust). While some of

these emotion-focused coping responses may be considered maladaptive, the current research

shows they are common strategies employed when an individual feels powerless to prevent

ongoing abuse and are likely to cause significant harm if not addressed.

Our findings support observations that when an individual feels they are in an

uncontrollable situation they are likely to revert to avoidance-focused coping strategies, such

as taking leave (O'Driscoll et al., 2009; Yagil et al., 2011). This is particularly relevant for

the design of research investigations when conducted in a workplace where employees may

not feel in control of the situation and/or do not feel supported by the organisation. In such

circumstances, affected employees are (at best) unlikely to participate in any investigation of

this organisational problem.

Implications for Practice

At least a third of respondents reported being as distressed by the lack of support

received from their organisations, as they were by the behaviour to which they were

subjected. Instead of seeking advice and support from Human Resources (HR) most

respondents sought support from colleagues, social networks, and experts outside the

organisation. As victims are often reluctant to report abuse, this suggests a proactive

approach to gathering information on instances of toxic leadership is required, because

organisational representatives are likely to hear about such difficulties anecdotally, rather

than through formal channels. Employees who chose to remain in the workplace, those who

136
could not deal with leader toxicity, nor felt able to leave the situation, reported learned

helplessness, chronic health problems, and long-term harm. Given the potential liability for

failure to provide a safe workplace, together with the cost of reduced productivity, and

increased absenteeism, presenteeism and unwanted turnover, empirical research supports the

need to take prompt action (including disciplinary action or removal of destructive leaders)

upon evidence of frequent, ongoing demonstration of toxic leadership behaviours (Lipman-

Blumen, 2005; Richards & Freeman, 2002).

When their assistance is sought, organisational representatives have a significant role

in advising and supporting victims of toxic leadership behaviours. Employees are just as

likely to try ineffective coping strategies (including ruminating, feeling shame and confusion)

as they are to try more effective direct coping strategies. For many respondents, once their

preferred coping strategies failed, they took leave or left the organisation. Employees who

remained in the workplace reported a variety of adverse consequences. Promotion of

organisational policies and procedures, codes of conduct, Employee Assistance Programs and

other external counselling services are basic requirements to promote employee well-being,

but are often inadequate to deal effectively with this specific problem (Bond et al., 2010;

Brough, O'Driscoll, Kalliath, Cooper, & Poelmans, 2009). Instead it is recommended that

proactive organisational training programs focusing on effective coping strategies to deal

with toxic behaviours, advice on the social and professional support available, and an

emphasis on the importance of taking responsibility for maintaining personal health and well-

being, may equip employees with the knowledge and skills needed to prevent them from

coming to harm, or to deflect harm when it first occurs (Brough et al., 2009).

Research Limitations and Suggestions for Future Research

In this study there was potential for a biased sample given the use of the convenience

method, recruiting only those participants reporting particularly negative experiences with

137
their manager/s. It may be that some respondents had their own personal difficulties with

their manager, e.g., interpersonal conflict, being under performance management, or

disciplinary processes. These factors were not assessed here and are recommended to be

controlled for in future research. However, it is important to note that the reported leader

behaviours were all described as harmful by the respondents, irrespective of the relationship

and circumstances between the respondent and their manager. Alternatively, individual

characteristics and attributions of followers may inform whether a manager’s behaviour is

perceived as harmful, and may also influence the coping strategies followers use

(Thoroughgood et al., 2012). Future research investigating follower characteristics that

mediate the relationship between perceived toxic leadership behaviours and the specific

coping strategies chosen by victims will improve our understanding of this complex

relationship.

In this study respondents were not asked to rate the effectiveness of the coping

strategies they used, although their responses gave some indication of their perceived

efficacy. Given that some reported strategies appear maladaptive, it is recommended future

research test the effectiveness of the coping strategies identified here (Skinner et al., 2003;

Yagil et al., 2011).

Conclusion

This study furthers our understanding of how followers cope with the psychological,

emotional, and/or physical impact of toxic leadership behaviours. The responsibility of

organisations to effectively address destructive leadership is clear: to prevent employee

exposure to toxic behaviours and to support employees who become targets of such

behaviours. This research informs the development of organisational interventions to

increase levels of employee coping and well-being by helping followers to identify and

become resilient to toxic leadership behaviours.

138
Chapter 6. Intervention: Resilience-Building Training Session to protect Followers’

Well-being (Study 3)

In Australia, like many countries, workplace health and safety legislation effectively

holds employers responsible for ensuring the psychological, emotional, and physical well-

being of employees (Safe Work Australia, 2011). If it is proven that an employer has

breached this responsibility, stress compensation claims may be lodged by affected

employees. Costs of health insurance premiums form a significant proportion of employment

costs in organisational budgets and premiums rise when there is an increase in claims. If the

claim is disputed, litigation costs are also incurred. Therefore, there are both legislative and

financial reasons for organisations to effectively manage occupational stress. The aim of

Study 3 was to design and evaluate a brief, theory-based, practical intervention to assist

followers to enhance their well-being by becoming resilient to the occupational stressor of

destructive leadership behaviours (e.g., abusive supervision behaviours). The purpose of this

chapter is to discuss the aims and rationale of Study 3, the design and methodology

employed, the key findings, and how they can inform theory and practice.

The design of Study 3 was informed by the findings of Study 1 and Study 2. Study 1

researched leaders’ personality traits to better understand the antecedents and mediators to

derailing leadership behaviours. Results of this cross-sectional study suggested leaders with

dispositional negative affectivity were more likely to be predisposed to derailing behaviours

through reduced self-regulatory processes. Therefore, if a leader was unable to self-regulate

to avoid engaging in destructive leadership behaviours, even if they were made aware of such

behaviours through leadership development activities, the abuse was likely to continue.

Based on these findings, the research focus then shifted to follower responses to destructive

leadership behaviours. Study 2 researched the leadership behaviours followers found to be

abusive, and the mechanisms by which followers responded and coped with destructive

139
leadership. The results showed that being regularly exposed over time to even one leadership

behaviour that was perceived by followers as abusive caused harm to psychological,

emotional, and ultimately physical well-being. Qualitative responses reported that significant

harm was experienced by respondents who remained working with the destructive leader over

time. A key contributor to such harm was a lack of knowledge of how to deal effectively

with the situation, negatively affecting their well-being.

Study 3 focused on the evaluation of a stress management intervention designed to

build resiliency in followers within a private healthcare organisation (nurses and support

staff) to deal effectively with abusive leadership behaviours at work, thereby enhancing their

psychological, emotional, and physical well-being. While there is debate about the

comparative efficacy of individual versus organisational stress prevention interventions, the

decision about which type of intervention to employ should be based on a thorough

assessment of the specific situation (Briner & Reynolds, 1999; DeFrank & Cooper, 1987).

To prevent or address the emotional and psychological distress caused to individual followers

by abusive leadership behaviours, an individual, secondary prevention intervention is likely

to be the most appropriate. Secondary prevention interventions have been found to be most

effective in reducing stress and enhancing psychological well-being in individuals when they

include a short group training session based on cognitive behavioural principles. Secondary

prevention interventions focus on improving individuals’ stress responses and coping (e.g.,

employee training programs to increase well-being through enhanced resiliency and coping

skills to deal with the stressor of abusive leadership behaviours). Secondary prevention

interventions promote the recognition of dysfunctional cognitions, allow a positive reframing

of cognitions, and increase personal resources and perceptions of control (Bond & Bunce,

2000; Lamontagne et al., 2007; Murphy, 1996; Richardson & Rothstein, 2008; van der Klink

et al., 2001).

140
This stress management intervention was based on the premise that resiliency

involves the processes and characteristics by which an individual is able to return to a state of

well-being following an adverse event, i.e., being the target of abusive supervision

(McLarnon & Rothstein, 2013). Resiliency includes learning to deal with adversity by self-

managing beliefs, cognitions and emotions, and engaging in behavioural strategies that

provide a sense of personal control. Once engaged, these adaptive responses will result in

outcomes that demonstrate resilience has occurred (e.g., psychological, emotional, and

physical well-being). In this research context, psychological well-being refers to

experiencing positive thoughts and affective states, with an absence of psychological distress.

Individuals with good psychological well-being report feeling happy, capable, well-

supported, and satisfied with life (Deci & Ryan, 2008). Emotional well-being refers to

experiencing positive emotions, with an absence of emotional distress (Warr, 1990). Physical

well-being refers to feeling healthy and energetic at work (Randall et al., 2009).

Study 3 Research Aims

This quasi-experimental research evaluates the effectiveness of a secondary

prevention stress management intervention to improve well-being in the context of abusive

supervision. The association between destructive leadership behaviours, psychological

distress and reduced affective and physical well-being has been documented (Pelletier, 2010;

Schyns & Schilling, 2013; Webster et al., 2016). Individual-level stress management

interventions typically aim to increase well-being by enhancing employees' capacity to cope

with occupational stressors (Richardson & Rothstein, 2008), and destructive leadership

behaviours, such as abusive supervision, have been conceptualised as an occupational

stressor (Nandkeolyar et al., 2014; Schyns & Schilling, 2013). To the author’s knowledge no

stress management training interventions have been conducted, evaluated, and reported

within the context of coping with abusive supervision.

141
Time 1 Time 2 Time 3

Resiliency training session – 4 hours 3-month time-lag

Experimental group
Survey 1 Survey 2 Survey 3
(immediately (at the
before the conclusion of
session) the session)

Control group
Survey 1 Survey 2

Figure 9. Study 3 design.

The research also seeks to address a limitation identified in the abusive supervision

literature: the pre-dominance of cross-sectional studies (Martinko et al., 2013; Yagil et al.,

2011). In this study, repeated-measures data for psychological, emotional, and physical well-

being were collected at three time points. Figure 9 outlines the Study 3 design. The

experimental group took part in a 4 hour resiliency training session and completed a pen and

paper survey to assess well-being outcomes immediately pre and post the training session.

They then completed the same survey 3 months later. As the control group did not attend the

training session, and there would be no change at a Time 2 survey, they completed their

surveys at the same time as the experimental group completed their Time 1 and Time 3

surveys. By employing a longitudinal research design, the researcher was able to measure the

effect of the intervention on well-being over time. As a result, where a significant change

142
was measured from Time 1 to Time 3, a causal relationship could be inferred between the

intervention and the significant well-being outcomes (Biggs, Brough & Barbour, 2014).

In addition, much of the destructive leadership and abusive supervision research has

been conducted in the US (Martinko et al., 2013), with increasing contribution from Norway,

UK, and Europe (Schyns & Hansborough, 2010; Schyns & Schilling, 2013). Follower

perceptions of leadership in Australia have been investigated (e.g., research suggests

Australian followers’ need to know the leader is committed to their welfare, they have a

preference for genuine but understated praise, they desire to be kept informed, and to receive

direct and truthful feedback; Dalglish & Evans, 2007; Kouzes & Posner, 2015). Conducting

this research within an Australian context, therefore, provided an important contribution to

assessing the applicability of theory predominantly developed in the US to the Australian

culture (e.g., is an intervention informed by the positive psychology and stress management

literatures effective in assisting Australian workers to identify and cope with abusive

leadership behaviours; Lamontagne et al., 2007; Martinko et al., 2013; May et al., 2014).

Stress management interventions have been criticised for their lack of theoretical

foundations (Brough & Biggs, 2015; Murphy, 1996). Such interventions need to achieve a

balance between being tailored to the unique needs of the work situation and being

underpinned by a strong theoretical foundation (Ivancevich, Matteson, Freedman, & Phillips,

1990; Murphy, 1996). This intervention, a group training session to build and maintain

resilience, was designed based on the principles of four theoretical frameworks, three

originating from CBT: SIT (Saunders et al., 1996); psychosocial resilience strategies from the

discipline of positive psychology; and ACT (Burton et al., 2010; Flaxman & Bond, 2010a;

Pipe et al., 2009; Seligman, 1992, 2002). The fourth theoretical framework underpinning this

intervention was career resilience theory. Destructive leaders often negatively affect

followers’ careers, so techniques to maximise career resilience (London, 1997) were included

143
to enhance participants’ personal resources. Selection of techniques and tools included in the

training session was also informed by the results of Study 2 (e.g., coping strategies reported

to be helpful, such as accessing social support, participating in physical health and well-being

activities, and reframing negative thoughts.)

This intervention was conducted within the healthcare sector. Mental stress claims

are highest in Australia within the Health and Community Services sector at 20.5% of claims

(Safe Work Australia, 2013). While there has been some evaluation of resilience programs

conducted within a healthcare workforce (e.g., Pipe et al., 2009; Pipe et al., 2012), more

studies of SMIs within this industry have been called for (Dollard, LaMontagne, Caulfield,

Blewett, & Shaw, 2007). This study has responded to this call by conducting the Study 3

resiliency training program in a private, acute healthcare organisation. The program was

specifically designed to help employees identify positive ways of coping with stress and

abusive leadership behaviours in their workplace. The effects of the program were evaluated

over a three-month period.

Study 3 Intervention Evaluation Measures

Both intervention implementation process measures (participant attendance,

participant evaluation of the quality of the intervention, and the extent to which the

intervention was implemented as planned) and intervention outcome evaluation measures

(participant emotional well-being, psychological well-being, and physical well-being) were

employed in evaluating this resiliency training intervention (Biggs & Brough, 2015b).

Including measures to evaluate the intervention implementation process is important for two

reasons: (a) to acknowledge the complexity of conducting longitudinal designs in

organisational research, and identify ways to improve outcomes in future research, such as

reducing attrition over time to ensure sufficient sample size; and (b) to acknowledge other

144
factors that may have had an impact on employees’ experiences during the intervention

and/or affected the well-being outcomes.

Participants’ ratings of their knowledge, motivation, and confidence to undertake the

tasks learned were measured (Gist & Mitchell, 1992; Judge & Bono, 2001). Where

individuals believe they have the knowledge, motivation, and confidence to undertake the

tasks learned, they are likely to exert sufficient effort, coping, and persistence that, if well

executed, will lead them to successfully deal with occupational stressors. Conversely,

individuals with a perception of insufficient knowledge, low confidence and/or poor

motivation are more likely to avoid implementing the strategies or to give up prematurely.

Training methods to enhance knowledge included providing information about the required

tasks and the task environment. Training methods to enhance confidence and motivation

included discussions on how to use the new skills successfully in their work environment,

and how to implement behavioural and cognitive strategies that lead to better coping

performance, including the pros and cons of the various strategies (Gist & Mitchell, 1992). It

was expected that participants’ knowledge, confidence and motivation to build and maintain

their own well-being would be increased by attending the resiliency training session, as

measured pre- and post-training. However, while confidence and motivation may be

increased when measured immediately after an intervention, it may not continue to increase

over time in some situations, such as when the environment in which the skills are employed

evokes a sense of risk or anxiety, or if the individual perceives they have no control over the

environment, as is often the case when working for an abusive supervisor (Gist & Mitchell,

1992). Based on these findings, it is expected that knowledge, confidence and motivation

will be significantly increased immediately after the training session, but it is possible that

these may not significantly change from Time 2 to Time 3, three months post the

intervention.

145
H3.1: Participating in a resiliency training intervention based on cognitive

behavioural principles will increase participants’ knowledge, confidence, and

motivation in undertaking resilience-building activities in the experimental group,

with no significant change in the control group.

It was expected that well-being would significantly increase over time with practice.

As outlined above, the three well-being outcome measures were collected at three time

points: immediately prior to the training session, at the end of the training session, and three

months after the training session. Therefore, the well-being outcome evaluation component

of this study tested three hypotheses:

H3.2a: Participating in a resiliency training intervention based on cognitive

behavioural principles will reduce participant psychological distress for the

experimental group over time, measured post-intervention (Time 3), with no significant

change in psychological distress for the control group.

H3.2b: Participating in a resiliency training intervention based on cognitive

behavioural principles will improve participant emotional well-being for the

experimental group over time, measured post-intervention (Time 3), with no significant

change in emotional well-being for the control group.

H3.2c: Participating in a resiliency training intervention based on cognitive

behavioural principles will improve participant physical well-being for the

experimental group over time, measured post-intervention (Time 3), with no significant

change in physical well-being for the control group.

The Role of Manager Support and Well-being

Supportive leadership has been found to enhance employee well-being for Australian

employees and for healthcare workers (Australian Psychological Society, 2013; Bennett,

Lowe, Matthews, Dourali, & Tattersall, 2001; Brough & Pears, 2004). Manager attitudes and
146
actions have also been identified as a resource that enhances the application of stress

management interventions in the workplace to improve well-being. This relationship is likely

to be due to the role managers have in determining employees’ access to stress management

interventions and their ability to either enable or obstruct the extent to which the strategies

learned can be implemented in the work situation (Randall, Nielsen, & Tvedt, 2009). The

quality of the manager relationship is particularly relevant in the context of abusive

supervision. In addition to evaluating individual well-being outcomes of the resilience

intervention, the impact on well-being of the quality of participants’ relationship with their

direct manager was also investigated. It was important to verify that the resilience

intervention had significant outcomes for all participants, including those who did not have a

supportive manager relationship. In relation to manager support this study tested the

hypothesis:

H3.3a: Participants who report low levels of support from their manager will report

higher psychological distress, and lower affective and physical well-being.

The Role of Manager Interpersonal Justice and Well-being

Abusive supervision, the sustained display of hostile verbal and nonverbal behaviours,

is difficult to measure in organisational research; respondents participating in interventions

conducted within their organisation are typically reluctant to answer surveys truthfully. For

example, a five-item measure of one aspect of abusive supervision, active interpersonal abuse

developed from Tepper’s (2000) measure of abusive supervision (Mitchell & Ambrose, 2007)

asks respondents whether their manager interpersonally abuses them (e.g., ridiculing staff,

telling them their thoughts and feelings are stupid, putting them down in front of others,

making negative comments about them to others, telling them they are incompetent). To the

extent that abusive supervisors are frequently rude and publicly critical towards some team

members, there is likely to be member reports of low interpersonal justice treatment

147
(Ogunfowora, 2013). It was determined that interpersonal abuse would be a difficult measure

for respondents in this study to respond to. Therefore, to measure this aspect of abusive

supervision in a way that was deemed appropriate for this research study, respondents were

asked to assess the level of interpersonal justice they felt they received from their manager

(e.g., the extent to which their manager is polite, treats them with dignity and respect, and

refrains from making improper remarks or comments; Bies & Moag, 1986). The

interpersonal justice measure provided acceptable face validity in this study, while at the

same time measuring perceived low interpersonal justice from a leader (e.g., showing lack of

respect and dignity to followers) as a pseudo-measure of interpersonal mistreatment, one

reported characteristic of abusive supervision (Tepper, 2000). It was hypothesised that

participants with low levels of interpersonal justice from their manager would report lower

levels of well-being. In relation to interpersonal justice demonstrated by the manager this

study tested the hypothesis:

H3.3b: Participants who report receiving lower interpersonal justice from their

manager will report higher psychological distress, and lower affective and physical

well-being.

In addition to manager support, manager interpersonal justice, knowledge, confidence

and motivation to employ the resilience tactics, and the well-being outcomes (psychological,

affective, and physical well-being), three coping strategies were measured as mediating

variables in this research: active coping, avoidance coping, and social support coping strategies.

Accounting for Mediating Variables: Coping Strategies

In addition to examining the direct effect of the resiliency training program on well-

being outcomes, a mediated model was tested investigating the role of follower coping

strategies in the relationship between the resilience intervention and intervention outcomes

(Nandkeolyar et al., 2014; Webster et al., 2016). The hypothesised relationships are depicted

148
in Figure 10. The destructive leadership literature has paid relatively little attention to the level

of control or volition that rests with targeted employees through their use of effective or

ineffective coping strategies when dealing with the effects of abusive supervision (May et al.,

2014). The few studies investigating the relationship between abusive supervision and

employee coping propose that the negative effects of abusive supervision will be weaker for

individuals who implement active coping strategies, and stronger for individuals who

implement avoidance coping strategies (Nandkeolyar et al., 2014; Yagil et al., 2011).

Therefore, it is hypothesised that active and avoidance coping strategies will mediate the

relationship between the intervention and well-being:

H3.4a: The positive effect of participation in a resiliency training intervention on

well-being (affective, psychological, and physical) will be mediated by participants’

coping strategies, such that those reporting active coping strategies will report higher

levels of well-being, and those reporting avoidance coping strategies will report lower

levels of well-being.

Results of Study 2 of this thesis (see Table 10, Appendix D) found that a significant

proportion of respondents drew on social support from mentors, colleagues, family, and

friends for emotional support to assist them to cope with toxic leadership. They also reported

drawing on instrumental support by seeking expert advice from professionals (Webster et al.,

2016). Yagil et al. (2011), in their research study on coping with abusive supervision, found

that seeking social support mediated the negative effects of abusive supervision and lowered

respondents’ negative affect. Therefore, it is hypothesised that social support seeking coping

strategies will mediate the relationship between the intervention and well-being.

H3.4b The positive effect of participation in a resiliency training intervention on well-

being (affective, psychological, and physical) will be mediated by participants’ social

149
support coping strategies, such that those reporting they frequently sought social

support for emotional and instrumental reasons will report higher levels of well-being.

In the context of abusive supervision, active coping and seeking social support have

been proposed to be more effective in protecting well-being than avoidance coping (Yagil et

al., 2011). While avoidance coping may be adaptive in the short term, over a longer period it

can be harmful to well-being (Webster et al., 2016). In this study, the role of coping strategies

in mediating the association between manager relationship (e.g., manager support/

interpersonal justice) and well-being was investigated (see Figure 11).

Mediating variables

M1 Active Coping
M2 Avoidance Coping
M3 Social Support Coping

Well-being Outcomes
Intervention
Psychological well-being
Resiliency Training Session Emotional well-being
Physical well-being

Figure 10. Study 3 Intervention Multiple Parallel Mediation Model.

H3.4c: The positive relationship between manager support and participant well-being

(psychological, affective, and physical) will be mediated by participants’ coping

strategies, such that active coping and seeking social support will be positively related

to well-being, and avoidance coping will be negatively related to well-being.

150
H3.4d: The positive relationship between manager interpersonal justice and

participant well-being (psychological, affective, and physical) will be mediated by

participants’ coping strategies, such that active coping and seeking social support will

be positively related to well-being, and avoidance coping will be negatively related to

well-being.

Mediating
Variables M1
Active Coping
M2 Avoidance Coping
M3 Social Support Coping

Well-being Outcomes
Manager relationship
Psychological well-being
Manager support
Emotional well-being
Interpersonal Justice Physical well-being

Figure 11. Study 3 Manager Relationship/Well-being Multiple Parallel Mediation Model.

Controlling for Conscientiousness

Conscientiousness has been correlated with psychological (Kotov, Gamez, Schmidt &

Watson, 2010), emotional (Javaras et al., 2012), and physical well-being (Bogg & Roberts,

2013). In the stress management literature, conscientiousness is purported to be one trait

affecting how individuals react to work stressors. Individuals high in conscientiousness are

likely to have higher levels of motivation to act as they take goal directed activity to regain

personal control over the situations they find themselves in. People high in conscientiousness

may report higher levels of well-being because they are likely to select more proactive coping

strategies and apply better emotion regulation in aversive situations, such as in the context of

151
abusive supervision (Smith, Ryan, & Röcke, 2013). Nandkeolyar et al. (2014) found some

support for the proposition that conscientiousness influenced the employment of coping

strategies adopted in response to active interpersonal abuse by the supervisor (e.g.,

conscientiousness being positively related to active coping strategies and negatively related to

avoidance coping strategies).

Offering an alternative view, Lin, Ma, Wang and Wang. (2015) found that individuals

high in conscientiousness may also experience increased psychological strain when

responding to stressors, because stressful work situations require cognitive and emotional

effort (i.e., appraisal and coping) that results in forms of strain such as fatigue and anxiety. It

may well be that active, problem-focused coping activities, such as confronting the abusive

leader about their behaviour, or persevering under duress by working harder, may have

adverse effects on psychological well-being in the longer term. Lin et al.’s proposition was

partially supported by the findings in Study 2 of this research. Study 2 participants reported

challenging the leader and/or working harder were not effective in reducing anxiety and

feelings of stress. Recently a curvilinear relationship between conscientiousness and

psychological well-being was posited by Carter, Guan, Maples, Williamson and Miller

(2016), with extreme levels of conscientiousness related to obsessive compulsive tendencies.

Due to the range of suggested effects of conscientiousness on well-being and coping with

stressors and, conscientiousness was treated as a control variable when evaluating the well-

being outcomes of the intervention, and when investigating coping strategies as mediating

variables.

In summary, this research aims to evaluate the effectiveness of a secondary

prevention stress management intervention in improving well-being within the context of

abusive supervision, by designing a resiliency training session underpinned by four

152
theoretical frameworks: SIT, ACT, positive psychology, and career resilience, and employing

a longitudinal research design to measure effects over time.

Study 3 Method

Participants

Employees within a not-for-profit, private, acute care healthcare organisation,

comprising four hospitals and 4,000 employees, were invited to participate in an intervention,

comprising a four-hour Building Career Resilience training session. This workshop was

specifically designed by the researcher to teach participants positive coping strategies and

resilience-building techniques they could use at work and in their personal lives. Participants

were assigned either to the experimental group, who participated in the workshop, or a

waitlist control group. Units of employees to be included in the experimental group were

determined by the executive leadership team of each hospital. Information on the training

session was provided to supervisors of the units, who passed this information on to staff. A

list of attendees was then provided back to the trainer (researcher) by unit supervisors. This

process was employed in order to release staff from their occupational duties and ensure that

there was sufficient cover for them while they were participating in the workshop. Control

group participants were recruited via supervisors of units comparable to the experimental

group (e.g., nursing, administration, support services).

Intervention Design

Decisions about the content, structure, and facilitation techniques of the training

session were made with respect to the desired intervention outcomes within the constraints of

organisational requirements (e.g., due to shift and labour management requirements, releasing

staff for one four-hour training session was approved by hospital executive). The primary

purpose of the intervention was to increase psychological, emotional, and physical well-being

of participants when dealing with occupational stressors within the work environment, by

153
providing additional techniques they could employ to build personal and career resilience.

Firstly, a review of the destructive leadership, abusive supervision, resilience, coping, and

well-being literatures was conducted to identify outcomes of stress and coping mechanisms,

specifically in the context of dealing with abusive supervision. Secondly, the content and

format of evidence-based interventions within the stress management interventions and stress

management training literatures that were relevant to this specific occupational stressor were

investigated. Thirdly, a review of the grey literature was conducted to identify training tools

used by practitioners. These literature reviews were presented in Chapters 1 to 3 and inform

the content of the intervention implemented in this study. Table 12 outlines the specific

theoretical frameworks that informed the design of the intervention.

As the literature demonstrates that secondary prevention interventions based on CBT

principles have been found most effective, a balance had to be drawn by the researcher

between designing an intervention with a strong theoretical foundation and meeting the

requirements of the organisation by customizing the training sessions to the work context

(Murphy, 1996). The approach adopted incorporated psycho education (SIT), cognitive

behavioural skills training (ACT and mindfulness skills), and group facilitation techniques to

allow participants time to process and apply learnings to their situation and workplace (e.g.,

self-reflection on own stressors and coping strategies; identification of work-related problems

in relation to abusive leadership experiences through group discussions and pair activities).

Discussions were practical and relevant to participants’ workplaces, providing the

opportunity for participants to discuss stressful situations they were dealing with, and they

were encouraged to generate potential solutions to minimise stress resulting from the

workplace problems they identified. Problems raised by participants in the workshops were

kept confidential by the researcher, with only main themes being communicated back to unit

managers and executive.

154
Table 12

Theoretical basis for Intervention (Study 3)

Intervention component Underpinning theoretical Example

framework

Collaborative facilitator SIT (Meichenbaum, 1996) Facilitator demonstrated appreciation

relationship with of workplace context and

participants understanding of culture.

Career Resilience Career Resilience (Kidd, Definition of career resilience; self-

2008; London, 1997) assessment measures of career

resilience

Destructive leadership SIT (Meichenbaum, 1996) Education on nature and impact of

the stressor; indices to recognise

values-based versus destructive

leadership (abusive supervision) and

its impact

Follower styles SIT (Meichenbaum, 1996) Managing Up – what sort of follower

am I? Focus on what participants are

able to control/change.

Dealing with destructive SIT (Meichenbaum, 1996) Education on effective/ineffective

leadership coping tactics for dealing with

destructive leadership; skill

acquisition; identifying mitigating

strategies; setting coping and

resilience-building goals.

155
Personal Resilience Resiliency (Luthans et al., Definition of resiliency

2007)

CBT; Positive psychology Self-assessment measures of

(Seligman & resiliency: positive emotions,

Csikszentmihalyi, 2000) signature strengths, meaning and

purpose

Mindfulness ACT (Harris, 2009; Hayes et Education on ACT and mindfulness

al., 2006) techniques; skills acquisition

Social Support Support seeking (Yagil et Reflection activity on social networks

al., 2011) and support; encourage employees to

support each other

Application and follow- Peer learning (Salas & Interactive sessions and group

through Cannon-Bowers, 2001) discussions to encourage peer

SIT (Meichenbaum, 1996) learning; action plan to transfer

learnings to the workplace.

The literature also demonstrated that secondary prevention interventions are most

successful when conducted in an environment where primary prevention strategies have been

employed. The healthcare organisation where the research was conducted had a values-based

leadership framework, a code of conduct, policies and procedures for dealing with

unacceptable behaviour, and a disciplinary management process in place. Tertiary

interventions were also available to staff: counselors, chaplains, and an employee assistance

program. However, for the purpose of this thesis, Study 3 will only evaluate the impact of

the resilience workshops conducted as a secondary prevention intervention.

156
The primary aim of the workshop was to educate participants about building

resilience to stressors, in particular the impact of abusive leadership behaviours as a

workplace stressor, and provide strategies to mitigate the negative effects of this stressor.

The intervention was based on social cognitive and cognitive behavioural theories, in relation

to individual coping and resilience from the positive psychology, ACT, SIT, and career

resilience literatures (Harris, 2008; Meichenbaum, 1996; Seligman, 2002; Vuori et al., 2012).

The workshop was designed for a minimum of six participants and a maximum of 15

participants per session. At the beginning of each session, the objectives of the workshop

were presented. Personal resiliency and career resilience were defined and characteristics of

resilient people discussed. The session then provided education for employees to identify

values-based versus destructive leadership behaviours. Tactics for identifying and dealing

with frustrating or harmful leader behaviours were provided, based on the dark side of

leadership and followship literature and research (Babiak & Hare, 2007; Clarke, 2005;

Crowley & Elster, 2009; Kellerman, 2004a, 2008; Lubit, 2004). The workshop aimed to

equip participants with the skills to distinguish between adaptive and maladaptive responses

and coping strategies. It was also intended to enhance participants’ perception of personal

control within the workplace by focusing on strategies they could employ within their circle

of influence, such as drawing on social support (colleagues, friends, and family), taking

ownership of their career, and regularly participating in health and well-being activities.

The facilitator (researcher) created a supportive learning environment that encouraged

peer learning by modeling and acknowledging appropriate behaviour as it occurred in the

groups (Vuori et al., 2012). Facilitator-led interactive sessions encouraged peer learning by

allowing participants to discuss obstacles and share experiences (Salas & Cannon-Bowers,

2001). Group discussions can enable a greater depth of learning, and may promote attitude

changes as opinions and insights are challenged by other group participants (Blanchard &

157
Thacker, 2004). In peer groups, participants identified their own preferences, lifestyle, and

career-related goals; defined tasks for carrying out these goals; and practised the required

actions in small groups.

The facilitator also utilised active teaching methods, such as making use of

participants’ own life and career knowledge as part of the learning process, in small and large

group discussions (Vuori et al., 2012). Participants were assisted to understand their own

follower style, as well as their personal values, motivations, fears, and needs. Maladaptive

responses to fear triggers or unmet needs were explored. Strategies for building career

resilience to enhance job satisfaction and employability, irrespective of work environments

and situations, were included (e.g., understanding career phases, aligning work with

participants’ own values and sense of purpose, and a review of work/life integration).

Resilience was strengthened utilising the principles of SIT: participants shared

experiences of barriers in their work environment, empathised with the feelings aroused by

these discussions, and then suggested possible solutions for these setbacks (Vuori et al.,

2012). Techniques to prepare for setbacks, and to deal appropriately and assertively with

them as they arose, were provided. Strategies for building personal resilience, utilising

Martin Seligman’s (1992; 2002) positive psychology principles and techniques, were covered.

Mindfulness exercises, based on ACT principles to create a happier, more satisfying life,

were introduced (e.g., gratitude exercises, health and well-being evaluations, reviewing social

support networks; Chu, Koh, Moy, & Müller-Riemenschneider, 2014; Harris, 2008; Webster

et al., 2016).

Participants completed a Building Resilience Action Plan to take away with them.

Therefore, the focus of the intervention was on increasing participants’ confidence in their

ability to manage their own personal and career resilience generally and, specifically, when

confronted with abusive leadership behaviours.

158
Intervention Implementation

Twenty workshops were facilitated by the researcher between June and December

2011. One hundred and eighty-six healthcare employees attended the workshops. Of those

attendees, a total of N = 178 participants completed the Time 1 survey at the beginning of the

workshop. The Time 2 survey was administered at the conclusion of the workshop and was

completed by N = 171 (96%). The Time 3 survey was administered three months later and

was completed by N = 76 (43%). A total of N = 54 (30%) respondents completed (matched)

surveys. In the control group, N = 54 employees completed the survey at Time 1 and only N

= 8 employees completed a second survey in the same period the experimental group

completed Time 3 (15% return rate; refer Figure 10 for research design). As surveys were

delivered to managers of control group units for distribution at both Time 1 and Time 2, and

to the experimental group unit managers at Time 3, there is no way to accurately estimate

how many of the surveys were actually received by target recipients.

Respondents in the experimental group who attended the resilience session and

completed the survey at Time 1 and Time 2 (immediately pre- and post-training session)

consisted of 135 females (76%) and 43 males (24%). Respondents’ ages ranged from 18

years to over 60 years, with the mean range between 40 and 49 years (SD = 1.19).

Respondents reported tenure with the organisation ranging from less than five years to over

20 years, with a mean range of less than five years (SD = 1.21). The most common

occupational groups were nurses (n = 103; 58%) and administration and clerical staff (n = 28;

15%). Fifty-two (29%) held undergraduate degrees and 30 (17%) held post graduate

qualifications. Respondents in the experimental group at Time 3 consisted of 43 females

(80%) and 11 males (20%), and the most common occupational group was nurses (n = 43;

80%).

159
Control group respondents consisted on 44 females (82%) and 10 males (18%), with a

mean age range of between 18 and 29 years (SD = 1.18). Control group respondents also

reported a mean of less than five years tenure (SD = 1.15 years). The most common

occupational groups were nurses (n = 28; 52%) and HR staff (n = 13; 24%). Twenty-one

(39%) held undergraduate degrees and 10 (19%) held post graduate qualifications.

Procedure and Administration

Approval was obtained from the organisation’s executive team to implement the

intervention. The hospital executive nominated units they wished to participate in the

intervention and areas that would be comparable for recruitment of control subjects. The

executives in each participating hospital also confirmed the components of the intervention,

and the implementation and data collection processes. The researcher project managed and

facilitated the implementation of the intervention and the data collection. Participants in the

experimental group were asked to complete one pre- and two post-evaluations. The first

baseline measurement was administered at the start of the workshop, with a second

measurement being conducted at the end of the workshop. Participants completed the two

pen and paper self-report surveys in the training room and placed their completed surveys in

a large envelope at the back of the room. Copies of the third survey for the experimental

group and the surveys for the control group were sent out by internal mail and returned to a

centralized location in the corporate office. Control group participants were invited, via their

supervisors, to complete the survey twice, three months apart. A full explanation of the

research, its purpose, and participant requirements were outlined at the beginning of the

survey. Participation in the surveys was taken as informed consent. Surveys were matched

using a code participants were asked to construct for themselves, following instructions and

an example provided at the beginning of the survey: first two letters of participant’s father’s

surname, first two letters of their mother’s first name, and the day of their own birth (e.g.,

160
WEGW20.) Using this confidential code for tracking purposes was preferable to collecting

identifying information (e.g., name or employee number) in order to ensure anonymity of

respondents. Reply-paid envelopes addressed to Griffith University researchers were

provided with the survey to the experimental group at Time 3 and to the control group.

Measures

Manager relationship - manager support. Manager support was measured using

two items (r = .77, p < .01). Participants were asked to rate the quality of their working

relationship with their immediate manager on a 5-point scale from 1 (poor) to 5 (very

effective). They were also asked to what extent they agreed their immediate manager is

positive and supportive of strategies to maintain well-being and resilience at work on a 5-

point scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree).

Manager relationship - interpersonal justice. Interpersonal justice, a subscale of

the interactional justice scale (Bies & Moag, 1986), was measured using four items (a = .96).

Respondents were asked to rate the extent to which their immediate manager treated them in

a polite manner, with dignity and respect, and to what extent their manager refrains from

making improper comments. Respondents used a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (to a small

extent) to 5 (to a large extent) to indicate the extent to which each item accurately described

the treatment they received from their immediate manager.

Intervention evaluation - implementation process. The effectiveness of the

implementation process was measured by participant attendance recorded with signed

attendance sheets noting any apologies, written participant evaluation of the resiliency

training session, and the post-implementation review of the extent to which the intervention

was implemented as planned.

Intervention evaluation - effectiveness using pre- and post-measures.

Effectiveness of the intervention was assessed using pre- and post-evaluation measures for

161
the experimental group. Due to a lack of access to computers by employees, pen and paper

surveys were used. In addition to an information cover sheet on the survey, containing

details of the research process (e.g., that is was voluntary, confidential, and adhered to the

university’s ethical requirements), the research aspects of the study were also explained to the

experimental group verbally at the beginning of the training session. It was reiterated that

participation in completing the research surveys was voluntary, that participation in

completing the survey would be accepted as consent to use the data for the research purposes

outlined in the information sheet, and that participants could withdraw at any time. For the

experimental group the pre- and post-surveys were delivered at the beginning and conclusion

of the workshop, and distributed to their workplace three months post the workshop.

Knowledge, Confidence, Motivation. These items were designed for this study

intervention by the researcher. Respondents were asked to rate their knowledge, confidence

and motivation (r = .32 to .64, p < .001) in using resilience strategies on a 5-point Likert scale

from Strongly Disagree to Strongly Agree. The three items included: “You are motivated to

use strategies to maintain your well-being and resilience”; “You have the knowledge you

need to maintain your career and personal resilience”; and “You feel confident to implement

the strategies and techniques you need to in order to maintain your career and personal

resilience.”

Well-being. Three well-being variables were measured: psychological well-being

was measured using Kessler’s Psychological Distress Scale, rating frequency of distress on a

five point scale from none of the time to all of the time (K10: a = .88; Furukawa, Kessler,

Slade, & Andrews, 2003); emotional well-being was measured using eight items measuring

job-related affective well-being (a = .84; Warr, 1990), for example, “In the past four weeks

about how often have you felt the following at work: depressed, enthusiastic, miserable,

optimistic, gloomy, cheerful?” on a six point scale from never to all of the time; and two

162
items for physical well-being (a = .85), asking participants to rate how often they felt

physically well and healthy, and active and energetic over the past four weeks on a six point

scale from never to all of the time (Randall et al., 2009). Survey items were contextualized to

the work environment (Lievens, De Corte, & Schollaert, 2008). For example, the K10 item

“Please indicate how often you have had these feelings over the past four weeks: so sad that

nothing could cheer you up” was changed to “so sad at work that nothing could cheer you

up.”

Mediating Variable Measures

Active and avoidance coping strategies. Coping strategies were measured with the

14-item Cybernetic Coping Scale (CSS; Brough et al., 2005; Edwards & Baglioni, 1993). A

factor analysis, conducted using Time 1 data (n = 144), resulted in three factors: avoidance

coping (8 items), active coping (4 items), and symptom reduction (2 items). Based on the

proposed mediating role of coping strategies in the literature (Nandkeolyar et al., 2014), in

this research the avoidance coping (a = .82) and active coping (a = .93) factors were

employed in the data analysis. The items measuring avoidance coping incorporated the

avoidance and devaluation subscales of the CCS. Sample items included: “Told yourself the

problem was unimportant”; “Refused to think about the problem”; and “Tried to keep

yourself from thinking about the problem.” The items measuring active coping incorporated

the change the situation and accommodation scales of the CCS. Sample items included

“Worked on changing the situation to get what you wanted” and “Tried to adjust your

expectations to meet the situation.” Participants were asked to indicate the coping strategies

they employed at work in response to stressful events on a scale ranging from 1 (did not use)

to 6 (used very much).

Social support coping strategies. Social support coping strategies were measured

using eight items from the COPE subscales for emotional support and instrumental support

163
(a = .91; Carver et al., 1989). Sample items included “Talked to someone about how you

felt” and “Asked people who have had similar experiences what they did.” Participants were

asked to indicate the coping strategies they employed at work in response to stressful events

on a scale ranging from 1 (did not use) to 6 (used very much).

Control Variables

Conscientiousness. Conscientiousness was measured using a 10 item NEO scale (a =

.70; Goldberg, 1992). Respondents used a 5-point frequency scale ranging from 1 (never) to

5 (always) to indicate the extent to which they felt each item accurately described the way

they preferred to work, such as “Get tasks done straight away.”

Age and job role. These variables were also treated in the analyses as control

variables where they had a significant relationship with the dependent variables.

Study 3 Data Analysis

Testing of assumptions for statistical analysis. Due to the high attrition rate at

Time 2 for the control group, this data could not be used for statistical analysis. Therefore,

data analysis was conducted on experimental group data only. Within group differences in

knowledge, confidence and motivation over time were tested with univariate analysis of

variance (ANOVA), while within-group differences in well-being over time were tested with

multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) and multiple regression analyses using the

SPSS statistical program. Prior to conducting the analyses, the data were screened to ensure

the assumptions for ANOVA, MANOVA, and multiple regression analyses were met: the

presence of multivariate normality, linearity, and homogeneity of variance, the absence of

missing data, absence of univariate and multivariate outliers, and absence of multicollinarity

and singularity, following the recommendations made by Tabachnick and Fidell (2013).

Psychological distress displayed minor heterostasticity, but this was not unexpected given the

use of field experimental data. The results for this variable should therefore be interpreted

164
with caution. Transformations were carried out on seven variables that demonstrated

skewness (skewness statistic greater than 1.0): psychological distress Time 1, Time 2, and

Time 3 was positively skewed; and affective well-being Time 3 was negatively skewed;

interpersonal justice Time 1 and Time 3 was negatively skewed. These non-normal

distributions were subjected to logarithmic transformations. Eight multivariate outliers were

identified as cases with Mahalanobis distance values greater than 15 (Field, 2013). Seven

univariate outliers were identified as items with standardised scores outside the range of z =

+3.29 to z = -3.29 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2013). Analyses were run using transformed and

untransformed research variables. Analyses run with univariate and multivariate outliers

removed, and using transformed variables, did not show significantly different results. Given

these factors, together with the small sample at Time 3, it was decided to use the raw data for

interpretation of results. Scale descriptives and bivariate correlations were run for all

research variables. The hypothesised mediating variables of active coping, avoidance coping,

and social support coping were found to have significant relationships with the dependent

variables.

Statistical analysis. Once data screening was completed, Hypotheses 3.1 and 3.2a –

3.2c were tested using ANOVA and MANOVA: the independent variable being the

intervention (difference Time 1 to Time 3) and the dependent variables being knowledge,

confidence, motivation to use the tactics, psychological, emotional, and physical well-being.

These analyses were conducted using the SPSS statistical program version 18.0. In respect to

the association between the manager relationship and the dependent variables, Hypotheses

3.3a – 3.3b were tested using multiple regression analyses, and hierarchical regression

analyses were conducted when control variables (i.e., age and role) were included.

Tests of parallel multiple mediation (Hypotheses 3.4a – 3.4d) were undertaken via

regression analyses with bootstrapping using PROCESS Model 4 in the PROCESS macro for

165
SPSS (Hayes, 2012). Parallel multiple mediation assumes the independent variable (the

intervention; manager relationship) affects the mediator variables (coping strategies) and the

mediator variables are causally linked to the dependent variables (well-being), but the

mediators are assumed not to affect each. All hypothesised mediators were added at once, to

account for the variance each explained in mediating the IV/DV relationship. For example,

when testing possible mediators between the intervention and well-being, the coping

strategies (active coping, avoidance coping, and seeking social support) were included

simultaneously as mediators between Time 1 well-being and Time 3 well-being (Hypotheses

3.4a and 3.4b). This statistical process estimates all mediator variables (coping strategies) are

operating in parallel. Research supports the premise that individuals engage in multiple

coping strategies at the same time when dealing with stress caused by abusive supervision

(Webster et al., 2016; Yagil et al., 2011). Therefore, by adding all mediators simultaneously,

any significant indirect effect found for a mediating coping strategy is independent of the

variance explained by the other mediators.

A significant indirect effect is deemed to be present when the confidence intervals for

the indirect effect do not contain zero (Hayes, 2013; Preacher & Hayes, 2004; Preacher &

Kelley, 2011; Zhao, Lynch, & Chen, 2010). For each of the bootstrapped analyses, 95% bias

corrected confidence intervals and 5,000 bootstrap resamples were specified. Control

variables were added as covariates (e.g., conscientiousness, for affective well-being (job

role), and physical well-being (age and job role). Mediations analyses between the

intervention and the well-being dependent variables were tested both cross-sectionally (Time

1) and over time (Time 1, Time 3).

To test Hypotheses 3.4c and 3.4d the independent variables were manager support and

interpersonal justice respectively, the dependent variables were psychological, emotional, and

physical well-being, and the mediators were active, avoidance, and social support coping.

166
The three coping measures were included simultaneously as mediators between the

independent variables of manager support and manager interpersonal justice and each of the

three well-being variables at Time 1 only, as a test of the multiple parallel mediation model.

Due to the small Time 3 sample, this analysis could not be conducted over time (Hypotheses

3.4c – 3.4d). Mediation effect sizes were reported using indirect effect (ab). As the

hypothesised mediators were added in the analysis together it was not possible to report

Kappa-squared (k2) statistic, recommended by Preacher and Kelley (2011) as the best

measure of effect size, because this statistic is currently only available for simple mediation

analyses.

Study 3 Results

Descriptive Statistics

Bivariate relationships between the research variables at Time 1, Time 2, and Time 3

for the experimental and control groups are presented in Tables 13A, 13B, 14 and 15. Age

and job role were significantly related to physical well-being, and job role was significantly

related to affective well-being. Significant demographic variables were controlled for in the

data analysis. Gender, education, and tenure were not found to be significantly correlated

with the well-being dependent variables, and as a result were not included as control

variables in further data analysis.

Table 16 presents the scale descriptives for the research variables at all three phases of

the research. The internal reliability for all measures exceeded the minimum recommended

cutoff of .70, with the exception of two variables at Time 3 for the control group only:

manager support (.57) and conscientiousness (.56). However, due to the high level of

attrition (n = 8 at Time 2), control group data could not be used in the data analysis.

167
Evaluation Results of Implementation Process

The training session intervention was implemented with the experimental group as

intended, with a high percentage of attendees participating in the research at the

commencement and staying to the end of the workshops. Attendance sheets for participants

in the experimental group confirmed attendance levels met expectations (N = 186) and

provided a good sample size for this research. On a few occasions individual staff were

contacted by their pager and called out of the session to the ward/theatre, but returned as soon

as they were able. Participant attendance at workshops was maximised by offering short, 4-

hour sessions, scheduled at times that fitted in well with shifts. Night shift staff were rostered

at times during the day that allowed them to attend the sessions. Sessions were conducted on

site at hospital training centres or theatre meeting rooms. Participants could join a morning

session before they attended an afternoon shift, or come off shift and join a morning or

afternoon session. Lunch was provided between morning and afternoon sessions to

encourage attendance and punctuality.

Perceived quality of the intervention was measured using participant evaluation forms

at the conclusion of the training session (refer Appendix E). The majority of participants

reported that their knowledge and skills in implementing resilience strategies had improved

from adequate (54%) to good (67%), and 84% felt the workshop was relevant to their needs.

The workshop overall was rated as excellent by 50%, and by 43% as good (93%). Participants

reported the most beneficial parts of the workshop were learning about workplace resilience,

learning about themselves, learning techniques for dealing with obstacles, and the group

discussion.

168
Table 13A

Study 3 Correlations for Full Sample: Experimental and Control Groups at Time 1 (N = 232)

Variable 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. Mean SD

1. Age - 2.65 1.21

2. Job role .03 - 3.78 1.81

3. Manager support -.12 -.13 - 3.67 .97

4. Interpersonal Justice -.02 -.12 .76** - 4.00 1.10

5. Psychological Distress -.04 .11 -.36** -.34** - 1.56 .59

6. Affective Well-being .01 -.20** .41** .32** -.68** - 4.77 .78

7. Physical Well-being .14* -.15* .33** .25** -.47** .59** - 4.45 1.02

8. Conscientiousness .01 -.05 .15* .10 -.28** .31** .25** - 4.19 .39

9. Coping – avoidance -.05 .12 -.30** -.37** .52** -.37** -.26** -.17* - 2.61 1.23

10. Coping - active -.07 .18* .00 -.03 .33** -.13 -.19* -.03 .45** - 3.19 1.20

11. Coping – social support -.23** -.21** .01 -.05 .21** -.05 -.10 .02 .36** .72** 3.55 1.27

*p < .05; **p < .01

169
Table 13B

Study 3 Correlations of Research Variables for Experimental Group at Time 1 (n = 178)

Variable 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. Mean SD

1. Age - 2.76 1.20

2. Job role -.04 - 3.97 1.73

3. Manager support -.04 -.07 - 3.42 .95

4. Interpersonal Justice .03 -.08 .76** - 3.81 1.14

5. Psychological Distress -.10 .03 -.34** -.32** - 1.60 .61

6. Affective Well-being .10 -.15* .40** .31** -.65** - 4.71 .78

7. Physical Well-being .27** -.19* .35** .25** -.51** .63** - 4.39 .96

8. Conscientiousness .02 -.05 .08 .02 -.25** .34** .24** - 4.16 .41

9. Coping – avoidance -.05 .11 -.31** -.38** .51** -.37** -.26** -.15 - 2.64 1.23

10. Coping - active -.06 -.12 -.05 -.05 .36** -.16 -.19* -.02 .48** - 3.13 1.19

11. Coping – social support -.20* -.15 -.06 -.09 .25** -.10 -.12 .03 .40** .74** 3.48 1.3

*p < .05; **p < .01

170
Table 14

Study 3 Correlations of Research Variables for Experimental Group at Time 2 (n = 178)

Variable 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. Mean SD

1. Manager support - 3.50 .95

2. Interpersonal Justice .76** - 3.71 1.06

3. Psychological Distress -.29** .26** - 1.53 .55

4. Affective Well-being .35** .30** -.73** - 4.80 .78

5. Physical Well-being .29** .29** -.52** .65** - 4.45 .99

6. Conscientiousness .08 .05 -.44** .39** .31** - 4.19 .45

7. Coping – Avoidance -.27** -.26** .38** -.24** -.10 -.26** - 2.64 1.34

8. Coping – Active .00 -.00 .24** -.10 -.05 -.14 .46** - 3.00 1.09

9. Coping – Social Support -.03 .02 .21* -.07 -.08 -.05 .42** .64** - 3.59 1.28

*p < .05; **p < .01

171
Table 15

Study 3 Correlations of Research Variables for Experimental Group at Time 3 (n = 54)

Variable 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. Mean SD

1. Manager support - .374 .87

2. Interpersonal Justice -.77** - 4.09 1.06

3. Psychological Distress -.36** -.45** - 1.37 .46

4. Affective Well-being .33 .58** -.65** - 5.10 .73

5. Physical Well-being .15 .35** -.62** .68** - 4.80 .94

6. Conscientiousness -.11 -.01 -.23 .23 .21 - 4.35 .36

7. Coping – Avoidance -.26 -.41* .54** -.38* -.20 -.06 - 2.68 1.11

8. Coping - Active -.37* -.39* .48** -.40* -.16 .00 .51** - 3.34 1.21

9. Coping – Social Support -.48** -.53** .45** -.41* -31 .07 .38* .69** - 3.78 1.35

*p < .05; **p < .01

172
Table 16

Study 3 Means and Standard Deviations of Research Variables for Experimental and Control Groups (Full sample N = 232; Experimental

n=178; Control = 54)

Full sample Experimental Control

Variable M SD a M SD a M SD a

T1 Manager Support 3.67 .97 3.42 .95 4.47 .49

T2 Manager Support 3.54 .96 3.50 .95

T3 Manager Support 3.74 .87 3.74 .87

T1 Interpersonal Justice 4.00 1.10 .96 3.81 1.14 .96 4.62 .63 .91

T2 Interpersonal Justice 3.75 1.06 .96 3.71 1.06 .96

T3 Interpersonal Justice 4.09 1.06 .97 4.09 1.06 .97

T1 Psychological Distress 1.56 .59 .88 1.60 .61 .89 1.42 .49 .86

T2 Psychological Distress 1.52 .55 .91 1.53 .55 .91

T3 Psychological Distress 1.37 .46 .88 1.37 .46 .88

173
T1 Affective Well-being 4.77 .78 .84 4.71 .78 .83 4.98 .76 .80

T2 Affective Well-being 4.81 .77 .85 4.80 .78 .85

T3 Affective Well-being 5.10 .73 .84 5.10 .73 .84

T1 Physical Well-being 4.45 1.02 .85 4.39 .96 .81 4.64 1.16 .94

T2 Physical Well-being 4.45 .99 .83 4.45 .99 .83

T3 Physical Well-being 4.80 .94 .87 4.80 .94 .87

T1 Conscientiousness 4.19 .39 .70 4.16 .41 .72 4.28 .33 .56

T2 Conscientiousness 4.19 .45 .85 4.19 .45 .85

T3 Conscientiousness 4.35 .36 .77 4.35 .36 .77

T1 Coping – Avoidance 2.61 1.23 .93 2.63 1.23 .93 2.38 1.29 .97

T2 Coping – Avoidance 2.64 1.33 .93 2.68 1.12 .92

T3 Coping – Avoidance 2.64 1.13 .92 2.68 1.12 .92

T1 Coping – Active 3.20 1.20 .82 3.13 1.19 .81 3.85 1.08 .86

T2 Coping – Active 3.02 1.09 .84 3.00 1.09 .84

174
T3 Coping – Active 3.34 1.21 .78 3.34 1.21 .78

T1 Coping – Social Support 3.55 1.27 .90 3.48 1.30 .91 4.26 .51 .43

T2 Coping – Social Support 3.61 1.27 .94 3.59 1.28 .94

T3 Coping – Social Support 3.78 1.35 .93 3.78 1.35 .93

Note: as Manager Support consisted of two items, the correlation was calculated and no alpha co-efficient was included in this table.

175
Qualitative responses included: “how to identify a toxic manager,” “what is my ‘follower

style’,” “discussing how to talk to/approach your manager,” ‘knowing that others have the

same/similar experiences,” “learning how to achieve a quality of life in the workplace,” and

“writing our action plan for building resilience.” Actions participants advised they will do as

a result of the workshop included: “having a discussion with my manager,” “being more

positive,” “practising strategies learned in the workshop,” “think about discussing my work

issues with the people who cause my discomfort,” “speak up when I feel issues need to be

addressed,” “talk more to resolve problems,” and “try different ways to build career/life

resilience and stress relief measure.” In addition to the positive feedback, some candidates

expressed doubt that things could change, “I understand the organisation is trying to change

and has good intentions, however it will take many years to undo and remove harmful

elements.” Qualitative responses can be viewed in detail in Appendix E.

The method for collecting Time 3 evaluation data for the experimental group, and for

collecting data from the control group was not implemented as intended. In these cases reliance

on unit managers to distribute and collect pen and paper surveys resulted in high attrition

(reduction from 176 participants at Time 1 to 54 at Time 3 for the experimental group, and from

54 at Time 1 to 8 for the control group at Time 2). Despite planning a control group for

comparison to the experimental group, the control group attrition rate at Time 2 led to the

exclusion of control group data from the statistical analysis due to the inadequate sample. The

small sample size for Time 3 experimental group also limited the statistical analyses that could

be conducted.

176
Evaluation Results of Intervention Outcomes

ANOVA analysis showed a significant increase in knowledge, confidence, and

motivation to use the learned resilience techniques, reported by the experimental group

between Time 1, immediately before the intervention, and Time 2, immediately after the

intervention, F(1,175) = 69.88 , p < .001; Wilks’ Λ = .175, partial η2 = .285, T1 M = 3.77,

SD = .64, T2 M = 4.16, SD = .51 (Hypothesis 3.1). No significant difference in knowledge,

confidence, and motivation was reported between Time 1 and Time 3, three months post the

intervention.

MANOVA analyses showed a significant increase in reported well-being over time for

the experimental group: from Time 1 to Time 2, F(3,171) = 4.72, p < .01, Wilks’ Λ = .92,

partial η2 = .08; and between Time 1 and Time 3 F(3, 50) = 4.80, p < .01; Wilk’s Λ = .77,

partial η 2 = .22. Univariate analysis found significant differences from Time 1 to Time 2 in

psychological distress, F(1,173) = 6.65, p < .01, partial η2 = .04, and affective well-being,

F(1,173) = 9.56, p < .01, partial η2 = .05; and significant differences from Time 1 to Time 3 in

psychological distress, F(1,52) = 5.57, p < .05, partial η2 = .10, T1 M = 1.53, SD = .49, T3 M =

1.37, SD = .46; and affective well-being, F(1,52) = 14.53, p < .001, partial η2 = .22, T1 M =

4.78, SD = .72, T3 M = 5.10, SD = .73 (Hypotheses 3.2a – 3.2b). There were no significant

changes in physical well-being over time (Hypothesis 3.2c).

Mediation effects of coping strategies on intervention outcomes. No significant

indirect effects were observed for active coping, avoidance coping, or seeking social support

with affective well-being or psychological distress from Time 1 to Time 2, or Time 1 to Time

3. Therefore, Hypotheses 3.4a and 3.4b were not supported. The nil effects found could be

due to the short duration of four hours between Time 1 (immediately before the training

177
session) and Time 2 (immediately after the training session) being insufficient time for the

mediators to take effect, and lack of power due to the small sample at Time 3 (three months

later).

Manager Relationship and Well-being

Before investigating the potential mediating effect of follower coping strategies on the

association between abusive supervision, measured as low manager support/interpersonal

justice, and well-being, regression analyses were conducted to confirm the hypothesis that lower

levels of manager support and manager interpersonal justice had a negative effect on well-being.

Manager Support was significantly positively associated with well-being at Time 1 for the

experimental group: affective well-being, after job role was controlled for (R = .43, R2 = .19,

F(1,222) = 40.52, p < .001); psychological distress (β = -.34, F(1,176) = 22.66, p < .001); and

physical well-being, after age and job role were controlled for (R = .39, R2 = .15, F(1,217) =

28.21, p < .001). However, no significant relationships were found, with or without control

variables, at Time 3 (Hypothesis 3.3a). Significantly higher manager support was reported from

Time 1 (M = 3.46, SD = .94) to Time 3 (M = 3.74, SD = .87), F(1,53) = 4.67, p < .05; Wilks’

Λ = .92, partial η2 = .08.

Interpersonal Justice was also significantly positively associated with well-being at

Time 1: affective well-being, after job role was controlled for (R = .36, R2 = .13, F(1,222) =

23.42, p < .001); psychological distress (β = -.32, F(1,175) = 19.61, p < .001); and physical

well-being, after age and job role were controlled for (R = .32, R2 = .09, F(1,217) = 13.55, p <

.001). Again, no significant relationships were found, with or without control variables, at Time

3 (Hypothesis 3.3b). No significant difference in interpersonal justice was reported from Time

1 (M = 3.89, SD = 1.16) to Time 3 (M = 4.09, SD = 1.06), F(1,53) = 2.39, p > .05; Wilks’ Λ =

178
.96, partial η2 = .04. Therefore, respondents reporting higher levels of manager support and

interpersonal justice at Time 1 also reported higher levels of affective and physical well-being

and lower level of psychological distress. Conversely, as expected, respondents reporting lower

levels of manager support and interpersonal justice at Time 1 reported lower levels of well-

being.

One explanation for the non-significant results for a relationship between manager

support/manager interpersonal justice and well-being at Time 3 could be that only

participants with supportive managers were encouraged to participate and complete the

survey, leading to ceiling effects in levels of manager support and interpersonal justice. This

explanation is supported by the significant increase in manager support reported at

Time 3. Another explanation is that the training intervention may have been successful in

reducing dependency on the manager relationship for well-being. Alternatively, due to the

small sample size at Time 3, there was insufficient power to find a significant result.

Mediation effects of coping strategies on manager relationship and well-being

outcomes. Results of mediation analyses for coping strategies mediating the association

between manager relationship and well-being are set out in Table 17, and

between manager interpersonal justice and well-being in Table 18. After controlling for

conscientiousness and correlated demographic variables (age and job role), cross-sectional

analyses found significant indirect effects for avoidance coping: between manager support

Time 1 and psychological distress Time 1, estimated to be -.07 (95% CI [ -.12, -.03]); and

between manager support Time 1 and affective well-being Time 1, estimated to be .04 (95%

CI [ .01, .12]).

179
Table 17

Study 3 indirect effects for the parallel multiple mediation model for manager support and well-being at Time 1

IV Mediator DV a b c’ ab SE ab CI LL, UL

Manager support Active coping Affective well-being -.05 -.08 .26* .00 .01 -.01, .05

Manager support Active coping Psychological distress .06 .12* .09 .01 .01 -.04, .01

Manager support Active coping Physical well-being -.03 -.21* .30* .01 .02 -.03, .07

Manager support Avoidance coping Affective well-being -.35** -.13 .26* .04 .03 .01, .12

Manager support Avoidance coping Psychological distress -.37*** .18** -.09 -.07 .02 -.12, -.03

Manager support Avoidance coping Physical well-being -.31* -.04 .30* .01 .02 -.03, .07

Manager support Social coping Affective well-being -.09 .03 .26* -.00 .01 -.05, .01

Manager support Social coping Psychological Distress -.08 -.03 .09 .00 .01 -.01, .04

Manager support Social coping Physical well-being -.08 .10 .30* -.01 .02 -.08, .01

Note. CI = 95% bias corrected confidence interval. a = IV  Mediator; b = Mediator  DV; c’ = direct effect IV  DV; ab = indirect

effect. Significant indirect effects are bolded. *p<.05; **p<.01; ***p<.001.

180
Table 18

Study 3 indirect effects for the parallel multiple mediation model for manager interpersonal justice and well-being at Time 1

IV Mediator DV a b c’ ab SE ab CI LL, UL

Interpersonal justice Active coping Affective well-being -.05 -.08 .13 .00 .01 -.00, .04

Interpersonal justice Active coping Psychological distress -.05 .13** -.08 -.01 .01 -.04, .01

Interpersonal justice Active coping Physical well-being -.03 -.22* .19* .01 .02 -.03, .06

Interpersonal justice Avoidance coping Affective well-being -.38*** .14 .13 .05 .03 .01, .13

Interpersonal justice Avoidance coping Psychological distress -.40*** .17*** -.08 -.07 .02 -.12, -.04

Interpersonal justice Avoidance coping Physical well-being -.35*** -.05 .19* .02 .03 -.03, .07

Interpersonal justice Social coping Affective well-being -.13 -.04 .13 -.00 .01 -.05, .01

Interpersonal justice Social coping Psychological Distress -.11 -.04 -.08 .00 .01 -.01, .04

Interpersonal justice Social coping Physical well-being -.10 .11 .19* -.01 .02 -.07, .01

Note. CI = 95% bias corrected confidence interval. a = IV  Mediator; b = Mediator  DV; c’ = direct effect IV  DV; ab = indirect

effect. Significant indirect effects are bolded. *p<.05; **p<.01; ***p<.001.

181
However, no significant indirect effect was found for avoidance coping between manager

relationship and physical well-being. A significant indirect effect was also found for avoidance

coping between interpersonal justice Time 1 and psychological distress Time 1, estimated to be -.07

(95% CI [-.12, -.04]); and interpersonal justice Time 1 and affective well-being Time 1, estimated to

be .05 (95% CI [.01, .13]), with no significant indirect effect for physical well-being Time 1. No

significant indirect effects were found for active coping or social support coping between manager

relationship (manager support or interpersonal justice) and the three well-being outcomes at Time 1.

Overall, avoidance coping significantly mediated the association between low manager support and

low manager interpersonal justice and psychological and affective well-being at Time 1. Contrary

to expectations, avoidance coping significantly reduced psychological distress and increased

affective well-being. This suggests that in the context of abusive supervision avoidance is likely to

be a common coping strategy, and may even be adaptive in reducing distress in these circumstances.

Therefore, Hypotheses 3.4c and 3.4d were not supported.

Analyses conducted over time, with Time 1 well-being, correlated demographic variables,

and conscientiousness controlled for as covariates, found no significant indirect effects for active

coping, avoidance coping, or social support coping strategies between manager relationship

(manager support and interpersonal justice) and the well-being outcomes (affective, psychological,

and physical) at Time 3. These non-significant results are likely to be due to the small sample at

Time 3. A second explanation is that, given the small indirect effects in the cross-sectional analysis

at Time 1, the effects of avoidance coping strategies became insignificant once the resiliency

training intervention had been conducted. Thirdly, avoidance coping may be adaptive in the short-

term but becomes insignificant in reducing stress symptoms over time.

182
Study 3 Discussion

When evaluating the effectiveness of the intervention in increasing participants’ knowledge,

confidence and motivation to use the tactics, and to increase their well-being, the results provided

support for three of the four hypothesised main effects. Participants reported significantly increased

knowledge, motivation, and confidence to employ learned resiliency-building strategies in the

workplace immediately after the training session (Hypothesis 3.1). Participants also reported

significantly increased psychological and affective well-being three months after the resiliency

training session (Hypothesis 3.2a and 3.2b).

No change to physical well-being over time resulted from the intervention (Hypothesis 3.2c).

There are three possible explanations for this lack of effect on physical well-being: (a) there was no

direct relationship between the resilience tactics in the training session and physical well-being; (b)

the negative impact of abusive supervision on physical well-being results from long-term

psychological and emotional distress (supported by the significant correlational relationship

between affective well-being, psychological distress, and physical well-being), and the three-month

evaluation period may have been too short to show the effects of the intervention on physical well-

being; and (c) the correlation between age and physical well-being suggests that this dependent

variable may have been directly related to the demographic variable of age and, as a result, was not

likely to change with a training session intervention.

Contrary to expectations, employees’ choice of coping strategies did not mediate the effect

of the intervention on well-being over time (Hypotheses 3.4a and 3.4b). This may have been due to

the intervention focusing on techniques to build resilience, rather than teaching specific coping

skills. Alternatively, given the small sample at Time 3, there may not have been enough variation in

participants’ coping skills or enough power to find an effect. Overall, this study provides initial

183
support for the utility of conducting short resiliency training sessions in the context of abusive

supervision to improve employee well-being over time, irrespective of individual differences in

participants.

Intervention Process Evaluation

Difficulty was experienced in finding a compromise between administering an effective

intervention with an optimal research design, and one that facilitated active participation in the

intervention, while fulfilling organisational requirements. While it is acknowledged that a true

experimental design is the most effective methodology to evaluate interventions because it controls

for potentially confounding factors, true experimental designs are extremely difficult to apply in

organisational settings (Nielsen et al., 2010; Wickstrom, 2000). In this research a quasi-

experimental design was adopted because random allocation to intervention sessions was not

practical. Unlike most preventative interventions, this study did not rely on voluntary participation.

Executive and management selection of participants and support for the program ensured good

attendance rates at the workshops. Quasi-experimental designs have been adopted in many studies

and have been regarded as an acceptable compromise for evaluating interventions if using a pre- and

post-test methodology (Lipsey & Cordray, 2000). One of the benefits of adopting the quasi-

experimental approach was being able to carry out the research in a natural organisational setting,

where reactions of test subjects are more likely to be genuine, because they are not in an artificial,

unrealistic research environment. It also enhanced the feasibility of the research, removing time,

resources, and logistical constraints often associated with true experimental designs.

A methodological strength of this study was the evaluation of both micro processes (e.g.,

increased participant knowledge, confidence, motivation, and well-being) and macro processes (e.g.,

participant attendance and participant appraisal of quality of intervention) because such evaluations

184
can partially explain why or how the intervention affected outcomes. Such evaluation can also

identify the influence micro and macro processes have on the variation in the outcomes of the study,

beyond the effects of the intervention (Biggs & Brough, 2015b; Randall et al., 2009). Participant

evaluations of the intervention at the end of the training session were positive in relation to the

quality of the workshop and the benefits gained (Appendix E).

Intervention process evaluation also allows insight into how stress management

interventions could be better designed for future studies. The main design complication in this

research study was the process for collecting longitudinal evaluation data due to the attrition for the

experimental group at Time 3 and the control group at Time 2. Relying on unit/ward managers to

ensure pen and paper surveys were completed and submitted was not an effective strategy, and it

resulted in sub-optimum sample sizes, negatively affecting the statistical analysis that could be

conducted (e.g., analysing mediation effects over time). This research intended to address the

limitations of stress management intervention research by adopting a longitudinal design with a

control group, but this could not be accomplished due to the constraints discussed.

Manager Relationship and Well-being

The results provided support for the hypothesised associations between manager relationship

and well-being (Webster et al., 2016): lower levels of manager support and manager interpersonal

justice were significantly related to lower levels of psychological, affective, and physical well-being

(Hypotheses 3.3a and 3.3b). Similarly to Nandkeolar et al.’s (2014) findings on mediators between

abusive supervision and job performance, support was found for the hypothesised mediation of

avoidance coping on the relationships between manager support and manager interpersonal justice,

but only for affective and psychological well-being. No significant mediating relationship was

found for active coping or social support coping as mediators between manager relationship and the

185
well-being outcomes (Hypotheses 3.4c – 3.4d). The higher the levels of manager support and

interpersonal justice reported, the less frequently avoidance coping was reported. Tepper’s (2007)

review of the abusive supervision literature found that individuals employing direct communication,

such as confronting an abusive leader with their concerns, experienced less psychological distress

than those who employed avoidance coping tactics to reduce their exposure to abusive supervision

(Tepper, 2007). However, although Yagil et al. (2011) confirmed Tepper’s findings, in their study

most employees did not confront supervisors, resorting to avoidance coping strategies. In Study 3,

those participants who reported lower levels of manager support and interpersonal justice and higher

levels of avoidance coping also reported significantly reduced psychological distress and increased

affective well-being. These findings suggest that while avoidance coping is not considered an

effective strategy for dealing with occupational stressors, it may relieve distress in the context of

coping with an abusive manager relationship, at least in the short term. Active coping strategies,

such as speaking up in such a situation, or trying to negotiate to change the situation, may trigger

further mistreatment, leading to increased abuse by the leader and negative career consequences for

the individual, resulting in significant harm over time (May et al., 2014; Webster et al., 2016). An

extended evaluation period of 12 to 24 months may demonstrate whether avoidance coping is an

adaptive response over the longer term.

Theoretical Implications

Stress management intervention for abusive supervision. The results of Study 3 support

the utility of secondary stress prevention interventions in assisting followers to deal with abusive

leadership behaviours in a way that maintains well-being. The Study 3 intervention and evaluation

addressed the call for theory-driven, longitudinal studies into the efficacy of SMIs. The intervention

was developed using evidence-based practice, underpinned by two main theoretical frameworks:

186
cognitive behavioural principles and career resilience theory. Educating employees to recognise

abusive leadership behaviours and inoculate them to potential harmful effects (SIT) may be

particularly effective in dealing with destructive leadership, when abuse occurs over time in an

environment that the employee cannot control, and when such behaviour may be tolerated by the

organisation. SIT provides a mechanism to increase awareness of potentially harmful situations and

validates follower experiences, giving some control back to potential targets. Combining SIT

techniques with career resilience strategies further enhances followers’ personal resources and

facilitates an avenue of potential escape from an abusive situation. This may prevent learned

helplessness or chronic mental health problems. The increase in affective well-being and the

reduction in psychological distress found at Time 3 post-intervention supports the proposition that

individuals with career resilience are better able to cope with the strong emotions generated by a

negative work situation (London, 1997; Vuori et al., 2012).

ACT techniques allow followers to review their values and priorities, which may protect

them from falling victim to manipulation by their manager or being caught up in the leader’s

agenda. It also provides mechanisms for accepting and tolerating uncomfortable and distressing

thoughts and emotions in a way that maintains well-being. This is particularly salient when dealing

with abusive leadership behaviours in situations where the victim has a less powerful position when

trying to address or change the situation. Supporting previous findings that SIT and ACT are

equally effective in reducing psychological distress (Flaxman & Bond, 2010a), the combined

techniques in the Study 3 intervention were found to be effective in increasing psychological and

affective well-being in the context of coping with abusive supervision.

187
Coping with Abusive Supervision

Targets of abusive supervision are likely to engage in several coping strategies (May et al.,

2014; Yagil et al., 2011; Webster et al., 2016). This study investigated three strategies: avoidance

coping (avoiding thinking about the problem), active coping (accommodating and changing the

situation), and social support coping (seeking emotional and instrumental support). The proposition

that avoidance coping can be adaptive in some contexts was confirmed (Skinner et al., 2003).

Indeed, avoidance coping may alleviate distress in stressful circumstances. Directing thoughts away

from the source of stress (i.e., the manager) may lead to behaviour that is perceived by the leader as

submissive, which could relieve abusive behaviour in the short-term. However, it may increase

abusive behaviour over time when the leader experiences no resistance to their negative behaviour

(May et al., 2014). In this study, the avoidance coping measure focussed on avoiding thinking about

the stressor. It would be difficult to avoid thinking about a leader’s abusive behaviour if the

follower had to interact with their manager on a daily basis. Yagil et al.’s (2011) scale for coping

with abusive supervision measured avoidance of contact with the abusive supervisor and found

avoidance coping to be maladaptive. Study 2 also found that while avoidance strategies, such as

ignoring or bypassing the leader, relieved distress in the short term, such strategies often led to

increasing periods of absenteeism (i.e., taking extended stress leave) and to psychological harm in

the longer term.

The coping literature suggests that both active and avoidance coping may be complementary

processes, with individuals cycling between the two, and that the emotional and psychological

respite gained through avoidance coping may provide the energy and resources needed for more

active coping responses. However, in this intervention, active coping to change the situation and to

accommodate or adapt to the stressor was not found to be an adaptive mechanism to enhance well-

188
being in this context. This could be because active coping may be considered confrontational by an

abusive manager, which could lead to an increase in abusive leadership behaviours (May et al.,

2014). Alternatively, if the follower felt the situation was outside their control, they may not feel

enabled to engage in active coping strategies.

Despite the findings of Study 2, that social support reduced distress, and the results of Yagil

et al.’s (2011) study, that seeking support (talking with others about abusive occurrences and

consequent emotions) mediated the effect of abusive supervision on negative affect, social support

coping was not found to mediate the effects of the quality of manager relationship on affective well-

being or psychological distress in this study (Hypotheses 3.4c and 3.4d). One explanation may be

that there are both positive and negative consequences of seeking social support when coping with

abusive supervision, negating a significant effect. The ability to vent emotions to a third-party and

seek advice may reduce negative affect and distress, while social reinforcement of the adverse

nature of the stressor may increase negative affect and distress. Additional field research using

consistent coping measures across studies is required to better understand the role of active,

avoidance, and social support coping in the context of abusive supervision.

Practical Implications

It is incumbent on organisations to minimise the detrimental effects to well-being of abusive

leadership, when it exists in the workplace, by implementing intervention strategies to build

employees’ coping, and resilience (Nandkeolyar et al., 2014). Human Resources professionals are

well placed to detect signs of stress in employees that may be caused by abusive supervision, such

as anecdotal feedback, grievance reporting, reduced engagement by unit, and high levels of

absenteeism. As well as addressing the leader’s behaviour, a brief resiliency training intervention

can be offered to staff in the affected areas. Adding psycho-education and self-evaluation

189
components to recognised resilience building techniques in the literature enabled design of an

effective intervention that caused minimal disruption to business. Brief interventions meet

organisational needs, especially when pressure on budgets and labour management is experienced in

relation to releasing staff for training. This is particularly relevant in a healthcare environment,

where training is treated as non-productive hours (hours away from patient care), negatively

affecting key performance indicator measures. Therefore, executives are more likely to invest in

individual, short interventions that fit into shift cycles (NytrØ, Saksvik, Mikkelsen, Bohle, &

Quinlan, 2000).

In addition to addressing the lack of evidence-based interventions for the stressor of abusive

supervision, this research also addressed the lack of evidence-based workplace interventions for

nurse stress (Pipe et al., 2009). Occupational stress has been identified as an dilemma for nurses

and healthcare workers, and the importance of providing a work environment where employees can

learn to use effective coping skills that enables a safe, effective, and highly satisfying care

environment for patients has been discussed (Pipe et al., 2012). The cost of unplanned absenteeism

or employee turnover related to occupational stressors, such as abusive supervision, has been found

to be significant. If the absent nurse is not replaced, the staff members who are left behind to carry

the load without the absent team member will experience an increased workload. An increased

workload is likely to incrementally increase the stress in an already challenging work environment,

potentially having a negative impact on patient care (Pipe et al., 2012). This study confirmed the

effectiveness of a four-hour training session in improving well-being within the context of abusive

supervision. Participants of units were uniformly exposed to the planned intervention,

contextualized to their workplace, maximizing the opportunity for colleague support, which has

been found to contribute to an optimal transfer climate for training (Randall et al., 2009).

190
Limitations and Future Research

As discussed in the intervention process evaluation section, the main limitation for Study 3

was sample attrition over time for both the experimental and control groups. A future research

design would benefit from employing a mechanism to bring participants back together to complete

the longitudinal surveys as a way to reduce attrition during the data collection period. For example,

the experimental group could attend a short refresher session, and the control group could be invited

to an information session on an unrelated topic, such as communication skills.

While participants did complete a resilience action plan prior to leaving the session, due to

the brief nature of the intervention, no follow-up process was put in place to assess whether the

action plans were enacted; nor were relapse prevention strategies included in the training session.

Autonomy within a hospital workplace is limited and may inhibit healthcare workers’ ability to

enact workplace change, or to minimise exposure to the stressor of abusive leadership (van der

Klink et al., 2001). Despite a three-month evaluation period being deemed sufficient when

measuring individual outcomes (Giga et al., 2003), the short evaluation period may have precluded

an analysis of possible relapse effects post the data collection period (Ivancevich et al., 1990). If

experimental groups were to be brought back together briefly for data collection purposes in order

to reduce attrition, then relapse prevention strategies could be included at that time (e.g., refresher

on key points from the training session, reflecting on the actions they have taken since the training

session, or asking participants to share what they have learned with others.) Collecting additional

measures, such as absenteeism and performance data, pre and 12 months after the intervention may

also demonstrate the sustainability of participants implementing the learned strategies in the

workplace.

191
Another challenge was the choice of measures for the abusive supervision and coping

constructs. When conducting research within one organisation, where the results will be shared

with executive staff, it is difficult to obtain approval to employ dark side measures, such as the

Abusive Supervision Scale, the active interpersonal abuse sub-scale, or the Abusive Supervision

Coping Scale (Mitchell & Ambrose, 2007; Yagil et al., 2011). It may be that if these measures were

employed, in place of the pseudo measures of quality of manager relationship, interpersonal justice,

active coping, avoidance coping, and social support coping, the efficacy of this intervention to

directly address abusive supervision, and mediators that affect the intervention-well-being

relationship, could have been better evaluated. Collecting data from one organisation reduced the

range of potential confounding variables, such as work environment and culture, while potentially

limiting generalisability of findings. Future studies collecting data from participants drawn from

more than one organisation, especially when results are not shared with those organisations in a way

that identifies their own employees, would facilitate the use of more direct measures of abusive

supervision and coping with abusive supervision and their impact on well-being (e.g., Nandekeolyar

et al., 2014; Yagil et al., 2011.)

Additionally, there is little consensus among coping researchers on how best to measure and

classify ways of coping when dealing generally with work stress, let alone the specific occupational

stressor of abusive supervision (Skinner et al., 2003). Nandekeolyar et al. (2014) recommended

using specific measures of coping with abusive supervision in future research, such as that

developed by Yagil et al. (2011). Yagil et al. (2011) did investigate five coping styles with abusive

supervision based on two classifications, problem-focused coping and emotional-focused coping.

Consistent with Nandekeolyer et al. (2014) and Yagil et al. (2011), in this research, avoidance

coping was found to be effective in reducing psychological distress and improving affect. However,

192
the avoidance coping scales used in the two examples discussed above were different from each

other (e.g., Yagil et al. measured avoidance of contact with the abusive supervisor; Nandekeolyer et

al. employed the 17-item avoidance coping scale in the Ways of Coping Checklist), and both were

different from the measure used in the current research (e.g., avoidance and devaluation scales of

the CCS.) Furthermore, due to sample attrition, this part of the Study 3 analysis also relied on

cross-sectional data, precluding the ability to draw any causal inferences from the data. Future

longitudinal research, using a consistent measure of coping specific to this occupational stressor,

such as the Coping with Abusive Supervision Scale, is recommended to enable researchers to gain a

better understanding as to why avoidance coping may be an adaptive coping mechanism to maintain

follower well-being in the context of abusive supervision. Employing the Coping with Abusive

Supervision Scale would also allow a broader examination of coping with abusive supervision

strategies beyond what was evaluated in this research, such as physically avoiding the abusive

supervisor, and employing ingratiation tactics.

Chapter Conclusion

Building on the findings from Study 1 and 2, Study 3 evaluated an evidence-based, quasi-

experimental intervention that informed participants within an acute healthcare organisation on how

to recognise and deal with abusive leadership behaviours, as well as to build and maintain their

personal and career resilience. Results showed that their knowledge, confidence and motivation in

employing resilience strategies increased at the end of the training session, and psychological and

emotional well-being were significantly improved three months post the intervention. No effects

were found for physical well-being.

Participant coping strategies did not mediate the effect of the intervention on their well-

being. However, avoidance coping did significantly mediate the association between poor manager

193
relationship (low levels of manager support and interpersonal justice) and well-being by reducing

psychological distress and increasing affective well-being. Contrary to expectations, no significant

effects were found for active coping or seeking social support (emotional and instrumental support)

mediating the impact of a poor manager relationship on well-being. While a robust research design

was intended, including evaluating micro/macro implementation processes and intervention

outcomes, and comparing experimental group data with control group data over time, a significant

limitation of this study was the inability to analyse control group data due to the high attrition rate.

The theoretical and practical implications of all three research studies in this thesis are discussed in

Chapter 7.

194
Chapter 7. General Discussion

The purpose of this thesis was to identify evidence-based strategies to prevent the damage and

costs destructive leadership behaviours cause to organisations, through their negative effect on

followers. This research sought to inform practical, realistic interventions that practitioners can

employ to prevent and/or address destructive leadership in organisations. As was outlined in Chapter

2, for the purposes of this thesis destructive leadership was defined by the researcher as:

Systematic, volitional behaviour by a leader over a period of time (verbal or non-verbal,

active or passive), that can inflict or intends to inflict serious and enduring harm on

subordinates by the use of methods of influence that are perceived as hostile or obstructive

by subordinates, and that result in the undermining of the effectiveness, motivation and/or

well-being of their subordinates, regardless of justifications for such behaviour.

Three studies were conducted to advance current theoretical and empirical knowledge of the dark

side of leadership and its impact on followers, each study contributing to achieving the aim of this

thesis. Study 1 provided information on antecedent personality traits that predispose leaders to

engage in destructive behaviours. Study 2 investigated behaviours followers perceived as toxic and

ways of coping employed by followers when attempting to mitigate the psychological, emotional,

and physical consequences of those perceived toxic leadership behaviours. Study 3 evaluated an

intervention to enhance follower well-being through improved resilience to abusive supervisory

behaviours. Figure 2 (see p. 17) outlined the contribution of the three studies of this thesis:

antecedent personality traits may predispose a leader to engage in destructive leadership behaviours,

which are likely to lead to emotional, psychological, and physical harm, when abusive leadership

behaviours are perceived by a follower as an occupational stressor. By participating in a secondary

stress prevention intervention (e.g., a resiliency training session), followers may increase their

195
ability to cope with this stressor, reducing the potential harm caused and improving well-being.

This final chapter discusses the theoretical and practical implications of the three studies’ findings,

together with the strengths and limitations of each research methodology. This chapter also

identifies areas for future research that will contribute to better understanding the complex

relationship between perpetrators and targets of destructive leadership in the context of the

workplace.

Theoretical Research Implications

The documented negative consequences of destructive leadership behaviours for followers

and organisations have not been ignored by researchers (Martinko et al., 2013). Indeed, interest and

focus in this research field has grown exponentially over the past decade (Krasikova et al., 2013;

Schyns & Schilling, 2013). By focusing on the dark side of leadership this research increases

understanding of the leader/follower leadership process. It goes beyond the assumption of

traditional leadership theory that leaders strive to be noble, by acknowledging the continuum from

constructive, values-based leadership to destructive leadership, with a specific focus on antecedents

to derailed leadership (see Figure 4, p.30). The two specific areas of interest this research aimed to

explore were, firstly, specific antecedent personality traits that predispose leaders to engage in

destructive behaviours (bright and dark side leader trait theory and trait activation theory), and,

secondly, theoretical processes by which the harm these destructive leadership behaviours cause for

followers can be mitigated (coping, resiliency, and stress management intervention theories).

This thesis research builds on previous studies that have provided some initial testing of

theoretical frameworks relevant to destructive leadership. In the first instance, this research builds

on studies on the dark side of leaders’ bright side traits theory (e.g., charisma, Hogan et al., 1990;

assertiveness, Ames & Flynn, 2007) by researching the negative effects of high levels of the bright

196
side trait, independence. It also explores the role of intrinsic rewards in trait activation, e.g. the need

to express strong personality traits (see Figure 3, p. 26; van Knippenberg, 2012), and the role of

self-regulatory traits in curbing that expression. Secondly, this research builds on previous

taxonomies of leader behaviours perceived by followers to be harmful (e.g., investigation of toxic

leader behaviour, Pelletier, 2010). Thirdly, this research extends previous coping research by

investigating follower coping in the context of destructive leadership to better understand which

strategies may protect followers from harm (e.g., coping with abusive supervision, Yagil et al.,

2011), including coping strategies that may be considered maladaptive in other contexts (Skinner et

al., 2003). Finally, it furthers stress management intervention research by investigating the utility of

a number of theoretical frameworks when designing interventions to reduce employee stress and

enhance well-being in the context of destructive leadership. The theoretical frameworks utilised

were based on cognitive behavioural principles (e.g., positive psychology, Meyers et al., 2013;

stress innoculation therapy and acceptance commitment training, Flaxman & Bond, 2010a) and

mechanisms for increasing personal resources (e.g., career resilience, Vuori et al., 2012).

Theoretical implications derived from the results of this research are discussed in more detail in the

following chapter sections.

Antecedent personality traits to destructive leadership. While the leader trait paradigm

has come under some criticism, the results of Study 1 support the argument that there is value in

attempting to understand the role of individual differences in leadership effectiveness versus

derailment, and specifically how some traits may be associated with positive outcomes in some

circumstances but negative outcomes in others (Judge et al., 2009). Study 1 provided support for

the theory that bright side leader traits can be overdone, with high levels of the bright side trait,

independence, significantly associated with derailing leadership traits (ego-centred, manipulating,

197
intimidating, micro-managing, and passive-aggressive traits). This is consistent with trait activation

theory, which purports that an individual will seek out opportunities for expressing his or her

personality traits. Leaders with high preferences for a trait are, therefore, likely to express it more

often and across more situations, which is likely to lead them to express dominant traits

inappropriately in some situations (van Knippenberg, 2012).

In addition, Study 1 provided support for part of Krasikov et al.’s (2013) proposed

theoretical model of factors that contribute to the enactment of destructive leadership. Dispositional

negative affectivity, likely to be triggered when achievement of a leader’s goals is threatened or

frustrated, was found to suppress self-regulatory traits (self-awareness and stress tolerance),

increasing the leader’s predisposition to four derailing leadership traits (intimidation, passive-

aggressiveness, micromanaging, and manipulating). Study 1 results suggested that the trait of

negative affectivity had an important role to play in mediating the relationship between dispositional

tendencies that emphasise personal achievement (e.g., high levels of the personality trait

independence), and a predisposition to employ a leadership style that involves the use of harmful

methods of influence (e.g., derailing leadership traits). This provides support for the proposition

that negative affectivity has a role in its own right as an independent or mediator variable to

destructive leadership, rather than as a control variable (Spector et al., 2000). While the cross-

sectional nature of Study 1 does not enable exploration of whether individual leaders with high

levels of the trait negative affectivity are more likely to perceive personal goal blockage, it does

provide confirmation for the mechanism that, when combined with high levels of bright or dark side

traits, negative affectivity plays a role in suppressing self-regulation traits and, as a result, may

prevent the leader from choosing a constructive response.

198
Despite a strong emphasis in the literature on the importance of emotional regulation in the

workplace (Jordan & Lindebaum, 2015; Lawrence et al., 2011) and previous findings that leaders

with emotional instability experience negative moods and create toxic environments, limited support

for these theories was found in Study 1. Emotional control was not found to be a significant

mediating trait between independence and derailing traits. The findings from Study 1 suggest leader

traits of self-awareness and stress tolerance may be more effective in facilitating self-regulation than

merely trying to suppress or control negative emotion (Kaiser et al., 2015; Rothstein & Burke,

2010). Longitudinal research is required to study over time hypothesised mechanisms of leader

enactment of high levels of bright or dark side traits into destructive leadership behaviours, in order

to fully understand the role of traits in the leader’s choice between constructive and destructive

leadership behaviours (Hogan & Kaiser, 2005; Judge et al., 2009).

Neither negative affectivity nor self-regulation traits were found to mediate the relationship

between independence and a predisposition for ego-centredness. This supports previous findings

that leaders with narcissistic traits are unlikely to develop self-awareness, because they are resistant

to feedback. Leaders who lack insight are less likely to manage their counterproductive tendencies,

especially if they are prone to experience negative emotions when feeling threatened or under stress

(Kaiser et al., 2015). The narcissism literature also suggests ego-centred leaders will be recalcitrant

to change their negative behaviour if such behaviour is meeting their self-interests, even at the

expense of others’ welfare (Padilla et al., 2007). The lack of significant findings for self-regulation

traits and ego-centredness also provides support for the theory that narcissistic leaders may employ

different self-regulatory mechanisms to less ego-centred leaders (e.g., self-enhancement, bragging,

accessing self-serving biases, seeking fame; Campbell et al., 2011). This hypothesis of differing

self-regulation mechanisms is supported by the leader behaviours reported in Study 2 (see Table 11,

199
Appendix D): self-enhancement, “They constantly seek and need praise.”, bragging “Lapses into

numerous, time consuming, self-praising anecdotes.”, and accessing self-serving biases, “He has to

win at all costs and it is pointless arguing with him.” Research findings from Study 1 and 2 suggest

sub-clinical narcissism should continue to be researched independently, as a sub-category of

destructive leadership, to attempt to identify strategies that can mitigate the negative impact of this

specific form of dark side leadership.

Behaviours perceived by followers as toxic. Study 2 investigated specific leadership

behaviours that followers perceived as toxic. Although these were summarised briefly in Chapter 5,

specific theming of participant respondents was not included in the journal article, and is therefore

outlined in detail in Table 11, Appendix D. Respondents reported harm from experiencing Dark

Triad behaviours (narcissism, intimidation, manipulation; Boddy, 2015) and behaviours consistent

with those outlined in the abusive supervision literature (emotional volatility and abuse; Tepper

2007).

Results are also consistent with Pelletier’s (2010) typology of toxic leader behaviour.

Pelletier separated out many of the behaviours themed in Study 2 as manipulative into specific

dimensions: lack of integrity (being deceptive), social exclusion, divisiveness, and promoting

inequity (favouritism). Pelletier categorised two dimensions for intimidating behaviours, called

attack on self-esteem and threat to followers’ security (aggression and threats), and one for

abusiveness (emotional volatility). Destructive leadership behaviours identified in Study 2 that are

not included under Pelletier’s typology include ego-centredness, “They demonstrated that they do

not have any care for anyone other than themselves.”, and micromanaging “fault finding” and “nit

picking”. Therefore, Study 2’s inventory of leadership behaviours, perceived by followers to cause

200
harm to their well-being, includes a broad range of behaviours from destructive leadership theory,

beyond just abusive supervision and/or toxic leadership.

Study 2 confirmed that it is not just active behaviour that is harmful. Respondents provided

examples of negative outcomes resulting from a leader’s non-action and passive-aggressive

behaviour, such as when their manager failed to provide them with important information, or

delayed decisions. Passive leader behaviour is likely to lead to follower perceptions of low-quality

interpersonal treatment by the leader, which, in turn, leads to stress (Skogstad et al., 2007). Study 1

found that high levels of independence was positively associated with passive-aggressiveness,

especially if respondents reported high levels of negative affectivity. Pelletier (2010) cited laissez-

faire as one of the toxic leader behaviour dimensions. Debate continues as to whether passive and

indirect styles of leadership (e.g., laissez-faire, passive management-by-exception, passive-

aggressive leadership) should be categorised as destructive leadership (Einarsen et al., 2007; Hogan

& Hogan, 2001; Skogstad et al., 2007). Longitudinal studies focused on leader/follower dyads or

leader/teams, measuring engagement, well-being, and performance as dependent variables, are

required to study the effects of passive leadership styles in order to establish whether they become

destructive over time.

Follower strategies for coping with destructive leadership. Study 2 responded to the call

to employ qualitative methods to better understand followers’ responses to manifestations of

destructive leadership, the consequences of follower reactions, and the effect of follower

perceptions of complaint mechanisms and organisational support for victims (Krasikova et al.,

2013). There is now a considerable body of research on the consequences of destructive leadership,

and Study 2 confirmed the psychological, emotional, and physical harm that destructive leadership

behaviours have been found to cause respondents (Einarsen et al., 2010; Martinko et al., 2013;

201
Tepper, 2007). Study 2 found that perceived toxic behaviours by managers led to negative

individual reactions such as inter-personal conflict, work withdrawal behaviours (absenteeism and

resignation from the organisation), psychological distress (anxiety and depression), emotional

distress (anger and fear), and poor physical health (insomnia, stomach upsets, and skin rashes;

Webster et al., 2016). Study 2 also found that even single destructive leadership behaviours may

incur substantial harm if they are not addressed immediately by the organisation and allowed to

become chronic. This confirmed the proposition that, based on stressor-strain theory, destructive

leadership behaviours are consistent with the stressor of anti-social behaviours and interpersonal

mistreatment, and cause strain to recipients (Beehr, 1998). This potentially adds destructive

leadership as another key component to the anti-social behaviour research, over and above the

stressors of workplace violence, aggression, bullying, and harassment (Martinko et al., 2013;

O'Driscoll & Brough, 2010). Respondents in Study 2 reported the stressor of toxic leadership as a

threat to their wellbeing and one that they did not feel they could control, e.g., a hindrance stressor

(Lazarus & Folkman, 1984).

An area that has received less attention in the literature is the coping mechanisms followers

employ when confronted with destructive leadership (May et al., 2014). Study 2 investigated the

coping strategies followers employed to deal with the negative consequences of perceived toxic

behaviour by leaders, based on a combination of two existing coping frameworks (Skinner et al.,

2003; Yagil et al., 2011). Data analysis provided support for 10 families of coping strategies for this

specific stressor (refer Table 10 in Appendix D). Reported coping strategies included instances of

problem-focused approach coping (e.g., seeking instrumental support), emotion-focused approach

coping (e.g., cognitive reframing), problem-focused avoidance coping (e.g., avoiding the leader),

and emotion-focused avoidance coping (e.g., denial; Nes & Segerstrom, 2006). As suggested in the

202
literature, choice of coping strategies was found to be influenced by individuals’ perception of

control over the stressor and their appraisal of the success of their coping strategies (Goh et al.,

2010; Yagil et al., 2011). Respondents who initially tried active coping or problem-focused

approach strategies resorted to emotion-focussed strategies, in particular avoidance tactics, when

they found their first attempts did not work. Many changed their situation by choosing to take

extended leave and/or resign (Edwards & Baglioni, 1993).

Followers’ coping skills determined their resiliency in recovering from the experience of

destructive leadership (McLarnon & Rothstein, 2013). As depicted in Figure 6 (see p. 77) and

discussed above, respondents in Study 2 experienced negative cognitions and emotions when they

interpreted their manager’s behaviour as a threat, and chose their coping strategies based on their

perception of personal control. The psychological, emotional, physical, and career outcomes they

experienced depended on the effectiveness of their coping skills, and the level of peer, social and

organisational support they received.

It is proposed that if an individual finds their coping strategies are unable to resolve the

stressor, this leads to even more negative outcomes over time (Goh et al., 2010). This proposition

was supported in Study 2. Those who did not change their situation reported experiencing

significant harm over time. Respondents in Study 3 also reported employing avoidance coping

strategies when receiving low levels of interpersonal justice and support from their manager. In

Study 3, avoidance coping significantly mediated the relationship between a poor manager

relationship and poor well-being, reducing psychological and emotional distress. These findings

support two previous studies that found avoidance coping reduced distress caused by abusive

supervision (Nandkeolyar et al., 2014; Yagil et al., 2011). There are two possible explanations for

these findings that require further investigation: (a) avoidance coping is adaptive when dealing with

203
the occupational stressor of destructive leadership, at least in the short term (Suls & Fletcher, 1985);

or (b) given the power differential and organisational context in which destructive leadership takes

place, there is little opportunity for active coping to be demonstrated.

There is limited evidence for the role of social support in reducing the negative effects of

destructive leadership (Martinko et al., 2011). Seeking social support, posited as an important

coping strategy to alleviate strain from an occupational stressor, was confirmed as a useful coping

strategy in Study 2. Those who had access to social support from co-workers, friends, or family

reported it reduced distress and improved their sense of well-being (Viswesvaran et al., 1999).

However, in Study 3 social support coping was not found to be a significant mediator between

manager relationship (low interpersonal justice) and psychological and emotional well-being. More

research is required to understand the specific role of seeking social support in the context of

destructive leadership (e.g., main effect, mediator and/or moderator; Dewe et al., 2010).

Stress management interventions and follower well-being. Study 3 provided initial

validation for the implementation of a secondary stress prevention intervention, based on cognitive

behavioural principles, within the context of abusive supervision, to enhance well-being

(Richardson & Rothstein, 2008). In an attempt to identify strategies that can prevent the damage

caused by destructive leadership behaviours, Study 3 evaluated the efficacy of a four-hour workshop

to teach followers how to identify abusive leadership behaviours early and to build their coping

skills, so that they can remain resilient when dealing with stressors, including an abusive supervisor.

Study 3 responded to the criticism that many stress management intervention designs are not

underpinned with theoretical frameworks. Support was found for adopting a multidisciplinary

approach to designing the intervention, utilising techniques based on theoretical frameworks from

CBT, positive psychology, ACT, SIT, stress management, resilience, coping, and career resilience

204
literatures and practices. Participants reported significantly higher knowledge, confidence and

motivation to use the coping and resilience strategies at the end of the workshop, and significantly

improved psychological and affective well-being over time. These results support the strategy used

of combining resiliency-building techniques with tactics to build personal resources, such as career

management exercises (Richardson & Rothstein, 2008; Vuori et al., 2012).

In addition, Study 3 confirmed the importance of undertaking program implementation

process evaluation to better understand context and process factors (macro-process factors) and

theoretical mechanisms between the intervention and outcomes (micro-process factors) that

influence the effectiveness of stress management interventions (Biggs & Brough, 2015b). As a

result, factors that may have had an impact on employees’ experiences during the intervention

and/or affected the well-being outcomes were acknowledged, explanations for non-significant

effects were provided, and improvements for future intervention designs identified, such as reducing

attrition over time to ensure sufficient sample size. Participant evaluation forms confirmed that

when designing SMIs ensuring sufficient time is allowed for reflection, group discussion,

interaction, skills practice, and learning are effective techniques for transfer of knowledge and

potential application of stress management tactics. Strategies that enhanced participation included

management support, conducting short sessions that fitted in well with shifts, and conducting

sessions on site. However, relying on third parties to collect pen and paper surveys was not found to

be conducive to acquiring adequate sample sizes.

Implications for Organisational Practice

A lack of scientific knowledge is detrimental to developing effective organisational

strategies to deal with destructive leaders and the effects of their behaviour (Martinko et al., 2013).

Despite significant research documenting the harmful consequences of destructive leadership

205
behaviours, destructive leadership is still relatively under-researched. In addition, with employers

held liable for maintaining the well-being of their employees, employing a combination of primary,

secondary, and tertiary stress management interventions is recommended to effectively deal with the

consequences of destructive leadership, and to avoid financial and legal repercussions (DeFrank &

Cooper, 1987; Murphy, 1988).

Practical implications of this research in relation to preventing destructive leadership include

providing organisations with suggested strategies to constrain destructive leader behaviour (e.g.,

rigorous selection processes, governance frameworks, policies, and sanctions) and enhancing

leadership development programs with activities designed to protect leaders from derailing (e.g.,

leadership style profiles, resiliency training, reflective practice, self-regulatory mechanisms).

In relation to managing occupational stress as a result of destructive leadership, three key

processes are recommended based on this research, namely: (a) preparing employees for stress

experiences (e.g., provision of training interventions to better prepare followers to identify and deal

effectively with destructive leadership behaviours; and provision of clear guidelines for escalating

problems, lodging grievances, and formal complaints); (b) minimising the risk of stress exposure,

via the removal of leaders who regularly demonstrate destructive leadership behaviours; and (c)

providing support for exposed employees, including formal support systems (e.g., EAP, counselling

services, and workplace conferencing; Brough et al., in press; Webster & Brough, 2015). Practical

implications derived from the results of this research are discussed in more detail in the following

chapter sections.

Primary interventions - organisational constraints to destructive leadership. This

research, in particular Study 2, emphasises the importance of organisations dealing effectively with

managers displaying destructive leadership behaviours, no matter what power or status they hold.

206
Organisations can foster destructive behaviour by focusing on achievement of strategic and

operational goals at any cost, by only measuring outcomes, and/or by punishing failures and non-

achievement of key performance indicators. Setting difficult goals or placing pressure on managers

to achieve their individual goals may lead to engagement in dispositional negative affectivity

leading to negative affect, if failure or underperformance is likely to result in punitive measures

and/or non-achievement of an incentive, such as a financial bonus (Bardes & Piccolo, 2010;

Krasikova et al., 2013; Mawritz, Folger & Lathan, 2014). Creating an environment where negative

affectivity is likely to be triggered in leaders (e.g., when their goals are thwarted) increases the

likelihood of activation of dark side traits, leading to engagement in derailing behaviours through

inhibited self-regulation (Study 1). Organisations that place equal emphasis on how organisational

goals are achieved, that is, the behaviours demonstrated in goal achievement, together with goal

outcome, are less likely to encourage destructive behaviours to ensure goal attainment.

In addition, primary interventions to prevent organisations becoming conducive

environments for destructive leadership, such as standard governance frameworks with codes of

conduct, clearly articulated organisational values, policies and procedures, and controls in place to

detect and deal with leader’s destructive behaviour, are a minimum requirement (Tepper, 2007).

Cultural and climate surveys, multisource feedback, high rate of complaints and grievances, and

patterns of leave or turnover in a particular department or team can provide indicators of potential

destructive leadership that require further investigation. Once detected, boards, executives, and HR

professionals then have to decide how they are going to deal with destructive leaders. Removing

such leaders can be challenging, time consuming, and costly. Therefore, prevention is a more

effective option for organisations.

207
A key prevention strategy is screening out leaders predisposed to engage in destructive

leadership behaviours during selection processes for recruitment and succession management.

While it is acknowledged this practice is more favourably viewed in Australia than some other

countries, such as the US, such testing provides an important mechanism to identify extreme levels

of personality traits that may derail leaders, such as those found in Study 1. Assessment results can

flag potential obstacles to leadership effectiveness and allow further investigation of candidates,

through behavioural simulations, behavioural interviewing, and reference checks based on

behavioural criteria, to prevent poor selection decisions.

Despite these preventative measures, organisations are likely to experience their share of

destructive leaders. The financial and personal cost of ignoring the behaviour of such leaders was

clearly demonstrated in Study 2. Executives and HR professionals have a duty of care to their

employees to remove such leaders, through performance management or disciplinary processes, as

soon as repeated and systematic behaviours that cause distress and harm to followers are identified.

Destructive leaders often hold relationships with people in powerful positions, are politically and

socially astute, and are likely to become litigious when challenged; thus, removing them can be

difficult. Careful planning, with expert legal advice, is often necessary (Babiak & Hare, 2007;

Boddy, 2015; Clarke, 2005).

Practical implications for leadership development programs. Relationships between

narcissism, Machiavellianism, subclinical psychopathy, and leader emergence have been

demonstrated, but no relationship has been found between the dark triad and leadership

effectiveness (Babiak et al., 2010; Grijalva, Harms, Newman, Gaddis, & Fraley, 2015). Indeed,

leaders with these traits may be viewed as having leadership potential, despite having negative

performance reviews and low ratings on leadership by peers and subordinates, providing some

208
evidence of the ability of such individuals to manipulate decision makers (Babiak et al., 2010;

Boddy, 2015). Individuals with these traits are likely to perform well in traditional selection

methods (i.e., interviews, executive sponsorship). Taking into account the resources and finances

invested by organisations in leadership development programs, it is recommended that psychometric

profiling continue to be included as part of the application and selection processes for such

programs. Rigorous selection processes may include a combination of assessment of personality

traits, multi-source feedback, team engagement and performance indicators, and leader performance

ratings (Babiak et al., 2010). For those identified with some potential leadership derailers,

providing an in-depth debrief of assessment data will be important, to increase the individual’s self-

awareness of development areas, and to identify contexts where their strengths will lead to effective

leadership versus contexts where they may derail (Kaiser et al., 2015).

Should an organisation decide to hire a candidate with extreme derailing personality traits,

perhaps due to the individual’s rare technical expertise and skills, or other unique characteristics

required by the organisation, additional support and professional development strategies may be

required during their onboarding, such as providing coaching by a psychologist, to prevent

engagement in destructive leadership behaviours. There has been some support for the proposition

that psychologist coaches are more likely to effectively assist managers with skill application and

setting behavioural change goals (Bono, Purvanova, Towler, & Peterson, 2009). Coaching can

assist the coachee to understand their implicit mental models or schemas and identify faulty

assumptions that lead to self-defeating strategies or engagement in counterproductive behaviours.

Participants may be coached through cognitive restructuring to replace faulty mental models and

counterproductive behaviours with more constructive alternatives (Gaddis & Foster, 2015; Kaiser et

al., 2015). Developing self-regulatory strategies requires commitment by the individual leader to an

209
ongoing cycle of practice, feedback, reflection, and guidance. Therefore, before investing time and

money into developing a leader with significant potential derailing traits, an assessment of the

individual’s motivation to engage in ongoing self-development should be conducted. This should

be combined with an assessment of whether the leader is likely to be able to engage the newly

learned strategies in the workplace, or whether they are more likely to use the tactics learned to

manipulate the coach, their manager, and their followers, and to better mask their dark side (Nelson

& Hogan, 2009).

To develop leadership styles that enhance employee well-being, it is recommended

practitioners include training in their leadership development programs on both constructive

leadership behaviour (such as transformational and transactional leadership styles, that take into

account the needs and wants of leaders and followers by focusing on democratic leadership and

democratic followership) and destructive leadership behaviour (Kellerman, 2008). Leader

development should also include mechanisms to increase executive and managers’ self-awareness

of the impact of their leadership style and their capacity for self-regulation to effectively self-

manage their behaviour (Kaiser et al., 2015). Techniques to increase leaders’ self-awareness could

include feedback from their selection assessments, self-reflection journaling, and multi-source

observer feedback (Kaiser et al., 2015; Kelloway & Barling, 2010; Sparks et al., 2001). In addition,

providing a mentor to facilitate learning from failures and mistakes may reduce psychological

distress, if leaders are willing to learn (Boies, Robinson, & Robertson, 2010).

Practitioners should also bear in mind that not all dark side traits lead to derailment. In some

contexts, leaders with moderate levels of these traits are successful, such as in sales roles (Furnham

et al., 2012). Study 1 provided important information on components to be included in leadership

programs to ensure leaders utilise their strengths, while self-regulating impulses that may lead to

210
derailment. This research identified the importance of building leaders’ resilience and, specifically,

their ability to deal with negative affectivity (King & Rothstein, 2010). The more leaders can deal

with frustration, thwarted personal goals, and failure effectively, the less likely they are to employ

destructive leadership behaviours. Engaging in moderate levels of physical exercise has been found

to buffer the relationship between supervisor stress and abusive behaviour, so including physical

exercise components into leadership development programs may be beneficial (Burton, Hoobler, &

Scheuer, 2012). In addition, teaching and coaching advanced interpersonal communication skills,

and resilience strategies, such as mindfulness techniques, cognitive reframing, and ways of coping

with uncertainty, enables leaders to better employ self-regulation tactics when under stress or

fatigued in order to enable an effective response (Atkins, 2008; Moran, 2011). The resulting

improvement in managers’ ability to build effective relationships with their staff and garner the

support and co-operation of their workforce is, in turn, likely to reduce managers’ stress levels and,

therefore, their predisposition to employ destructive leadership behaviours (Sparks et al., 2001).

Interventions to enhance follower well-being. In addition to protecting followers from

exposure to destructive leadership behaviours by organisational leaders, organisations can also take

measures to protect followers from the negative effects of destructive and abusive leadership

behaviours. Study 3 demonstrated that a short training program, designed to increase followers’

ability to quickly identify destructive leadership behaviours when they occur, and to take steps to

build their resilience to it, can be effective in raising participants’ perceived knowledge, confidence

and motivation to use resiliency strategies leading to improved emotional and psychological well-

being over time. It is recommended such programs include educational components to recognise

destructive leadership behaviours, learn effective responses to destructive leadership behaviours,

become aware of follower needs and styles, learn mindfulness techniques and gratefulness activities,

211
review and assess current health and well-being, identify social support networks, and participate in

career management activities. Results from Study 3 suggest attending this type of training can

enhance staff well-being and reduce psychological and emotional distress. The literature suggests

that this, in turn, is likely to lead to reduced absenteeism and/or turnover (Krasikova et al., 2013;

Schyns & Schilling, 2013). In summary, organisations can implement a number of strategies (e.g.,

primary, secondary, and tertiary interventions) to guard against incidents of destructive leadership

and to protect followers from abuse (prevention). At the same time, organisations can ensure

governance mechanisms are in place that allow boards and executives to act promptly to deal with

or remove a leader exhibiting destructive behaviours as soon as they are identified.

Research Strengths of this Thesis

Reviewing the strengths and limitations of this research allows identification of useful

theoretical frameworks and/or methodologies that should be repeated in future research into the dark

side of leadership. It also identifies what could be changed in future research design to increase the

validity and reliability of results (e.g., to address the challenges the researcher experienced in

accessing adequate field samples).

Five primary strengths of the thesis research were identified. Firstly, by studying destructive

leadership, this research broadens the scope of traditional leadership theory. As Kellerman (2004b)

and Alvesson and Spicer (2012) purported, the study of the dark side of leadership informs an

understanding of the bright side of leadership. Taking a critical approach to the study of leadership,

this research aimed to advance knowledge of the dark side of leadership, such as antecedent traits

that may lead to manager derailment, and to understand why followers may conform to domination

and relinquish their autonomy and agency (Alvesson & Spicer, 2012). This research also adopted

functionalist and interpretive approaches to the leadership phenomenon. Functionalist studies view

212
leadership as an objective phenomenon amenable to scientific enquiry. From a leader-centric

perspective, they focus on traits and behaviours of leaders (e.g., in this research, traits and

characteristics that may derail a leader) and, from a follower-centric perspective, they acknowledge

the role of followers in the leadership process (e.g., in this research avoidance coping to reduce

distress, which may allow destructive leadership behaviours to continue). Interpretive studies

employ qualitative measures to understand how leadership is enacted and interpreted from ongoing

interactions between leaders and followers (e.g., in this research, understanding the leadership

behaviours that followers perceive to be toxic). This research demonstrated that drawing on

multiple theoretical approaches (critical, functional, and interpretive) and multiple perspectives

(leader-centric, follower-centric, and followership) can improve our understanding of the

interactional leadership process between the leader-follower dyad (Uhl-Bien et al., 2014).

Secondly, this research was designed to address a significant real-world organisational

problem with the objective that the research results would be informative to both practitioners and

theorists; that is, the findings contribute to theory development that can inform solutions applied in

the workplace and tested in future field studies. Given this research was primarily informed by two

epistemological knowledge claims, pragmatism and positivism, a range of methodologies and

methods were employed across the three studies, including: quantitative self-report, qualitative self-

report, cross-sectional, and longitudinal designs. The program of research in this thesis addressed

two organisational problems across three studies that informed each other. Study 1 showed that

some leaders have traits that are associated with leadership derailing traits and may predispose them

to engage in destructive leadership behaviours. It also confirmed that some, such as leaders with

high levels of ego-centredness, are likely to be resistant to modifying their behaviour. Study 2

showed the harm caused by destructive leadership behaviour and the difficulty followers had coping

213
with even one toxic behaviour. Respondents also reported distress at the lack of action by

organisations to protect them from harm. If the demonstration of destructive leadership behaviours

is a fact of organisational life, as the evidence suggests (Aasland et al., 2010; Martinko et al., 2013),

then followers are likely to be subjected to destructive behaviours by leaders at some time in their

career. Therefore, more interventions, such as that used in Study 3, are called for - to pilot and

evaluate programs designed to assist followers to identify and be resilient to abusive behaviours in

the workplace, in order to maintain well-being.

Thirdly, there is a dearth of field studies on destructive leadership, both from the leader and

follower perspective, with convenience, snowballing, or student samples often used in cross-

sectional research designs (Martinko et al., 2013). All three studies collected data from participants

employed in organisations across a range of industries and sectors. In addition, this research

includes one of the few studies that have directly investigated follower coping in response to

destructive leadership (e.g., May et al., 2014; Pelletier, 2012; Yagil et al., 2011), using both cross-

sectional and longitudinal study designs. Using field samples increased generalisability of findings

and allowed inferences to be drawn that inform a scientist-practitioner approach to dealing with the

problem of destructive leadership in organisations.

Fourthly, this research draws on multiple theoretical frameworks within the psychology and

sociology literature: specifically, destructive leadership/abusive supervision, coping, resiliency,

stress management interventions, and program implementation evaluation theories. Given the

complex relationship between destructive leaders and their followers, within the context of their

behaviour in the workplace, it is unlikely that one theoretical perspective can explain the full range

of antecedents, interactions, causes, and consequences. For example, Study 2 confirmed that one

coping framework alone was insufficient to explain the range of coping instances reported by

214
respondents who perceived they worked for a toxic leader. These three studies informed a number

of theoretical areas for future research (e.g., engaging in destructive leadership versus constructive

leadership, followership, coping with destructive leadership, and stress management interventions).

Finally, most existing field studies investigating destructive leadership or abusive

supervision are based in the US (Martinko et al., 2013). Of 21 non-US examples, Martinko et al.

(2013) could only identify one Australian study examining abusive supervision (Kiazad, Restubog,

Zagenczyk, Kiewitz, & Tang, 2010). While the studies in this thesis do not look specifically at

cultural differences, they do allow an investigation of whether the theories, models, and frameworks

in the destructive leadership and coping literatures are supported by research findings using

Australian samples.

Research Limitations of this Thesis

There were two main limitations of this research: common method variance through the use

of single source, self-report data, a method employed in all three studies in this thesis; and the high

attrition rate in the longitudinal research, Study 3. The sole use of self-report data is a limitation

which is frequently noted in the literature (Podsakoff et al., 2012). Firstly, it has been argued that it

tends to increase the reported size of relationships (correlations) between measures employing

common methods, although this assumption has recently been challenged (Conway & Lance, 2010).

Secondly, it is considered to be subjective and raises the problem of social desirability. However,

because the constructs of interest related to innate dispositions (e.g., leader personality traits, and

follower self-perceptions of stressors and coping responses), self-report measures were considered

most appropriate to generate accurate representations of respondents’ inner thoughts, feelings,

motivations, and behaviours (Chi & Liang, 2013; Eatough & Spector, 2013). Therefore, in this

context, when leaders and employees are in the best position to report on their appraisal of others’

215
behaviour, their own attitudes, affect, and responses, it was not possible to obtain measures of key

variables from different sources (Cash & Gardner, 2011).

The possibility that the results may be influenced by social desirability bias (e.g.,

respondents wanting to appear a certain way) is often raised as a critique of using self-report

instruments. In Study 1, impression management was controlled for. Nevertheless, objective

measures of outcome variables (e.g., performance review data, multi-source data, number of

grievances or complaints filed, team turnover by leader, behavioural observations in the workplace)

would have provided a useful insight into the accuracy of self-report data. In Study 2 and Study 3

procedural design remedies were used (e.g., assurances of anonymity and confidentiality, and

reversing the measure order of items in the questionnaires) to alleviate potential common-source

bias (Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Lee, & Podsakoff, 2003; Podsakoff et al., 2012). In Study 2

respondents may have answered desirably in relation to their reported responses to toxic leadership

behaviours, as none reported using aggression or retaliation. However, as noted by Cash and

Gardner (2011), the use of anonymous survey reporting can reduce this bias by giving participants

little incentive to misrepresent themselves. Again, collection of objective measures (e.g., co-worker

reports of follower behaviour, absenteeism, and turnover records) would have provided

confirmation of self-report data; however these data are not always available or easy to collect. In

Study 3, participants in the experimental group completed the self-report survey three times, making

it more difficult to manipulate their answers across time.

The most important limitation of Study 3 was the modest response rate at Time 3 in the

experimental group (approximately 30%), and the poor response rate at Time 2 for the control group

(approximately 15%). Chapter 6 provided an analysis of the reasons for this attrition. This is an

often cited deficiency of applied research, and the difficulty of applying rigorous research

216
methodologies, such as longitudinal designs, using field studies has been frequently discussed

(Biggs & Brough, 2015a, 2015b; Nielsen, 2000). Sufficiently large sample sizes are important to

ensure the appropriate analyses can be conducted for the particular research question being

examined. Research conducted in healthcare organisations is particularly challenging, given the

combination of shift work, lack of access to computers on the wards, and reliance on supervisors

and managers to release staff and encourage them to participate in the research intervention. It is

possible that the reported response rate obtained at Time 3 for the experimental group and Time 2

for the control group represented a lower bound estimate of the actual response rate, because the pen

and paper surveys were distributed by supervisors of the department/ward of respondents, and it was

impossible to assess whether the surveys were actually received by all employees. Difficulties with

matching the longitudinal data sets also contributed to the poor response rate. Respondents were

asked to provide a unique code that allowed surveys to be matched over time, while preserving

anonymity of their responses. Numerous respondents failed to produce consistent codes, making it

difficult to match their data. Similar challenges have been cited in previous research (Pipe et al.,

2012). Due to insufficient longitudinal (matched) sample sizes for the Study 3 control group, a

comparison of experimental and control group results was not possible. While this limited the

conclusions that could be drawn regarding the causal relationships between the variables, the results

are still deemed to be informative because of their contribution as one of the few field studies

available investigating an intervention to increase well-being, resilience and, in particular, coping

with destructive leadership behaviours (Pipe et al., 2012; Yagil et al., 2011).

Future Directions for Research

Reviews of the current destructive leadership literature have identified research gaps that

warrant further investigation, for example, the need for more field studies, more longitudinal

217
studies, and more research conducted with sample populations outside the US to assess cultural

differences (Martinko et al., 2013; Tepper, 2007). While the consequences of destructive leadership

have been studied, more research is required into the antecedents of destructive leadership (Schyns

& Schilling, 2013) and the mechanisms by which followers can effectively cope with the effects of

destructive leadership behaviours (May et al., 2014; Pelletier, 2010; Webster et al., 2016; Yagil et

al., 2011). In addition, the research discussed in this thesis has raised additional research questions

for investigation, which are discussed next.

Destructive leadership. Firstly, to better understand the role of leader personality trait

activation in the engagement of destructive leadership behaviours, longitudinal field studies that

follow leaders and their subordinates over time are required. Such studies enable an investigation of

follower and environmental triggers of emotions, such as frustration and anger, or negative

affectivity, which can lead to reduced self-regulation, and ultimately engagement in destructive

leadership behaviours. Objective outcome variables, such as: leader performance reviews; number

of grievances and complaints against the leader; achievement of leader KPIs; and subordinate

engagement, coping, and well-being will enable a better understanding of the relationships between

leader traits, leader performance, and outcomes of leadership style. Such research may also

highlight the situations when strategic use of dark side leadership behaviours, such as strategic

bullying, are effective in achieving goals (Ferris et al., 2007) and when the effects of passive styles

of leadership, such as laissez-faire leadership, are indeed destructive (Skogstad et al., 2007). This

methodology would enable researchers to study trajectories of leader-follower interactions over

time, including how follower coping behaviours and leader reactions to subordinate coping develop

in the course of the interactional process. These may either trigger or prevent further interpersonal

mistreatment by leaders (May et al., 2014).

218
Secondly, this thesis demonstrates the benefits of introducing resilience-building

interventions for both leaders and followers (Study 1 and Study 3). An optimal longitudinal

research design would study three groups of leader/follower dyads or leader/team groups: (a)

leaders of teams with high performance and high engagement style; (b) leaders of teams with low

performance and low engagement; and (c) a control group. Such a design would enable a better

understanding of the link between antecedent personality characteristics and destructive leadership

behaviours. Experimental groups may be assigned to a resiliency-building intervention, while the

control group undertakes a non-related activity. Pre- and post-measures of leader traits (e.g.,

abusive supervision scale, GPI – derailing leadership traits), leader performance reviews,

achievement of leader KPIs, and engagement and well-being of subordinates by team within each of

the three groups, may provide confirmation of the role resiliency-building interventions play in

improving follower well-being by mitigating the effects of destructive leadership.

Susceptible followers. There is much discussion in the literature on the need for a unified

and comprehensive coping structure (Skinner et al., 2003), and this is also a problem for the study of

subordinate coping with the occupational stressor of destructive leadership (May et al., 2014; Yagil

et al., 2011). A framework for coping with destructive leadership may be further developed by

conducting field research to validate the coping frameworks investigated in Study 2 (see Table 10 in

Appendix D), with followers reporting the coping strategies they employed, and an evaluation of the

perceived effectiveness of each coping response. Such studies could also investigate which

followers are best able to cope with destructive leadership behaviours and how followers may

contribute to, or hinder, the process of toxic leadership (Krasikova et al., 2013; May et al., 2014).

An examination of followers’ ingroup status would also be informative, to understand whether

219
leaders are more likely to translate dark side personality traits into abusive behaviours towards

individuals in their outgroup (Babiak & Hare, 2007; Schyns, 2015).

Longitudinal field studies are needed that include a detailed description of follower

interventions for replication across sectors and cultures, a thorough evaluation of the effectiveness

and efficacy of interventions, and a methodology to reduce attrition in both experimental and

control groups over time.

Organisational responses to destructive leadership. Study 2 demonstrated that the harm

to well-being caused by destructive leadership behaviours is increased when senior managers in

organisations fail to act on complaints and grievances lodged by followers. One reason for the

silent response by organisations may be that they do not feel equipped to deal with the effects of

destructive leadership. This could be due to a range of factors, such as an avoidance of

confrontation, fear of retaliation by a destructive leader, negative career implications if they

challenge the destructive leader, the difficulty of obtaining objective evidence of the destructive

behaviours due to observers’ fear to speak up, an unwillingness to engage in disciplinary processes,

or a reluctance to terminate the offending party (Perlow & Williams, 2003). One area of research

and practice that is showing promise when dealing with workplace conflict is the use of workplace

conferencing based on restorative justice principles (Fitzgerald, 2006). This could be an effective

tertiary intervention for organisations to undertake when they would like to acknowledge the harm

caused to employees by one or more of their leaders. This process may allow organisations to

identify ways to prevent harm happening in future, while supporting the complainant/s, without

becoming liable to litigation or compensation claims. While there have been papers outlining the

workplace conferencing process and its application to a range of sectors such as IT, education, legal

and government, with supporting case studies (Davey, 2007; Thorsborne, 2009), these have

220
generally been conducted by practitioners, and they would benefit from replication using more

rigorous experimental designs. In the context of destructive leadership, such a process could

provide a safe avenue for each party to tell their story, to discuss what happened, to describe the

impact of the harmful behaviours, and explore whether there is a way forward that can ensure the

future psychosocial safety and well-being of affected employees in the workplace.

An investigation of the utility of workplace conferencing in mitigating the negative impact

of destructive leadership, using a longitudinal field study methodology, would require the target

organisation to bring representatives from their executive (as discussed in Chapter 3, in this context

it would not be appropriate to conduct the workplace conference with the offending leader present)

together with complainant/s of destructive leadership and their support people, to have a discussion

facilitated by an expert in restorative justice techniques. Dependent variables measured pre- and

post the intervention could include employees’ well-being, coping, resiliency, and performance. If

proved effective, this intervention would provide an additional option for organisations to

effectively deal with the occupational stressor of destructive leadership.

In addition to the three specific areas for future research outlined above, there are many

additional theoretical areas identified for future research within the destructive leadership literature.

These include: developing a comprehensive theoretical model of destructive leadership, integrating

leader, follower, and organisational characteristics (Krasikova et al., 2013; Schyns & Schilling,

2013); the role of leader intent and/or perceived intent on follower experience of destructive

leadership (Krasikova et al., 2013; Schyns & Schilling, 2013); validation of assessment tools to

screen dark side personality traits, and validation of abusive supervision scales with actual

behaviours (Martinko et al., 2013; Schyns, 2015); the role of implicit leadership theories in shaping

follower perceptions of their leader and perpetuating their leader’s power (Hansbrough & Schyns,

221
2010; Judge et al., 2009); role of followers in the process of destructive leadership, including

subordinate characteristics and behaviours that may lead to abuse (Krasikova et al., 2013; Martinko

et al., 2013; May et al., 2014); cultural factors such as varying power distances between abusive

supervisor and subordinate (Tepper, 2007); validation of follower coping scales (Webster et al.,

2016; Yagil et al., 2011); moderators of choice of follower coping strategy (May et al., 2014); and

moderators between destructive leadership and well-being outcomes, such as organisational support

(Martinko et al., 2013).

Research Conclusions

In conclusion, the research in this thesis identified traits as antecedents to predisposing

leaders to exhibit destructive leadership (Study 1), and the negative consequences experienced by

followers of leaders displaying toxic leadership behaviours (Study 2; Aasland et al., 2010; Martinko

et al., 2013; Schyns & Schilling, 2013; Webster et al., 2016). This thesis has contributed to a more

complete understanding of the dynamics of destructive leadership, from both leader-centric and

follower-centric perspectives, to provide a starting point for the development of leader and follower

interventions to counteract the negative effects of destructive leadership in organisations. This

research contributed to a better understanding of the antecedent traits that predispose a leader to

engage in destructive leadership behaviours, highlighting the importance of developing resilience in

leaders to tolerate negative emotions and respond effectively to goal blockages, frustrations, and

failures (Study 1). In addition, the results of this research demonstrated that followers are likely to

employ ineffective coping strategies when dealing with toxic leadership (Study 2). This research

has demonstrated that a multidisciplinary approach, from psychological and sociological

perspectives, is beneficial when designing interventions to build follower resilience against the

negative effects of destructive leadership. Research results demonstrated that teaching followers

222
effective ways to deal with destructive leadership behaviours, and to build personal and career

resilience, was effective in improving well-being over time (Study 3). We return to the example

from Chapter 1, which highlights how necessary it is for organisations to address destructive

leadership in an effective way that mitigates harm to followers.

Well, my work stress claim was accepted, and I went on extended leave. A few months

later my boss was “headhunted” to a senior executive role overseas. My organisation

offered me a part-time “return to work” arrangement, but I couldn’t face going back. By

then I was on medication for chronic depression. Eventually my partner gave me the

ultimatum – get off the couch and find a new job or the relationship was over. It took me six

months to find this job. I investigated the culture and leadership team thoroughly before I

accepted this role, and my new boss seems OK. Still, there are no guarantees. I find it hard

to trust – I keep waiting for that Jekyll and Hyde moment! Once I’m through the probation

period, I plan to gradually come off my medication. I’ll keep seeing my counselor though. I

look forward to getting my life back.

Imagine. Imagine if my claims had been acknowledged by my employer, and my boss

counseled or terminated. Imagine if my organisation had provided me with training and

support on how to deal more effectively with my manager’s toxic behaviours. Imagine…

Collaboration between academics and practitioners in influencing organisations to address

the occupational stressor of destructive leadership by employing a combination of primary,

secondary, and tertiary stress prevention interventions is necessary if leader derailment and

enactment of destructive leadership behaviours are to be prevented, and the psychosocial safety and

well-being of followers is to be protected. The implementation of evidence-based resilience-

building interventions across organisations provides a real opportunity to improve leader’s self-

223
regulation mechanisms, while at the same time optimising well-being outcomes for all employees,

including those working with destructive leaders.

224
References

Aasland, M. S., Skogstad, A., Notelaers, G., Nielsen, M. B., & Einarsen, S. (2010). The prevalence

of destructive leadership behaviour. British Journal of Management, 21, 438-452.

Ackerman, R. A., Witt, E. A., Donnellan, B., Trzesniewski, K. H., Robins, R. W., & Kashy, D. A.

(2011). What does the Narcissistic Personality Inventory really measure? Assessment, 18,

67-87.

Alvesson, M., & Spicer, A. (2012). Critical leadership studies: The case for critical performativity.

Human Relations, 65(3), 367-390.

Ames, D. R., & Flynn, F. J. (2007). What breaks a leader: The curvilinear relation between

assertiveness and leadership. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(2), 307-324.

American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders.

DSM-IV. Washington: American Psychiatric Association.

Australian Psychological Society. (2013). Stress and wellbeing in Australia 2013: A state-of-the-

nation survey. from http://www.psychology.org.au/NPW/survey/

Aquino, K., & Thau, S. (2009). Workplace victimization: Aggression from the target's perspective.

Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 717-741.

Aryee, S., Chen, Z. X., Sun, L., & Debrah, Y. A. (2007). Antecendents and outcomes of abusive

supervision: Test of a trickle-down model. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 191-201.

Ashforth, B. (1994). Petty tyranny in organizations. Human Relations, 47, 755-778.

Aspinwall, L. G., & Taylor, S. (1997). A stitch in time: Self-regulation and proactive coping.

Psychological Bulletin, 121, 417-436.

225
Atkins, P. (2008). Leadership as response not reaction: Wisdom and mindfulness in public sector

leadership. In P. Hart & J. Uhr (Eds.), Public leadership - perspectives and practices.

Canberra, ACT: ANU Press.

Avolio, B. J., & Gardner, W. L. (2005). Authentic leadership development: Getting to the root of

positive forms of leadership. The Leadership Quarterly, 16, 315-338.

Avolio, B. J., Walumbwa, F. O., & Weber, T. J. (2009). Leadership: Current theories, research,

and future directions. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 421-449.

Babiak, P., & Hare, R. D. (2007). Snakes in suits. When psychopaths go to work. New York:

Harper Collins.

Babiak, P., Neumann, C. S., & Hare, R. D. (2010). Corporate psychopathy: Talking the walk.

Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 28, 174-193.

Bandura, A. (1991). Social cognitive theory of self-regulation. Organizational Behavior and

Human Decision Processes, 90, 248-287.

Bandura, A. (2001). Social cognitive theory: An agentic perspective. Annual Review of

Psychology, 52, 1-26.

Barbuto, J. E. (2000). Influence triggers: A framework for understanding follower compliance.

Leadership Quarterly, 11(3), 365-387.

Barbuto, J. E., Scholl, R. W., Hickox III, C. F., & Boulmetis, J. (2001). A field study of the

relation between leaders' anticipation of targets' resistance and targets' reports of influence

tactics used by leaders in dyadic relations. Psychological Reports, 88, 835-843.

Barbuto, J. E., & Xu, Y. (2006). Sources of motivation, interpersonal conflict management styles,

and leadership effectiveness: a structural model. Psychological Reports, 98, 3-20.

226
Bardes, M., & Piccolo, R. F. (2010). Goal setting as an antecedent of destructive leader behaviors.

In B. Schyns & T. Hansborough (Eds.), When leadership goes wrong: Destructive

leadership, mistakes and ethical failures (pp. 3-22). Chicago: Information Age Publishing.

Bass, B. M., & Avolio, B. H. (1993). Transformational leadership: Response to critiques. In M.

M. Chemers & R. Ayman (Eds.), Leadership theory and research: Perspectives and

directions. Sydney: Academic Press Inc.

Beehr, T. A. (1998). An organizational psychology meta-model of occupational stress. In C. L.

Cooper (Ed.), Theories of organizational stress. New York: Oxford University Press.

Bennett, P., Lowe, R., Matthews, V., Dourali, M., & Tattersall, A. (2001). Stress in nurses:

Coping, managerial support and work demand. Stress and Health, 17, 55-63.

Bennis, W. (2003). On becoming a leader. USA: Perseus Books.

Benson, M. J., & Campbell, J. P. (2007). To be, or not to be, linear: An expanded representation of

personality and its relationship to leadership performance. International Journal of

Selection and Assessment, 15(2), 232-249.

Berridge, J., Cooper, C. L., & Highley, C. (1992). Employee assistance programs and workplace

counseling. Chichester: Wiley.

Bies, R. J., & Moag, J. F. (1986). Interactional justice: Communication criteria of fairness. In R. J.

Lewicki, B. H. Sheppard & M. H. Bazerman (Eds.), Research on negotiations in

organizations, Volume 1 (pp. 43-55). Greenwhich, CT: JAI Press.

Biggs, A., & Brough, P. (2015a). Challenges of intervention acceptance in complex, multifaceted

organisations: The importance of local champions. In M. Karanika-Murray & C. Biron

(Eds.), Derailed organizational stress and well-being interventions: Confessions of failure

and solutions for success (pp. 151-158). UK: Springer.

227
Biggs, A., & Brough, P. (2015b). Explaining intervention success and failure: What works, when,

and why? In M. Karanika-Muray & C. Biron (Eds.), Derailed organizational stress and

well-being interventions: Confessions of failure and solutions for success (pp. 237-244).

UK: Springer.

Biggs, A., Brough, P., & Barbour, J. (2014). Relationships of individual and organizational support

with engagement: Examining various types of causality in a three-wave study. Work &

Stress, 28(3), 236–254. 10.1080/02678373.2014.934316.

Blanchard, N., & Thacker, J. W. (2004). Effective training: Systems, strategies, and practices. 2nd

edition. New Jersey: Pearson Education Inc.

Blase, J., & Blase, J. (2002). The dark side of leadership: Teacher perspectives of principal

mistreatment. Educational Administration Quarterly, 38(5), 671-727.

Bligh, M. C., Kohles, J. C., Pearce, C. L., Justin, J. E., & Stovall, J. F. (2007). When the romance

is over: Follower perspectives of aversive leadership. Applied Psychology: An International

Review, 56(4), 528-557.

Board, B. J., & Fritzon, K. (2005). Disordered personalities at work. Psychology, Crime & Law,

11(1), 17-32.

Boddy, C.R. (2011a). Corporate psychopaths, bullying and unfair supervision in the workplace.

Journal of Business Ethics, 100, 3, 367-379.

Boddy, C.R. (2011b). Corporate Psychopaths: Organisational Destroyers. Palgrave Macmillan,

Basingstoke.

Boddy, C.R. (2015). Organisational psychopaths: A ten year update. Management Decision, 53,

10, 2407-2432.

228
Boddy, C.R., Ladyshewsky, R. & Galvin, P.G. (2010). The influence of corporate psychopaths on

corporate social responsibility and organizational commitment to employees. Journal of

Business Ethics, 97, 1, 1-19.

Boddy, C.R., Miles, D., Sanyal, C., & Hartog, M. (2015). Extreme managers, extreme workplaces:

Capitalism, organisations and corporate psychopaths. Organization, 22, 4, 530-551.

Bogg, T., & Roberts, B.W. (2013). The case for conscientiousness: Evidence and implications for a

personality trait marker of health and longevity. Annals of Behavioural Medicine, 45, 278–

288.

Boies, K., Robinson, M. A., & Robertson, M. C. S. (2010). Leaders' personal experience and

response to failure. A theoretical framework and initial test. In B. Schyns & T.

Hansborough (Eds.), When leadership goes wrong: Destructive leadership, mistakes and

ethical failures (pp. 357-382). Charlotte, North Carolina: Information Age Publishing, Inc.

Bond, F. W., & Bunce, D. (2000). Mediators of change in emotion-focused and problem-focused

worksite stress management interventions. Journal of Occupational and Health Psychology,

5(1), 156-163.

Bond, S. A., Tuckey, M. R., & Dollard, M. F. (2010). Psychosocial safety climate, workplace

bullying and symptoms of posttraumatic stress. Organization Development Journal, 28(1),

37-56.

Bono, J. E., & Judge, T. A. (2004). Personality and transformational and transactional leadership:

A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89(5), 901-910.

Bono, J. E., Purvanova, R. K., Towler, A. J., & Peterson, D. B. (2009). A survey of executive

coaching practices. Personnel Psychology, 62, 361-404.

229
Briner, R. B., & Reynolds, S. (1999). The costs, benefits, and limitations of organizational level

stress interventions. Journal of Organizational Behaviour, 20, 647-664.

Brinkborg, H., Michanek, J., Hesser, H., & Berglund, G. (2011). Acceptance and commitment

therapy for the treatment of stress among social workers: A randomized controlled trial.

Behaviour Research and Therapy, 49, 389-398.

Brough, P., & Biggs, A. (2015). The highs and lows of occupational stress intervention research:

Lessons learnt from collaborations with high-risk industries. In M. Karanika-Murray & C.

Biron (Eds.), Derailed organizational stress and well-being interventions: Confessions of

failure and solutions for success (pp. 263-270). UK: Springer.

Brough, P., Brown, J., & Biggs, A. (in press). Improving criminal justice workplaces: Translating

theory and research into evidenced-based practice. UK: Routledge.

Brough, P., O'Driscoll, M., & Kalliath, T. (2005). Confirmatory factor analysis of the Cybernetic

Coping Scale. Journal of Occupational and Organisational Psychology, 78, 53-61.

Brough, P., O'Driscoll, M., Kalliath, T., Cooper, C. L., & Poelmans, S. A. Y. (2009). Workplace

Psychological Health. Current Research and Practice. Cheltenham, UK: Edward Elgar

Publishing Limited.

Brough, P., & Pears, J. (2004). Evaluating the influence of the type of social support on job

satisfaction and work related psychological well-being. International Journal of

Organisational Behaviour, 8(2), 472-485.

Brown, D. (1964). Subordinates views of ineffective executive behaviours. Academy of

Management, 7(4), 288-299.

Brown, K. W., Ryan, R. M., & Creswell, J. D. (2007). Mindfulness: Theoretical foundations and

evidence for its salutary effects. Psychological Inquiry, 18(4), 211-237.

230
Burke, R. J. (2006). Why leaders fail: Exploring the dark side. International Journal of

Manpower, 27(1), 91.

Burns, J. M. (1978). Leadership. New York: Harper & Row.

Burton, J. P., Hoobler, J. M., & Scheuer, M. L. (2012). Supervisor workplace stress and abusive

supervision: The buffering effect of exercise. Journal of Business Psychology, 27, 271-279.

Burton, N. W., Pakenham, K. I., & Brown, W. J. (2010). Feasibility and effectiveness of

psychosocial resilience training: A pilot study of the READY program. Psychology, Health

& Medicine, 15(3), 266-277.

Byrne, A., Dionisi, A. M., Barling, J., Akers, A., Robertson, J., Lys, R., et al. (2014). The depleted

leader: The influence of leaders' diminished psychological resources on leadership

behaviors. The Leadership Quarterly, 25, 344-357.

Campbell, W. K., Hoffman, B. J., Campbell, S. M., & Marchisio, G. (2011). Narcissism in

organizational contexts. Human Resource Management Review, 21, 268-284.

Cannon, W.B. (1929). Bodily changes in pain, hunger, fear, and rage. New York: Appleton-

Century-Crofts.

Carson, M. A., Shanock, L. R., Heggestad, E. D., Andrew, A. M., Pugh, S. D., & Walter, M.

(2012). The relationship between dysfunctional interpersonal tendencies, derailment

potential behavior, and turnover. Journal of Business Psychology, 27, 291-304.

Carsten, M. K., Uhl-Bien, M., West, B. J., Patera, J. L., & McGregor, R. (2010). Exploring social

constructions of followership: A qualitative study. The Leadership Quarterly, 21, 543-562.

Carter, N.T., Guan, L., Maples, J. L., Williamson, R. L., & Miller, J. D. (2016). The downsides of

extreme conscientiousness for psychological well-being: The role of obsessive compulsive

tendencies. Journal of Personality, 84, 4, 510-522.

231
Carver, C. S., & Connor-Smith, J. K. (2010). Personality and coping. Annual Review of

Psychology, 61, 679-704.

Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., & Weintraub, J. K. (1989). Assessing coping strategies: A

theoretically based approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(267-283).

Cash, M. L., & Gardner, D. (2011). Cognitive hardiness, appraisal and coping: Comparing two

transactional models. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 26(8), 646-664.

Centre for Creative Leadership. Benchmarks 360 degree feedback from www.ccl.org

Chaleff, I. (2009). The courageous follower. Standing up to and for our leaders. San Francisco,

CA: Berrett-Koehler Publishers, Inc.

Chi, S-C. S., & Liang, S-G. (2013). When do subordinates' emotion-regulation strategies matter?

Abusive supervision, subordinates' emotional exhaustion, and work withdrawal. The

Leadership Quarterly, 24, 125-137.

Christiansen, N. D., Quirk, S. W., Robie, C., & Oswald, F. L. (2014). Light already defines the

darkness: Understanding normal and maladaptive personality in the workplace. Industrial

and Organizational Psychology, 7, 138-143.

Christie, R., & Geis, F. L. (1970). Studies in Machiavellianism. New York: Academic Press.

Chu, A. H. Y., Koh, D., Moy, F. M., & Müller-Riemenschneider, F. (2014). Do workplace

physical activity interventions improve mental health outcomes? Occupational Medicine,

64, 235-245.

Clarke, J. (2005). Working with monsters. How to identify and protect yourself from the workplace

psychopath. Sydney: Random House.

Clements, C., & Washbush, J. B. (1999). The two faces of leadership: Considering the dark side of

leader-follower dynamics. Journal of Workplace Learning, 11(5), 170-175.

232
Coleman, J. W. (2006). The causes. The criminal elite: understanding white-collar crime (pp. 193-

233). New York: Worth Publishers.

Conger, J. A. (1990). The dark side of leadership. Organizational Dynamics, 19(2), 44-55.

Conger, J. A., & Kanungo, R. N. (1987). Toward a behavioral theory of charismatic leadership in

organizational settings. Academy of Management Review, 12, 637-647.

Connor-Smith, J. K., & Flaschsbart, C. (2007). Relations between personality and coping: A meta-

analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(6), 1080-1107.

Conway, J. M., & Lance, C. E. (2010). What reviewers should expect from authors regarding

common method bias in organizational research. Journal of Business Psychology, 25, 325-

334.

Cooper, C. L., & Cartwright, S. (1997). An intervention strategy for workplace stress. Journal of

Psychosomatic Research, 43(1), 7-16.

Cotton, P. (2008). Psychological injury in the workplace. InPsych, 30(2), 8-11.

Cotton, P., & Hart, P. M. (2003). Occupational wellbeing and performance: A review of

organisational health research. Australian Psychologist, 38(2), 118-127.

Creswell, J. (2009). Research design: Qualitative, quantitative and mixed methods approaches.

London: Sage Publications.

Crocker, J. (2002). Contingencies of self-worth: Implications for self-regulation and psychological

vulnerability. Self and Identity, 1, 143-149.

Crowley, K., & Elster, K. (2009). Working for you isn't working for me. New York: Penguin

Group.

Dahling, J. J., Whitaker, B. G., & Levy, P. E. (2009). The development and validation of a new

Machiavellianism scale. Journal of Management, 35(2), 219-257.

233
Dalglish, C., & Evans, P. (2007). Leadership in the Australian context. Prahan: Tilde University

Press.

Davey, L. (2007). Restorative practices in workplaces. Paper presented at the IIRP 10th

International Institute for Restorative Practices World Conference, Budapest, Hungary,

November.

Day, D. V. (2000). Leadership development: A review in context. Leadership Quarterly, 11(4),

581-613.

Deci, E.L., & Ryan, R.M. (2008). Hedonia, eudaimonia, and well-being: An introduction. Journal

of Happiness Studies, 9, 1–11.

De Fruyt, F., Wille, B., & Furnham, A. (2013). Assessing aberrant personality in managerial

coaching: Measurement issues and prevalence rates across employment sectors. European

Journal of Personality, 27, 555-564.

De Hoogh, A. H. B., & Den Hartog, D. N. (2008). Ethical and despotic leadership, relationships

with leader's social responsiblity, top management team effectiveness and subordinates'

optimism: A multi-method study. The Leadership Quarterly, 19, 297-311.

DeFrank, R. S., & Cooper, C. L. (1987). Worksite stress management interventions: Their

effectiveness and conceptualisation. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 2(1), 4-10.

Dewe, P. (1994). EAPs and stress management. Personnel Review, 23(7), 21-32.

Dewe, P. J., O'Driscoll, M. P., & Cooper, C. L. (2010). Coping with work stress. A review and

critique. UK: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Dinh, J. E., & Lord, R. G. (2012). Implications of dispositional and process views of traits for

individual difference research in leadership. The Leadership Quarterly, 23, 651-669.

234
Dollard, M. F., LaMontagne, A. D., Caulfield, N., Blewett, V., & Shaw, A. (2007). Job stress in

the Australian and international health and community services sector: A review of the

literature. International Journal of Stress Management, 14(4), 417-445.

Dollard, M. F., Shimazu, A., Bin Nordin, R., Brough, P., & Tuckey, M. R. (Eds.). (2014).

Psychosocial factors at work in the Asia Pacific. Dordrecht Springer.

Dubrin, A. J. (2010). Leadership. Research findings, practice, and skills. Mason, USA: South-

Western Cengage Learning.

Duffy, M. K., Ganster, D., & Pagon, M. (2002). Social undermining in the workplace. Academy of

Management Journal, 45, 331-351.

Eatough, E. M., & Spector, P. E. (2013). Quantitative self-report methods in occupational health

psychology research. In R. R. Sinclair, M. Wang & L. E. Tetrick (Eds.), Research methods

in occupational health psychology: Measurement, design, and analysis (pp. 248-267). New

York, NY: Routledge.

Edwards, J. R. (1992). A cybernetic theory of stress, coping and wellbeing in organizations.

Academy of Management Review, 17, 238-274.

Edwards, J. R., & Baglioni, A. J. (1993). The measurement of coping with stress: construct validity

of the Ways of Coping Checklist and the Cybernetic Coping Scale. Work & Stress, 7(1), 17-

31.

Einarsen, S., Aasland, M. S., & Skogstad, A. (2007). Destructive leadership behaviour: A

definition and conceptual model. The Leadership Quarterly, 18, 207-216.

Einarsen, S., Skogstad, A., & Aasland, M. S. (2010). The nature, prevalence, and outcomes of

destructive leadership - a behavioral and conglomerate approach. In B. Schyns & T.

235
Hansborough (Eds.), When leadership goes wrong: Destructive leadership, mistakes and

ethical failures (pp. 145-171). Chicago: Information Age Publishing.

Ferris, G. R., Zinko, R., Brouer, R. L., Buckley, M. R., & Harvey, M. G. (2007). Strategic bullying

as a supplementary, balanced perspective on destructive leadership. The Leadership

Quarterly, 18, 195-206.

Fiedler, F. E. (1967). A theory of leadership effectiveness. New York: McGraw-Hill.

Field, A. (2013). Discovering statistics using IBM SPSS statistics and sex and drugs and rock 'n'

roll. (3rd ed.). London: Sage Publications.

Fitzgerald, M. (2006). Corporate circles: Transforming conflict and building trusting teams.

USA: Quinn Publishing.

Flaxman, P. E., & Bond, F. W. (2010a). A randomised worksite comparison of acceptance

commitment therapy and stress inoculation training. Behavior Research and Therapy, 48,

816-820.

Flaxman, P. E., & Bond, F. W. (2010b). Worksite stress management training: Moderated effects

and clinical significance. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 15(4), 347-358.

Foley, J., & Stone, G. L. (1988). Stress inoculation with nursing students. Western Journal of

Nursing Research, 10(4), 435-448.

Fowlie, J., & Wood, M. (2009). The emotional impact of leaders' behaviours. Journal of

European Industrial Training, 33(6), 559-572.

French, J. R. P., & Raven, B. (1959). The bases of social power. In D. Cartwright (Ed.), Studies in

social power (pp. 150-167). Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Institute for Social

Research.

236
Fry, L. W. (2003). Toward a theory of spiritual leadership. The Leadership Quarterly, 14, 693-

727.

Furnham, A. (2007). Personality disorders and derailment at work: The paradoxical positive

influence of pathology in the workplace. In J. Langan-Fox, C. L. Cooper & R. J. Klimoski

(Eds.), Research companion to the dysfunctional workplace: Management challenges and

symptoms. (pp. 22-39). UK: Edward Elgar Publishing Limited.

Furnham, A., Richards, S.C., & Paulhus, D.L. (2013). The dark triad of personality: A 10 year

review. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 7, 3, 199-216.

Furnham, A., Trickey, G., & Hyde, G. (2012). Bright aspects to dark side traits: Dark side traits

associated with work success. Personality and Individual Differences, 52, 908-913.

Furukawa, T. A., Kessler, R. C., Slade, T., & Andrews, G. (2003). The performance of the K6 and

K10 screening scales for psychological distress in Australian National Survey of Mental

Health and Well-Being. Psychological Medicine, 33, 357-362.

Gaddis, B. H., & Foster, J. L. (2015). Meta-analysis of dark side personality characteristics and

critical work behaviors among leaders across the globe: Findings and implications for

leadership development and executive coaching. Applied Psychology: An International

Review, 64(1), 25-54.

Gallup. (2013). State of the American workplace. Employee engagement insights for U.S. business

leaders. U.S.A.: Gallup, Inc.

Gander, F., Proyer, R. T., Ruch, W., & Wyss, T. (2012). The good character at work: an initial

study on the contribution of character strengths in identifying healthy and unhealthy work-

related behaviour and experience patterns. International Archives of Occupational and

Environmental Health, 85, 895-904.

237
Gentry, W. A., Hannum, K. M., Ekelund, B. Z., & de Jong, A. (2007). A study of the discrepancy

between self- and observer-ratings on managerial derailment characteristics of European

managers. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 16(30), 295-325.

George, B. (2004). Authentic leadership. Rediscovering the secrets to creating lasting value.

USA: Jossey-Bass.

George, B., Sims, P., McLean, A. N., & Mayer, D. (2007). Discovering your authentic leadership.

Harvard Business Review, February, 129-138.

Giga, S. I., Noblet, A. J., Faragher, B., & Cooper, C. L. (2003). The UK perspective: A review of

research on organisational stress management interventions. Australian Psychologist, 38(2),

158-164.

Gist, M. E., & Mitchell, T. R. (1992). Self-efficacy: A theoretical analysis of its determinants and

malleability. Academy of Management Review, 17(2), 183-211.

Goh, U. W., Sawang, S., & Oei, T. P. S. (2010). The revised transactional model (RTM) of

occupational stress and coping: An improved process approach. The Australian and New

Zealand Journal of Organisational Psychology, 3, 13-20.

Goldberg, L. R. (1992). The development of markers for the Big-Five factor structure.

Psychological Assessment, 4, 26-42.

Goleman, D. (2004). What makes a leader? Harvard Business Review, January 2004, 82-91.

Graen, G. B., & Uhl-Bien, M. (1995). Relationship-based approach to leadership: Development of

leader-member exchange (LMX) theory of leadership over 25 years: Applying a multi-level

multi-domain perspective. The Leadership Quarterly, 6(2), 219-247.

Greenleaf, R. (1998). The power of servant leadership. San Francisco: Berret-Kohler Publisher

Inc.

238
Grijalva, E., & Harms, P. D. (2014). Narcissism: An integrative synthesis and dominance

complementarity model. Academy of Management Perspectives, 28, 108-127.

Grijalva, E., Harms, P. D., Newman, D. A., Gaddis, B. H., & Fraley, R. C. (2015). Narcissism and

leadership: A meta-analytic review of linear and nonlinear relationships. Personnel

Psychology, 68(1), 1-47.

Grijalva, E., & Newman, D. A. (2015). Narcissism and counterproductive work behaviour (CWB):

Meta-analysis and consideration of collectivist culture, Big Five personality, and

narcissism's facet structure. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 64(1), 93-126.

Guenole, N. (2014). Maladaptive personality at work: Exploring the darkness. Industrial and

Organizational Psychology, 7, 85-97.

Hannah, S. T., & Avolio, B. J. (2010). Moral potency: Building the capacity for character-based

leadership. Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice and Research, 62(4), 291-310.

Hannah, S. T., Woolfolk, R. L., & Lord, R. G. (2009). Leader self-structure: A framework for

positive leadership. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 30, 269-290.

Hansbrough, T., & Schyns, B. (2010). Heroic Illusions. How implicit leadership theories shape

follower attributions about poor leader performance. In B. Schyns & T. Hansbrough (Eds.),

When leadership goes wrong. Destructive leadership, mistakes and ethical failures (pp.

513-524). USA: Information Age Publishing, Inc.

Hargrove, M. B., Quick, J. C., Nelson, D. L., & Quick, J. D. (2011). The theory of preventative

stress management: A 33-year review and evaluation. Stress and Health, 27(3), 182-193.

Harland, L., Harrison, W., Jones, J. R., & Reiter-Palmon, R. (2005). Leadership behaviours and

subordinate resilience. Journal of Leadership & Organisational Studies, 11(2), 2-14.

239
Harms, P. D., Spain, S. M., & Hannah, S. T. (2011). Leader development and the dark side of

personality. The Leadership Quarterly, 22, 495-509.

Harris, R. (2008). The Happiness Trap. How to Stop Struggling and Start Living: A Guide to ACT.

USA: Trumpeter Books.

Harris, R. (2009). ACT made simple. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger Publications, Inc.

Hartley, J., & Hinksman, B. (2003). Leadership development: A systematic review of the literature.

A report for NHS Leadership Centre. UK: Warwick Institute of Governance and Public

Management, Warwick Business School, University of Warwick.

Harvey, P., Harris, K. J., Gillis, W. E., & Martinko, M. J. (2014). Abusive supervision and the

entitled employee. The Leadership Quarterly, 25, 204-217.

Harvey, P., Stoner, J., Hochwarter, W., & Kacmar, C. (2007). Coping with abusive supervision:

The neutralizing effects of ingratiation and positive affect on negative employee outcomes.

Leadership Quarterly, 18, 219-239.

Hayes, A. F. (2012). PROCESS: A versatile computational tool for observed variable mediation,

moderation, and conditional process modelling [White paper]. Retrieved from

http://www.afhayes.com/public/process2012.pdf

Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A

regression-based approach. New York: The Guildford Press.

Hayes, S. C., Luoma, J. B., Bond, F. W., Masuda, A., & Lillis, J. (2006). Acceptance and

commitment therapy: Model, processes and outcomes. Behaviour Research and Therapy,

44, 1-25.

Heatherton, T. F., & Baumeister, R. F. (1996). Self-regulation failure: Past, present, and future.

Psychological Inquiry, 7(1), 90-98.

240
Higgins, E. T. (1987). Self-discrepancy: A theory relating self and affect. Psychological Review,

94(3), 319-340.

Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52(12), 1280-1300.

Higgins, E. T. (1999). When do self-discrepancies have specific relations to emotions? The

second-generation question of Tangney, Niedenthal, Covert, and Barlow. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 77(6), 1313-1317.

Hinkin, T. R., & Schriesheim, C. A. (1989). Development and application of new scales to

measure the bases of social power. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 561-567.

Hobman, E. V., Restubog, S. L. D., Bordia, P., & Tang, R. L. (2009). Abusive supervision in

advising relationships: Investigating the role of social support. Applied Psychology: An

International Review, 58, 233-256.

Hogan, R., Curphy, G. J., & Hogan, J. (1994). What we know about leadership: Effectiveness and

personality. American Psychologist, 49, 493-504.

Hogan, R., & Hogan, J. (2001). Assessing leadership: A view from the dark side. International

Journal of Selection and Assessment, 9(1&2), 40-51.

Hogan, R., & Kaiser, R. B. (2005). What we know about leadership. Review of General

Psychology, 9(169-180).

Hogan, R., Raskin, R., & Fazzini, D. (1990). The dark side of charisma. In K. E. Clark & M. B.

Clark (Eds.), Measures of leadership. West Orange, NJ: Leadership Library of America.

Hollander, E. P. (1992). The essential interdependence of leadership and followership. Current

Directions in Psychological Science, 1, 71-75.

Hollander, E. P., & Julian, J. W. (1969). Contemporary trends in the analysis of the leadership

process. Psychological Bulletin, 71, 387-397.

241
House, R. J., & Howell, J. M. (1992). Personality and charismatic leadership. Leadership

Quarterly, 3(2), 81-108.

Hsieh, J., & Shannon, S. (2005). Three approaches to qualitative content analysis. Qualitative

Health Research, 15(9), 1277-1288.

Ivancevich, J. M., Matteson, M. T., Freedman, S. M., & Phillips, J. S. (1990). Worksite stress

management interventions. American Psychologist, 45, 252-261.

Javaras, K.N., Schaefer, S.M., van Reekum, C.M., Lapate, R.C., Greischar, L.L., Bachhuber, D.R.,

et al. (2012). Conscientiousness predicts greater recovery from negative emotion. Emotion,

12, 875–881.

Jonason, P. K., & Webster, G. D. (2010). The dirty dozen: A concise measure of the dark triad.

Psychological Assessment, 22(2), 420-432.

Jones, D. N., & Paulhus, D. L. (2011). The role of impulsivity in the Dark Triad of personality.

Personality and Individual Differences, 51, 679-683.

Jordan, P. J., & Lindebaum, D. (2015). A model of within person variation in leadership: Emotion

regulation and scripts as predictors of situationally appropriate leadership. The Leadership

Quarterly, 26, 594-605.

Judge, T. A., & Bono, J. E. (2001). Relationship of core self-evaluations traits - self-esteem,

generalized self-efficacy, locus of control, and emotional stability - with job satisfaction and

job performance: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86(1), 80-92.

Judge, T. A., Bono, J. E., Illies, R., & Gerhardt, M. W. (2002). Personality and leadership: A

qualitative and quantitative review. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87(4), 765-780.

Judge, T. A., & LePine, J. A. (2007). The bright and dark sides of personality: Implications for

personnel selection in individual and team contexts. In J. Langan-Fox, C. L. Cooper & R. J.

242
Klimoski (Eds.), Research companion to the dysfunctional workplace: Management

challenges and symptoms. (pp. 332-355.). UK: Edward Elgar Publishing Limited.

Judge, T. A., & Piccolo, R. F. (2004). Transformational and transactional leadership: A meta-

analytic test of their relative validity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89(5), 755-768.

Judge, T. A., Piccolo, R. F., & Kosalka, T. (2009). The bright and dark sides of leader traits: A

review and theoretical extension of the leader trait paradigm. The Leadership Quarterly,

20(6), 855-875.

Junker, N. M., & van Dick, R. (2014). Implicit theories in organisational settings: A systematic

review and research agenda of implicit leadership and followership theories. The

Leadership Quarterly, 25, 1154-1173.

Kaiser, R. B., & Hogan, J. (2011). Personality, leader behavior, and overdoing it. Consulting

Psychology Journal: Practice and Research, 63(4), 219-242.

Kaiser, R. B., & Kaplan, R. B. (2006). The deeper work of executive development: Outgrowing

sensitivities. Academy of Management Learning and Education, 5(4), 463-483.

Kaiser, R. B., LeBreton, J. M., & Hogan, J. (2015). The dark side of personality and extreme

leader behavior. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 64(1), 55-92.

Kaplan, R. E., & Kaiser, R. B. (2008). The versatile leader. Make the most of your strengths

without overdoing it. USA: Pfeiffer.

Kaplan, R. E., & Kaiser, R. B. (2009). Stop overdoing your strengths. Harvard Business Review,

February 2009.

Karoly, P. (1993). Mechanisms of self-regulation: A systems view. Annual Review Psychology,

44, 23-52.

243
Kellerman, B. (2004a). Bad leadership: What it is, how it happens, why it matters. New York:

Harvard Business Review Press.

Kellerman, B. (2004b). Leadership - warts and all. Harvard Business Review, January 2004.

Kellerman, B. (2008). Followership: How followers are creating change and changing leaders.

USA: Harvard Business School Publishing.

Kelloway, E. K., & Barling, J. (2010). Leadership development as an intervention in occupational

health psychology. Work & Stress, 24(3), 260-279.

Khoo, H., & Burch, G. (2008). The 'dark side' of leadership personality and transformational

leadership. Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 86-97.

Kiazad, K., Restubog, S. L. D., Zagenczyk, T. J., Kiewitz, C., & Tang, R. L. (2010). In pursuit of

power: The role of authoritarian leadership in the relationship between supervisors'

Machiavellianism and subordinates' perceptions of abusive supervisory behavior. Journal of

Research in Personality, 44, 412-519.

Kidd, J. M. (2008). Exploring the components of career well-being and the emotions associated

with significant career experiences. Journal of Career Development, 35(2), 166-186.

King, G. A., & Rothstein, M. G. (2010). Resilience and leadership: The self-management of

failure. In M. G. Rothstein & R. J. Burke (Eds.), Self-Management and Leadership

Development. Cheltenham: Edward Elgar Publishing.

Knox Haly, M. (2008). Managing bullying at work. InPsych, 30(2), 14-15.

Koole, S. L. (2009). The psychology of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Cognition and

Emotion, 23(1), 4-41.

244
Kotov, R., Gamez, W., Schmidt, F., & Watson, D. (2010). Linking “big” personality traits to

anxiety, depressive, and substance use disorders: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin,

136, 768–821.

Kouzes, J., & Posner, B. (2015). Extraordinary Leadership in Australia and New Zealand. The

five practices that create great workplaces. Melbourne: John Wiley & Sons Australia, Ltd.

Krasikova, D. V., Green, S. G., & LeBreton, J. M. (2013). Destructive leadership: A theoretical

review, integration, and future research agenda. Journal of Management 39(5), 1308-1338.

Kupers, W., & Weibler, J. (2005). How emotional is transformation leadership really? Leadership

& Organization Development Journal, 27(5), 368-383.

Lamontagne, A. D., Keegel, T., Louie, A. M., Ostry, A., & Landsbergis, P. A. (2007). A

systematic review of the job-stress intervention evaluation literature, 1990-2005.

International Journal of Occupational and Environmental Health, 13(3), 268-280.

Lange, D. (2008). A multidimensional conceptualization of organizational corruption control.

Academy of Management Review, 33(3), 710-729.

Lawrence, S. A., Troth, A. C., Jordan, P. J., & Collins, A. L. (2011). A review of emotion

regulation and development of a framework for emotion regulation in the workplace. The

role of individual differences in occupational stress and well being research in occupational

stress and well being, 9, 197-263.

Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal and coping. New York: Springer

Publications.

LePine, J. A., Podsakoff, N. P., & LePine, M. A. (2005). A meta-analytic test of the challenge

stressor-hindrance stressor framework: An explanation of inconsistent relationships among

stressors and performance. Academy of Management Journal, 48(5), 764-775.

245
Leslie, J. B., & Velsor, E. V. (1996). A look at derailment today: North America and Europe.

USA: CCL Press.

Liebmann, M. (2007). Restorative Justice. How it works. USA: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.

Lievens, F., De Corte, W., & Schollaert, E. (2008). A closer look at the frame-of-reference effect

in personality scale scores and validity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93(2), 268-279.

Lilienfield, S. O., & Andrews, B. P. (1996). Development and preliminary validation of a self-

report measure of psychopathic personality traits in noncriminal populations. Journal of

Personality Assessment, 66(3), 488-524.

Lin, W., Ma, J., Wang, L., & Wang, M. (2015). A double-edged sword: The moderating role of

conscientiousness in the relationships between work stressors, psychological strain, and job

performance. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 36, 94-111.

Lincoln, K. D. (2000). Social support, negative social interactions, and psychological well-being.

Social Service Review, 74(2), 231-252.

Lipman-Blumen, J. (2005). The allure of toxic leaders: Why we follow destructive bosses and

corrupt politicians - and how we can survive them. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Lipsey, M. W., & Cordray, D. S. (2000). Evaluation methods for social intervention. Annual

Review of Psychology, 51, 345-375.

London, M. (1983). Toward a theory of career motivation. Academy of Management Review, 8(4),

620-630.

London, M. (1997). Overcoming career barriers: A model of cognitive and emotional processes for

realistic appraisal and constructive coping. Journal of Career Development, 24(1), 25-28.

246
Lowe, K. B., Kroeck, K. G., & Sivasubramaniam, N. (1996). Effectiveness correlates of

transformational and transactional leadership: A meta-analytic review of the MLQ literature.

Leadership Quarterly, 7(3), 385-425.

Lubit, R. H. (2004). Coping with toxic managers, subordinates and other difficult people. USA:

Pearson Education Inc.

Luthans, F., Youssef, C. M., & Avolio, B. J. (2007). Psychological capital: Developing the human

competitive edge. New York: Oxford University Press.

Maak, T., & Pless, N. M. (2006). Responsible leadership in a stakeholder society - a relational

perspective. Journal of Business Ethics, 66(1), 99-115.

Maccoby, M. (2004). Narcisstic leaders: The incredible pros, the inevitable cons. Harvard

Business Review, 82(1).

Maccoby, M. (2007). Narcisstic leaders: Who succeeds and who fails. New York: Harvard

Business School Press.

Mackey, J. D., Parker Ellen III, B., Hochwarter, W. A., & Ferris, G. R. (2013). Subordinate social

adaptability and the consequences of abusive supervision perceptions in two samples. The

Leadership Quarterly, 24, 732-746.

Mackie, D. (2008). Leadership derailment and psychological harm. InPsych, 30(2), 12-13.

Manz, C. C. (1986). Self-leadership: Toward an expanded theory of self-influence processes in

organisations. Academy of Management Review, 11(3), 585-600.

Martinko, M. J., Harvey, P., Brees, J. R., & Mackey, J. (2013). A review of abusive supervision

research. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 34, S120-S137.

247
Martinko, M. J., Harvey, P., Sikora, D., & Douglas, S. C. (2011). Perceptions of abusive

supervision: The role of subordinates' attribution styles. The Leadership Quarterly, 22, 751-

764.

Mawritz, M. B., Folger, R., & Lathan, G. P. (2014). Supervisors' exceedingly difficult goals and

abusive supervision: The mediating effects of hindrance stress, anger, and anxiety. Journal

of Organizational Behavior, 35, 358-372.

May, D., Wesche, J. S., Heinitz, K., & Kerschreiter, R. (2014). Coping with destructive leadership.

Putting forward an integrated theoretical framework for the interaction process between

leaders and followers. Journal of Psychology, 222(4), 203-213.

McCall, M. W. J., & Lombardo, M. M. (1983). Off the track: Why and how successful executives

get derailed (Leadership Report No. 21). Greensboro: Center for Creative Leadership.

McCartney, W. W., & Campbell, C. R. (2006). Leadership, management and derailment: A model

of individual success and failure. Leadership & Organization Development Journal, 27(3),

190.

McClelland, D. C. (1970). The two faces of power. Journal of International Affairs, 24, 29-47.

McClelland, D. C. (1975). Power: The inner experience. New York: Irvington.

McClelland, D. C., & Boyatzis, R. E. (1982). Leadership motive pattern and long-term success in

management. Journal of Applied Psychology, 67(6), 737-743.

McConachie, D. A. J., McKenzie, K., Morris, P. G., & Walley, R. M. (2014). Acceptance and

mindfulness-based stress management for support staff caring for individuals with

intellectual disabilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 35, 1216-1227.

McFarlin, D. B., & Sweeney, P. D. (2010). The corporate reflecting pool - antecedents and

consequences of narcissism in executives. In B. Schyns & T. Hansborough (Eds.), When

248
leadership goes wrong: Destructive leadership, mistakes and ethical failures (pp. 247-283).

Chicago: Information Age Publishing.

McHoskey, J.W., Worzel, W., & Szyarto, C. (1998). Machiavellianism and psychopathy. Journal

of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1, 192-210.

McIntosh, G. L., & Rima, S. D. (2007). Overcoming the dark side of leadership. How to become

an effective leader by confronting potential failures. Michigan: Baker Books.

McLarnon, M. J. W., & Rothstein, M. G. (2013). Development and initial validation of the

Workplace Resilience Inventory. Journal of Personnel Psychology, 12(2), 63-73.

Medibank Private. (2008). The cost of workplace stress in Australia: August 2008. Australia:

Medibank Private.

Meichenbaum, D. (1996). Stress inoculation training for coping with stressors. The Clinical

Psychologist, 49, 4-7.

Meyers, M. C., van Woerkom, M., & Bakker, A. B. (2013). The added value of the positive: A

literature review of positive psychology interventions in organizations. European Journal of

Work and Organizational Psychology, 22(5), 618-632.

Mitchell, M. S., & Ambrose, M. L. (2007). Abusive supervision and workplace deviance and the

moderating effects of negative reciprocity beliefs. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92(4),

1159-1168.

Moore, D. B. (1996). Transformation in the workplace: Affect and script in the business world.

Transformative Justice Australia.

Moran, D. J. (2011). ACT for leadership: Using acceptance and commitment training to develop

crisis-resilient change managers. The International Journal of Behavioral Consultation and

Therapy, 7(1), 68-77.

249
Mulvey, P. W., & Padilla, A. (2010). The environment of destructive leadership. In B. Schyns &

T. Hansborough (Eds.), When leadership goes wrong: Destructive leadership, mistakes and

ethical failures (pp. 49-71). Chicago: Information Age Publishing.

Murphy, L. R. (1988). Workplace interventions for stress reduction and prevention. In C. L.

Cooper & R. Payne (Eds.), Causes, coping, and consequences of stress at work (pp. 301-

339). New York: Wiley.

Murphy, L. R. (1996). Stress management in work settings: A critical review of the health effects.

American Journal of Health Promotion, 11(2), 112-135.

Najar, M. J., Holland, B. D., & Van Landuyt, C. R. (2004). Individual differences in leadership

derailment. Chicago, Illinois: Paper presented at the 19th Annual Conference of the Society

of Industrial and Organisational Psychology, 2 April 2004.

Nandkeolyar, A. K., Shaffer, J. A., Li, A., Ekkirala, S., & Bagger, J. (2014). Surviving an abusive

supervisor: The joint roles of conscientiousness and coping strategies. Journal of Applied

Psychology, 99(1), 138-150.

Nelson, E., & Hogan, R. (2009). Coaching on the dark side. International Coaching Psychology

Review, 4(1), 7-19.

Nes, L. S., & Segerstrom, S. C. (2006). Dispositional optimism and coping: A meta-analytic

review. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10(3), 235-251.

Nielsen, K., & Abildgaard, J. S. (2013). Organizational interventions: A research-based framework

for the evaluation of both process and effects. Work & Stress: An International Journal of

Work, Health & Organisations, 27(3), 278-297.

250
Nielsen, K., Taris, T. W., & Cox, T. (2010). The future of organizational interventions: Addressing

the challenges of today's organizations. Work & Stress: An International Journal of Work,

Health & Organisations, 24(3), 219-233.

Nielsen, M. B., & Einarsen, S. (2012). Outcomes of exposure to workplace bullying: A meta-

analytic review. Work & Stress, 26(4), 309-332.

Nielsen, M. L. (2000). Challenges in performing work environment interventions for research

purposes. In G. Wickstrom (Ed.), Intervention studies in the health care work environment.

Sweden: The National Institute for Working Life.

Nieminen, L. R. G., Smerek, R., Kotrba, L., & Denison, D. (2013). What does an executive

coaching intervention add beyond facilitated multisource feedback? Effects on leader self-

ratings and perceived effectiveness. Human Resource Development Quarterly, 24(2), 145-

176.

Nyberg, D., & Sveningsson, S. (2014). Paradoxes of authentic leadership: Leader identity

struggles. Leadership, 10(4), 437-455.

NytrØ, K., Saksvik, P. O., Mikkelsen, A., Bohle, P., & Quinlan, M. (2000). An appraisal of key

factors in the implementation of occupational stress interventions. Work & Stress, 14(3),

213-225.

O'Boyle, E. H., & Forsyth, D. R. (2012). A meta-analysis of the dark triad and work behavior: A

social exchange perspective. Journal of Applied Psychology, 97(3), 557-579.

O'Connor, J., Mumford, M. D., Clifton, T. C., Gessner, T. L., & Connelly, M. S. (1995).

Charismatic leaders and destructiveness: An historiometric study. Leadership Quarterly,

6(4), 529-555.

251
O'Driscoll, M., Brough, P., & Kalliath, T. (2009). Stress and coping. In S. Cartwright & C. Cooper

(Eds.), The Oxford handbook of organizational well being (pp. 237-266). Oxford: Oxford

University Press.

O'Driscoll, M. P., & Brough, P. (2010). Work organization and health. In S. Leka & J. Houdmont

(Eds.), Occupational Health Psychology (pp. 57-87). Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.

Oc, B., & Bashshur, M. R. (2013). Followership, leadership and social influence. The Leadership

Quarterly, 24, 919-934.

Ogunfowora, B. (2013). When the abuse is unevenly distributed: The effects of abusive

supervision variability on work attitudes and behaviours. Journal of Organizational

Behaviour, 34, 1105–1123.

Öst, L. (2014). The efficacy of Acceptance and Commitment Therapy: An updated systematic

review and meta-analysis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 61, 105-121.

Padilla, A., Hogan, R., & Kaiser, R. B. (2007). The toxic triangle: Destructive leaders, susceptible

followers and conducive environments. The Leadership Quarterly, 18, 176-194.

Paulhus, D. L., & Williams, K. M. (2002). The dark triad of personality: Narcissism,

Machiavellianism, and psychopathy. Journal of Research in Personality, 36, 556-563.

Pearce, C. L., & Sims, H. P. (2002). Vertical versus shared leadership as predictors of the

effectiveness of change management teams: An examination of aversive, directive,

transactional, transformational, and empowering leader behaviours. Group Dynamics:

Theory, Research, and Practice, 6, 172-197.

Pelletier, K. L. (2010). Leader toxicity: An empirical investigation of toxic behaviour and rhetoric.

Leadership 6(4), 373-389.

252
Pelletier, K. L. (2012). Perceptions of and reactions to leader toxicity: Do leader-follower

relationships and identification with victim matter? The Leadership Quarterly, 23, 412-424.

Perlow, L., & Williams, S. (2003). Is silence killing your company? Harvard Business Review,

May 2003, 52-58.

Phillips, J. S., & Lord, R. G. (1981). Causal attribution and prescriptions of leadership.

Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 28, 143-163.

Pipe, T. B., Bortz, J. J., Dueck, A., Pendergast, D., Buchda, V., & Summers, J. (2009). Nurse

leader mindfulness meditation program for stress management. Journal of Nursing

Administration, 39(3), 130-137.

Pipe, T. B., Buchda, V. L., Launder, S., Hudak, B., Hulvey, L., Karns, K. E., et al. (2012).

Building personal and professional resources of resilience and agility in the healthcare

workplace. Stress and Health, 28, 11-22.

Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J. Y., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method

biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies.

Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(5), 879-903.

Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2012). Sources of method bias in social

science research and recommendations on how to control it. Annual Review of Psychology,

63, 539-569.

Potter, W., & Levine-Donnerstein, D. (1999). Rethinking validity and reliability in content

analysis. Journal of Applied Communication Research, 27(2), 258-284.

Powell, K. S., & Yalcin, S. (2010). Managerial training effectiveness. A meta-analysis 1952-2002.

Personnel Review, 39(2), 227-241.

253
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in

simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 36(4),

717-731.

Preacher, K. J., & Kelley, K. (2011). Effect size measures for mediation models: Quantitative

strategies for communicating indirect effects. Psychological Methods, 16(2), 93-115.

Previsor. (2001). PDI Global Personality Inventory (GPI) Technical Manual. USA: Personnel

Decisions International Corporation.

Probst, T. M. (2013). Conducting effective stress intervention research: Strategies for achieving an

elusive goal. Stress and Health, 29, 1-4.

Pundt, A. (2014). A multiple pathway model linking charismatic leadership attempts and abusive

supervision. Zeitschrift fur Psychologie, 222, 190-202.

Rafferty, A. E., & Restubog, S. L. D. (2011). The influence of abusive supervisors on followers'

organizational citizenship behaviours: The hidden costs of abusive supervision. British

Journal of Management, 22, 270-285.

Randall, R., Nielsen, K., & Tvedt, S. D. (2009). The development of five scales to measure

employees' appraisals of organizational-level stress management interventions. Work &

Stress, 23(1), 1-23.

Rate, C. R., & Sternberg, R. J. (2007). When good people do nothing: A failure of courage. In J.

Langan-Fox, C. L. Cooper & R. J. Klimoski (Eds.), Research companion to the

dysfunctional workplace: Management challenges and symptoms. UK: Edward Elgar

Publishing Limited.

254
Redeker, M., de Vries, R. E., Rouckhout, D., Vermeren, P., & de Fruyt, F. (2014). Integrating

leadership: The leadership circumplex. European Journal of Work and Organizational

Psychology, 23(3), 435-455.

Richards, H., & Freeman, S. (2002). Bullying in the workplace: An occupational hazard. NSW:

Harpers Collins.

Richardson, K. M., & Rothstein, H. R. (2008). Effects of occupational stress management

intervention programs: A meta-analysis. Journal of Occupational and Health Psychology,

13(1), 69-93.

Rogelberg, S. G., Justice, L., Braddy, P. W., Paustian-Underdahl, S. C., Heggestad, E., Shanock, L.,

et al. (2013). The executive mind: leader self-talk, effectiveness and strain. Journal of

Managerial Psychology, 28(2), 183-201.

Rosenthal, S. A., & Pittinsky, T. L. (2006). Narcissistic leadership. The Leadership Quarterly, 17,

617-633.

Rost, J., & Smith, A. (1992). Leadership: A postindustrial approach. European Management

Journal, 10(2), 193-201.

Roth, S., & Cohen, L. J. (1986). Approach, avoidance, and coping with stress. American

Psychologist, 41(7), 813-819.

Rothstein, M. G., & Burke, R. J. (Eds.). (2010). Self-management and leadership development.

UK: Edward Elgar Publishing Limited.

Roussi, P., Miller, S. M., & Shoda, Y. (2000). Discriminative facility in the face of threat:

Relationship to psychological distress. Psychology and Health, 15(1), 21-33.

Ruderman, M. N., Hannum, K., Leslie, J. B., & Steed, J. L. (2001). Making the connection:

leadership skills and emotional intelligence. LIA, 21(5), 3-7.

255
Safe Work Australia. (2011). Model Work Health and Safety Bill: Revised draft 23/6/11.

Canberra: Safe Work Australia.

Safe Work Australia. (2013). The incidence of accepted workers' compensation claims for mental

stress in Australia. Canberra: Safe Work Australia.

Safe Work Australia. (2014). Australian Workers' Compensation Statistics 2011-12. Canberra:

Safe Work Australia.

Salas, E., & Cannon-Bowers, J. A. (2001). The science of training: A decade of progress. Annual

Review of Psychology, 52, 471-499.

Saldana, J. (2009). The coding manual for qualitative researchers. London: Sage Publications.

Samnani, A., & Singh, P. (2012). 20 years of workplace bullying research: A review of the

antecedents and consequences of bullying in the workplace. Aggression and Violent

Behavior, 17, 581-589.

Sandler, C. (2012). The emotional profiles triangle: Working with leaders under pressure.

Strategic HR Review, 11(2), 65-71.

Saunders, T., Driskell, J. E., Johnston, J. H., & Salas, E. (1996). The effect of stress inoculation

training on anxiety and performance. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 1(2),

170-186.

Schabracq, M. J., & Smit, I. E. (2007). Leadership and ethics: The darker side of management. In

J. Langan-Fox, C. L. Cooper & R. J. Klimoski (Eds.), Research companion to the

dysfunctional workplace: Management challenges and symptoms. (pp. 110-124). UK:

Edward Elgar Publishing Limited.

Schmit, M. J., Kihm, J. A., & Robie, C. (2000). Development of a global measure of personality.

Personnel Psychology, 53(1), 153-193.

256
Schyns, B. (2015). Dark personaltiy in the workplace: Introduction to the special issue. Applied

Psychology: An International Review, 64(1), 1-14.

Schyns, B., & Hansborough, T. (2010). When leadership goes wrong: Destructive leadership,

mistakes and ethical failures. USA: Information Age Publishing.

Schyns, B., & Schilling, J. (2013). How bad are the effects of bad leaders? A meta-analysis of

destructive leadership and its outcomes. The Leadership Quarterly, 24, 138-158.

Seligman, M. (1992). Learned Optimism. NSW: Random House.

Seligman, M. (2002). Authentic Happiness. NSW: Random House.

Seligman, M., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology: An introduction. American

Psychologist, 55, 5-14.

Shapiro, P. L., & Von Glinow, M. A. (2007). Why bad leaders stay in good places. In J. Langan-

Fox, C. L. Cooper & R. J. Klimoski (Eds.), Research Companion to the dysfunctional

workplace: Management challenges and symptoms. (pp. 3-21.). UK: Edward Elgar

Publishing Limited.

Skinner, E. A., Edge, K., Altman, J., & Sherwood, H. (2003). Searching for the structure of

coping: A review and critique of category systems for classifying ways of coping.

Psychological Bulletin, 129(2), 216-269.

Skogstad, A., Einarsen, S., Torsheim, T., Aasland, M. S., & Hetland, H. (2007). The

destructiveness of laissez-faire leadership behaviour. Journal of Occupational and Health

Psychology, 12(1), 80-92.

Smith, J., Ryan, L. H., & Röcke, C. (2013). The day-to-day effects of conscientiousness on well-

being. Research in Human Development, 10(1), 9-25.

257
Smith, S. F., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2013). Psychopathy in the workplace: The knowns and

unknowns. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 18, 204-218.

Sosik, J. J., & Cameron, J. C. (2010). Character and authentic transformational leadership

behaviour: Expanding the ascetic self toward others. Consulting Psychology Journal:

Practice and Research, 62(4), 251-269.

Sparks, K., Faragher, B., & Cooper, C. L. (2001). Well-being and occupational health in the 21st

century workplace. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 74, 489-509.

Spreier, S. W., Fontaine, M. H., & Malloy, R. L. (2006). Leadership run amok: The destructive

potential of overachievers. Harvard Business Review, June 2006, 73-82.

Stafford-Brown, J., & Pakenham, K. I. (2012). The effectiveness of an ACT informed intervention

for managing stress and improving therapist qualities in clinical psychology trainees.

Journal of Clinical Psychology, 68(6), 592-613.

Steger, M. F. (2012). Spiritual Leadership. In P. C. Hill & B. J. Dik (Eds.), Psychology of religion

and workplace spirituality (pp. 223-238). Charlotte: Information Age Publishing.

Stogdill, R. M. (1948). Personal factors associated with leadership. Journal of Psychology, 25, 35-

71.

Suls, J., & Fletcher, B. (1985). The relative efficacy of avoidant and nonavoidant coping strategies:

A meta-analysis. Health Psychology, 4(3), 249-288.

Sutton, R. (2007). The no asshole rule. Building a civilized workplace and surviving one that isn't.

Great Britain: Sphere.

Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2013). Using Multivariate Statistics. Sixth Edition. USA:

Pearson Education Inc.

TalentQ. (2013). Dimensions Psychometric Review: Hay Group.

258
Tanner, C., Brugger, A., van Schie, S., & Lebherz, C. (2010). Actions speak louder than words.

The benefits of ethical behaviours of leaders. Journal of Psychology, 218(4), 225-233.

Tepper, B. J. (2000). Consequences of abusive supervision. Academy of Management, 43, 178-

190.

Tepper, B. J. (2007). Abusive supervision in work organisations: Review, synthesis, and research

agenda. Journal of Management Development, 33(3), 261-289.

Tepper, B. J., Duffy, M. K., Henle, C. A., & Lambert, L. S. (2006). Procedural injustice, victim

precipitation, and abusive supervision. Personnel Psychology, 59, 101-123.

Tepper, B. J., Moss, S. E., Lockhart, D. E., & Carr, J. C. (2007). Abusive supervision, upward

maintainance communication, and subordinates' psychological distress. Academy of

Management Journal, 50(5), 1169-1180.

Tett, R. P., & Burnett, D. D. (2003). A personality trait-based interactionist model of job

performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(3), 500-517.

Thoroughgood, C. N., Padilla, A., Hunter, S. T., & Tate, B. W. (2012). The susceptible circle: A

taxonomy of followers associated with destructive leadership. The Leadership Quarterly,

23, 897-917.

Thoroughgood, C. N., Tate, B. W., Sawyer, K. B., & Jacobs, R. (2012) . Bad to the bone:

Empirically defining and measuring destructive leader behavior. Journal of Leadership &

Organizational Studies, 19(2), 230-255.

Thorsborne, M. (1999). Beyond punishment - workplace conferencing: An effective organisational

response to incidents of workplace bullying. Paper presented at the Beyond Bullying

Association's 'Responding to Professional Abuse Conference', Brisbane, July.

259
Thorsborne, M. (2009). Workplace Conferencing: An effective organisational response to

transforming workplace conflict. Sunshine Coast: Transformative Justice Australia

(Queensland).

Trickey, G., & Hyde, G. (2009). A decade of the dark side. Fighting our demons at work. UK:

Whitepaper by Psychological Consultancy Limited.

Tsui, A. S., & Ashford, S. J. (1994). Adaptive self-regulation: A process view of managerial

effectiveness. Journal of Management, 20(1), 93-121.

Uhl-Bien, M., Riggio, R. E., Lowe, K. B., & Carsten, M. K. (2014). Followership theory: A review

and research agenda. The Leadership Quarterly, 25, 83-104.

Valliant, G. (1977). Adaptation to life. USA: Little, Brown & Company.

Van der Hek, H., & Plomp, H. N. (1997). Occupation stress management programmes. A practical

overview of published effect studies. Occupational Medicine, 47(3), 133-141.

van der Klink, J. J. L., Blonk, W. B. R., Schene, A. H., & van Dijk, F. J. H. (2001). The benefits of

interventions for work-related stress. American Journal of Public Health, 91, 270-276.

van Emmerik, I. H., Euwema, M. C., & Bakker, A. B. (2007). Threats of workplace violence and

the buffering effect of social support. Group and Organization Management, 32(2), 152-

175.

van Knippenberg, D. (2012). Leadership: A person-in-situation perspective. In K. Deaux & M.

Snyder (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Personality and Social Psychology (pp. 673-700).

Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Vazire, S., & Funder, D. C. (2006). Impulsivity and the self-defeating behaviour of narcissists.

Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10(2), 154-165.

260
Viswesvaran, C., Sanchez, J. I., & Fisher, J. (1999). The role of social support in the process of

work stress: A meta-analysis. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 54, 314-334.

Vuori, J., Toppinen-Tanner, S., & Mutanen, P. (2012). Effects of resource-building group

intervention on career management and mental health in work organisations: Randomised

controlled field trial. Journal of Applied Psychology, 97(2), 273-286.

Wachtel, T. (1999, ). Restorative justice in everyday life: Beyond the formal ritual. Paper

presented at the Reshaping Australian Institutions Conference: Restorative justice and civil

society, Canberra, February.

Wallis, P., & Tudor, B. (2008). The pocket guide to Restorative Justice. London: Jessica Kingsley

Publishers.

Wang, G., Oh, I. S., Courtright, S. H., & Colbert, A. E. (2011). Transformational leadership and

performance across criteria and levels: A meta-analytic review of 25 years of research.

Group and Organisation Management, 36(2), 223-270.

Wang, M., Sinclair, R., & Deese, M. N. (2010). Understanding the causes of destructive leadership

behaviour - a dual-process model. In B. Schyns & T. Hansborough (Eds.), When leadership

goes wrong: Destructive leadership, mistakes and ethical failures (pp. 73-97). Chicago:

Information Age Publishing.

Warr, P. (1990). The measurement of well-being and other aspects of mental health. Journal of

Occupational Psychology, 63, 193-210.

Waung, M. (1995). The effects of self-regulatory coping orientation on newcomer adjustment and

job survival. Personnel Psychology, 58(3), 633-650.

Webster, V., & Brough, P. (2015). Assisting organisations to deal effectively with toxic leadership

in the workplace. InPsych (June), 24-25.

261
Webster, V., Brough, P., & Daly, K. (2016). Fight, flight or freeze: Common responses for follower

coping with toxic leadership. Stress & Health, 32, 346-354. DOI: 10.1002/smi.2626

West, D. J., Horan, J. J., & Games, P. A. (1984). Component analysis of occupational stress

inoculation applied to registered nurses in an acute care hospital setting. Journal of

Counseling Psychology, 31, 209-218.

Whitman, M. V., Halbesleben, J. R. B., & Shanine, K. K. (2013). Psychological entitlement and

abusive supervision: Political skill as a self-regulatory mechanism. Health Care

Management Review, 38(3), 248-257.

Wickstrom, G. (Ed.). (2000). Intervention studies in the health care work environment. Sweden:

National Institute for Working Life.

Wotruba, T. R., Chonko, L. B., & Loe, T. W. (2001). The impact of ethics code familiarity on

manager behaviour. Journal of Business Ethics, 33(1), 59-69.

Wu, T. Y., & Hu, C. (2009). Abusive supervision and employee emotional exhaustion:

Dispositional antecedents and boundaries. Group and Organisation Management, 34, 143-

169.

Yagil, D., Ben-Zur, H., & Tamir, I. (2011). Do employees cope effectively with abusive

supervision at work? An exploratory study. International Journal of Stress Management,

18(1), 5-23.

Yukl, G., & van Fleet, D. D. (1992). Theory and research on leadership in organisations. In M. D.

Dunette & L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (pp.

147-199). Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press.

262
Zakowski, S. G., Hall, M. H., Klein, L. C., & Baum, A. (2001). Appraised control, coping, and

stress in a community sample: A test of the goodness-of-fit hypothesis. Annals of

Behavioural Medicine, 23(3), 158-165.

Zellars, K. L., Justice, L., & Beck, T. E. (2011). Resilience: New paths for building and sustaining

individual and organizational capacity. Research in Occupational Stress and Well Being, 9,

1-37.

Zhao, X., Lynch, J. G., & Chen, Q. (2010). Reconsidering Baron and Kenny: Myths and truths

about mediation analysis. Journal of Consumer Research, 37, 197-206.

263
Appendix A

Destructive Leadership Definitions and Research Foci

Table 1

Summary of destructive leadership definitions and focus

Author Construct Definition Focus of research

Lombardo et al. Leader derailment Plateaued, demoted, or fired below the level of Career derailment; harm to the leader

(1988)
anticipated achievement or reaching that level only to

fail unexpectantly.

Schmidt et al. Leader derailment The tendency to use quasi-leadership tactics or to Harm caused to followers:

(2000) engage in various behaviours that may prove


 Derailing personality traits (GPI)
successful in changing others behaviour in the short

term, but ultimately cause the leader to fail or lose

support of those around him or her.

Hogan and Leader derailment Managerial incompetence due to a dysfunctional Harm caused to followers:

Hogan (2001) disposition  Derailing personality traits (HDS)

264
Tepper (2000) Abusive Supervision A sustained display of hostile verbal and nonverbal Harm caused to followers:

behaviours, excluding physical contact.


 Behaviours perceived as abusive by

followers

Lipman-Blumen Toxic leadership Leaders who engage in numerous destructive Harm caused to followers and

(2005) behaviours and who exhibit certain dysfunctional organisations:

personal characteristics. To be toxic these


 Dysfunctional personality
behaviours and qualities of character must inflict characteristics.
some reasonably serious and enduring harm on their
 Destructive behaviours.
followers and their organisations.

Einarson et al. Destructive The systematic and repeated behaviour by a leader, Harm caused to the organisation and

(2007) Leadership supervisor or manager that violates the legitimate followers:

interest of the organisation by undermining and/or


 Tyrannical leadership;
sabotaging the organisation's goals, tasks, resources,
 Derailed leadership;
and effectiveness and/or the motivation, well-being
 Supportive – Disloyal;
or job satisfaction of subordinates.

265
 Constructive

Krasikova et al. Destructive Volitional behaviour by a leader that can harm or Harm caused to the organisation and

(2013) Leadership intends to harm a leader’s organisation and/or followers

followers by (a) encouraging

followers to pursue goals that contravene the

legitimate interests of the organisation

and/or (b) employing a leadership style that involves

the use of harmful methods of influence

with followers, regardless of justifications for such

behaviour

Schyns and Destructive A process in which over a longer period of time the Harm caused to followers

Schilling (2013) Leadership activities, experiences and/or relationships of an

individual or the members of a group are repeatedly

influenced by their supervisor in a way that is

perceived as hostile and/or obstructive.

266
Appendix B

Abstracts from papers presented in Chapters 4 & 5

Antecedents to derailing leadership traits: The mediating roles of negative affectivity and self-

regulation traits

Abstract

Scholarly discussions have proposed the association of extreme levels of some personality traits

with the “dark side of leadership”. This study investigates the relationship between high levels of

the personality trait of independence and the predisposition to exhibit negative leadership behaviors,

and the role of self-regulatory personality traits in mediating these relationships. A sample of senior

managers (N = 300) from a range of public and private industries responded to a self-report survey

assessing their personality traits. Managers reporting high levels of independence were more likely

to be predisposed to exhibit derailing leadership behaviors. These associations were significantly

mediated by the leaders’ levels of dispositional negative affectivity, which reduced the impact of the

self-regulation traits of self-awareness and stress tolerance. Understanding how intrapersonal

differences increase or inhibit the enactment of potentially derailing leadership behaviors furthers

our understanding of antecedents of the managerial derailment process and inform ways to address

this risk.

267
Fight, flight or freeze: Common responses for follower coping with toxic leadership.

Abstract

Sustained destructive leadership behaviours are associated with negative outcomes that produce

serious workplace problems, yet there is scant research into how followers effectively cope with

toxic leader behaviours. Despite numerous attempts to develop typologies of coping behaviours,

there remains much to learn, especially in relation to this specific workplace stressor. This mixed

method research investigates the coping strategies reported by 76 followers to cope with the

psychological, emotional and physical consequences of their leader’s adverse behaviour. Coping

instances were categorised using two existing theoretical coping frameworks, and the ability of

these frameworks to explain responses to real world experiences with toxic leadership are discussed.

Common coping strategies reported included assertively challenging the leader, seeking social

support, ruminating, taking leave, and leaving the organisation. Organisational interventions to

increase effectiveness of follower coping with the impact of toxic leadership are also discussed.

268
Appendix C

Consequences of Leadership Behaviours Survey (Study 2)

Have you worked with or for a leader who has engaged in destructive behaviours that inflict harm

on their followers and their organisation? We are keen to hear your story.

You are invited to participate in this survey, which is being conducted to investigate the impact of

poor leadership behaviours on the leader themselves and their followers and what strategies

followers use to protect themselves from bad leadership.

This research forms part of a PhD being conducted by Griffith University. The results of this survey

will form the basis for designing leadership and follower interventions to assist in the reduction of

the negative effects of poor leadership. The conditions for undertaking this research are outlined

below. Please print off this page and retain for future reference.

This survey should take about 20 minutes to complete. Participation in this survey is voluntary and

you may withdraw from completing the survey at any time.

All the information you provide in this survey will be treated in the strictest confidence. Data

collected will be kept confidential and stored in a secure location and password protected, with

access only granted to the research team. No individual respondent will be identified in reporting of

results.

Please be advised that by completing this survey you are giving consent for your responses to this

survey to be used as part of the researchers’ aggregated data for a Griffith University PhD research

project.

Do you give further specific permission for this story to be used as a deidentified,

anonymous case study? Y/N

269
Griffith University conducts research in accordance with the National Statement on Ethical

Conduct in Human Research. If you have any concerns about the ethical conduct of this

research project please contact the Manager, Research Ethics at research-

[email protected].

Thank you for taking the time to participate in this study. If you would like further

information about this research or to receive a summary of the overall results of the research,

please contact Vicki Webster at [email protected]

Tell us about your experience:

 What did the manager do? What specific behaviours did they demonstrate? Why? [TEXT

BOX – 200 word limit]

 What was the context or situation? [TEXT BOX – 200 word limit]

 What was your response? What impact did the incident/s have on you personally? [TEXT

BOX – 200 word limit]

 Are you still working for the organisation? Y/N

 Overall, what was the result of their behaviour (for individuals, team/s and the

organisation)? [TEXT BOX – 200 word limit]

 What were the consequences for the leader/manager themselves, if any? [TEXT BOX – 200

word limit]

 Is the leader/manager, the subject of your story, still working for the organisation?

Y/N/Don’t know

On reflection, how do you feel about the incident/s now?

 How long ago did the incident/s happen?

270
o Under 2 years

o 2-5 years

o 6-10 years

o 11- 15 years

o Over 15 years

 How long did you stay with the organisation after the incident/s you have described?

o Under 2 years

o 2-5 years

o 6-10 years

o 11- 15 years

o Over 15 years

 What factors did you take into consideration, when deciding whether to stay or leave?

[TEXT BOX – 200 word limit]

 What is your view of this manager now? [TEXT BOX – 200 word limit]

 Looking back, what strategies did you use to deal with the poor behaviours? How have you

coped, both at the time of the incident/s and since? [TEXT BOX – 200 word limit]

The following demographic information is being collected for research analysis purposes only and

will not be used to identify respondents.

Age

Gender

Tenure with organisation

271
Sector

Industry

Occupation

Level of education

Country

Industry

Date event/s occurred – Year

Thank you for assisting with this research and contributing your story to be used as part of the

researchers’ aggregated data for a Griffith University PhD research project.

Please note, your story will only be used as a deidentified, anonymous case study if you have given

specific permission to do so at the commencement of this survey.

272
Appendix D

Results Tables (Study 2)

Table 9

Follower Effect on Well-being (Study 2)

Key Theme Example outcome Example respondent statements

Psychological Loss of self- “I became very confused and isolated and started to doubt what I knew to be

confidence/self-doubt true.”

(n = 22; 29) “It created some self-doubt when the employer used this incident to cast doubt on

my professionalism generally.”

Feeling highly stressed “I did move interstate and occasionally see a female figure that resembled my

(n = 17; 22%) boss and I still get a nasty adrenaline rush.”

“I have been physically sick on a Sunday night thinking about work and going to

work on a Monday.”

273
Anxious (n = 13; 17%) “The incident has led to me suffering a major depression and developing an

and depressed (n = 10; anxiety disorder, where every day I have to adjust my lifestyle to avoid anxiety

13%) attacks. I am also on numerous medications and have weekly visits to numerous

specialists. I am unable to work or function ‘normally’. I am withdrawn,

sensitive, lack confidence and motivation, agoraphobic, and suffer many side

effects of the medication.”

“I did not cope at all and became very depressed and unwell. I am still suffering

with my illness. I needed help to complete this survey.”

“Due to this series of events I suffered a nervous breakdown.”

Emotional Mistrust (n = 17; 22%) “… the seeds of mistrust and antipathy that the verbal attack had sown.”

“It served to engender an atmosphere of division and mistrust amongst

staff.” “Loss of communication and trust.”

Anger (n = 15; 20%) “I am angry and frustrated; my life is drastically changed.”

“To this day I still feel angered for the unfair treatment that I received.”

Fear (n = 10; 13%) “I experienced fear and anguish.”

274
“My outrage was silenced by my fear… I was terrified of taking on my

manager.”

“This led to a fear response by staff when they picked up the phone or opened

email, as all communications were negative.”

Physical Health problems (n = 22; “I suffered from 6 colds and took around 15 days off work, as my immune

29%), e.g. hair loss, colds, system had been affected by stress. My wisdom tooth became infected, and due

insomnia, rashes, weight to the reduction in my immune system, the infection took over and required

loss/gain, headaches, serious dental work. I also was grinding my teeth harshly as I slept, requiring the

digestive problems use of a plate. My bowel movements were also not consistent at this time. I

underwent blood tests to determine if there was a more serious cause, but was

informed that I must reduce stress.”

“I lost weight from an average size 10-12 I dropped to a size 8.”

“I was not sleeping. I was displaying stress signs by loss of hair and itches etc.”

275
Table 10

Follower Coping Strategies (Study 2)

Coping with Abusive Higher Order Ways of Coping Coping Instances reported by respondents

Supervision Scale Families of Coping

(Yagil et al., 2011) (Skinner et al., 2003)

Direct Communication Problem-solving Assertively “I used problem solving behaviours. I confronted the bullies.”

confronting or “I stood my ground and defended myself when confronted.”

challenging leader “My approach was to have straight talks with the manager.”

(n = 17; 22%)
“At the time I would not compromise my integrity and I would

still advocate for myself.”

“ … and on numerous occasions I have challenged her line of

thinking.”

Direct Communication Problem-solving Managing up/direct “At first I attempted to clarify expectations and roles, manage up.”

communication “I initially tried to coach my manager when certain behaviours

(n = 3; 4%) started to impact on the team.”

276
“Upward managing to attempt to get her to be more responsible.”

Problem-Solving Instrumental Action: “We attempted whistle blowing but it went nowhere through

Formal complaint (n = official channels. The only option was to leave.”

11; 14%), “I chose to document each incident and make sure it was

documenting incidents witnessed.”

(n = 9; 12%), informal “I made numerous attempts to discuss issues as they arose… I

complaint (n = 4; 5%) finally attempted a formal mediation process that failed

completely….”

“I openly challenged and raised the issues as a grievance through a

formal process and resigned.”

Problem-solving Instrumental Action: “I undertook activities such as yoga, breathing exercises, and

Undertaking health increased my walking activities.”

and well-being “I exercised by running every day, and I think this helped with the

activities: exercise stress.”

“NLP techniques on myself; yoga and meditation.”

277
(n = 6; 8%); yoga, “I exercise every day, and try to eat healthy, because it keeps me

meditation and/or feeling relaxed and fit during the day.”

mindfulness (n = 5;

7%), diet (n = 2; 3%)

Support Seeking Support Seeking Social Support: “I found one other colleague, with whom I could debrief and that

Mentor or colleague made things more bearable.”

(n = 26; 34%) “I had a small network of people I had worked with previously

with whom I was able to vent.”

“Seeking support by other team members.”

Support Seeking Support Seeking Social Support: “Spent a lot of time discussing the situation with my husband and

Family or friends other friends.”

(n = 20; 26%) “I coped by being supported by friends and family.”

“Support from other senior leaders in the company who had left.”

Avoidance of contact Escape Leaving the situation: “Ultimately it led to my taking 3 months stress leave.”

resigning, retiring or “I took some sick leave when it got too much.”

278
taking redundancy “…on advice from the HR Manager and my Dr I removed myself

package (n = 34; from the environment and have been on my accrued sick and

45%), taking leave (n recreational leave since.”

= 16; 39%); seeking a

transfer (n = 3; 4%)

Avoidance of contact Escape Bypassing leader “The section in which I worked tended to work around the person,

(n = 13; 17%) forming our own informal work groups to solve problems and

make the work happen.”

“I chose to not engage with the manager.”

“I just managed to get through each day with little or no personal

contact with him.”

Avoidance of contact Escape Ignoring the leader “Low profile - don't question or challenge.”

(n = 7; 10%) “I would ignore and not respond to the behaviour so I believe my

strategies were submissive, rather than assertive.”

279
Reframing Accommodation Cognitive “I have focused on what I can get out of a bad employment

restructuring: situation.”

depersonalizing issues “I had to work hard to maintain a sense of inner calm and manage

(n = 8; 10%); my state of mind.”

positive thinking and

reframing to keep

calm (n = 5; 7%)

Information Seeking Seeking professional “Saw my GP to have the incident and my reaction documented in

support and advice: case there are further incidents which may make me consider

Counselling (n = 6; stress leave.” “I sought treatment from an organisational

8%), GP (n = 6; 8%), psychologist who was very helpful. I also had a supportive GP.”

Psychologist (n = 5; “I have attended stress workshops, which teach you how to cope

7%), union (n = 4; with all the different stresses in your life.”

5%), HR
“I contacted the HR group manager for advice.”
(n = 2; 3%).

280
Self-education

/reading (n = 5; 7%)

Submission Rumination “I still feel disgust and outraged that basic human rights, like the

(n = 23; 30%) right to feel safe, to feel protected from bullying and harassment,

is not even represented in some of the institutions that purport to

study it.”

“…I had lost a sense of proportion or perspective because of how

odd and surreal everything was.”

Self-Reliance: Working harder (n = “I stayed calm and tried to keep the conversation focused on facts,

emotional and 10; 13%); maintaining diffuse his anger or finish the conversation quickly - although that

behavioural professionalism (n = was not always easy.”

regulation 4; 5%), humour (n = “Worked really hard, lots of extra hours, compliant… Worked

4; 5%) harder and harder.”

“I came in put my head down and worked.”

“We used humour…to try to deal with the stress.”

281
Helplessness Feelings of “…a sense of helplessness around the options.”

helplessness “Do I still doubt and feel insecure about my ability to do my work

(n = 4; 5%) - totally - there is nothing I do not question or analyse to death.”

Delegation Shame; self-blame “I actually feel sick when I look back on it. It was so shameful that

(n = 4; 5%) a group of intelligent, thoughtful articulate staff were completely

unable to meet the challenge of addressing this director’s

behaviour.”

“…still feel degraded and annoyed at myself for allowing that

behaviour to be conducted towards me.”

282
Table 11.
Leader Behaviours perceived as harmful by Respondents (Study 2) [note: this table was not included in the published journal article].

Behaviour Examples Example respondent statements

Manipulating/ Deception, denials and “Only over time realised that he actually had an amazing ability to charm,

Machiavellian cover ups; excluding cultivate and manipulate everyone.”

(n=56; 74%) staff; playing favourites; “At first, the manager appeared quite charming and appeared interested in

professional misconduct; knowing all employees at a ‘personal’ level. It soon became obvious,

taking credit for others’ however, that this simply a way to gather information that could later be

work; withholding used against the employee.”

information “When the manager identified a weakness in an employee, he would use

this against the employee.”

“She has her favourites.”

“The manager would have 'favourites of the week' where that team member

was given whatever they wanted and all others were ignored or excluded.”

“The manager ‘pitted’ his subordinates against each other.”

283
“Anyone who he believed was going to make him look bad was isolated.”

“Kept important information away from me.”

“Lying.”

“Not taking advice, then blaming others.”

“No deliverables were completed yet the manager wasn't held to account

yet was happy to take credit for things they had no involvement with/work

done in their ongoing sick absences.”

“Presented my presentations to the Board, replaced my name with his and

gave me no acknowledgement.”

“She persistently presented others’ work as her own.”

“He led a stacked board who seemingly rubber-stamped his activities,

some of which boarded on the fraudulent, or at the outside, represented a

misuse of the (public) funds entrusted to the organisation.”

“The manager had inappropriate "personal" relationships with several

female staff members below his level.”

284
“The manager made inappropriate comments to employees about their

personal lives.”

“…sidelining and discrediting staff…”

“The reason I found out he had blocked my forward progression was he

gossiped the fact to a staff member, who was concerned enough to

eventually tell me.”

Intimidating Authoritative “Constant use of fear based directives.”

(n=49; 64%) dictatorship; belittling “A dictating and bullying style of management.”

staff; bullying; targeting “They would reply with a cutting and nasty manner, resorting to personal
dissenters insults.”

“Criticising employees to others privately & publicly.”

“Would ‘target’ people who for some reason had fallen out of favour.”

“If an employee spoke up against the manager, he would "target" them and

systematically try to get rid of them.”

285
“He indulged in bullying of staff and had a tendency to dismiss - as in fire -

anyone who challenged him.”

“Bullying, overbearing and control freak.”

“Many were the targets of the bullying and harassment.”

“Basically demanded control of all facets of the business and absolute

obedience from his employees.”

“If he wanted me to do something outside of my normal job scope and I

didn't want to do it, he would make me feel guilty and threaten me with

various things such as redundancy or not being offered overtime if it came

up.”

“She would make fun of people when they got upset at her behaviour or

when she made very cutting comments.”

“Staff were afraid of him and would not provide any support. He would

pick winners in the team to get them onside in working against his bullying

victim.”

286
“Swearing and using intimidating language.”

Ego-centred Arrogant; no regard for “He was arrogant.” (5)

(Narcisstic) the needs of others “Arrogance - my way or the highway.”

(n=20; 26%) “He has to win at all costs and it is pointless arguing with him.”

“Lapses into numerous, time consuming, self-praising anecdotes.”

“They constantly seek and need praise.”

“I worked with a very narcissistic manager who was used to keep my

position distant from the CEO.”

“They demonstrated that they do not have any care for anyone other than

themselves.”

Emotionally Abusive “The manager had very erratic behaviour towards staff – peaks and lows of

volatile mood.”

(n=20; 26%) “Yelling, swearing and belittling managers in senior managers' meetings.”

“The person regularly became abusive towards me and my staff. He yelled

and swore, including the F word.”

287
“…abuse, tantrums and threatening behaviour…”

“Temper tantrums.”

“Her behaviour was erratic and she was prone to bad tempered outbursts,

shouting and crying.”

“She firstly cried a lot then got angry and used body language and other

subtle measures that made me feel resented and ignored.”

Micromanaging Controlling; “He cheerfully got out his red pen and scribbled all over my documents. He

(n=18; 24%) unreasonable demands; seemed to believe it was his responsibility to do it to EVERY document,

questioning staff’s regardless of how good it was.”

capability; critical – “Treated workplace interactions as a fault-finding exercise. It made me feel

‘fault finding’ and ‘nit pretty awful to feel under constant unfriendly scrutiny.”

picking’ “Had to know what I was doing every minute of every day.”

“He took all of my decision making authority away.”

“He would talk about me behind my back, go directly to my staff and

request work from them, and not include me. He would further assign my

288
staff to other duties without including me. I had a great working

relationship with my staff and they would come and tell me as soon as it

happened.”

Passive Aggressive Avoids important targets “…ignoring my questions or requests of him in a meeting. Suddenly

(n=17; 22%) and ignores issues; ending meetings he did not like…”

defensive when “If there were problems and he was approached concerning them, he would

questioned; unsupportive listen, not engage in any discussion, take no notes, and take no action.”

“If people persisted in raising issues, he would ignore them from that point

on – no greeting.”

“…the leader agreed to take action and then later negated on this”

“Ignore and avoid attempts to meet with them.”

“Manager overlooked destructive behaviour by one of the staff…The

behaviour is so bad that manager has decided to overlook it and has told

other staff to just ignore the behaviour.”

289
“She would appear to agree with staff… then imply, (not explicitly say no)

that she would act on staff requests, but then she would not act at all, and

also, not respond to any communication about follow up on agreed to

actions.”

“…over promised, under delivered and was unsupportive…”

“This leader was a smart guy, but basically lazy and uncaring.”

“No deliverables were completed yet the manager wasn't held to account.”

290
Appendix E

Participant Evaluations of Intervention (Study 3)

Workshop Title: Building Career Resilience

Date: Workshops conducted between 06/06/2011 and 23/11/2011

Facilitator: Vicki Webster

1. How would you rate your overall level of skill/knowledge before attending the

workshop?

Excellent Good Adequate Inadequate

3% 27% 54% 17%

2. How would you rate your overall level of skill/knowledge after attending the

workshop?

Excellent Good Adequate Inadequate

26% 67% 7% 0

3. What was the most beneficial part of the workshop?

Learning about workplace resilience (3)


 discussing how to talk to/approach your manager (5)
 knowing where to improve in our behaviour towards other team members
 career resilience education
 employability discussion
 knowing there is help in the workplace to effect change
 learning how to achieve a quality of life in the workplace
 building career resilience – career phases
 will help with my own assignments and upcoming performance reviews

291
 helping us learn more about moving ahead
 focusing on resilience and the importance of it in long-term success
 how to identify a toxic manager
 learning about personal and career resilience
 writing our action plan for building resilience

Learning more about myself/personal reflection (9)


 identifying areas where my resilience could be strengthened even more (2)
 learning what I can do to improve myself
 how to deal with life
 provision of ‘life’ scenarios
 what is my ‘follower style’
 accepted my problems and feelings when I was in stressful situations
 learned how to empower my feelings to put on positive sides

Strategies and techniques for problem solving/dealing with issues (21)


 mindfulness (4)
 conflict management (4)
 practise having difficult conversations (2)
 learning strategies we can use in our personal lives as well as at work
 identifying how to be resilient
 how to resolve situations calmly
 be aware of coping mechanisms and ways to address a problem
 identifying problems
 how to evaluate situations
 to look at a problem from a different perspective and deal with it in another way
 to reflect and write down issues to get a clearer picture
 got more confidence with dealing with issues
 encouraged to speak to colleagues about better communication

Interaction/Group discussion (36)


 being able to voice problems/vent (6)
 being able to say what we really thought openly (5)
 knowing that others have the same/similar experiences (3)
 hearing other people’s thoughts (2)
 discussing ways of approaching problems and people
 the opportunity to share some positive outcomes for SAWMH

292
 one on one and a small amount of people in the class

All of it was good (5)


 made me think out of the box (2)
 positive thinking (2)
 interesting topic (2)
 overview, being made aware
 the information was clear and well presented

Other
 the examples given made it easier to understand
 more understanding
 getting career resilience workbook
 the workbook activities
 access to literature and websites
 chatting informally with the facilitator throughout the session
 learning that resilience covered a wide gamut of issues in my personal and career
life

4. What was the least helpful?

 nothing – all helpful (35)


 too long (3)
 only having a 10 minute break (2)
 the abundance of material (2)
 having such short notice and leaving colleagues with less staff at short notice
 not knowing about the course until it happened
 quiet reflection time
 some people talking for too long
 pairing up with other people for activities
 don’t always feel comfortable talking with colleagues about some things
 having discussion with others or a partner about issues with career
 very sleepy time of day
 maybe have some physical exercises
 trying to be courageous

293
 signature strengths
 8 steps toward a happier more satisfying life as not credible source
 survey

5. The course content was relevant to my needs?

Excellent Good Adequate Inadequate

38% 46% 15% 1%

6. The course allowed opportunities for interaction, practice and learning?

Excellent Good Adequate Inadequate

59% 36% 5% 0

7. How would you rate this workshop overall?

Excellent Good Adequate Inadequate

50% 43% 6% 1%

8. How would you rate the facilitator/s?

Excellent Good Adequate Inadequate

76% 23% 1% 0

294
9. What is one thing you will do as a result of this workshop?

Be more positive (14)


 stay focused and positive in life
 think about how I can develop/broaden my work life so I feel more positive about
it
 make positive changes

Have a discussion with my manager (5)

Work on /maintain physical well-being (14)


 investigate meditation techniques

Practice strategies learnt in this workshop (12)


 use template
 complete the 8 steps on a regular basis
 use the ‘Dealing with Issues at work’ tool
 try different ways to build career/life resilience and stress relief measures
 try and achieve one of the career and personal action plan goals
 further investigations into concept and practice

Study/learn more (5)


 think more about further studies
 speak to my manager regarding further training/courses
 be more pro-active with learning opportunities and career development

Talk more to resolve problems (3)

 listen more (3)


 communicate better (2)
 be more mindful of my abilities and strategize before dealing with issues (2)
 speak up when I feel issues need to be addressed (2)
 think before I speak/jump in
 pass on to relevant person some feedback
 think about discussing my work issues with the people who cause my discomfort

295
 approach the situation and circumstances before they spiral out of control
 focus on addressing problems in department minus the emotion
 try and look at issues from different sides
 try to be diplomatic
 resolve conflicts
 handle conflict better
 be more vocal
 change the way I deal with issues

Make more time for me (3)


 focus on my own needs (2)
 take rest breaks and meal breaks
 plan time off

Reflect more (3)


 review my career and personal resilience status
 be more aware of personal and career resilience
 start a journal
 think more about decisions I need to make and the ability to make changes

Try and be more confident at work (3)


 do not be afraid of being in stressful situations

Seek further help (3)


 spend precious time with supportive networks

Update resume (2)


 focus on career future and change
 re-evaluate and look at what I want from my job
 take a risk and look at new roles

Other
 act/be proactive (3)
 spend time building my own personal resilience (2)

296
 access resources available to me (2)
 try to implement some of my findings
 try to implement changes that will effectively help my overall resilience
 try to change my ways
 try and fulfil my promises
 count my blessings more frequently
 social and emotional intelligence
 look at the bigger picture
 embrace change
 be more tolerant
 be more considerate to colleagues
 learn to forgive and be kind
 try harder
 look after my colleagues
 work on improving myself
 think about what was presented
 be mindful of how to be resilient
10. How could we improve this workshop?

 nothing/good as is (23)
 more audio/visual representation of the subject matter (5)
 more interaction with other staff (2)
 more interactive if possible
 hold more often
 provide a follow up in 12 months’ time
 have follow up workshops
 offer pre-read of information on the subject
 more time to reflect
 offer more hope and ability to make positive changes
 maybe more ways to deal with disappointment
 information on further training/courses that are available
 less emphasis on first half of the topics in the booklet and more in the latter
 bring managers into meeting with regular staff
 delve more into how to practice various strategies
 have more notice to enable correct numbers of staff attendance
 more advance notice from managers

297
 don’t pair people up for activities
 more time
 maybe a touch shorter
 break time up

11. Other comments

 thank you (4)


 enjoyed the workshop (3)
 well-presented (2)
 very good (2)
 relaxed and interesting
 very insightful
 The facilitator was very clear and provided a very structured learning experience
 allowed things to follow their course to conclusion which has helped with the
healing process - didn’t follow to the letter and stop the natural flow
 I found this session most beneficial
 managers need to have this input too
 look forward to the next workshop
 made me aware of how I can self help
 lots of good ideas
 good size group
 it was a good workshop that can be applied to both work and home life
 hope to have another full session of this workshop
 I really think this workshop has helped me and my co-workers
 keep up the good work
 I look forward to seeing some change "slowly" in the organisation
 I think these workshops offer a confidential opportunity to 'vent' but one still
wonders if change can actually happen
 unthreatening environment to discuss issues and felt it can be 'looked' into
 as I am at the end of my career the course wasn't as relevant, however for the
newer staff very good info
 I was not sure what the course was about but now very enlightened about tactics
to use in confronting other managers or staff
 did not find it particularly helpful
 I understand the org. is trying to change and has good intentions however it will
take many years to undo and remove harmful elements

298
299

You might also like