The Hungarian Palatal Stop: Phonological Considerations and Phonetic Data

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 23

The Hungarian palatal stop

The Hungarian palatal stop: Phonological


considerations and phonetic data
Christian Geng
ZAS Berlin
Christine Mooshammer
University of Kiel

This study examines the movement trajectories of the dorsal tongue movements during symmetrical /VCa/ -sequences, where /V/ was one of the Hungarian long or short
vowels /i,a,u/ and C either the voiceless palatal or velar stop consonants. General
aims of this study were to deliver a data-driven account for (a) the evidence of the
division between dorsality and coronality and (b) for the potential role coarticulatory
factors could play for the relative frequency of velar palatalization processes in genetically unrelated languages. Results suggest a clear-cut demarcation between the
behaviour of purely dorsal velars and the coronal palatals. Morevover, factors arising
from a general movement economy might contribute to the palatalization processes
mentioned.

Introduction

The palatal stop has been a matter of debate for at least two reasons: first it is not
clear whether it is articulated with a dorsal component, which would involve the
specification as a complex segment. The concurrent specification would favour a
classification as simple coronals and introduce at least one additional feature to
separate them from the other [-anterior] coronals (Keating, 1991). Secondly, on
a phonetic level, the relationship of the palatal stop to the velars remains unclear
(Recasens, 1990).
The data analyzed in the phonological discourse related to the notion of distinctive features has mainly utilized static X-Ray-images, linguo- and palatograms
to determine a featural description of palatal articulation. In contrast, the research work which has studied palatal articulation from the phonetically motivated
viewpoint of lingual coarticulation has mainly relied on electropalatographic data,
which has the merit of supplying time-varying data but only if there is a contact
ZAS Papers in Linguistics 37, 2004: 221-246

Christian Geng and Christine Mooshammer


and with the drawback of a very limited spatial interpretability. The first aim of
this study is to give a descriptive account of the articulatory realization of palatal
stops by making use of movement data acquired by the articulograph.
In recent years, the influence of phonetic mechanisms in shaping the
paradigmatic contrasts of a language has been refocused by the scientific community, as articulatory economy, perceptual discriminability and the maintenance
of contrast have been agreed on as the key phonetic factors shaping the sound
inventories of the worlds spoken languages. For the palatal articulation we are
dealing with this involves the reference to the natural phonological process of (velar) palatalization, and therefore the comparison of the palatal stop articulation
with the stop articulation at the velar place of articulation. Therefore, in the current study we relate the palatal to the velar stop articulation of the same language
to explore the articulatory economy principles governing the palatal/velar or coronal/dorsal contrast
The introduction is structured as follows: we will first summarize phonetic approaches for explaining sound change in general, taking palatalization processes
as a concrete example.1 We will see that these refer to different parts of the phonetic band and partially make reference to a clear-cut-distinction between true
assimilative change and coarticulatory variation. It will turn out that more recent
approaches assign a more prominent role to coarticulatory variation than did the
generative tradition. We will then summarize selected approaches to coarticulation and the different predictions they make about the concrete data we collected.

1.1

Accounts of phonetically induced sound change

Several accounts of phonetic sound change have been proposed in the literature. The main distinction between them lies in the identification of the factors
by which they are driven: Is the primary principle rooted in articulation, acoustics/perception or a kind of transaction between these two kinds of influences. Let
us exemplify these different standpoints on the basis of a subprocess of an extensively studied phonological process, palatalization. According to Bhat (1978),
at least three distinct processes contribute to palatalization processes, and Bhat
presents these as independent subprocesses of palatalization: These are tonguefronting, tongue-raising and spirantization. Of special concern here is the the
fronting of velars conditioned by the presence of a front vowel, i.e.
1

Note that the term palatalization has a different meaning in phonetics/phonology: it denotes
the addition of a secondary palatal articulation to a primary articulation, like in palatalized
labials. Here, the term palatalization is used in the way it is used in typology or historical
linguistics, i.e. as a phonological process (Bhat, 1978).

222

The Hungarian palatal stop


[k]

[c]

[i]

As mentioned, accounts in the neo-grammarian tradition would seek for articulatory factors underlying the fronting of the velar in front vowel context. A
contemporary theorist following this rationale is Recasens. He views palatalization as a gradual sound change mechanism proceeding from the unstable mediopostpalatal or medio-palatal articulations towards preferred alveolopalatal ones
through an increase in predorsal and laminal contact (Recasens, 2003).
An approach relying primarily on acoustic properties of the speech signal
is the theory and research program of acoustic invariance (Blumstein & Stevens,
1979), which was elaborated in a series of papers (Lahiri & Blumstein, 1984;
Blumstein, 1986; Keating & Lahiri, 1993). This theory makes the claims that (1)
there is acoustic invariance in the speech signal corresponding to the phonetic features of a language (Blumstein, 1986, p. 178) and that (2) the perceptual system
is sensitive to these invariant properties. These invariant properties are seen responsible for the natural processes in phonology, and, in particular can account
for why certain assimilation rules are more likely to occur. The conditions for
the occurrence of an assimilation (Blumstein, 1986, p. 186) are (a) that the two
contiguous segments must have some similar acoustic properties and (b) that the
original sound and the modified sound must also share a number of acoustic properties. In the case of velar fronting the acoustic property of the segments involved
are identified as the relative distribution of energy: Palatals exhibit a selective increase in energy between burst and voicing onset in the frequency band between
800 and 1800 Hz, whereas the distribution for velars is comparatively flat.
In other words, the assimilation of [k] to [c] involves a true assimilation
of the acoustic property of gravity from the vowel to the preceding consonant2
(Blumstein, 1986, p. 186). In short, this kind of theorizing views the sound change
under consideration as a true assimilative change by making reference to revived
Jakobsonian featural descriptions.
The third class of theories emphasizes the use of perceptual factors for sound
change. Among these are -on a metatheoretical level- the scenarios of generalizing mini sound changes as elaborated by Ohala (e.g. Ohala (1983), Ohala (1993))
and Lindbloms approach of adaptive dispersion (Lindblom et al., 1995). A concrete instantiation of Lindbloms approach is carried out in the work of Guion
(1998): She interprets the facts about velar palatalization in terms of a perceptual
reanalysis of fast speech. On a large scale, we see the explananda in the potential
influence and interaction of these factors in shaping language change, and for the
present purpose, in an exploration of its articulatory antecedentia.
2

/i/ has F2 und F3 in the same region.

223

Christian Geng and Christine Mooshammer


1.2

Theoretical Approaches to coarticulation

Classical generative theory makes a clearcut separation between coarticulation


and other context-dependent phenomena, such as assimilations: Coarticulation
deals with transitions between vowels and adjacent consonants, the adjustments
in the vocal tract shape made in anticipation of a subsequent motion (SPE:295,
after Farnetani & Recasens (1999)) In contrast, assimilations involve operations
on phonological features, and are accounted for by phonological rules, which map
lexical representations onto phonetic representations. This kind of reasoning comprises the acoustic invariance approach relying on Jakobsonian ideas as well as
the approach taken in SPE with extensive marking conventions. We skip a more
thorough discussion as these approaches are not laid out to make more detailed
predictions about articulatory surface behavior3 : Coarticulation and other properties of phonetic implementation are assumed to follow from universal principles
of speech physiology.
1.2.1

The window model of coarticulation

The window model of coarticulation elaborated by Keating (1990) blurs this


clear-cut distinction between the grammar and the physics of speech4. In Keatings view, the grammar has a phonological and a phonetic component. This
phonetic component of the grammar has the function to specify whether a feature
is relevant in the paradigm in a given language. For example, the voicing distinction is very systematic in English or German, but completely absent in Polish and
Czech. This kind of facts has to be accounted for in the grammar. Altogether,
there are three different ways to deal with underspecification: There is underspecification on the phonological level, which may persist in the phonetic domain.
Then, there is phonetic underspecification which is conceptualized as a continuous notion. Additionally, unspecified features may be left unspecified or specified
by rule. Now, if phonological assimilation rules assign a contextual feature to a
segment, its associated window will be narrow before that context and the contour
will have a plateau-like shape. If assimilation rules are not active, the key feature remains unspecified and the trajectories will be provided by interpolation.
Furthermore, inter-language differences in coarticulation can be of phonological
or phonetic origin. If phonological assimilation rules operate in one language and
not in the other, they are of phonological origin. They are of phonetic origin, if
different languages interpret an unspecified feature differently.
3

but see Lahiri & Blumstein (1984).


It has to be mentioned though that this clear-cut distinction between the grammar and the
physics of speech has been abandoned earlier by approaches stemming from within featural
phonology, e.g. in approaches as the theory of feature spreading (Hammarberg, 1976), the lookahead model, (Daniloff & Hammarberg, 1973), the model of coarticulatory resistance Bladon
& Al-Bamerni (1976).
4

224

The Hungarian palatal stop


In a different series of papers, Keating (1988,1991,1993) elaborated the status of
palatals and velars. In Keating (1988) it was proposed that palatals are complex
segments involving both coronal and tongue body articulations, such that they
would have a status like double-articulated labial-velars. The representation of
the palatal has specifications on the dorsal as well as on the coronal node5:
Place
coronal

dorsal

[ant] [+distr]

[back] [+high]

In yet another publication, Keating (1993) discusses the phenomena of velar


fronting in terms of surface features for English:
With reference to the work of Houde (1968), she states that velars lack
inherent specification for Back ((Keating, 1991, p. 17)):
V

Place

Place

Place

DORSAL DORSAL DORSAL

[+back]

[back]

Stated another way, velar fronting is something that happens gradually over
the course of the velar. Such temporal/spatial variation, or phonetic gradience,
can be interpreted as a transparency effect on the velar with respect to backness.
(Keating, 1991, p. 17) This seems to tacitly assume a large sliding movement
of the tongue during the closure interval, and, on the reverse, an absence of this
kind of movements during the realization of the palatals. A point, which we will
turn back on later.
For completeness, we reproduce the specification of the palatalized velars. In
contrast to the velar specification cited above, palatalized velars have an inherent
specification for Back.
Place
dorsal

[back]

[+high]

Keating (1991) offers another possibility for the representation of palatals: However, another option in the representation of palatals is to treat them as simple coronals, and introduce
at least one additional feature to distinguish them from the [-anterior] coronals. This is in fact
what Halle (1968) does with his new features Lower Incisors Contact. Actually, both options
could be exercised for more descriptive coverage (Keating, 1991, p. 45).

225

Christian Geng and Christine Mooshammer


1.2.2

Coproduction theory and derivatives

At this point, the necessity arises to review the very basic results of one of the most
influential studies and the accompanying model of coarticulation: hman (1966)
and hman (1967) proposed a model of vowel-to-vowel coarticulation, the basic
empirical evidence of which was articulatory and acoustic analysis of Swedish
VCV utterances produced in isolation, and similar speech material in American
English and Russian. The material on the Russian data was different with regard
to (secondary) palatalization. The major finding was that the consonantal transitions (V1 C and CV2 ) depend on the identity of the transconsonantal vowel. But:
this coarticulatory variability was reduced to almost random fluctuation in the case
of Russian. hman interprets these findings as follows: The tongue is considered
a system of independently operating articulators driven by invariant articulatory
commands. The apical articulator is involved in the formation of apical consonants, the dorsal articulator in the formation of palatal and velar consonants and
the tongue body articulator in the formation of vowels. The reduced coarticulatory variability for the palatalized F2-transitions is seen as the result of conflicting
vowel commands on the tongue body, i.e. an [i]-like palatalization commands
exerting a blocking effect on the following vowel.
On the basis of hmans work, the coproduction theory and articulatory
phonology have been elaborated.
Fowler (1980) argues against speech production theories in general which take
phonological features as input. The features used as input for the speech production mechanism are timeless, abstract and static and have to be translated into
articulatory movement. As Farnetani & Recasens (1999, p. 51) put it: In this
translation process,the speech plan supplies the spatial target and a central clock
specifies when the articulators have to move. In contrast, Fowlers intention is
to overcome this dichotomy and she suggests to modify the phonological units of
the plan: The phonological units become dynamically specified phonetic gestures,
with an intrinsic temporal dimension. In speech, these gestures are implemented
by coordinative structures, i.e. by temporary functional dependencies among the
articulators contributing to the goal the gestures want to achieve. For example, in
producing a bilabial stop, a temporal functional link is created between upper lip,
lower lip and jaw. Several gestures are allowed to be coproduced. The amount
of articulatory variability induced by this coproduction depends on the degree to
which the gestures involved share articulators. The case of minimal gestural interference is the production of /VbV/-sequences, where vocalic and consonantal
constriction gestures involve two independent sets of articulators. Conversely, a
gesture is defined along exactly the same set of tract variables and articulators
as the flanking vowels, if the consonant is a velar (Saltzman & Munhall, 1989).
However, the original work seems to make no reference to palatal articulation, but
a series of papers by Recasens (1997, 2002) was explicitly designed to make these
226

The Hungarian palatal stop


predictions6 : The goal of the DAC(Degree of coarticulatory constraint)-scale attempts to characterize phonetic segments according to the types of articulatory
constraints involved in their production. These values then can be used to predict
the coarticulatory resistance of the segments. As in Fowlers theory, the DACmodel assumes that articulatory gestures associated with consecutive segments
are coproduced and overlap to different degrees depending on their spatiotemporal properties, on prosodic factors and speech rate. According to DAC-scale,
consonants differ in DAC value according to the following order: dorsals (alveolopalatals, palatals, velars), lingual fricatives (/s/,/ /), dark /l/, which can be assigned a maximum DAC value (DAC=3); dentals and alveolars such as /n/ and
clear /l/ (DAC=2); and bilabials, with the lowest DAC value (DAC=1)(...) It is hypothesized that dorsal consonants are highly constrained based on the observation
that their primary contact or constriction location stays relatively fixed in line with
the large contact size involved and perhaps the sluggishness of the tongue dorsum.
The same observation may even apply to velars provided that at least two targets
in front and back vowel contexts are accounted for (Recasens, 2002).
1.3

Hypotheses

To sum up the selected theories presented and predictions concerning palatals and
velars:
1. Keating: Palatals -if the description as complex segments is not outdatedare dorsals and coronals likewise. This amounts to relatively little sensitivity
to vowel-induced contextual coarticulation for palatals in contrast to velars.
At the same time, front velars should be distinct from palatals in their shape
configurations, velars lacking the /i/-like component. If the surface underspecification Keating adopts for English is also valid for Hungarian velars,
then the velars exhibit relatively large vowel-dependence in place of articulation. Furthermore, let us shortly review her statements on the phonetic implementation on contextual velar fronting: Stated another way, velar fronting
is something that happens gradually over the course of the velar. As mentioned, Keating seems to view this as a transparency effect of the velar with
respect to Backness. (Keating, 1993, p. 17) . We need to add here, that this
implies that this transparency effect should be absent over the the course of
the palatal. Putting this together, according to Keating, velars and palatals
should be distinguished by the amount of coarticulatorily induced variability
and their behavior during these stops.
2. In contrast, Recasens (1990) rejects Keatings (1988) claim that palatals are
complex segments produced with the blade and the tongue dorsum: Contrary
6

Recasens explicitly mentions his model as being a concrete instantiation of coproduction


theory.

227

Christian Geng and Christine Mooshammer


to Keating, we believe that those consonants which have been characterized
as alveolopalatals and front palatals in this paper belong to different articulatory classes. Moreover, front velars are back palatal articulations, and a mid
palatal class is also needed in the light of the articulatory facts. (Recasens,
1990, p. 276).
This implies the following predictions: Contextual fronting of velars should
amount in a convergence with the tongue shapes of palatals. Furthermore,
according to the DAC-scale presented above, palatals and velars should be
equally prone to effects induced by variation of the vowel context.
The aim of this study is to tease apart these contradictory predictions by a comparison of the palatal and velar stops in Hungarian with respect to (a) vowel-contextspecific effects on tongue posture analyzed by factor-analytic techniques and (b)
the vowel-context-specific movement patterns during the realization of these consonants.
2

Method

V0F2OFF1

(a) (b)

(c)

0.5

(d)

(e)

(f)

1.2
Time (s)

Figure 1. The figure shows an Illustration of segmentation criteria. For a more detailed description see text. The example sound is a voiceless palatal in the context of /i /.

Tongue, jaw and lower lip movements of one female and two male speakers
of Hungarian were recorded by means of Electromagnetic Midsagittal Articulography (EMMA, AG100, Carstens). The two male speakers natively were from
Budapest, the female speaker came from the North of Hungary, from the region of
Szeged. The choice of the Hungarian language arose from articulatory reasons, as
the Hungarian palatal stops have been described as the palatal stops with the po228

The Hungarian palatal stop


tentially strongest dorsal colouring (Keating & Lahiri, 1993)7 . Four sensors were
attached to the tongue, one as far back as possible (TB), one approximately 1 cm
behind the tongue tip (TT). The two middle sensors, tongue dorsum (TD) and
tongue mid (TM) were located at equidistant points between them. Additional
sensors were glued on the vermillion border of the lower lip (LLIP) and on the
lower incisors (JAW). Two sensors on the nasion and on the upper incisors served
as references for compensation of head movements relative to the helmet and definition of an intermediate coordinate system. The final coordinate system was defined by recordings of two sensors on a T-bar acquired in order to rotate the data to
the occlusion plane for each speaker individually (Hoole, 1996). Original sample
frequencies were 400 Hz for EMMA data and 48 kHz for the acoustical signal.
For the analysis, the EMMA signals were low-pass filtered and downsampled to
200 Hz. The material consisted of /CVCa/ nonsense words with either velar or
palatal voiceless stops as consonantal context and one of the long or short corner
vowels /i,a,u/. Two realizations of the test words were embedded in the carrier
sentence Most a ... es a ... volt (This was a ... and a ... now) and repeated six
to 10 times. The following temporal landmarks were extracted manually from the
acoustic signal by means of the software package PRAAT by Boersma & Weenink
(19922004): (a) the burst of the initial consonant (b) the onset of the second formant of the first vowel, (c) the offset of the second formant of the first vowel,
(d) the burst of the the medial vowel. (e) the onset of the second formant of the
second vowel (f) the offset of the second formant of the second vowel. For the analyzes of the distances reported below -we will analyze the medial consonants of
the /CVCa/-sequences- four different intervals of the VCa-sequence were defined:
the movement from the midpoint of the first vowel to the onset of closure, where
the midpoint of the vowel is defined as the central sample of the temporal landmarks of (b) and (c),the interval during closure defined as the time between (c)
and (d)), the interval between the burst and voicing onset as the time between (d)
and (e) and, the interval during the /a/,where the vowel mid of the second vowel
is defined in analogy to the interval of the first vowel. These intervals serve as
the basis for the articulatory analyzes reported below. In our opinion, the use of
acoustic landmarks seems satisfied, because the commonly applied definition of
articulatory landmarks from the speed signal was not consistently possible in all
vowel environments. The distances traveled during the intervals defined above are
calculated by summing the distances traveled by selected sensors over the course
of these trajectories.

As far as we can see, diachronically it has not emerged from velar palatalization: Palatals
are a rather rare outcome of velar fronting (Bhat, 1978).

229

Christian Geng and Christine Mooshammer


3

Results

3.1

Preliminary qualitative evaluation on palatal stop realisations in Hungarian

There was been a long-standing debate whether the palatal stop in Hungarian is
a stop or an affricate (see Siptr & Mikls (2000) for a summary). Realisations
of the three speakers presented here showed yet another pattern: During the stop
interval no full silence was achieved but the whole interval was accompanied by
frication. Additional we found a portion which we interpreted as a (residual) burst.
This was followed by a second frication portion with a change in the spectral
energy distribution. Two more speakers have been recorded, one with and without
EMMA. Preliminary inspection gave evidence of a clear palatal stop realisation.
Since for the former speakers frication during the closure phase only occured for
the palatal but not for the velar stop we assume that it is not an artefact of the
recording procedure but a speaker-dependent allophonic variation. More thorough
and detailed spectral analyses are needed before a conclusive categorisation of the
observed patterns are possible.
3.2

Positional data

In the two top panels and the bottom left panel of fig.2, results of Principal Component analyzes of the covariance matrices of the averaged articulatory configurations excluding the lips during (a) the initial burst, (b) the medial burst and (c) the
vowel configurations at the midpoint of V1 are shown. Principal component analysis (PCA) involves a decomposition of a a larger number of usually correlated
variables into a (usually smaller) number of not directly observable uncorrelated
variables. These are the so- called principal components. One drawback of these
single-speaker factor solution is the fact of rotational indeterminacy, i.e. there exists an infinite number of solutions which explain the same amount of variance. In
the bottom right panel of fig. 2, the solution of the multispeaker factors analytic
model PARAFAC which avoids this problem is shown8 .
The aim of both methods is to reveal new meaningful underlying variables,
in our case, we can reduce the articulatory configuration containing separate xand y- positions for four tongue and one jaw sensor into a two-dimensional representation still representing the gross topology of the articulatory space analyzed.
The first principal component accounts for as much of the variability in the data as
8

PARAFAC is a type of multi-mode analysis procedure and therefore contrasting (PCA) or


factor analysis, which are two mode representations. PARAFAC requires an at least threedimensional data structure with the third dimension usually being represented by different
speakers, i.e. if all speaker weights are fixed to be one, then PARAFAC reduces to PCA.
The advantage of PARAFAC is that there is no rotational indeterminacy as in PCA, in other
words, PARAFAC gives unique results. The PARAFAC-model was introduced in experimental
phonetics by Harshman et al. (1977).

230

The Hungarian palatal stop

[u]

[u]

kuuk
1

cu
2

cu
ciu c
ca ki
a ci c

ikka
ak

6
rn

10

ik

ki

ak
ka

[a]

ap

[a]
10

uc
cicc
aica

4
820

ku
uk

8
20
10
13

10

15

[u]
6

ku uk

cu

12.5

ku

4
2

cu

uci
iaccc
ca ki

uca
cic

12

ik
ak

11.5

ic
ac
ki

ka

uk

ik

ka
ak

11

6
10.5
8

[a]

lt

10
2

10
3

14

16

18

20

Figure 2. The two top panels and the lower left panel show speaker-dependent Principal Component analyzes of the mean tongue configurations at different temporal landmarks: the initial
release, the medial release. The light gray triangles are the midpoints of the long corner vowels in the context of velar consonants, the dark gray triangles the corresponding projections of
the long corner vowels in the palatal contexts. The lower right figure is a speaker-independent
PARAFAC projection of the same data

231

Christian Geng and Christine Mooshammer

1
0.5
0
0.5
1
1.5

rn

0
x[cm]

2
x[cm]

y[cm]

1
0
1
2

ap

1.5
1
0.5
0
0.5
1
lt

x[cm]
Figure 3. 1--ellipses as indication for the variation of palatal and velar consonants as calculated by the means over all vowel contexts at burst time for initial and medial cononants. Small
ellipses: Palatal configuration; large ellipses: velar configuration

232

The Hungarian palatal stop


possible, and so does each succeeding component. For the single speaker Principal
Component Analyses, the first factors explain between 65 and 80% of the variance
and the second factors between 17 and 27 %. The total amount of variance varies
between 93 and 98%. This means that the most substantial portion of the variance
is accounted for in the PCA representations, but the amount of variance explained
is partitioned in different ways by different speakers. The grey triangles shown
in fig. 2 are the vowel configurations at the midpoint of V1, with the dark grey
triangle for the palatal context and the light grey triangle for the palatal context.
The most salient pattern of all these plots seems that the velars show much more
inclination to coarticulate with their vocalic environment in comparison with the
palatals: For speakers ap and lt (top right and bottom left panel) all the palatals
except the initial palatal in the context of /u/ cluster close together, not far from
the /i/- edge of the palatal triangle. Compared to the place variation for the
velar stop this gives the impression of a stable palatal configuration very close
to the first factor score of /i/. This finding is further substantiated by the plot of
1 -ellipse shown in figure 3. The velars substantially show more variation for
all three speakers.9 In particular, only the second factor on the y-axis distinguishes
between the palatals. The first factor is almost constant for the palatals and in line
with the classic interpretation as front-raising to an /i/-like shape (Harshman
et al., 1977). The most consistent pattern for the velars is that the shape of the
velar in /u/-context is close to the /u/-corner. Of particular interest are the projections of the /i/ in velar context and initial position. And here, the patterns do
not seem to be fully conclusive: For ap, the /k/ is separated from the main palatal
cluster as well as from the initial /c/ in /u/-context. For speaker lt, the initial /k/ in
/i/-context is dissolved in the main palatal cluster. The same holds for speaker
/rn/ although the whole single-speaker solution seems more diffuse than for the
other speakers. So if we would have to be decisive about these factor solution,
we would prefer to interpret the PARAFAC solution in the bottom right panel, because to be identified, certain assumptions have to be met: "The basic assumption
is that, if a factor corresponds to some real organic unity, then from one study to
another it will retain its pattern, simultaneously raising or lowering all its loadings
according to the magnitude of the role of that factor under the different experimental conditions of the second study." (Cattell & Cattell (1955), after Harshman
& Lundy, 1984, p.151). This means that this solution has to be judged higher in
terms of its validity. This is substantiated in a clearer interpretability: The medial
palatal consonants in /i/- and /a/-context show the strongest coarticulation with
the vowel /i/ in the palatal context, a finding that could have been expected. Then,
there is a second palatal cluster with initial /i/ and /a/ and medial /u/-context. This
analysis substantiates and enhances the finding of the initial palatal /u/ as having a
9

One could raise the objection that the place of maximal constriction is not reliably measured
even by the EMA tongue back sensors, but the parallel orientation of the ellipses for the rearmost
sensor indicates that the constriction is mostly caught.

233

Christian Geng and Christine Mooshammer


bias towards the vowel /u/. And finally, the initial velar in /i/-context is the closest
to the palatals of all velars. Moreover, another finding concerning the vowel triangles is remarkable: The sizes of the triangles are reduced in the palatal context,
indicating, that the palatal consonants themselves exert a stronger coarticulatory
influence on the vowels than the velars: in comparison with the velars, this results
in a fronting of /u/ and a rising of the /a/.
3.3

Kinematic Characteristics

Figure 4 shows distances traveled during the four intervals which were defined
earlier for the tongue dorsum sensor. Similar results were obtained for the tongue
back sensor. The most salient aspect of these plots is that the total distances traveled by the TD sensor during the /VCa/-sequences are larger in the palatal contexts
for /a/ and /u/-contexts; the reverse holds for the /i/-contexts.
Concerning the velar contexts, we found some relatively surprising patterns
in comparison with the data we analyzed earlier. In particular, in Geng et al.
(2003) we found consistently bigger total amplitudes in the /a/-contexts. We interpreted this finding in agreement with Munhall et al. (1991), who observed a
reduction in movement complexity after removing of the jaw influence. This tendency is weaker in this corpus and even reversed for speaker ap. So if the patterns
for the /a/-contexts can at least partly be explained through a contribution of the
jaw for both consonants, this explanation is not justified for the large total amplitudes for the palatals observed in the context of /u/. If we cannot attribute these
large movements to an influence of the jaw, then this pattern must be attributed to
a strong movement component by the tongue itself. We will return to this point
later. Another quite general observation in these plots is that the movement amplitudes during the stop, -i.e. the black parts of the bars- are usually larger for
the velar consonants. This holds with the exception for the /a/ and /u/-contexts,
where this finding is blurred, most probably due to large amplitudes aforementioned. Similar observations can be made for the distances traveled between the
stop release and the onset of the second vowel /a/.
As a crude method for quantifying the direction the tongue paths travel during the closure interval, we weighted the distances the sensors traced in the closure
interval by a direction coefficient, which was determined as the sign function of
the difference between the x-coordinates of the first sample of the closure interval
and the last sample of the closure interval, i.e., negative values indicate the tendency to make a movement in backward direction during the closure. The error
bars in figure 5 indicate the standard deviations of this composite for the tongue
dorsum sensor. Again, similar results were obtained for the tongue back sensor.
Note that this is a very gross measure, in particular, a mainly vertical movement
during closure would have the consequence of making the sign function, which is
only based on the horizontal movement in this interval relatively arbitrary. So note
234

The Hungarian palatal stop

3.5
rn

tdorsum

2.5
2
1.5
1
0.5
0
3.5
ap

tdorsum

2.5
2
1.5
1
0.5
0
3.5
lt

tdorsum

2.5
2
1.5
1
0.5
0

uc uc

ic

ic

ac ac

uk uk

ik ik

ak ak

Figure 4. Distances the tongue dorsum sensor traveled during the four different intervals described. The stack bars indicate from bottom to top: first stack, white, distance traveled during
the first vowel; second stack, black, distance traveled during oral closure; third stack, gray,
distance traveled between stop release and the onset of the second vowel; fourth stack, white,
distance traveled during the second vowel. Upper case characters: lax vowels, lower case: tense
vowels.

235

Christian Geng and Christine Mooshammer

1.5
rn

tdorsum

0.5
0
0.5
1
1.5
ap

tdorsum

0.5
0
0.5
1
1.5
lt

tdorsum

0.5
0
0.5
1

USU+

USU

USI+

USI

USA+

USA

Figure 5. Distances the tongue dorsum sensor traveled during the the closure interval weighted
by a direction coefficient. The direction coefficient was determined as the sign function of the
difference between the first sample of the closure interval and the last sample of the closure
interval, i.e., a negative value indicates the tendency to make a movement in backward direction
during the closure.

236

The Hungarian palatal stop


that the partially large standard deviations in these plots underlyingly might represent (a) heterogeneity in the true movement as an expression of token-to-tokenvariability or (b) the predominance of a vertical component causing a noisy sign
function. So we will limit ourselves to the results which do not have this uncertainty and exclude the patterns which have large standard deviations and values
above and below zero. For example, this concerns the results for speaker lt in the
context of /a/: As due to the influence of the jaw, a vertical movement dominates,
which results in a noisy sign function and therefore in large standard deviations.
Following the front vowel /i/ (long and short) and short /u/, very little movement was found during the palatal closure. A higher degree of forward movement
occured during the palatal following long /u/ and /a/. For the velar stop movement during closure was consistently largest following long and short /u/. After
/i/ two speakers showed almost no movement during /k/ which is consistent with
the data on German (Geng et a. 2003) and a relatively large backwards movement
for speaker AP. Movement directions following long /a/ varied inter- and intraindividually (e.g. large standard deviations for speaker LT) These results indicate
that the palatal stop is produced close to the constriction location for /i/, therefore
no movement is required during closure.
Figures 6 shows the correlations between the positions in the mid of the
first vowel and the distances traveled during the stops for tongue dorsum sensor.
Again, similar tendencies were observed for the tongue back sensor. The left panel
shows the correlation of distance and x-position and the right panel of distance and
y- position. For two out of our three speakers, the correlations for velar and palatal
articulations have the same sign, i.e. the more anterior the sensor location during
the following vowel, the larger the distance traveled during the stop closure (left
panels), and, the higher the position of the sensor during the preceding vowel,
the smaller the distance traveled during the preceding stop. Note that for RN
and LT, the correlations between horizontal position and the distance traveled are
spurious, only the height of the sensor during the preceding vowel seems to be
related to the distance traveled during the stop. For one speaker, ap, the sign of
the correlations is reversed for both sensors in the velar condition. This reverse in
sign for the velar seems to contradict the results for German we presented in Geng
et al. (2003). For the three speakers we presented there, no such sign reversal was
observed10 , rather similar patterns to those of the other two speakers of this study
were found.

Discussion

The results of this experiment can be summarized as follows:


10

note that the vowel environment was richer in this study.

237

Christian Geng and Christine Mooshammer

ac

uc ac ak
ik ik
ic
ic

1.5

ac
ak

0.5

uc
0

uk uk
a c
ak

ak

Position x[cm]

4.5

uk
ak

r, pal: 0.98 1.16


r, vel: 0.64 1.79

ap

uk

ic
ic

ikik

uc

uc
ak
ac

ic
ic

0.5

ukuk
ak

uc

ac uc

ac

3.5

ik
ik

ac 0
ak
0.5

2.5

Position x[cm]

lt

1.5
Position y[cm]

r, pal: 0.87 1.09


r, vel: 0.88 3.33

ap

4.5

0.5
1

lt

r, pal: 0.84 0.89


r, vel: 0.28 0.94

r, pal: 0.98 2.34


r, vel: 0.95 5.02

icic iik
k

uk
uk ak
uc
ac
uc
ac ak
ic k
ic iik

uc
ucac
uk
uk
ak

2
1.5
1

ac

0.5

Position y[cm]

Position x[cm]

ic
ic
ik
uc ik

uc uk
uk

2.5

r, pal: 0.94 2.2


r, vel: 0.84 3.02

rn

Position y[cm]

r, pal: 0.47 1.09


r, vel: 0.09 0.28

rn

ak

0
Distance during Closure [cm]

Distance during Closure [cm]

Figure 6. Correlations between the positions in the mid of the first vowel and the distances traveled during the stops, both for the tongue dorsum sensor. The left panel shows the correlation
of distance and x-position and the right panel of distance and y- position.

238

The Hungarian palatal stop

Coarticulatory resistance of the palatal stop: Not very prone to vowel-like


influences (a) the palatal itself exhibits a very stable configuration in comparison to the velar. This stable configuration is similar to an /i/-like shape (b)
the palatal itself exerts an influence on the vowel articulation inasmuch as the
size of the vowel space is shrunk in comparison to vowel space in the context
of the velar stop.
The initial palatal in the context of the back vowel /u/ gets isolated from the
relatively stable configuration we observed for the remaining palatals.
Another important finding were the extremely large distances the palatal has
to travel in the context of the long /u/ over the whole VCV-sequence.
For two out of our three speakers, the movement amplitude of the palatal in
/u/-context during closure is among the highest amplitudes altogether.
In the introductory section, we contrasted predictions about the gross coarticulatory variability patterns made by different approaches: Keatings approach
creates its prediction from underspecification at different phonological or phonetic
levels. This amounts to relatively little sensitivity to vowel-induced contextual
coarticulation for palatals in contrast to velars if the derivations we used are the intended ones for the Hungarian language. In contrast, the DAC-scale by Recasens,
adapting ideas from the theory of coproduction proposed by Fowler, predicts similar patterns in the tendency to coarticulate with the vowel environment for both
palatals and velars: It assigns palatals and velars the same values on the DACscale. On this macro-level, Keatings approach makes better descriptions. Here,
the necessity arises to summarize the influences Keating sees at work in shaping coarticulatory patterns. These are the following factors: (a) production constraints, operating both within and across languages,(b) contraints deriving from
language-specific phonological structure and (c) language-particular contraints,
unrelated to production or phonology and therefore unpredictable. Farnetani &
Recasens (1999, p.49) claim that cross-consonant differences and cross-language
similarities in patterns of coarticulatory resistence (under-)specification held constant point to consonant-specific production constraints going beyond language
peculiarities. We found the intial palatal stop in the context of /u/ strongly coarticulating with the following vowel. We do not have cross-language data on palatal
stops, but tried to relate this pattern to consonant-specific production constaints:
To meet the objective of a palatal closure, the tongue has to travel a long distance
in the context of /u/. These were present in the form of long distances the tongue
back and dorsum were moving. As these cannot be attributed to the influence
of the jaw, we interpret them as consonant-specific production constraints for the
palatal in back vowel context.
In the light of the data presented in this paper, Recasens assignment of the same
DAC-value for palatals and velars is unlikely because (a) the palatals themselves
239

Christian Geng and Christine Mooshammer


are articulated more consistently, and (b) they exert a bigger influence on the neigbouring vowel than the velars. For further substantiating our finding of a higher
DAC value for the palatal we plan to analyse the postconsonantal vowel /a/ in the
/CVCa/ sequences. If our hypothesis is true then the vowel /a/ should vary with the
preceding medial vowel quality to a greater degree for the velar as compared to the
palatal. In our view, several reasons could be responsible for the lack of fit to the
data the DAC exhibits: One reason could lie in a too strong emphasis on the idea
of the coupling of articulators neglecting a general articulatory movement economy aiming at the avoidance of long traveling paths. This might be augmented
by Recasens preference of the palatographic acquisition method, which does not
take into account contacts behind the hard palate and also movements leading to
tongue-palate contact.
A second important claim we elaborated in the introductory section is that
articulatory velar fronting is a "transparency effect of the velar with respect to
Backness" and something that happens gradually over the course of the velar as
stated by Keating. She explicitly refers to Houde (1968), who first observed sliding movements over the course of velar consonants. If these sliding movements
were an effect of surface underspecifications, then these movements should be unobservable over the course of palatals. This claim was strongly disconfirmed by
our data: The movement amplitudes during the palatals, -especially in the context
of the vowel /a/- can exceed the amplitudes in velar context. Note that these movements, beforehand only observed for the velars and called loops (see Mooshammer et al., 1995), have stimulated a whole research program in speech production
as it has been seen as a paradigmatic case for the most different influences simultaneously shaping planning of the movement trajectory. Accordingly, several
competing explanations were given for the phenomenon: The looping patterns are
seen as a passive forward movement of the tongue due to airstream mechanisms
Kent & Moll (1972) or as a result of an active gesture aiming at the maintenance of
voicing by Houde (1968), sometimes termed cavity enlargement (Ohala, 1983).
Lfqvist & Gracco (2002) to explain looping patterns in more general principles
of motor control, postulating the entire movement to be planned in terms of cost
minimization principles. A recent modelling study by Perrier, Payan, Zandipour
and Perkell (2003) emphasizes the role of tongue biomechanics. We think that
palatal stops can essentially contribute to this research program.
We began the introduction with reference to the mechanisms that might underlie diachronic velar fronting 11 . Recasens (1990) argues that the front velar
converges to a back palatal with respect to its articulatory configuration. Concerning this question, the results are ambiguous. While the speaker-independent
factor solution and the single factor solution suggest a relatively strict separation
in the shapes of the fronted velar and the palatal, this situation is ambiguous for
11

let us note again here that the Hungarian palatal is probably not the output of velar fronting.

240

the two other speakers. What we have more robustly, though, is the change in the
articulatory configuration for the initial palatal stop in the context of /u/, which
we interpret as an excess of a threshold of maximum coarticulatory resistance. So
if the diachronic process of velar palatalization has an articulatory grounding at
all, this grounding could as well be afforded by a bidirectional articulatory change
of contextual fronting of the velar and contextual backing of the palatal, rather
than unidirectionally triggered by the velar.
5

ACKNOWLEDGEMENT

Work supported by German Research Council(DFG) grant GWZ 4/8-1, P.1


6

References

References
Bhat D. (1978). A general study of palatalization. In: J. Greenberg (ed.) Universals of Human Language. Vol. 2.: Phonology, 4792. Stanford: Stanford
University Press.
Bladon R. & Al-Bamerni A. (1976). Coarticulation reistance in english /l/. Journal of Phonetics, 4:137150.
Blumstein S. (1986). On acoustic invariance in speech. In: Invariance and
Variability in Speech Processes, 179197. Hillsdale, New Jersey, London:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Blumstein S. & Stevens K. (1979). Acoustic invariance in speech production:
Evidence from measurements of the spectral characteristics of stop consonants.
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 10011017.
Boersma P. & Weenink D. (19922004). Praat, a system for doing phonetics by
computer. www.praat.org. URL www.PRAAT.org.
Cattell R. & Cattell A. (1955). Factor rotation for proportional profiles: analytical
solution and an example. British Journal of Statistical Psychology, 8:8392.
Daniloff R. & Hammarberg R. (1973). On defining coarticulation. Journal of
Phonetics, 1:239248.
Farnetani E. & Recasens D. (1999). Coarticulation in recent speech production
theories. In: W.J. Hardcastle & N. Hewlett (eds.) Coarticulation; Theory, Data
and Techniques, 3165. Cambridge: University Press.
Fowler C. (1980). Coarticulation and theories of extrinsic timing. Journal of
Phonetics, 8:113133.

Geng C., Fuchs S., Mooshammer C., & Pompino-Marschall B. (2003). How does
vowel context influence loops? In: Proceedings of the SPS6: Sydney.
Guion G. (1998). The role of perecption in the sound change of velar palatalization. Phonetica, 55:1852.
Hammarberg R. (1976). The metaphysics of coarticulation. Journal of Phonetics,
4:353363.
Harshman R., Ladefoged P., & Goldstein L. (1977). Factor analysis of tongue
shapes. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 693707.
Harshman R. & Lundy M. (1984). Data preprocessing and the extended parafac
model. In: H. Law (ed.) Research Methods for Multimode Data Analysis, 216
284. New York: Prager.
Hoole P. (1996). Issues in the acquisition, processing, reduction and parameterization of articulographic data. Forschungsberichte des Instituts fr Phonetik
und Sprachliche Kommunikation der Universitt Mnchen, 34:158173.
Houde R. (1968). A study of tongue body motion during selected consonant
sounds. Speech Communications Research Laboratory, Santa Barbara,SCRL
Monograph 2.
Keating P. (1993). Phonetic representation of palatalization versus fronting. UCLA
Working Papers in Phonetics, 85:621.
Keating P. & Lahiri A. (1993). Fronted velars, palatized velars, and palatals.
Phonetica, 73101.
Keating P.A. (1988). Palatals as complex segments: X-ray evidence. UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics, 69:7791.
Keating P.A. (1990). The window model of coarticulation: articulatory evidence.
In: J. Kingston & M.E. Beckman (eds.) labphon1, 451470. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Keating P.A. (1991). Coronal places of articulation. In: C. Paradis & J.F. Prunet
(eds.) Phonetics and Phonology 2: The special status of coronals: Internal and
external evidence, 2948. Academic Press.
Kent R. & Moll K. (1972). Cinefluographic analyses of selected lingual consonants. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 15:453473.
Lahiri A. & Blumstein S.E. (1984). A re-evaluation of the feature coronal. Journal
of Phonetics, 12:133146.
Lindblom B., Guion S., Hura S., Moon S., & Willerman R. (1995). Is sound
change adaptive? Rivista di linguistica, 7:537.

Lfqvist A. & Gracco V.L. (2002). Control of oral closure in lingual stop consonant production. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 111:28112827.
Mooshammer C., Hoole P., & Khnert B. (1995). On loops. Journal of Phonetics,
23:321.
Munhall K., Ostry D., & Flanagan J. (1991). Coordinate spaces in speech planning. Journal of Phonetics, 19:293307.
Ohala J.J. (1983). The origin of sound patterns in vocal tract constraints. In: P.F.
MacNeilage (ed.) The production of speech, 189216. New York: Springer.
Ohala J.J. (1993). The phonetics of sound change. In: C. Jones (ed.) Historical
linguistics: Problems and perspectives, 237278. London: Longman.
hman S. (1967). Numerical model of coarticulation. Journal of the Acoustical
Society of America, 41:310320.
hman S.E.G. (1966). Coarticulation in VCV utterances: Spectrographic measurements. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 39:151168.
Perrier P., Payan Y., Zandipour M., & Perkell J. (2003). Influences that shape
tongue biomechanics on speech movements during the production of velar stop
consonants: A modeling study. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,
114:15821599.
Recasens D. (1990). The articulatory characteristics of palatal consonants. Journal of Phonetics, 18:267280.
Recasens D. (1997). A model of lingual coarticulation based on articulatory constraints. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 102:544561.
Recasens D. (2002). An ema study of vcv coarticulatory direction. Journal of the
Acoustical Society of America, 111:28282840.
Recasens D. (2003). Articulation and sound change in romance. In: Proceedings
of the 15th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, 231234.
Saltzman E. & Munhall K. (1989). A dynamical approach to gestural patterning
in speech. Haskins Status Report on Speech Research, 99/100:3868.
Siptr P. & Mikls T. (2000). The phonology of Hungarian. Oxford: Oxford Univ.
Press.

You might also like