Edward Vajda
9 Number in Ket (Yeniseian)
Abstract: Ket is a critically endangered language spoken in Siberia in the Yenisei
river basin and is the last surviving member of the once widespread Yeniseian family whose typological profile is very different from its closest neighbours. Nouns and
pronouns distinguish singular and plural number, usually by adding a plural suffix,
with singular number left morphologically unmarked. Plural suffixes have distinct
allomorphs for inanimate class nouns, kinship terms, and other animate class
nouns, but there are many exceptions and irregular forms. Attributive adjectives
and other modifiers are normally left unmarked for number, though a few adjectives
have a plural suffix. Demonstrative pronouns, however, regularly express plurality
when modifying animate class plural nouns. The Ket verb expresses agreement in
singular and plural number with its subject and object and also has a variety of
morphological means for expressing pluractionality, resulting in various patterns of
multiple exponence of number on the verb. Among other topics, this chapter focuses on the relationship between the expression of number and animacy, which variously manifests itself in the morphology of nouns, pronouns and finite verbs. It also
explains how certain irregularities in Ket number marking developed and includes
comparisons with the extinct Yeniseian languages.
1 Overview
Ket is spoken today by a dwindling number of elders, mostly in remote Siberian
villages near the Yenisei river or its tributaries in Turukhansk District of Krasnoyarsk Krai, the Russian Federation’s second largest administrative unit. Alongside
its extinct sisters – Yugh, Kott, Assan,1 Arin, and Pumpokol – Ket belongs to the
Yeniseian (Yeniseic) language family. Substrate river names and 17th century Tsarist
fur tax records indicate that languages or dialects related to modern Ket were once
spoken across much of south and west-central Siberia, from northern Mongolia and
the forests southwest of Lake Baikal westward to the Ob river watershed and northward along the Yenisei to the Arctic Circle (Map 1).
Three Ket dialects have survived into the early 21st century: Southern Ket (spoken in several villages, including Kellog, Sulomai, and Alinskoye), Central Ket (Sur-
1 In terms of linguistic similarity, Assan could easily be considered a dialect of Kott. However, as
in the case of Yugh, the speakers considered themselves to be ethnically distinct. For this reason,
Assan and Kott, like Ket and Yugh, are generally regarded today as closely related pairs of languages.
https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110622713-010
308
Edward Vajda
Map 1: Language distribution in central Siberia during the 17th century.
gutikha and Baklanikha), and Northern Ket (Maduika and Kureika). At the time of
this writing, these dialects together have no more than a few dozen fully fluent
native speakers, all over the age of 65. There are only small differences in how each
Ket dialect expresses number, and most are of a predictable phonological nature.
Except where otherwise noted, this chapter cites Southern Ket forms. Number marking in Ket broadly resembles Yugh (Werner 1997a), a distinct but closely related
language that disappeared in the 1970s. Most 20th century Soviet scholarship on
Yeniseian languages treated Yugh as another Ket dialect, calling it “Sym-Ket” after
a river where most speakers lived. By contrast, Southern, Central and Northern Ket
were called “Imbat Ket” after Inbak – a 19th century ethno-geographic term for several downriver groups of Kets. The other documented Yeniseian language varieties,
all of which disappeared between 1730 and 1850, are more distantly related and
sometimes provide deeper insights into the historical development of number marking in the family. The extant material representing the Kott language, which belongs
to a different primary branch than Ket and Yugh, is the most useful in this regard
because it contains many inflected word forms, including plurals, thanks to a gram-
Number in Ket (Yeniseian)
309
mar sketch and dictionary recorded by the Finnish field linguist M. A. Castrén
(1858).
Yeniseian languages are in many ways structurally unlike the “Ural-Altaic” belt
of language families originally spoken across nearly all of the remaining portions
of northern and Inner Eurasia. The most striking difference is the Ket polysynthetic
finite verb, which is based on a template of ten morpheme classes, most of which,
at least historically speaking, are represented in prefixal positions. By contrast,
verbs in Turkic, Tungusic, and Uralic languages are exclusively suffixing in their
inflectional morphology. Possession in Yeniseian is likewise expressed using markers placed in front of the possessed noun (or noun phrase), rather than by suffixes
as in the neighboring languages.
This chapter covers all aspects of how Southern Ket expresses number categories. It also considers the diachronic origins – whether genealogical or contactinduced – behind the various formal systems of number marking across the Yeniseian family. In keeping with the volume’s general structure, section 2 provides descriptions of number marking across the language’s major form classes. Subsection 2.2
examines the Ket pronoun system, which manifests all of the language’s core number distinctions in one way or another. It also provides a general overview of how
animacy intersects with plural marking patterns in several areas of the morphosyntax. Subsection 2.3 investigates the complex motivations behind how plural suffix
allomorphs are distributed across the noun lexicon, and also looks at sporadic examples of dual number marking that arose through morphological reanalysis of certain noun stems. Subsection 2.4 describes morphological techniques used for expressing pluractionality. Section 3 discusses agreement morphology. Subsection 3.1
examines number agreement in demonstrative pronouns and adjectives. Subsection 3.2 examines how adjective and action nominal suffixes have been reanalyzed
as plural markers in a number of stems. Subsection 3.3 discusses the number agreement suffixes found on certain lexical classes of subject complements in the predicates of clauses with no finite verb form. Finally, subsection 3.4 turns to the highly
complex polysynthetic Ket verb to explain its intricate system of class and number
agreement with subjects and objects. Section 4 remarks on what is known about
the discourse functions of number marking. Section 5 summarizes the typology of
number marking in modern Ket and attempts to sort out which techniques were
inherited from Proto-Yeniseian and which were later innovated. The discussion
throughout this chapter also points out noteworthy features and categories related
to number that are found in other languages but absent in Yeniseian.
Much of the material here was covered earlier in Porotova (1990), the only existing monograph devoted to the expression of plurality in Yeniseian. Accessible treatments of most aspects of Ket number marking can also be found in Werner (1997b),
Vajda (2004), Georg (2007), and Nefedov & Vajda (2015).
310
Edward Vajda
2 Pronominal, nominal, and verbal number
2.1 Generalities
Number marking features shared widely with other Siberian language families include the productive use of suffixes to mark plurality in nouns. The expression of
number in Ket and other Yeniseian languages is based on a formal distinction between singular (one) and plural (two or more), and this dichotomy strongly affects
the morphosyntax of nouns, pronouns, and finite verbs. Yeniseian also has a noun
class (grammatical gender) system based on the contrast between inanimate and
animate class, with animate class further subdivided into masculine and feminine
gender. Ket noun class (animacy and gender) interacts with number marking in
several important ways, with inanimate class nouns showing an overt distinction
between singular and plural only in the noun form itself, but not in the possessive
markers or verb agreement affixes that cross-reference them. Finally, modern Ket
has also developed a typologically unusual system of phonemic tones based on an
amalgam of features involving melody, length and phonation type.2 Tone is marginally germane to the topic of number in Ket because a small irregular set of singular
vs. plural noun forms are formally distinguished solely by tonal differences.
2.2 Pronominal number
2.2.1 Independent pronouns
The system of personal and anaphoric pronouns in Ket offers a good starting point
for discussing number marking. These pronouns convey all of the core number distinctions present in Yeniseian morphosyntax (singular vs. plural) and provide clear
examples of the most typical formal means used to mark them (plurality expressed
2 The Southern Ket data in this chapter derive from the author’s own fieldwork. Monosyllabic
phonological words distinguish four phonemic tones. These are transcribed using a macron for
high-even tone (sūl ‘blood’), apostrophe for rising laryngealized tone (su’l ‘white salmon’), double
vowel letter for rising-falling tone on a geminate vowel (suul ‘snow sled’), and grave accent for
falling tone (sùl ‘cradle hook’). Most polysyllabic phonological words have instead an accent-like
pitch on the first syllable, as in qópqun ‘cuckoo bird’. In a much smaller number of polysyllabic
words the pitch peak falls on the second syllable, one example being the plural form qopqún ‘cuckoo birds’. Because syllable-initial pitch in polysyllables is far more common, it is left unmarked
except in disyllabic noun stems like qópqun ‘cuckoo bird’, where it is included to call attention to
its relevance for number marking. Finally, each of the three Ket mid vowel phonemes have two
regular allophones. The symbol /ǝ/ is used to transcribe the mid back unrounded vowel, which is
realized phonetically as [ɤ̄ˑ] under high-even tone and as [ʌ] elsewhere. In a similar fashion, /o/
transcribes both [ōˑ] and [ɔ] and /e/ transcribes [ēˑ] and [ɛ].
Number in Ket (Yeniseian)
311
through suffixes that normally contain a nasal consonant, either /n/ or /ŋ/). Table 1
shows the system of singular and plural animate personal pronouns in Southern
Ket:
Tab. 1: Ket personal and anaphoric pronouns.
1sg ād
1pl ǝtn
2sg ū
2pl ǝkŋ
3sg.anim bū
3pl.anim būŋ
3inan tude
The 3rd person anaphoric pronouns bū ‘he / she’ and būŋ ‘(animate class) they’
normally refer back to nouns denoting human beings, but sometimes animals as
well. Other nouns are generally replaced anaphorically by tude ‘that (one)’, ‘those
ones’ (pronounced [tuɾɛ] in Southern Ket), which can also be used adnominally as
a demonstrative pronoun.3
The simplicity of this paradigm stems in part from the fact that Yeniseian pronouns do not distinguish dual number. Nor is there any inclusive/exclusive distinction. The 1st person plural pronoun ǝtn ‘we’ can mean ‘you and me’ or ‘you and us’
(inclusive); or it can mean ‘me with another or others’ (exclusive). In contrast to
many modern Indo-European languages, the Ket 2nd person plural pronoun ǝkŋ
‘you’ was not traditionally used to address a single individual to show respect or
mark social distance, though recent Russian interference influenced such occasional usage among the last generation of speakers.
Ket pronoun forms also exemplify the number marking strategy that is overwhelmingly favored across Yeniseian nominal morphology. The core distinction between singular (one) and plural (more than one) is formally expressed by augmenting the singular form with a suffix containing either the velar nasal /ŋ/ or alveolar
nasal /n/. There is no grammatical means for marking singular number, aside from
the absence of plural marking. Plural suffixation in nouns sometimes entails irregular morphophonemic changes, as seems to be the case with Ket 1st and 2nd plural
pronouns as well. Finally, although Yeniseian morphosyntax lacks a grammatical
category of dual number, there are sporadic instances where a nasal coda in certain
noun stems denoting paired objects such as mittens has been reanalyzed as a dual
marker (see Section 4.3).
The personal pronoun system likewise affords a convenient introduction to the
important role played by animacy in Ket morphosyntax. We have already seen that
3 Possessive markers are considered clitics because they normally attach to the preceding word if
one is available rather than to the following possessum noun, as shown here. Substitutions of
3rd person animate class bū for inanimate class tude with reference to inanimate class entities have
occasionally been documented, possibly due to Russian interference.
312
Edward Vajda
considerations of animacy determine which third-person anaphoric pronouns are
used. Third-person singular bū ‘he / she / him / her’ and plural būŋ ‘they / them’
normally reference animate entities, while the demonstrative form tude ‘that’ is
used when referring back to singular or plural inanimate class entities. The biological gender distinction in third-person animate singular bū is covert in the pronoun
itself, but surfaces elsewhere in the clause by triggering different masculine and
feminine forms of singular possessive clitics (1), subject concord suffixes on predicate adjectives (2), and subject/object agreement affixes in the finite verb complex (3):
(1)
Animate singular gender distinction in possessive marking: (a) masculine,
(b) feminine
qu’s
a. bu=da4
3sg.pron=m.poss tent
‘his tent’
b. bu=d
qu’s
3sg.pron=f.poss tent
‘her tent’
(2)
Animate singular gender distinction in predicate adjectives: (a) masculine,
(b) feminine
a. hīk [hīˑɣ] sel-du
man
bad-m.sbj
‘The man is bad.’
b. qīm
sel-da
woman bad-f.sbj
‘The woman is bad.’
(3)
Animate singular gender distinction in verb-internal subject/object agreement:
a. masculine subject with feminine object,
hīk qīm
d-i-toŋ
man woman 3m.sbj-3.obj-see
‘The man sees the woman.’
4 The combination bu=da is pronounced [buɾa] in Southern Ket. To keep the underlying forms
more transparent, the transcription ignores the allophonic intervocalic lenition of /d/ to [ɾ], /b/ to
[v], /k/ to [ɣ] and [q] to [ʁ], as well as voicing of /k/ to [g] next to a voiced consonant. It also ignores
word-final devoicing of /d/ to [t] and /b/ to [p]. Note that word-final /p/ in words such as dāp
‘shoulder’ belongs to the same phoneme as word-initial /h/ and not to /b/. The symbols /p/ and
/h/ here could have been unified by choosing one or the other in all cases; however, doing so
would have given a misleading impression of how these allophones are actually pronounced in
modern Ket. Phonetic forms are sometimes provided in square brackets, especially in less transparent cases such as certain Southern Ket high-even tone words and all falling-tone words, where
Number in Ket (Yeniseian)
b.
313
feminine subject with masculine object
qīm
hīk da=a-toŋ
woman man 3 f.sbj=3m.obj-see
‘The woman sees the man.’5
The agreement contrast triggered by masculine and feminine animate singular
forms does not extend to expressions of plurality. However, the difference between
inanimate as opposed to animate entities is regularly maintained in the same three
environments of possessive constructions (4), subject concord (5), and verb-internal
subject or object agreement (6):
(4)
3p inanimate (a) and animate (b) possessive clitics
a. tu-de=d
kīd
that-inan=inan.poss price
‘its price’ / ‘their.inan price’
b. buŋ=na
kīd
they=anim.pl.poss price
‘their.anim price’
(5)
3p plural inanimate (a) and animate plural (b) subject concord in predicate
adjectives
a. tu-de
sel-am
that-inan bad-3inan.sbj
‘It is bad.’ / They.inan are bad.’
b. būŋ
sel-aŋ
they.anim bad-3anim.pl.sbj
‘They.anim are bad.’
(6)
Verb-internal agreement with inanimate (a) and animate (b) plural objects
a. būŋ
tu-de
du-Ø-toŋ-n
they.anim that-inan 3anim.sbj-3inan.obj-see-anim.pl.sbj
‘They see it.’ / ‘They see them.inan.’
intervocalic lenition persists even though the final vowel has elided: hīk [hīˑɣ] ‘man’, qòq [qχɔ̀ʁ]
‘star’.
5 The feminine class subject marker /da/ is also a clitic that prefers to attach to a preceding word,
whenever one is available, just like the possessive markers described earlier. Enclisis is less likely
when the preceding word ends in an obstruent, as in this example. All Ket morphemes marked as
clitics in this chapter behave in a similar way.
314
Edward Vajda
b. būŋ
de’ŋ
d-aŋ-toŋ-n
they.anim people 3sbj-3anim.pl.obj-see-anim.pl.sbj
‘They see the people.’
Examples (1) to (6) provide a good introduction to how animacy and gender (noun
class) interacts with the expression of number across Ket morphosyntax. Only singular referents trigger the overt expression of gender, while the contrast between
animate and inanimate extends to patterns of plural agreement, as well. The pervasive role of animacy in Ket number marking is further explored in the next section.
2.2.2 Number in possessive constructions
One key Yeniseian typological feature absent from other families of northern Eurasia outside of Indo-European is the presence of a noun class system, examples of
which have already been provided above. Unlike Russian and other Indo-European
languages with grammatical gender, however, Ket agreement classes reflect foremost a distinction between animate and inanimate entities, with animate class
nouns secondarily divided into masculine and feminine sub classes in singular
forms – a division of much less significance to the grammar overall. The animate
superclass subsumes not only humans, animals, birds, fish, reptiles, amphibians,
and insects, but also certain body parts, as well as trees species. A few animate
class nouns denote culturally salient inanimate objects such as pinecones, tent
poles, buttons, and cradle hooks, with the masculine designation typically favoring
objects of special positive value in traditional Ket lifeways. Grammatical animacy
and gender are usually covert in the noun stem itself.
The class division between inanimate and animate has broad ramifications for
number marking in several domains of Yeniseian morphosyntax. The next several
paragraphs analyze the role of animacy and gender in the shape of possessive markers and various word forms that evolved on the basis of possessed noun constructions.
The expression of number in possessive markers intersects with animacy and
gender distinctions. The forms in Table 2 illustrate how this pattern is reflected in
the possessive forms of Ket personal pronouns:
Tab. 2: Possessive forms of personal pronouns.
1sg a=b
1pl ǝtn=na
2sg u=k
2pl ǝkŋ=na
3sg.m bu=da
3pl.anim būŋ=na
3sg.f bu=d
3inan tu-de=d
Number in Ket (Yeniseian)
315
Singular possessive markers, shown as enclitics in Table 2, distinguish person and
gender of the possessor, except that the 3rd person feminine animate singular and
inanimate markers formally coincide. More importantly, the same inanimate class
possessive clitic is used for singular and plural number. Clitics only differentiate
masculine and feminine and person categories with regard to singular entities,
while the plural possessive markers distinguish animacy only. In the absence of a
preceding pronoun to disambiguate person, constructions with an animate plural
possessor such as na=ām could therefore be understood to mean either ‘their mother’, ‘our mother’, or ‘your.pl mother’. In cases where no possessor is named, the
possessive marker can attach either to the following word as proclitic or to the preceding word as enclitic, depending on factors such as sentence structure and intonation. Enclisis is favored whenever a preceding word is available as host, particularly those that end in a vowel or sonorant:
(7)
Possessive marker as (a) proclitic or (b) enclitic
a. da=ām
his=mother
‘his mother’
b. ǝqaj=da
am-as
d-ol-daq
formerly=his mother-with 3m.sgj-pst-live
‘He used to live with his mother.’
Several types of Ket morphological structures derive from possessed nouns, including some case suffixes, most postpositional constructions, and a minor lexical class
known as directional adverbs. All of them retain the same interplay between noun
class and the expression of number displayed in possessed noun constructions.
Three Southern Ket case suffixes – dative (to, toward), adessive (at, in, having),
and ablative (from, out of)6 – are preceded by a possessive augment expressing
the same characteristic gender and animacy distinctions as those associated with
possessed nouns. When animate class plural nouns are used with these case suffixes, the resulting word forms exhibit multi-site plural marking:
6 The so-called benefactive case forms of -dita ‘feminine singular or inanimate’, -data ‘masculine
singular’, and -nata ‘animate plural’, appear to be idiolectal reductions of adessive case forms.
316
(8)
Edward Vajda
Adessive (a–b), dative (c–d), and ablative (e–f) case suffixes
i. with inanimate nouns
a. suul-aŋ-di-ŋten
snow.sled-pl-inan-ades
‘in the snow sleds’
ii. with animate plural nouns
b. am-aŋ-na-ŋten
mother-pl-anim.pl-ades
‘at the mothers’
c. baŋ-n-di-ŋa
land-pl-inan-dat
‘to our (your, their) lands’
d. hun-aŋ-na-ŋa
daughter-pl-anim.pl-dat
‘to our (your, their) daughters’
e. bes-n-di-ŋal
rabbit-pl-inan-abl
‘(made) out of rabbit pelts’7
f. bes-n-na-ŋal
rabbit-pl-anim.pl-abl
‘(motion) from rabbits (living
animals)’
The markers -di- and -na- in (8) are glossed simply as inan and anim.pl, respectively,
since the earlier possessive meaning was lost. Vajda (2013b: 80–84) has argued that
the segment /ŋ/ located at the beginning of the modern dative, adessive and ablative
case suffixes is a vestige of an ancient possessive connector once present in all possessive constructions. Regardless of whether this interpretation is valid, structures
such as (8a), (8b), and (8c) suggest that the three possessive-augmented case suffixes
may derive from underlying noun forms or postpositions, though the precise etymologies remain unclear. The other case suffixes – locative, comitative-instrumental
(with), prolative (along or though), and caritive (without) – attach to nouns or pronouns without a preceding possessive augment, for reasons that are likewise unclear.
Ket postpositions, most of which require a case suffix, form complex constructions that are often preceded by a possessive marker that agrees in number, animacy and gender with the preceding noun or pronoun stem; they preserve all of the
same morphosyntactic distinctions expressed in true possessive constructions.
Some Ket postpositions transparently derive from body-part nouns, suggesting that
postpositional constructions arose via the grammaticalization of possessed nouns.
For example, the Southern Ket postposition -ɯn- ‘below’, is cognate with a noun
root meaning ‘belly’ or ‘underside’ in the extinct Yeniseian languages. In Ket it no
longer expresses its original anatomical meaning, having been replaced by hɯ̄j ‘belly’, though Ket does preserve a cognate noun ɯ’n ‘sled runner’. The examples below
use inanimate and animate class clitics to show the homology of the structures
displayed by possessed nouns (9) and by the postpositional constructions grammaticalized on the basis of possessed nouns (10):
7 See Werner (1997b: 107) for the full paradigms of animate class be’s ‘rabbit (live animal)’ vs.
inanimate class be’s ‘rabbit (pelt)’. The inanimate class compound besiŋolt ‘rabbit pelt’ (with iŋolt
‘pelt’, ‘fur’) also exists.
Number in Ket (Yeniseian)
(9)
317
Possessed nouns with case suffixes
a. suul=d
ɯ’n
snow.sled=inan.poss runner (< underside)
‘sled runner (one of two long pieces of wood beneath a sled for gliding on
snow)’
b. ked=da
hɯ̄j
person=m.poss belly
‘person’s belly’
(10) Contrast between (a) possessed noun construction and (b) postpositional construction
a. ked=da=hɯj-di-ŋal
person=m.poss=belly-inan-abl
‘from the person’s belly’
b. ked=da=hɯj=d=ɯn-di-ŋal
person=m.poss=belly-inan.poss=under (< underside)-inan-abl
‘out from under the person’s belly
Directional adverbs, which express orientation relative to a specific point in space,
are built exactly like possessed noun constructions. The two most common directional roots are ikda [igda] ‘downland’, ‘orientation from forest to river’ and aka
[aɣa] ‘upland’, ‘orientation from river to forest’. Directional word forms are adverbs
that consist of a possessive clitic and directional root, followed by a case suffix. If
the required case is ablative or dative, the case suffix must be preceded by the
connector -di-, originally the inanimate class possessive marker (the adessive case
is not used in directional constructions).
(11) Directional adverbs
a. assanos-in na=ikda-di-ŋal
hunter-pl anim.pl.poss=downland-inan.poss-abl
‘(movement) from a location downland with respect to the hunters’
b. qu-ŋ
d=aka-di-ŋa
tent-pl inan.poss=upland-inan.poss-dat
‘(movement) toward a location upland with respect to the tents’
Ket possessed nouns, certain case forms, postpositional constructions, and directional adverbs all display multi-site marking of number, animacy, and what is often
called “gender” (masculine vs. feminine animate singular agreement). Instances of
multiple exponence in all of these structures arose through the coalescence of formerly discrete words into single morphological forms. The expression of noun class
in connection with number in certain case forms and postpositional constructions
318
Edward Vajda
is unique among the indigenous language families of landlocked northern Asia. The
feature of multi-site plural marking in some of these word forms (cf. 8 above) is
likewise unique to Yeniseian across this broader geographic area.
The effect of animacy on number marking patterns extends to other parts of Ket
morphosyntax, as well. Section 2.3 explores how considerations of both referential
and grammatical animacy influences the choice of plural suffix allomorphs in Ket
noun forms. Section 3.3 analyzes the effect of animacy on the subject concord suffixes required by certain subject complements in linking verb clauses, which lack a
finite verb. Section 3.4.1 examines the role played by noun class in assigning subject
and object number agreement markers in finite verb forms.
2.3 Nominal number
Yeniseian nouns have no grammatical marker of singular number. Count nouns
typically distinguish number by augmenting the bare singular stem with a plural
suffix containing a nasal consonant – either /ŋ/ or /n/. There are no nominal plural
prefixes. Stem reduplication is absent as a means of expressing plurality or pluractionality, being limited in Yeniseian morphology to a few expressive interjections.
Overall, the choice of noun plural suffix shape follows several broad patterns that
together form a complex interweave of semantics (animate vs. inanimate class) with
certain formal features of stem structure, the most important being whether the
noun stem already ends in /ŋ/ or /n/. There are also many irregularities caused by
diachronic processes of reductions in the noun stem’s final syllable or coda, some
of which have caused the loss of the plural suffix itself. The subsections below discuss the motivations that determine plural noun forms (2.2.1), dual number marking
arising sporadically through reanalysis of noun stem coda nasals (2.2.2), as well as
pluralia tantum, singularia tantum, singulatives, and a few other number-related
lexical categories at work in Ket noun morphology (2.2.3). This section goes into
considerable detail because the expression of plurality in nouns is fundamental to
number marking throughout Yeniseian morphology, given that nearly all other
number marking categories reflect the singular/plural dichotomy inherent to nominal morphology.
2.3.1 The morphology of noun pluralization
Animacy plays a fundamental role in how nouns build their plurals in Yeniseian
languages. Animate and inanimate class nouns typically have different plural suffix
shapes, except where phonological or diachronic factors intervene. Inanimate class
nouns normally take the suffix -(V)ŋ. The symbol (V) here and elsewhere represents
a phonologically undetermined vowel that is often inserted between stem coda and
nasal suffix:
Number in Ket (Yeniseian)
(12) Examples of inanimate class noun pluralization using -(V)ŋ
qobad
‘back’
qobad-aŋ
bǝtl
‘bubble’
bǝtl-aŋ
sɯ’k
‘bowl’
sɯk-ŋ
qoŋloq
‘bell’
qoŋloq-ŋ
asl
‘ski’
asl-iŋ
àj
‘bag’
aj-éŋ
319
‘backs’
‘bubbles’
‘bowls
‘bells’
‘skis’8
‘bags’
The animate class noun plural suffix is -(V)n, with alveolar rather than velar nasal.
It too is sometimes augmented by a vowel after a stem-final consonant:
(13) Examples of animate class noun pluralization using -(V)n
hīk
‘man’
hik-n
ostɯ́k
‘Ket’
ostɯ́k-ǝn
be’s
‘rabbit’
bes-n
qīm
‘woman’
qim-n
hǝ́ mka
‘Evenki’
hǝ́ mka-n
la’t
‘beaver’
lat-n
‘men’
‘Kets’
‘rabbits’
‘women’
‘Evenkis’
‘beavers’
The distribution of the alveolar as opposed to velar nasal consonant in noun plural
suffixes is determined by much more than grammatical animacy, however. In fact,
this deceptively elegant dichotomy has so many exceptions that recognizing a
noun’s inherent class membership based on its surface plural form is unreliable.
(Noun class can be detected more reliably from observing the form of possessive
markers or agreement morphology). The quality of the vowel before either of these
basic noun plural suffixes is often unpredictable, as well. Some of these vowels
appear to be epenthetic, which often appears true for [ǝ] or [ɯ]. Others reflect preservation of an original stem vowel lost in the singular form, as tends to be true of
many plurals that augment the nasal suffix using [i], [e], or [a]. In other cases, it
remains unclear what determines the choice of vowel in noun plural suffixes (or
even whether any vowel appears at all).
One fairly consistent semantic exception to the basic pattern of animate plurals
marked with -(V)n involves nouns denoting kinship. Despite obviously belonging to
the animate class, most kinship terms build their plural by adding the suffix form -aŋ:
(14) Kinship nouns pluralized with -aŋ
ām
‘mother’
ōb
‘father’
hɯ’b
‘son’
hu’n
‘daughter’
am-áŋ
ob-áŋ
hɯb-aŋ
hun-aŋ
‘mothers’
‘fathers’
‘sons’
‘daughters’
8 The nouns bǝtl ‘bubble’ and asl ‘ski’ both end in a syllabic sonorant and thus are disyllables
with rising tone on the initial syllable.
320
Edward Vajda
Full vowel forms of the kin noun plural suffix are consistently found in other Yeniseian languages, as well, though not necessarily with the same vowel (cf. Yugh améŋ ‘mothers’, ob-éŋ ‘fathers’. Given that this vowel usually draws the pitch peak
onto itself in disyllables, the Ket kinship plural suffix -aŋ is probably etymologically
distinct from inanimate class -(V)ŋ, which does not normally attract the accent. However, as with every general pattern of Ket noun plural formation, kin nouns also
include a few exceptional plural forms. Some nouns that express kinship take the
regular animate class suffix -(V)n instead of the kin noun plural suffix -aŋ. Two
examples are qīm ‘wife, woman’ → qim-n ‘wives, women’ and tēd ‘husband’ → tad-n
‘husbands’, which do not seem to be treated as primary kin terms, at least as regards the morphology of plural formation. Conversely, a few animate class nouns
that are not kinship-related take -aŋ rather than the expected suffix -(V)n: ēs ‘god’,
‘deity’ → esáŋ ‘gods’, ‘deities’; qān ‘khan’ → qanáŋ ‘khans’. The fact that the suffix
vowel attracts the pitch peak in the latter two plurals suggests that they share the
same suffix -aŋ used by most kinship nouns. Most Ket personal pronouns (shown
earlier in Table 1) also use the velar nasal as a plural ending. It should be mentioned
here that Ket lacks dyadic kinship forms altogether.
Nouns in (15) take the regular inanimate class suffix -(V)ŋ yet trigger masculine
animate or feminine animate agreement. This pattern occurs with some animate
nouns that do not denote people or animals, and is observed with certain body part
nouns, nouns denoting tools or other important objects, and some nouns denoting
lower forms of life, which tend to belong to the feminine subclass:
(15) Grammatically animate class nouns that pluralize using inanimate class -(V)ŋ
Body part nouns that are grammatically (m) masculine or (f) feminine
animate
bɯ’s
‘penis’ (m)
bɯs-aŋ
‘penises’
huud
‘tail’ (f)
hud-aŋ
‘tails’
tɯ̄l
‘navel’ (f)
tɯl-aŋ
‘navels’
Other inanimate objects that are grammatically (m) masculine or (f) feminine
animate
quu
‘pole’ (m)
quu-ŋ
‘poles’
álal
‘(boat) seat’ (f)
álal-aŋ
‘(boat) seats’
Lower forms of life that are grammatically feminine animate (f)
biilt
‘martin’ (f)
bilt-aŋ
‘martins’
hankó
‘toadstool’ (f)
hankó-ŋ
‘toadstools’
ulól
‘leech’ (f)
ulól-aŋ
‘leeches’
Although such nouns are grammatically animate as indicated by anaphoric pronoun usage, possessive morphology, and subject or object agreement, most take the
Number in Ket (Yeniseian)
321
inanimate class plural suffix -(V)ŋ rather than animate class -(V)n, except where
additional phonological factors such as nasal dissimilation play a role (see below,
this subsection). In this case, plural formation is governed by referential considerations of animacy, whereas nominal categorization as reflected in agreement patterns
is determined on a lexical basis.
There is also a tendency for recent loanwords to take the “inanimate class”
plural suffix -(V)ŋ, even where such nouns denote human beings or animals:
(16) Examples of animate class loanwords pluralized using -(V)ŋ.
bótaj
‘rich man’ (< Russian bogatɨj ‘rich’) bótaj-aŋ
ópsa
‘sheep’
(< Russian ovtsa id.)
ópsa-ŋ
mína
‘pig’
(< Russian svin’ja id.)
mína-ŋ
ondátɯr
‘muskrat’
(< Russian ondatr id.)
ondátɯr-aŋ
‘rich men’
‘sheep.pl’
‘pigs’
‘muskrats’
This pattern seems to have become relatively productive in the last phase of Ket
language usage, though exceptions can be found, such as koska ‘cat’ → koska-n
‘cats’, where animate -(V)n pluralizes a 20th century borrowing of Russian koška
‘cat’. In 2008, during a conversation with her mother, fluent speaker V. A. Romanenkova spontaneously used the Russian noun sobolj ‘sable’ in the hybrid Russian/Ket
plural form sobolj-aŋ in place of native Ket et-n ‘sables’ with its canonical animate
class suffix -(V)n. The common usage of -(V)ŋ to pluralize recent loanwords regardless of their meaning perhaps underscores a more general trend away from the originally fundamental morphosyntactic dichotomy in Yeniseian languages between animate and inanimate class entities.
The system of Ket noun plural formation also involves numerous phonological
and morphological exceptions that override the underlying triple contrast between
generic animate -(V)n vs. kinship -aŋ vs. inanimate -(V)ŋ described above. The most
regular among these patterns involves dissimilation of the plural suffix nasal consonant in stems that end in either /n/ or /ŋ/, and sometimes /m/. Noun stems ending
in /n/ nearly always take -(V)ŋ even when they belong to the animate class:
(17) Pluralization of animate class nouns ending in /n/
bɯ́sten
‘wasp’
bɯ́sten-aŋ
tuln
‘lizard’
tuln-eŋ
lūn
‘grayling (fish)’
lun-áŋ
‘wasps’
‘lizard’
‘graylings’
Exceptions such as kùn ‘wolverine’, kun-en ∼ kunn ‘wolverines’ probably involve
the diachronic loss of a final consonant other than /n/ that originally occupied the
stem-final position.
Through an analogous process of dissimilation, inanimate class noun stems
ending in either /ŋ/ or /m/ nearly always require the plural suffix -(V)n even where
-(V)ŋ would be expected.
322
Edward Vajda
(18) Pluralization of inanimate class noun stems ending in /ŋ/ or /m/
ka’ŋ
‘hole’
kaŋ-en
‘holes’
hǝ’ŋ
‘fishing net’
hǝŋ-en
‘fishing nets’
àŋ
‘rope’
aŋ-en
‘ropes’
qām
‘arrow’
qam-en
‘arrows’
kulém
‘lid’
kulém-n
‘lids’
One exception is bōŋ ‘corpse’ → boŋ-á ŋ ‘corpses’, where dissimilation fails to occur
for unknown reasons. This noun belongs to the feminine animate class in terms of
agreement, but even if the velar nasal was chosen through semantic influence from
the word’s lack of logical animacy, the suffix should still undergo nasal dissimilation to -an, but it does not. Instead, it seems to pattern formally more like a kinship
noun, including the syllable-final accent shift.
The kin noun plural suffix -aŋ is likewise never affected by nasal dissimilation,
and its velar nasal appears even after /m/: ádbam ‘sister-in-law’ → ádbam-aŋ ‘sisters-in-law’. It is also noteworthy that other animate class nouns ending in /m/
preserve the canonical animate class suffix form -(V)n: tēm ‘goose’ → tem-n ‘geese’.
The stem coda /m/ has no effect on the form of the animate class plural suffix -(V)n
either.
A different process, possibly historically a type of assimilation, replaces the inanimate class suffix -(V)ŋ with -(V)n in many though not all nouns with stems ending in the segment /s/:
(19) Inanimate class noun stems ending in /s/ pluralized with -(V)n rather than
-(V)ŋ
kūs
‘ringworm’
kus-en
‘ringworms’
qāks
‘wound’
qaks-en
‘wounds’
ádes
‘iron nail’
ádes-n
‘iron nails’
qóles
‘hoof’
qóles-n
‘hooves’
dǝ́ les
‘willow thicket’
dǝ́ les-n
‘willow thickets’
Other inanimate class nouns with stems ending in /s/, however, form their plurals
with the expected velar nasal, sometimes with loss of /s/, rather than by assimilating the nasal plural segment to alveolar /n/: qu’s ‘birchbark tent’ → qu’ŋ ‘birchbark
tents’, tɯ’s ‘rock’ → tǝ’ŋ ‘rocks’. Finally, the velar nasal is doubly unexpected in the
plural form of the masculine animate class noun ùs ‘birch tree’ → us-eŋ ‘birch trees’
since the animate class suffix -(V)n would have been expected on the grounds of its
animacy. Even if inanimate class -(V)ŋ appears in this plural because it does not
denote a person or animal, the lack of /ŋ/ to /n/ assimilation after stem-final /s/
remains unaccounted for. The same observations are germane in the case of the
plural form of masculine animate class bɯ’s → bɯs-aŋ ‘penises’. The reasons behind all of these phenomena remain at present unelucidated.
Number in Ket (Yeniseian)
323
A far more frequent and completely regular departure from the underlying semantic patterning of Yeniseian plural allomorphs affects any noun stem built by
adding the universal nominalizing suffix -s. All of these stems are pluralized with
the suffix form -in. The vowel /i/ derives from the historic form of the nominalizing
suffix *-si, which has eroded to -s in singular stems. Virtually any non-noun form,
including postpositional constructions and inflected finite verbs, can be nominalized by adding the word-final suffix -s. The plural of such nominalizations always
ends -in, regardless of animacy considerations.
(20) Use of -in to pluralize nominalizations in -s
a. aq-s
aq-s-in
rot-nmlz
rot-nmlz-pl
‘something rotten’
‘things that are rotten’
b. d=b-il-bet-in-s
3anim.sbj=inan.obj-pst-makeanim.pl.sbj-nmlz
‘something that they made’
d=b-il-bet-in-s-in
3anim.sbj=inan.obj-pst-makeanim.pl.sbj-nmlz-pl
‘things that they made’
c. ul-di-ŋal-s
water-inan-abl-nmlz
‘the one (pulled) from the water’
ul-di-ŋal-s-in
water-inan-abl-nmlz-pl
‘the ones (pulled) from the water’
There are no exceptions to this highly productive pattern. In example (23c) the plural suffix -in would be used regardless of whether what emerged from the water was
a person, animal, or inanimate object.
Other nouns undergo an array of irregular changes when a plural suffix is added.
Their full range is too broad to be covered exhaustively here since each exceptional
form probably has its own idiosyncratic historical explanation. The difference between the plural vs. singular stem phonology in some of these number pairs is drastic
enough to warrant being called partial suppletion. The examples in (21) only minimally represent the gamut of exceptional noun plurals that exist in modern Ket:
(21) A few examples of irregular stem changes triggered by plural suffixation
uu
‘meadow’
oo-ŋ
‘meadows’
qàj
‘elk’
qii-n
‘elk.pl’
sa’q
‘squirrel’
saa-n
‘squirrels’
qǝ’q
‘corner’
qǝ̄ -n
‘corners’
dāp
‘shoulder’
daa-n
‘shoulders’
dīd [dīˑt]
‘spruce grouse’
dek-ŋ
‘spruce grouses’
taal
‘otter’
tak-ŋ
‘otters’
This brief sample suffices to illustrate that irregular plurals involve not only vowel
and coda consonant changes, but sometimes an unexplained form of the nasal plu-
324
Edward Vajda
ral suffix, as well. The pair sa’q ‘squirrel’ → saan ‘squirrels’ involves the intervocalic
loss of uvular /q/, which occurs elsewhere in Ket phonology; however, there are
also plurals where this does not occur: tǝ’q ‘finger’ → tǝqin [tʌʁin] ‘fingers’. In the
case of qǝ’q ‘corner’ → qǝ̄ -n ‘corners’, where /q/ does elide, the appearance of -(V)n
rather than -(V)ŋ is unexpected since this noun belongs to the inanimate class. The
choice of -(V)ŋ rather than -(V)n for animate class taal ‘otter’ → tak-ŋ ‘otters’ and
dīd ‘spruce grouse’ → dek-ŋ ‘spruce grouse.pl’ is equally unexpected. A possible
explanation is that the choice of suffix allomorph originally followed the general
rules, and the surface appearance of velar /ŋ/ in the plurals of ‘otter’ and ‘spruce
grouse’ as well as /n/ in ‘corners’ arose through some sort of formerly predictable
phonological interaction. A full account of such diachronic changes would require
a separate study.
In contrast to the relatively large number of cases of partial suppletion among
Ket count noun singular and plural pairs, full suppletion is limited to the following
two examples:
(22) Suppletive number pairs
ke’d
‘person’
ōks
‘tree’, ‘pole’
de’ŋ
a’q
‘people’
‘trees’, ‘poles’
The origin of these two full suppletive pairs is at present likewise unexplained.9
Some plural nouns do not show a suffix at all, but instead express plurality
entirely through irregular tonal alternations, with or without concomitant vowel
ablaut:
(23) Tonal or vowel ablaut plurals in Ket
kɯ̄l
‘raven’
kǝ́ qin
‘fox’
qópqun
‘cuckoo’
tīb
‘dog’
tōk
‘axe’
ēj
‘tongue’
sēs
‘river’
hās
‘shaman’s drum’
kɯɯl
kǝqín
qopqún
ta’b
tòk [tɔ̀ɣ]
èj
sàs
hàs
‘ravens’
‘foxes’
‘cuckoo birds’
‘dogs’
‘axes’
‘tongues’
‘rivers’
‘shaman’s drums’
9 Porotova (1990: 48) and later Georg (2007: 100) suggested that de’ŋ ‘people’ represents a contraction of a regular plural ked-eŋ. Castrén (1858: 167) in fact did record the form keädeŋ as the plural
of ket ∼ kiet ‘person’, alongside djeŋ ‘people’. However, the modern Ket word for person, which is
pronounced [kɛ’t], contains a coda that reflects Proto-Yeniseian *d, whereas the onset /d/ of de’ŋ
is a reflex of PY *dj, a different phoneme. In any event, there are no other plausible cases of initial
syllable collapse in Ket roots. The origin of this suppletive pair thus remains unexplained.
Number in Ket (Yeniseian)
325
There is evidence that at least some of these suffixless exceptions arose when one
of the canonical plural suffix forms -(V)n or -(V)ŋ was absorbed into the noun root
(for reasons as yet unclear), similar to the historical development of English ablaut
plurals like mice or teeth. The cognate plural of ‘river’ in some 18th century word
lists representing extinct Yeniseian languages appears with the expected inanimate
class suffix -(V)ŋ. This interpretation is also supported by the frequency of falling
tone in irregular ablaut plurals, since the Ket falling tone [v̀] generally arose through
the loss of a final stem element. Zero affixation or internal flection does not appear
to have been an original technique of inflection in Yeniseian languages. Nevertheless, some of these irregular plural forms are shared across the family’s primary
branches and undoubtedly existed in Common Yeniseian, showing that irregularities in noun plural formation are deeply rooted in the family.
The few exceptional cases where the singular and plural noun form are identical can also be explained as resulting from the elision of a plural suffix. In these
stems, however, suffix absorption did not cause tonal or vowel ablaut, again for
reasons that are not clear:
(24) Number syncretism in a few Southern Ket nouns
bǝ’n
‘duck’
bǝ’n
sūj
‘mosquito’
sūj
tɯ̄t
‘black midge’
tɯ̄t
‘ducks’
‘mosquitoes’
‘black midges’
Grammatical number in this small set of nouns is covert, appearing only in the
morphosyntax as part of verb agreement or possessive marking (compare the expression of number in English this deer – these deer). The other Ket dialects occasionally retain the original plural suffix in a few of these words, as when Central
Ket tɯtn is compared to Southern Ket tɯ̄t, both of which are animate class plurals
meaning ‘black midges’. This suggests that number syncretism in such pairs arose
from plural suffix erosion. Why the loss of a plural suffix triggered tonal or vowel
changes in some stems (23) but not in others (24) requires further investigation of
the language’s historical phonology.
Though exceptions to almost every general rule of Ket noun plural formation
abound, it is still possible to provide a cogent overall description. The list in (25)
summarizes the various intersecting patterns and exceptions that are at play in assigning plural markers to Ket count nouns, beginning with lexically marked plurals
and ending with the most general patterns.
(25) Summary of plural formation techniques in Ket count nouns
a. A random assortment of lexically marked exceptions to the rules listed below,
which involve suppletion, tonal or vowel ablaut, and number syncretism
b. Stems ending in /n/ usually pluralize with -(V)ŋ regardless of meaning
c. Inanimate class stems ending in /ŋ/ or /m/ and many ending in /s/ pluralize
with -(V)n
326
Edward Vajda
d. Other inanimate class nouns and many grammatically animate class nouns
denoting body parts, inanimate objects, or some lower forms of life pluralize
with -(V)ŋ
e. Recent loanwords almost always pluralize with -(V)ŋ regardless of meaning
f. Most kinship terms pluralize with -aŋ, which typically attracts the pitch
peak
g. Most other nouns denoting people or animals pluralize with -(V)n
h. S-nominalizations always pluralize with -in regardless of meaning or form
Irregularities in Ket noun plural formation offer a wealth of evidence for reconstructing the Proto-Yeniseian consonant inventory, which included more phonemes than
modern Ket. Unstable final-stem /l/ that truncates in plural forms like sèl ‘reindeer’
→ se’-n ‘reindeer (plural)’, and sa’l ‘crucian fish’ → sa’n ‘crucian fish (plural)’ can
be reconstructed as a Proto-Yeniseian rhotic (or retroflex) sound rather than a lateral, as supported by its reflexes in Yugh seħr ‘reindeer’ and sa’r ‘crucian (fish)’. Stable
/l/ in nouns like būl ‘leg’ → bul-aŋ ‘legs’ and ool ‘container’ → ol-aŋ ‘covers’ traces
back to an original lateral, appearing also as /l/ in the Yugh cognates būl ‘legs’
and o’l ‘cover’. Unstable coda /d/ in Ket, which participates in morphophonemic
alternations with velar /k/ and corresponds to /dj/ in Yugh and /r/ in Kott, reflects
the retroflex stop *dr in Proto-Yeniseian. One example is Kott (feŋ-)čera ‘(female)
spruce grouse’ and Ket dīd, Yugh dīdj ‘spruce grouse’, where the velar nasal in the
Ket and Yugh irregular plural form dekŋ ‘spruce grouse.pl’ presumably arose because the alveolar nasal of the animate class suffix *-(V)n interacted with the retroflex stem coda.
The foregoing discussion does not do justice to the full variety of irregular noun
plurals in Yeniseian. The extensive coverage of dialectal forms in (Porotova 1990:
22–61), as well as the lucid presentations of plural allomorphy in Georg (2007: 92–
101) and Werner (1997b: 96–102), consider many more exceptional plurals than
could be analyzed here. The most complete reference for Southern Ket noun plurals
is the two-volume Comprehensive dictionary of Ket (Kotorova & Nefedov 2015). Porotova’s dialectal noun dictionary (Porotova 2002) is additionally useful because it
includes plurals in Yugh as well as from all three Ket dialects.
2.3.2 Dual number marking and apparent pleonastic plural marking in Ket nouns
A diachronic perspective also explains how dual number marking arose in a few
Ket nouns, and why other nouns appear to add two consecutive plural suffixes.
Many Ket monosyllabic singular noun stems result from contractions of disyllabic
root compounds or involve elided final-stem consonants. These changes sometimes
have implications for understanding number marking in the modern language.
Plural noun forms that seem to contain two consecutive plural suffixes include:
Number in Ket (Yeniseian)
327
kū ‘opening / mouth (of object)’ → kuniŋ ‘openings’, dɯ’ ‘hat’ → dɯniŋ ‘hats’, and
kǝ́ jka ∼ kǝjá ‘head’ → kǝ́ jkeniŋ (alongside kǝ́ jken) ‘heads’. In the case of kū ‘opening’
and dɯ’ ‘hat’, the stem originally ended in *x, which elided in the bare singular
form but was preserved through nasalization before the plural suffix -(V)ŋ. The
forms dɯ’ ∼ dɯn- ‘hat’ and kū ∼ kun- ‘opening’ are nothing more than irregular
root allomorphs. The noun kǝ́ jka ‘head’, on the other hand, is a compound of *kɯj
‘center / inner’ + *gen ‘brain’, so that kǝ́ jken is the original singular stem, from
which the final /n/ was later dropped in the singular form after being reanalyzed
as a plural suffix. This yielded the truncated stem kǝjká ‘head’, with kǝ́ jken reanalyzed as a plural form alongside the original plural kǝ́ jkeniŋ ‘heads’. Other apparent
examples of plural suffix concatenations listed by Porotova (1990: 155–158) or Werner (1997b: 96–97) can be explained in similar fashion.
In a similar way, dual marking developed in a few Ket nouns denoting naturally
paired objects. In modern Ket usage the noun meaning ‘sleeve’ has a singular form
bánno, a dual form bánno-n, and a plural (three or more) form bánno-n-iŋ. Etymologically, ‘sleeve’ is a compound of *ben ‘double’ + *gowx ‘opening’. Its original
structure is better preserved in the Yugh cognate bengóu ‘sleeve’. The same process
created ókde ‘one ear’, ókde-n ‘pair of ears’, ógde-n-iŋ ‘(three or more) ears’, where
the “dual” /n/ probably developed via nasalization of a fricative final-stem coda
(analogous to *gowx ‘opening’). Another example is ólta ‘one testicle’, ólta-ŋ ‘pair
of testicles’, ólta-ɣ-in ‘(three or more) testicles’ (< ol ‘covered’ + tǝ’ŋ ‘head’). This did
not occur in holtǝ́ ŋ ‘button’ → holtǝ́ kin [hɔltʌ́ɣin] ‘buttons’ (< hol(ad) ‘leather’ + tǝŋ
‘head’), since buttons are not frequently paired objects. Only nouns that happened
to denote naturally paired objects innovated the expression of dual number via semantic reanalysis of a nasal final coda as a dual marker. The rise of dual number
in the Ket noun meaning ‘eye’, however, remains unexplained: dēs ‘one eye’, dès
‘pair of eyes’, and destáŋ ‘three or more eyes’.
Nouns expressing naturally paired objects also developed a means to specially
emphasize singular number by adding the modifier qóleb ‘half’: qóleb dès ‘one single eye, one-eyed’ (literally ‘half eye’), qóleb būl ‘one single leg (būl)’, ‘one-legged’.
Georg (2007: 92) points out that this pattern, which is shared with Uralic, is likely a
calque from one of the surrounding Siberian language families, where it is ancient
and widespread.
A few remarks should be made here about the grammatical expression of number in determiner phrases consisting of an attributive numeral followed by quantified noun, as this topic is also tangentially relevant to the expression of dual number in Ket. Cardinal numeral words exist in two forms in Yeniseian. Forms used as
attributive modifiers of nouns are normally unmarked for grammatical number. After qo’k ‘(animate class) one’ and qūs ‘(inanimate class) one’, nouns appear in their
unmarked singular form: qo’k ke’d ‘one person’, qūs qu’s ‘one tent’. After ɯ̄n ‘two’,
dōŋ ‘three’, sīk ‘four’, qāk ‘five’, and so forth, the plural form is used: ɯ̄n de’ŋ ‘two
people’, dōŋ qu’ŋ ‘three tents’, etc. Porotova (1990: 171) observes, however, that
328
Edward Vajda
singular noun forms sometimes appear after ɯ̄n ‘two’, and still less often after dōŋ
‘three’ or sīk ‘four’: ɯ̄n / dōŋ / sīk to’q ‘two / three / four steps’; after numbers five
and higher, Ket nouns invariably appear in the expected plural form: qāk / à / òn
toq-ŋ … ‘five / six / seven steps’, etc. Also, nouns that have developed a dual form
show a tendency to use it after the numeral ‘two’, as in ɯ̄n kǝ́ jken ‘two heads’ when
compared to qūs kǝ́ jka ‘one head’ and qāk kǝ́ jkeniŋ ‘five heads’. This usage does not
appear to be fully regular, however, as dōŋ kǝ́ jken ‘three heads’ (containing the socalled dual form of the noun) have also been recorded. Because heads are not a
naturally paired object, perhaps the original stem kǝ́ jken came to be treated as a
sort of paucal form by certain individual speakers. It is also possible that interference from Russian, with its special treatment of using the genitive singular rather
than the plural after the numbers two, three and four, may have influenced the rise
of such forms.
Porotova (1990: 120) also lists a few rare examples where the nominalized form
of the numeral is used attributively with a plural noun, leading to multi-site marking in the number phrase, including ɯn-aŋ de’ŋ ‘two people’, with animate plural
–aŋ on the numeral, in place of the expected ɯn deŋ ‘two people’, where the numeral form is unmarked. Such cases do not appear to represent canonical usage. These
rare examples also violate the typically strong head marking profile of Yeniseian
morphosyntax. Only attributive forms of the numeral ‘one’ regularly and canonically reflect the grammatical class of the head noun and thus constitute genuine examples of dependent marking in modern Ket. See, however, Section 3.2 below for a
description of two more widespread instances of marking on both modifier and
noun in Ket noun and determiner phrases.
2.3.3 Other number-related aspects of Ket noun morphology
Pluralia tantum nouns are rare in Ket. One example is kǝ’t ‘children of the same
mother’; the stem dɯ̄l ‘child (in general)’ is singular to the formally related forms
dɯlkat ∼ dɯlkitn ‘children (in general)’, forming a pair that displays the typical
singular/plural number contrast, like nearly all Ket nouns denoting countable objects. Two other possible examples are obáŋ, the plural of ōb ‘father’, and amáŋ, the
plural of ām ‘mother’, when used in the secondary meaning of ‘parents, ancestors’.
Because the singular nouns ōp and ām can only mean ‘father’ or ‘mother’ and not
‘(generic) parent’ or ‘ancestor’, the plural forms obáŋ and amáŋ when used to mean
‘parents’, ‘ancestors’ could be regarded as pluralia tantum nouns.
Mass nouns in Ket are often singularia tantum lacking plural forms, as is typical
for many languages with a number distinction in their noun morphology: ūl ‘water’,
aal ‘broth’, altǝq ‘mud’, ho’q ‘filth’, na’n ‘bread’, īm ‘pine nuts’, hu’s ‘whortleberries’, tulit ‘red currants’, etc. Many of these words derive count nouns by adding a
suffix expressing a quantifiable amount of the given item. Such mass-quantifying
Number in Ket (Yeniseian)
329
suffixes express specific amounts, shapes and consistencies. The most productive
is the singulative suffix -des ∼ -dis, which attaches to mass nouns to express a
countable quantity in the form of a droplet of liquid or a single berry or pine nut.
The examples in (26) juxtapose the underlying mass noun with its derived count
noun and its plural form:
(26) Count nouns derived with the mass-quantifying suffix -des ∼ -dis
ūl
‘water’, ‘rain’
úldis
‘droplet’, ‘raindrop’ (plural: úldisn)
īm
‘pine nuts’
ímdis
‘(single) pine nut’
(plural: ímdisn)
eel
‘lingonberries’
éldis
‘(single) lingonberry’ (plural: éldisn)
óŋniŋ
‘roe’
óŋndis ‘(single) fish egg’
(plural: óŋndisn)
The meaning of a single droplet or single discrete berry, nut, or grain of roe accrues
from the suffix’s etymological origin in the anatomical noun root dēs ‘eye’, which
accounts for why the countable items it expresses are normally roundish is shape.
Only rarely is this suffix used to convert a mass noun into a countable object of
radically different shape, perhaps the sole example being daan ‘grass’ → daandis
‘blade of grass (plural: dandisn).
There are a number of other, less productive mass-quantifying suffixes in Ket,
all derived from roots or combinations of roots. The noun lamtǝ ∼ lamt ‘piece’ can
be added to Ket mass nouns to convey a congealed lump or broken off piece, thus
deriving another set of countable noun stems:
(27) Count nouns derived by adding the suffix -lamt ‘piece’ to mass noun stems
kɯ̄d
‘fat’
kɯtlámt
‘lump of fat’
(plural: kɯtlámtaŋ)
sūl
‘blood’
sullámt
‘blood clot’
(plural: sullámtaŋ)
na’n
‘bread’
nanlámt
‘piece of bread’
(plural: nanlámtaŋ)
ōks
‘wood’
okslámt
‘(small) piece of wood’ (plural: okslámtaŋ)
qō
‘ice’
qoklámt
‘(small) chunk of ice’
(plural: qoklámtaŋ)
Yugh uses the suffix -lap in a similar way: sur-láp ‘blood clot’ (plural sur-láf-ɯn).
The Ket noun dápqul ‘heap’, which originated via metathesis involving the roots
*daq ‘put / laid down’ + *pɯl ‘growth, mass, quantity’, is added to a few mass
nouns to denote countable larger pieces: qokdápqul ‘(large) chunk of ice’ (plural:
qokdápqulaŋ). Finally, the Ket suffix -les, of unknown etymology, derives count
nouns expressing pieces of flat flexible objects: si’k ‘rawhide’ → síkles ‘piece of rawhide’ (plural sikles-n).
Some of these Yeniseian mass-quantifying suffixes have been called ‘singulatives’ (Porotova 1990: 65). Note, however, that they are derivational suffixes and
cannot be regarded as grammatical markers of singular number because they remain when the stems they derive are inflected for plural meaning. In this connection, the count noun óŋndis ‘(single) fish egg’, which is derived from óŋniŋ ‘roe’ is
330
Edward Vajda
noteworthy because it involves the truncation of the final-stem segments -iŋ, probably via reanalysis as a plural marker. The same process also seems to be at play in
hǝ́ naŋdis ∼ hǝ́ ndis ‘grain of sand’, where the final portion of the stem hǝ́ naŋ ‘sand’
is optionally deleted, probably due to its resemblance to a plural marker. The mass
noun hǝ́ naŋ ‘sand’ also unusually allows pluralization as hǝnaŋ-an in the meaning
‘expanses of sand’, as recorded by Werner (2002, vol 1: 338). Still, the mass-quantifying ‘singulative’ suffix -dis in hǝ́ naŋdis ∼ hǝ́ ndis ‘grain of sand’ cannot be viewed
as a grammatical marker of singular number, since it is compatible with the addition of a plural suffix: hǝ́ naŋdis-n
There is some evidence, however, that a genuine grammatical singulative suffix
once operated in Yeniseian. Helimski (2016) identifies the /s/ element at the end of
several noun stems as a fossilized singulative. This interpretation is supported by
the fact that the element in question does disappear when a plural suffix is added.
(28) Possible examples of a fossilized singular marker (the “S-singulative”) in Ket
tɯ’s
‘stone’, ‘rock’
tǝ’ŋ
‘stones’, ‘rocks’
qu’s
‘birchbark tent’, also ‘house’
qu’ŋ
‘birchbark tents’
qe’s
‘sandbar (in river)’
qedeŋ
‘sandbars’
ōks
‘tree’
a’q
‘trees’
On the other hand, the disappearance of word-final /s/ in these forms might instead
be interpreted as simply another irregular stem change in Yeniseian plural formation, with the elision of /s/ due to some as yet unexplained morphophonemic process. In particular, the forms for ‘tree’ are suppletive, which complicates any attempt
to argue that /s/ in the singular form is a morpheme separable from the rest of the
word. In any event, none of these forms can be used as mass nouns by deleting the
final /s/; for instance, there is no bare form like [tɯ’] that means ‘rock’ as a generic,
uncountable mass. The existence of an S-singulative in an earlier stage of Yeniseian,
along with its possible etymological connection to the highly productive nominalizing suffix *-si, remains plausible but unproven. Solving this problem would require
additional diachronic investigation of irregular noun plural morphology.
Ket nouns normally have only one plural suffix, with apparent cases of consecutive nasal suffixes explainable as resulting from the loss of a final stem nasal from
what was originally the full singular stem. However, genuine pleonastic (multi-site)
marking of plurality does occur in some noun + noun compounds made through
coalescence of two formerly independent word forms. A few examples, adapted
from Porotova (1990: 158), are shown in (29), with hyphens inserted to show the
morpheme breakdown:
(29) Noun compounds with multi-site number marking
búl-dal ‘knee tendon’
< būl ‘leg’ + da’l ‘tendon’ → plural: búl-aŋ-dál-eŋ
ál-bes ‘waist-length fur coat’ < āl ‘half’ + be’s ‘rabbit’ → plural: ál-aŋ-bés-n
sés-tas ‘suede boot’
< sàs ‘suede’ + tès ‘boot’ → plural: sás-eŋ-tés-aŋ
Number in Ket (Yeniseian)
331
The first element in such compounds modifies the second to express a contiguitybased relationship involving parts and wholes. Their internal structure reflects the
general syntactic pattern of modifier + head required for all Yeniseian noun and
determiner phrases.
2.4 Verbal number
The polysynthetic Ket finite verb has a diverse arsenal of morphological means for
expressing pluractionality. Only a few examples will be examined here.
Many stems use suppletion (full or partial) of the base (a term used by Ketologists to refer to the stem’s rightmost lexical morpheme) in order to signal a contrast
between a single event and multiple events affecting multiple entities separately:
(30) Base suppletion expressing types of pluractionality
a. d-aŋ-i-b-to
1sbj-hanging-pres-3inan.obj-put.once
‘I hang it up.’ ‘I hang them up (all together, in one action)’
b. d-aŋ-i-b-uk
1sbj-hanging-pres-3inan.obj-put.more.than.once
‘I hang them up (two or more separately, perhaps one after the other).’
c. t-a-b-qut
down-pres-3inan.sbj-one.lies
‘It lies (there).’
d. t-a-b-damin
down-pres-3inan.sbj-many.lie
‘They (many things) lie (there).’
The form (30b) d-aŋ-i-b-uk ‘I hang them up’ expresses multiple events, while the
form (30a) d-aŋ-i-b-to ‘I hang it up’ expresses a single event and could also mean ‘I
hang them up’ in cases where a handful of inanimate objects are simultaneously
placed in a hanging position. The form (30d) t-a-b-damin ‘They (objects) lie there’
denotes a single situation, but with emphasis on more than one object lying in
different places. This form is also interesting in that its composite base, -damin,
seems to contain a fossilized plural suffix -in, which in all other types of finite verb
forms correlates with animate class subjects only.
Along with base suppletion, the pluractional (pltc) prefix d- sometimes appears
in multiple-action stems.
332
Edward Vajda
(31) Example of (a) single- and (b) multiple-action pair involving pluractional da. d-es-a-b-daq
1sbj-surface-pres-3anim.pl.obj-put.once
‘I lay it down.’ / ‘I lay them down (objects placed together in one action).’
b. da=d-a-b-da
3 f.sbj=plct-pres-3anim.pl.obj-put.many
‘I lay it down many times.’ / ‘I lay them down (objects, one after another).’
In another pair, the pluractional (plct) prefix n- distinguishes the multiple-action
stem, in addition to base morpheme suppletion:
(32) a. d-aŋ-baq
1sbj-3anim.pl.obj-give.once
‘I give them (something) once.’
b. d-aŋ-n-bu
1sbj-3anim.pl.obj-plct-give.many.times
‘I give them (something) many times.’
The pluractional prefix n- is unique to this pair, while d-, though also largely unproductive, is found in many stems, where it sometimes surfaces as t- through phonological merger with adjacent morphemes.
Some verbs express pluractionality by using two pluractional affixes as well as
a suppletion of the morpheme:
(33) a. da=don-ba-h-ol-ted
3 f.sbj=knife-1sg.obj-area-pst-hit.endwise
‘She stabbed me (once).’
b. da=don-an-ba-t-ol-do
3 f.sbj=knife-pl-1sg.obj-plct.area-pst-gouge
‘She kept stabbing me.’
The pluractional suffix form /t/ in (33b) derives from d- merged with the area prefix h-, which remains visible in the single-action form in (33a). Notice also that the
plural suffix -an on the incorporated instrument noun do’n ‘knife’ is a pluractional
marker in that it expresses multiples acts of stabbing (done by one person or by
many people) rather than stabbing with multiple knives. The noun plural suffix in
this mildly productive verb stem pattern therefore marks pluractionality in conjunction with the addition of the pluractional prefix d- as well as base morpheme suppletion – though both -ted ‘hit endwise’ and -do ‘cut, gouge, hew’ when used on
their own can refer to either single or multiple actions.
Number in Ket (Yeniseian)
333
Just as multi-site grammatical agreement is not infrequent in Ket, the lexical
category of pluractionality is also frequently expressed using combinations of multiple morphemes in the same verb form (31–33). There is no evidence that Ket pluractionality involves a morphological distinction between a small number events (two
or three) and a larger number.
3 Agreement and syntax of number
The marking of grammatical categories on modifiers is generally atypical for Yeniseian, being limited to a few types of noun or determiner phrases. As mentioned in
2.2.3, attributive forms of the numeral ‘one’ regularly agree with their head noun in
animacy but not gender: animate class qo’k qīm ‘one woman’ and qo’k hīk ‘one man’
vs. inanimate class qūs tɯ’s ‘one rock’. Marking of plural number on both the numeral form and the noun has been documented sporadically in a few phrases with
higher numbers like ɯn-aŋ de’ŋ ‘two people’ in place of the canonical ɯ̄n de’ŋ ‘two
people’, which shows the typical pattern of plural marking on the noun only. This
section examines two additional instances where number is marked on modifiers as
well as on the noun. In the first instance, examined in 3.1 below, demonstrative
pronouns are grammatically marked for noun class (both animacy and gender) as
well as animate plural number, while the nouns they modify are marked only for
plural number, but not for class. In the second, a small minority of adjectives and
action nominals (the Ket equivalent of participles) take a plural suffix that distinguishes them from their singular forms (3.2). There are no other instances in Ket
nominal morphology where modifiers take grammatical affixes. Case suffixes in
noun or determiner phrases attach to the head noun only and never appear on
modifying words. Adjectives and adverbs that undergo S-nominalization can take
case inflections, but only as the head of a noun phrase. Finally, subsection 3.3 examines number marking in the subject complements of locative and existential
clauses.
3.1 Class and number agreement in demonstrative pronouns
The most frequently encountered and straightforward exception to the general pattern of Ket head marking appears in determiner phrases, where demonstrative pronouns agree in class and number with their head noun. Ket demonstrative roots
express a three-way contrast between proximal ki- (close to speaker), medial tu(farther from speaker but earlier mentioned or in the general vicinity) and distal qa(distant from speech situation). When modifying a singular masculine animate class
noun, all three demonstrative roots take the suffix -d (pronounced as the rhotic flap
[ɾ] in southern Ket). When modifying a feminine singular animate class or inanimate
334
Edward Vajda
Tab. 3: Class and number agreement forms of demonstrative pronouns.
-d
kīd / tūd / qād hīk
‘this / that (near) / that (far) man’
-de
kide / tude / qade qīm
‘this / that (near) / that (far) woman’
kide / tude / qade qu’s
‘this / that (near) / that (far) tent’
-ne
kine / tune / qane hikn
‘these / those (near) / those (far) men’
kine / tune / qane qimn
‘these / those (near) / those (far) women’
-de
kide / tude / qade qu’ŋ
‘these / those (near) / those (far) tents’
class noun of either number, they take the suffix -de (pronounced [ɾɛ] in Southern
Ket). When modifying any plural animate class noun, they take the suffix -ne.
Plural marking in demonstratives that modify animate class nouns represents
another case where grammatical marking appears on the modifier as well as its
head noun. Inflection for class (masculine animate, feminine animate, and inanimate) in singular forms of demonstrative modifiers constitutes dependent marking,
however, since these categories are covert in the head nouns themselves.
Vajda (2013a: 87–88) argued that the agreement suffixes in modern Ket demonstrative pronouns are vestiges of ancient possessive morphology, which explains
why they exactly mirror the grammatical categories expressed by 3rd person possessive clitics.
3.2 Plural marking in adjective and action nominal forms
While most Ket adjectives keep the same form regardless of whether they apply to
a single or plural entity, about a dozen mark plurality by adding the suffix -(V)ŋ
(Bibikova 1976: 91). Although this suffix, with its epenthetic vowel and velar nasal,
resembles that used on inanimate class plural nouns, these exceptional adjective
plural forms modify inanimate as well as animate class nouns, as can be seen in
these attributive phrases. Example (34) provides an exhaustive list of Ket adjective
forms that typically occur in different singular and plural forms:
(34) Examples of plural agreement in adjectives
qà ke’t
‘big person’
qē-ŋ de’ŋ
ugd būl
‘long leg’
ugd-eŋ bulaŋ
ho’l qa’d
‘short coat’
hol-aŋ qadaŋ
bo’l dɯlkit ‘fat child’
bol-aŋ dɯlkitn
ka’t ke’d
‘old person’
kat-aŋ de’ŋ
sīn ōks
‘old / rotten tree’ sin-aŋ a’q
‘big people’
‘long legs’
‘short coats’
‘fat children’
‘old people’ [human age]
‘old / rotten trees’ [objects]
Number in Ket (Yeniseian)
dǝ́ qta qɯ̄t
uul la’m
ǝǝl hīk
èt ko’p
ēt ìt
bɯ̄d àŋ
to’t de’
‘fast wolf’
‘smooth board’
‘unmarried man’
‘living chipmunk’
‘sharp tooth’
‘strong rope’
‘shallow lake’
dǝ́ qta-ŋ qǝtn
uul-aŋ lemiŋ
ǝǝl-aŋ hikn
et-iŋ koon
et-iŋ iteŋ
bɯd-eŋ aŋen
tot-iŋ dēŋ
335
‘fast wolves’
‘smooth boards’
‘unmarried men’
‘living chipmunks’
‘sharp teeth’
‘strong ropes’
‘shallow lakes’
Using internal reconstruction, Vajda (2013b: 20–22) demonstrated that the element
-(V)ŋ in these words was originally an adjective-deriving suffix that was reanalyzed
as a marker of plurality through false analogy with the common noun plural suffix
-(V)ŋ. For reasons that are not entirely clear, some adjectives absorbed the suffix
into their roots in plural as well as singular forms, which explains the unusually
high prevalence of falling tone in monosyllabic Ket adjectives. Other adjectives retain the suffix in all contexts, suggesting it was originally part of the stem rather
than a plural marker. Two examples of the latter type of adjective stems are Ket
sokŋ ‘thick’, údokŋ ‘lazy’, which modify either singular or plural nouns: údokŋ ke’t
‘lazy person’, údokŋ de’ŋ ‘lazy people’. Other adjectives show free variation between
forms with or without the suffix. One example is Yugh súrbes ∼ surbèːħs ∼ súrbesiŋ
‘red’ < *sū r ‘blood’ + *wes ‘resemble’ + *(V)ŋ ‘adj suffix’, with any one of these
variants capable of modifying either singular or plural nouns. The Ket cognate
súlem ‘red’, which shows a more radical reduction, also occurs with either singular
or plural nouns – súlem tɯ’s ‘red stone’, súlem tǝ’ŋ ‘red stones’, providing yet another indication that the final nasal element was originally part of the stem. Recognizing this semantic reanalysis of the adjective derivational suffix *-(V)ŋ, which appears to be an innovation that is limited to the Ket-Yugh branch of the family, makes
it unnecessary to posit adjective number agreement as an original feature of Yeniseian morphology and also explains the origin of this highly idiosyncratic trait in a
strongly head marking language like Ket.
An identical process affected the stem phonology of action nominals, a lexical
class of modifiers that serves a participial as well as infinitival function in Ket. Action nominal derivation originally involved attaching a nasal suffix to a finite verb
root or complex lexical stem. This suffix is homonymous with the adjective-deriving
suffix -(V)ŋ and may be etymologically identical to it; in any event, both adjectival
and action nominal -(V)ŋ underwent a parallel evolution in the historical development of Ket. In some action nominals this suffix remained unchanged in all forms:
bágdeŋ ‘pulling’, táseŋ ‘getting up’, ániŋ ‘playing’, qódeŋ ‘dying’, íliŋ ‘eating’, éjiŋ
‘going’, háleŋ ‘wrapping up’, etc. In other stems it was absorbed into the root, usually creating falling tone: bǝ̀ k [bʌ̀ɣ] ‘finding’, tàd [tàɾ] ‘beating’, tòs ‘raising’, kàl ‘fighting’, dàq [dàʁ] ‘laughing’, dùd [dùɾ] ‘burning’, èj ‘killing’, etc. Less often, absorption
of the suffix into the verb root yields another tone: kīt ‘rubbing’, ǝ̄ n ‘cooking’, kǝ’j
‘going hunting’, dǝ’q ‘living’, etc. A full explanation for why the action nominal and
336
Edward Vajda
adjective suffix was sometimes retained and sometimes absorbed into the stem
would require a detailed excursion into diachronic phonology, similar to what
would be required to explain the origin of exceptional ablaut noun plurals. Paralleling the development of a number contrast in a few adjective stems, this suffix was
similarly reanalyzed as a marker of plurality or pluractionality in a minority of action nominal stems. This has resulted in a sporadic contrast between a contracted
form used with reference to single actions or singular entities, and a suffix-augmented
form that refers to multiple actions or modifies plural nouns. Example (35) shows
three Ket action nominals that typically use distinct singular and plural forms.
(35) Number marking in action nominals
suulbèd ‘sled-making (one event)’
suulbed-eŋ ‘sled-making (in general)’
tǝǝl i’ ‘freezing day’
tǝl-iŋ ekŋ ‘freezing days’
ūs i’
‘warm day’10
us-eŋ ekŋ ‘warm days’
The same subset of adjective and action nominal stems that show a number contrast
when used as modifiers in noun phrases carries this opposition into the stem morphology of nominalizations. Example (36) shows compound words containing adjective suffixes that have been reanalyzed as plural markers:
(36) a. noun phrase
bol-eŋ lon-eŋ
thick-pl lip-pl
‘thick lips’
b. noun phrase
ukd-eŋ bul-aŋ
long-pl leg-pl
‘long legs’
compound adjective
bol-eŋ-lon-eŋ ke’d
thick-pl-lip-pl
person
‘thick-lipped person’
S-nominalization
bol-eŋ-lon-eŋ-s
thick-pl-lip-pl-nmlz
plural of S-nominalization
bol-eŋ-lon-eŋ-s-in
thick-pl-lip-pl-nmlz-pl
compound adjective
ukd-eŋ-bul-aŋ assel
long-pl-leg-pl
animal
‘long-legged animal’
S-nominalization
ukd-eŋ-bul-aŋ-s
long-pl-leg-pl-nmlz
plural of S-nominalization
ukd-eŋ-bul-aŋ-s-in
long-pl-leg-pl-nmlz-pl
‘one with long-legs’
‘ones with long legs’
‘ones with thick lips’ ‘ones with thick lips’
In complex nouns or noun phrases containing more than one plural marker, it is
the number category at the noun’s rightmost edge that signals whether a singular
or plural entity is being referred to. The retention of this type of multi-site number
marking in complex stems, however, is highly lexicalized, as can be seen from the
forms in (37):
(37) singular phrase
ukd a’d
long bone
long bone
plural phrase
ukd-eŋ ad-eŋ
long-pl bone-pl
‘long bones’
S-nominalization
ukd-ad-s
long-bone-nmlz
‘one with big bones’
plural of S-nominalization
ukd-ad-s-in
long-bone-nmlz-pl
‘ones with big bones’
10 Though unclear from the English translation ‘warm day’, the morpheme us is a verb root that
heads finite forms like b-il-us ‘it turned warm’, in addition to appearing in the action nominal ūs ∼
useŋ ‘turning / being warm’.
Number in Ket (Yeniseian)
337
Here the use of plural marking in both modifier and noun fails to carry through to
the complex stem form nominalized by -s, in contrast to the presence of precisely
two lips or precisely two (or four) legs. The NP ‘long bone’ therefore acts more like
a mass noun in the complex nominalizations shown in (37). At the same time, note
the lack of plural adjective form in ukde-hǝŋn-eŋ ke’d ‘long-armed man’, a compound recorded by Werner (2002, vol. 2: 322). This type of variation probably follows
from the originally derivational nature of the adjective suffix -(V)ŋ, whose grammatical function of marking plural agreement developed only later through semantic
reanalysis and remained sporadic.
This secondary singular/plural distinction in the same subset of adjective and
action nominal stems also shows up when these forms are incorporated into finite
verbs (3.4.1) or are used as subject complements in copular clauses (3.3).
3.3 Number marking in the subject complements
of linking verb clauses
While most Ket clauses are headed by a finite verb, some copular clauses contain a
subject complement and no tense-bearing verb form at all. The predicates in clauses
of this type have three different kinds of subject complements: 1) a bare noun or
noun phrase; 2) an S-nominalization derived from another part of speech; or 3) a
qualitative adjective or locational stem inflected with a subject agreement suffix.
Bare nouns used as subject complements may be singular or plural in form but lack
agreement in person with their clausal subject:11
(38) Nouns used predicatively as subject complements
a. tu-de
tɯ’s
that-inan rock
‘That is a rock’
b. tu-de
tǝ’-ŋ
that-inan rocks-pl
‘Those are rocks.’
c. tū-d qòj ben ke’d
that-m bear really person
‘That bear is really human.’
11 During his 19th century fieldwork with Ket and Yugh speakers, Castrén (1858: 100–103) recorded
noun forms like uob-di ‘I am a father’ and uob-du ‘He is a father’ with subject concord suffixes, but
such forms are no longer used in any of the three modern Ket dialects (Werner 1997b: 306).
338
Edward Vajda
d. ǝtn de’ŋ
we people
‘We are people.’
The second morphological type of subject complement in copular clauses with no
finite verb form is the S-nominalization, which can be derived from any non-noun.
Used predicatively, S-nominalizations regularly show number agreement with the
clause subject; however, similar to bare nouns they lack person agreement. Adjectives like qà ∼ qēŋ (or qàŋ) ‘big’ that display a morphological number contrast when
used attributively, maintain this formal contrast when nominalized by -s:
(39) S-nominalizations used predicatively as subject complements in copular
clauses
a. tɯ’s qà-s
rock big-nmlz
‘The rock is big.’
b. tɯ’-ŋ qe-ŋ-s-in
rock-pl big-pl-nmlz-pl
‘The rocks are big.’
c. ke’d
qà-s
person big-nmlz
‘The person is big.’
d. de’ŋ
qe-ŋ-s-in
people big-pl-nmlz-pl
‘The people are big.’
S-nominalizations show plural agreement with inanimate as well as animate class
subjects. In addition, forms like qe-ŋ-s-in ‘they (either animate or inanimate class)
are big’ display multi-site plural marking, while S-nominalizations derived from
non-variable stem adjectives (the majority of forms) mark plurality word-finally
only: ke’d udokŋ-s ‘the person is lazy’, de’ŋ udokŋ-s-in ‘the people are lazy’, at aqta-s
‘I am good’, ǝtn aqta-s-in ‘we are good’.
The third type of non-finite predicate contains an adjective or locational adverb
that takes a suffix agreeing in person, number, and class (animacy and gender) with
the clause subject, as shown in (44):
(40) Concord suffixes on predicate adjectives used as subject complements
a. tɯ’s qa-am [qaɣam]
rock big-inan.sbj
‘The rock is big.’
Number in Ket (Yeniseian)
339
b. tǝ’-ŋ
qe-ŋ-am
rock-pl big-pl-inan.sbj
‘The rocks are big.’
c. at qa-di
I big-1sg.sbj
‘I am big.’
d. ǝtn qe-ŋ-dǝŋ
we big-pl-1pl.sbj
‘We are big.’
In contrast to S-nominalizations, there is no singular/plural differentiation in the
inanimate class forms, except in the minority of stems where adjectival -ŋ has been
reanalyzed as a plural marker. The concord suffixes that appear on predicate adjectives, locational adverbs, or action nominals12 used as subject complements mark
person and class, in addition to number. Just as in other realms of Yeniseian morphology, the masculine and feminine animate singular classes merge into a single
animate plural class.
The examples in Table 4 show the full array of predicate concord suffixes on
Ket and Yugh forms of the adverb *qapǝ, which originally meant something like
‘inside one’s tent’.13 The Yugh forms are adapted from Werner (1997a: 209).
Vajda (2019: 68) argued that modifying words with predicate concord suffixes
were originally finite verbs constructed from an incorporated modifier followed by a
Tab. 4: Paradigm comparing Ket and Yugh predicate concord suffix forms.
1sg ‘I am at home / in my tent.’
2sg ‘You are …’
3m.sg ‘He is …’
3 f.sg ‘She is …’
1pl ‘We are …’
2pl ‘You (all) are …’
3anim.pl ‘They (people, animals) are …’
3inan ‘It is …’ ‘They (things) are …’
Southern Ket
Yugh
qaˑ-di
qaˑ-ku
qaˑ-du
qaˑ-dǝ
qaˑ-dǝŋ
qaˑ-kǝŋ
qaɣ-aŋ
qaɣ-am
χaˑb-di’
χaˑp-ku’
χaˑb-du’
χaˑb-da’
χaˑb-dǝːħŋ
χaˑp-kǝːħŋ
χaf-eːħŋ
χaf-e’
12 Subject concord suffixes on action nominals express that the subject is able to perform the
given action’: il-di ‘I can sing’ (from i’l ‘singing’, ‘to sing’).
13 The phonemic form of this adverb in modern Ket is /qap/, which could also be transcribed as
/qah/, since word-final /p/ and word-initial /h/ represent a single phoneme in the transcription
used in this article. The forms in (45) show the surface allomorphs [qaˑ] and [qaɣ] rather than the
underlying phonemic form.
340
Edward Vajda
subject agreement prefix on an archaic word-final verb root meaning ‘be’ or ‘having
become’, probably shaped something like *eŋ. Phonological reductions in this
agreement prefix + verb root sequence produced the modern concord suffix forms,
in which the original verb root was reanalyzed as a nasal plural agreement suffix.
This interpretation also explains why predicate concord in modern Ket is suffixing,
whereas subject and object agreement in finite verbs (described in the next section)
follows a mostly prefixing template. If subject complements with predicate concord
do indeed derive etymologically from prefixed finite verb forms, their adjective or
locational adverb portions were once incorporated modifiers exactly like those still
found in many finite verb forms today.
3.4 Number marking in finite verb forms
Ket verb morphology is based on a discontinuous stem that allows the incorporation
of certain types of modifying elements (Vajda 2017a). Agreement affixes interdigitated between the stem’s lexical morphemes index the subject and direct object arguments. Other affixes in the verb complex distinguish past and non-past tense. Finite
verb clauses in Ket are exclusively head marking. While subjects and direct objects
are indexed verb-internally, any noun or pronoun arguments appearing in the finite
verb clause remain unmarked for grammatical relations. Yeniseian nominal morphology lacks any type of nominative, accusative, absolutive or ergative case markers altogether. As earlier described with reference to possessive clitics and other
morphologies evolved from them, plural number agreement in finite verbs is grammatically marked only with reference to animate class subjects or objects. Inanimate
class subjects or objects trigger the same affixal form regardless of whether they are
singular or plural. The discussion below skirts around much of the verb’s internal
polysynthetic complexity to concentrate on features relevant to the topic of number
marking. Subsection 3.4.1 examines how the finite verb template expresses grammatical subject and object number agreement. The main point is that plural agreement is normally limited to animate class subjects and objects. Subsection 3.4.2
draws on the description given earlier in 3.2 of how number marking in adjectives
and action nominals arose via reanalysis in order to examine features of verb morphology originally unconnected with number that later acquired plural or pluractional meaning based on coincidental homonymy with the common noun plural
suffix -(V)ŋ. Plural markers that arose in Ket via reanalysis of derivational or aspectual affixes differ from the inherited Yeniseian agreement system in occasionally
reflecting the plurality of inanimate class subjects.
3.4.1 Verb-internal class, person, and number concord
Subject/object concord in finite verb forms is accomplished using verb-internal affixes that express the same grammatical categories of agreement in person, class
341
Number in Ket (Yeniseian)
and number found in possessive constructions. Intransitive verb forms normally
require subject agreement, while transitive verbs agree with their subject and direct
object. The following template underlies every Ket finite verb form:
Tab. 5: Ket finite verb template, with agreement slots shaded.14
8
7
sbj
incorperson porated
noun,
modifier,
or action
nominal
6
5
4/3
2
1
0
-1
obj
or sbj
thematic
consonant
prefix(es)
3 sbj or obj
(originally in
slot 4)
now fused with
tensemood
aspect
(n ∼ l)
sbj
or
obj
BASE
(verb
root or
lexical
aspect
marker)
anim
pl sbj
suffix
conjugation
marker (s/i ∼ a/o)
(originally in
slot 3)
Each stem lexically chooses which of the shaded slots must be filled to express
subject or object agreement. No verb form fills all of the template’s five (shaded)
agreement slots, though multi-site subject marking involving two or even three of
these slots filled simultaneously in single verb forms is not uncommon. There are
five productive intransitive agreement configurations and three productive transitive configurations, in addition to a smattering of unproductive types – a system
first explained in Vajda (2001) and Vajda (2004: 44–76) and now most succinctly
described in Nefedov & Vajda (2015: 38–48). Vajda (2017b) explains in detail the
diachronic processes that gave rise to this typologically unusual system. Table 6
shows basic allomorphs found in each agreement slot.15
Of the positions left blank in Table 6, slots 7, 5, and 0 contain lexical morphemes, while slot 2 is used to mark tense, mood and aspect. Slots 6, 4, and 1 are
used variously to express class, person, and number concord. By contrast, the wordinitial marker in slot 8 expresses person and class agreement only, since it works
in tandem with the template’s only productive suffix, located word-finally, which
expresses plurality of any animate class subject in verbs that use prefixal slot 8 for
subject person and class agreement:
14 Subject complements with predicate concord suffixes of the type examined in 3.3 above, are
composed of morphemes that once occupied slots 7-1-0, which reflects an intransitive configuration
still productive today for several other kinds of finite verb stems.
15 See Nefedov & Vajda (2015: 38–40) for more detail on agreement marker allomorphy, including
rules for when P8 markers surface as prefixes and when they surface as clitics.
342
Edward Vajda
Tab. 6: Agreement marking series in the Ket finite verb template.
8
7
sbj
di- 1person
ku- 2person
du- 3m.sg
dǝ- 3f.sg
6
5
obj or sbj
ba- ∼ bo- 1sg
ku- 2sg
a- ∼ o- 3m.sg
i- ∼ u- 3f.sg
Ø- ∼ u- 3inan
dǝŋ- 1pl
ku- 2pl
aŋ- ∼ oŋ3anim.pl
4/3
2
3 sbj or obj
(d)a- 3m.sg16
(d)i- 3f.sg
(d)aŋ- 3anim.pl
b- 3inan
1
sbj or obj
di- 1sg
ku- 2sg
ǝ- 3anim.sg
or 3inan
dǝŋ- 1pl
kǝŋ- 2pl
ǝŋ- 3anim.pl
0
–1
anim
pl sbj
suffix
-in
(41) a. di-k-a-qut [diɣaʁut]
1sbj-up-pres-walk
‘I ascend / go upward.’
b. di-k-a-qut-n
1sbj-up-pres-walk-anim.pl.sbj
‘We ascend / go upward.’
c. ku-k-a-qut
2sbj-up-pres-walk
‘You.sg ascend / go upward.’
d. ku-k-a-qut-n
2sbj-up-pres-walk-anim.pl.sbj
‘You.pl ascend / go upward.’
e. du-k-a-qut
3sbj-up-pres-walk
‘He ascends / goes upward.’
f. du-k-a-qut-n
3sbj-up-pres-walk-anim.pl.sbj
‘They (animate, either gender) ascend / go upward.’
g. dǝ-k-a-qut
3f.sg.sbj-up-pres-walk
‘She ascends / goes upward.’
16 The prefixes d- (anim) and b- (inan) once filled slot 4. Today b- fills slot 3, having metathesized
rightward ahead of the a-conjugation marker. The innovative Ket/Yugh gender contrast between
masc.sg a- and fem.sg i- has eclipsed generic anim d-, which survives vestigially in only a small
minority of forms.
Number in Ket (Yeniseian)
343
In this common intransitive agreement pattern, the marker P8 dǝ- ∼ da= indexes
3rd person singular feminine animate class subjects.17 It is never used to index animate plural subjects, even those specifically denoting groups of females. Instead,
the prefix du- appears in verb forms with animate plural subjects. The position 8
marker du- also indexes a 3rd person masculine animate subject.
The rest of this subsection examines number marking differences between animate and inanimate class arguments, as well as several cases of multi-site animate
plural agreement.
Animacy plays just a pervasive role in Ket finite verb as elsewhere in Ket morphology. Despite this fact, Yeniseian lacks true semantic (active) alignment: roughly
synonymous verbs often require different agreement configurations, while active
and inactive intransitives with completely different semantics often follow an identical agreement pattern. Animate singular nouns display a formal dichotomy between masculine and feminine subclass, as has already been observed in predicate
concord suffixes and other areas of the morphosyntax. Inanimate class subjects and
objects, on the other hand, require a mostly different set of agreement markers that
lack any number contrast between singular and plural. Finally, it is worth mentioning that inanimate class entities rarely serve as the subjects of transitive verbs in
Ket. The sentences in (42) show examples of verb-internal subject concord in clauses
containing arguments of different noun classes and grammatical numbers. The verb
stem in these examples is a linking verb that incorporates its subject complement
and uses slot 6 to express subject agreement:
(42) Intransitive verbs showing 3rd person animate class number agreement
a. qīm
baam-i-tonoq
woman old.woman-3 f.sbj-became
‘The woman became (very) old.’
b. hīk baat-a-tonoq
man old.man-3m.sbj-became
‘The man became old.’
c. qim-n
baam-aŋ-aŋ-tonoq
woman-pl old.woman-pl-3anim.pl.sbj-became
‘The women became old.’
d. hik-n
baat-aŋ-aŋ-tonoq
man-pl old.man-pl-3anim.pl.sbj-became
‘The men became old.’
17 The P8 marker dǝ- ∼ da= is also used to index an inanimate class subject in the few verbs of
this agreement configuration that allow inanimate subjects, such as qām hīk da=kasonam ‘an arrow
took [= killed] the man’.
344
Edward Vajda
Just as in possessive morphology, the masculine and feminine animate distinction
disappears in the plural: the same animate class plural affix -aŋ is used for plural
nouns that denote males as well as females. This set of forms also illustrates how
nouns that fulfill the role of subject complement retain their logical expression of
number, which in plural subject forms like (42c) and (42d) produces yet another
example of multi-site plural marking in Ket.
Inanimate class subjects and objects, by contrast, show no grammatical number
distinction, as shown in (43d) which sets them apart from the rich array of animate
class object agreement markers in (43a), (43b), and (43c):
(43) Class and number marking of objects in one set of Ket transitive verb forms
a. ku-a-ted [kuɣatɛt]
2sbj-3m.obj-hit
‘You hit him.’
b. ku-i-ted [kuˑtɛt]
2sbj-3 f.obj-hit
‘You hit her.’
c. k-aŋ-a-ted
2sbj-3anim.pl.obj-pres-hit
‘You hit them (people or animals).’
d. ku-b-ted
2sbj-3inan.obj-beat
‘You hit it.’ / ‘You hit them (inanimate objects).’
e. k-aŋ-a-teɣ-in18
2sbj-3anim.pl.obj-pres-hit-anim.pl.sbj
‘You (plural) hit them (people or animals).’
f. ku-b-teɣ-in
2sbj-3inan.obj-beat-anim.pl.sbj
‘You (plural) hit it.’ / ‘You (plural) hit them (inanimate objects).’
Other verbs require multi-site subject marking, with both person and number agreement marked twice in different slots in the verb form:
(44) a. Example of multi-site subject marking in an intransitive verb
d-aka-dǝŋ-t-l-aq-in
1sbj-upland-1pl.sbj-to-pst-go-anim.pl.sbj
‘We made a quick round trip from river to forest.’
18 The verb base -ted is replaced by the allomorph -tek whenever the animate class plural subject
agreement suffix -in, a combination that is pronounced [tɛɣin].
Number in Ket (Yeniseian)
345
b. Example of multi-site subject marking in a transitive verb
d-ǝla-dǝŋ-a-qos-n
1sbj-outside-1pl.sbj-3m.obj-take-anim.pl.sbj
‘We take him outside.’
Several reasons for the origin of multi-site agreement markers in Ket verb stems
have been identified (Vajda 2017a), including semantic reanalysis of an incorporate
element that happens phonologically to resemble an agreement marker, but the
origin of the phenomenon in examples (44a) and (44b) remains unclear.
All of the example verbs given so far are single-action stems. The next subsection addresses how the Ket verb expresses pluractionality.
3.4.2 Number marking in finite verbs arising through reanalysis
The number distinction developed in some adjectives and action nominals via reanalysis of the derivational suffix -(V)ŋ as a plural marker, as earlier described in
3.2, is maintained when these words are incorporated into finite verb forms. In verb
forms with inanimate class subjects or objects, which do not mark plural agreement
grammatically, the plural marker in the incorporated adjective is the sole marker of
plurality, as in (45b) and (46b):
(45) Plural marking of incorporated adjective that correlates with inanimate class
subject
a. qa-d-a-b-qan
big-transition-pres-3inan.sbj-become
‘It becomes big.’
b. qe-ŋ-d-a-b-qan
big-pl-transition-pres-3inan.sbj-become
‘They (inanimate objects) become big.’
(46) Plural marking of incorporated adjective that correlates with an inanimate
class object
a. d-ukde-t-a-p-sin
1sbj-long-cause-pres-3inan.obj-be
‘I lengthen it.’
b. d-ukde-ŋ-t-a-p-sin
1sbj-long-pl-cause-pres-3inan.obj-be
‘I lengthen them (inanimate objects).’
In verbs where such adjectives correlate instead with animate class subjects or objects, which do regularly express grammatical plural agreement, the extra plural
346
Edward Vajda
suffix in the incorporated adjective form results in multiple exponence of plural
marking, sometimes in tandem with other stem changes like base suppletion that
likewise express plurality of the argument or pluractionality, as in (47b).
(47) Plural marking of incorporated adjective that correlates with an animate class
subject
a. t=qa-d-a-qan
3m.sbj=big-transition-pres-become
‘He becomes big / grows up / matures.’
b. t=qe-ŋ-d-a-set-n
3anim.sbj=big-pl-transition-pres-many.become-anim.pl.sbj
‘They (people or animals) become big / grow up / mature.’
Generally, adjectives that lack a number contrast outside the verb do not vary to
express plurality when incorporated into a finite verb stem either.
(48) Examples of incorporated adjectives that lack a number distinction
a. d-aqta-a-qan [daqtaʁan]
3m.sbj-good-pres-become
‘He recovers.’
b. d-aqta-a-set-n
3anim.sbj-good-pres-many.animate.become-anim.pl.sbj
‘They (people or animals) recover.’
Like variable adjective stems, variable forms of action nominals incorporated into
finite verbs also signal a number contrast reflecting an intransitive subject or transitive object. Action nominal forms that retain the suffix -(V)ŋ tend to appear in multiple action verbs and contribute to the expression of the idea of pluractionality
(plct), while this element usually is absent in verbs expressing single events:
(49) Variable action nominal forms used to distinguish single from multiple events
a. d-ulad-q-a-it
1sbj-petting-into-3m.obj.pres-put.once
‘I pet / start petting him (male dog, once).’
b. d-ulad-iŋ-q-a-da
1sbj-petting-pl-into-3inan.obj.pres-put.many.times
‘I pet him (male dog, on many different occasions).’
The use of the reanalyzed action nominal suffix -(V)ŋ as a marker of pluractionality
is fairly sporadic. It sometimes remains in Southern Ket single-event verbs as well,
and there is even more variability across the Ket dialects. For example, Central Ket
Number in Ket (Yeniseian)
347
d-uladiŋ-q-a-it ‘I pet him once’ and Yugh d-uljadɯŋ-χ-á j-it ‘I pet him once’ show
that the suffix is retained in single-event verb forms.
One final instance of a morpheme reanalyzed as a plural (pluractional) marker
through coincidental homonymy with the common plural marking -(V)ŋ involves
the archaic change-of-state (inchoative) suffix -(V)ŋ. In a few dozen stems, this old
aspect suffix survived only in forms with plural subjects, where it was reinterpreted
as a subject plural marker. With the base -teel ‘freeze’, it is sporadically retained
when referring to plural entities, including inanimate class plurals.
(50) b-in-teliŋ
3inan.sbj-pst-freeze
(also recorded as b-in-teel, with loss of suffix causing root vowel lengthening)
‘They (inanimate class things) froze (once).’
Example (50) shows that plural markers arising through reanalysis can reference
inanimate as well as animate class entities, while canonical plural agreement in
finite verb forms only indexes animate class subjects or objects. Most other stems
where it survives, however, are generally restricted to animate class subjects, as is
the case with the base -doq ‘fly’.
(51) Animate plural marking expressed by a reanalyzed inchoative suffix
a. d-in-doq
3anim.sbj-pst-flew
‘He flew.’
b. d-in-aŋ-doq-ŋ
3anim.sbj-pst-3anim.pl.sbj-flew-pl
‘They flew (together as a group).’
c. d=t-ol-aŋ-doq-ŋ
3anim.sbj=plct-pst-3anim.pl.sbj-flew-pl
‘They flew (one after another).’
The use of reanalyzed adjective, action nominal, or aspect suffixes as plural or pluractional markers in Ket finite verb forms is sporadic and unpredictable. As already
mentioned, such reanalyzed affixes differ most strikingly from canonical agreement
morphology inherited from Proto-Yeniseian in occasionally expressing plurality of
an inanimate class subject.
4 Semantics and discourse
The semantics of number as category values have been described in the relevant
sections above. There is no additional data currently available to discuss the pragmatics of number in Ket or its role in Ket discourse.
348
Edward Vajda
5 Summary and conclusions
The preceding sections have covered all facets of number marking in modern Ket,
offering observations on the historical evolution of plural marking patterns to help
distinguish ancient features from more recent innovations. The core distinction between singular and plural in Ket encompasses most nouns and pronouns and is
typically expressed by augmenting the bare singular stem with a suffix containing
one of two nasal consonants: /n/ or /ŋ/. There is no evidence that plurals were
ever productively generated by other means. Although some irregularities in plural
formation, including ablaut and odd morphophonemic stem alternations, trace back
to the proto language, they appear to be morphophonological or otherwise secondary. The grammatical dichotomy between animate and inanimate class nouns influences the choice of plural suffix allomorph and also dictates the form of 3rd person
anaphoric pronouns. Because Yeniseian personal pronouns are the etymological
source of possessive markers and verb agreement affixes, these systems likewise
formally distinguish between inanimate and animate class entities. Due to its intrinsic connection with number marking, the grammatical factor of animacy thus casts
an influence over virtually all areas of the morphosyntax in one way or another. By
contrast, the subdivision within animate class nouns between masculine and feminine manifests itself in agreement morphology associated with singular nouns and
pronouns only and may represent a Ket and Yugh innovation not traceable back to
Common Yeniseian.
Both genetic and areal factors in the linguistic history of Yeniseian peoples enhance the value of Ket data for a cross-linguistic study of number marking. Yeniseian languages are genealogically isolated from all other families of northern Asia,
and some features of Ket number marking are not found in any of the neighboring
languages. This includes, in particular, the pervasive formal contrast evident
throughout Ket morphology between how animate class and inanimate class nouns
express and reflect number – a trait not observed anywhere else among the indigenous languages of Siberia. Other features, such as the use of plural suffixes on
nouns is found widely in other indigenous language families of northern and Inner
Eurasia.
In addition, the language’s genealogical isolation makes contact-induced
changes a particularly interesting topic of investigation. Ket speakers were the last
hunter-gatherer-fishers in landlocked northern Asia. They continued to move about
seasonally in small kin-based groups long after all of their neighbors had shifted
to reindeer herding or, in parts of south Siberia, to stockbreeding. For centuries,
demographic pressure induced the Ket hunting bands to take brides from pastoral
groups as marriage partners who presumably had to learn the language as young
adults. The influence exerted on Ket linguistic forms by young L2 speaking mothers
from Turkic, Ugric, Samoyedic, and Tungusic speaking groups could possibly be
implicated in the unusually high number of cases where plural marking developed
Number in Ket (Yeniseian)
349
via semantic reanalysis in areas of Ket morphology where it had originally been
absent. This includes the sporadic creation of dual number forms of nouns, the rise
of multi-site subject plural marking on the basis of a moribund change-of-state affix
in finite verb forms, pluractional marking developed from a reanalyzed action nominal suffix, as well as plural marking in a small subset of adjectives – a lexical class
originally entirely devoid of inflections of any kind within the strongly head marking Yeniseian family. The expression of pluractionality in Ket could conceivably
have been influenced by Selkup, with its rich inventory of Aktionsart forms that
relate to quantification of events. Analogous features need not have been present
as such in the neighboring languages originally spoken by Ket spouses for these L2
speakers to have influenced the spread of new patterns via semantic reanalysis.
Among languages in contact with Yeniseian, adjective agreement, for example, is
present only in Russian, and these patterns seem to have been established in Ket
and Yugh already before the arrival of Indo-European languages in central Siberia.
Finally, it need be stressed that although it is now possible to describe Ket number marking in great detail and to account for many intricate aspects of its diachronic evolution, some facets of its morphological development still remain unclear. This
particularly concerns the motivations behind certain irregular noun plurals, as well
as the reason why only certain adjectives and action nominal stems developed number marking while most did not. The origin of multi-site subject marking in some
Ket verb types also remains unclear.
Abbreviations
abl
ades
anim
dat
f
inan
m
nmlz
obj
pl
pltc
poss
pres
pron
pst
sbj
sg
ablative case
adessive case
animate class
dative case
feminine
inanimate class
masculine
nominalizer
object
plural
pluractional
possessive
present tense
pronoun
past tense
subject
singular
350
Edward Vajda
References
Bibikova, Vera S. 1976. O kategorii chisla v sisteme imeni prilagatel’nykh v ketskom jazyke
[Number marking in Ket adjectives]. In Andrej P. Dul’zon & Tel’mina I. Porotova (eds.), Jazyki i
toponimija Sibiri [Languages and toponymy of Siberia] 6, 90–96. Tomsk: TGPI.
Castrén, M. A. 1858. Versuch einer jenissei-ostjakischen und kottischen Sprachlehre.
St. Petersburg: Imperatorskaja Akademija Nauk.
Georg, Stefan. 2007. A descriptive grammar of Ket, part 1: Introduction, phonology and
morphology. Kent: Global Oriental.
Helimski, Eugene. 2016. S-singulatives in Ket. Journal of language relationship 14(3). 157–163.
Kotorova, Elizaveta & Andrey Nefedov. (eds.). 2015. Comprehensive dictionary of Ket. (2 vol.)
Munich: Lincom Europa.
Nefedov, Andrey & Edward Vajda. 2015. Grammatical sketch of Ket. In Elizaveta Kotorova &
Andrey Nefedov (eds.), Comprehensive dictionary of Ket. Munich: Lincom Europa.
Porotova, Tel’mina I. 1990. Kategorija mnozhestvennosti v enisejskikh jazykakh [The category of
plural in Yeniseian languages]. Tomsk: Tomsk State University.
Porotova, Tel’mina I. 2002. Slovar’ govornykh form ketskikh sushchestvitel’nykh (s formami
mnozhestvennogo chisla) [Dictionary of dialectal forms of Ket nouns (with plural forms)].
Tomsk: TGPU.
Vajda, Edward. 2001. The role of position class in Ket verb morphophonology. Word 52(3). 369–
436.
Vajda, Edward. 2004. Ket (Languages of the world/materials 204). Munich: Lincom Europa.
Vajda, Edward. 2013a. Metathesis and reanalysis in Ket. Tomsk journal of linguistics and
anthropology 1(1). 14–26.
Vajda, Edward. 2013b. Vestigial possessive morphology in Na-Dene and Yeniseian. In Sharon
Hargus, Edward Vajda & Danny Hieber (eds.), Working papers in Athabaskan (Dene)
languages 2012, 79–91. Fairbanks: ANLC.
Vajda, Edward. 2017a. Ket incorporation. In Michael Fortescue, Marianne Mithun, & Nicholas
Evans (eds.), Handbook of polysynthesis, 906–929. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Vajda, Edward. 2017b. Patterns of innovation and retention in templatic polysynthesis. In Michael
Fortescue, Marianne Mithun, & Nicholas Evans (eds.), Handbook of polysynthesis, 363–391.
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Vajda, Edward. 2019. Ket lexical typology. In Mikhail Kotkin (ed.), Wortschätze und Sprachwelten
(Sprach- und Kulturkontakte in Europas Mitte: Studien zur Slawistik und Germanistik 10),
59–72. Berlin: Peter Lang.
Werner, Heinrich. 1997a. Das Jugische. Wiesbaden: Harrassowitz.
Werner, Heinrich. 1997b. Die ketische Sprache. Wiesbaden: Harrassowitz.
Werner, Heinrich. 2002. Vergleichende Wörterbuch der Jenissej-Sprachen. Wiesbaden:
Harrassowitz.