David Kirsh, Dafne Muntanyola, R. Joanne Jao, Amy Lew, Matt Sugihara
kirsh @ ucsd.edu
Choreographic methods for creating
novel, high quality dance
Abstract
We undertook a detailed ethnographic study of the
dance creation process of a noted choreographer and
his distinguished troupe. All choreographer dancer
interactions were video’ed, the choreographer and
dancers were interviewed extensively each day, as
well as other observations and tests performed. The
choreographer used three main methods to produce
high quality and novel content: showing, making-on,
and tasking. We present, analyze and evaluate these
methods, and show how these approaches allow the
choreographer to increase the creative output of the
dancers and himself. His methods, although designed
for dance, apply more generally to other creative
endeavors, especially where brainstorming is involved,
WM’s hands, they have been remarkably successful at
generating novel, high quality dance creations. They are
also of interest because of what they teach us about
distributed creativity more generally, and non-verbal
interaction using multi-sensory imagery. Dance is a very
physical medium, both in performance and the way it is
created. When creating dance, choreographers often
engage their dancers in a bodily way. This is not unique
to dance but in other domains, it is less evident and
harder to study. We are finding that through careful
observation and analysis we are acquiring new insights
into creativity, and multi-modal communication.
Our discussion has four parts: Methodology, Findings,
Analysis, and Discussion.
and where the creative process is distributed over many
individuals. His approach is also a case study in multimodal direction, owing to the range of mechanisms he
Methodology
uses to communicate and direct.
archive of that process, and to analyze the methods,
outcomes and distributed cognition of WM and RD.
The dance team worked for thirteen days on a new
Keywords
Choreography, Multi-modal Instruction.
Our goal in this study was to exhaustively collect data
of the creative process in dance, to create a complete
Here we discuss our developing understanding of the
methods used by a world famous choreographer ,
dance piece at UCSD, the spring of 2009. Their time at
UCSD represents about 60% of the total time allocated
to creating the final work. Neither the music nor the
sets, both specially commissioned by WM, had yet
been made. So our first surprise was that music is not
(hereafter WM), when he works with his dance troupe,
(hereafter RD), to create highly original dance pieces.
These methods are of particular interest because, in
used in the early phase of creation as a mechanism
for generating dance phrases. Whenever the dancers
worked, there was music present. But they danced with
Introduction
188
Design and semantics of form and movement
the music, not to it. We will not discuss music further.
next step was to annotate snippets of video showing
The data we captured and used in our analyses fall into
seven categories. Jointly they comprise a thorough
documentation of the entire distributed creative process.
1. Video: The two dance venues used by the company
activities of particular interest. Students were allowed
to choose particular dance phrases from amongst the 14
phrases the troupe performed in the final review on the
last two days. They then tracked the activities that led
were instrumented with five high definition video
cameras on the walls and two standard camcorders
on the ceiling. See Fig 1. These cameras were run an
average of six hours a day for thirteen days, covering
the times when the choreographer was present and
whenever the dancers were practicing movements.
2. Field notes: Teams of students sat on the sidelines
during the entire process and took notes on
movements, interactions, and instructions each day.
For each dancer there was a dedicated team of 2-3
students, eleven teams in all. Their field notes helped
us to organize and annotate the video archive.
3. Choreographer interviews: The choreographer was
interviewed before and after each working day – a
total of 22 times in all – often for an hour at a time.
4. Dance interviews: Four dancers were interviewed
two at a time, each day. When appropriate, the
dancers ‘danced’ their answers to our specific
questions about the day’s activities. This was
especially helpful when the day’s activities required
the dancers to visualize, or use other sensory
imagery, to help create movement ideas.
5. Motion capture: Three dancers, each performing
several dance phrases, were captured using a
sixteen-camera VICON system. This produced 3-D
trajectories of the dancers in motion.
6. Psychological tests: Each dancer performed a
memory test for dance phases and identified key
positions or ‘movement anchors’. These anchors are
used to help recall attributes and positions and offer
insights into memory.
7. Diaries and notebooks: Photographs were taken
of all written artifacts used by the dancers and
choreographer. These notebooks and diaries are used
to help solve problems, record ideas, and remember
movements and phrases.
to the evolution of those phrases over the course of the
thirteen days, creating snippets that could be compared
on a split screen or spliced together to make a video of
the morphogenesis of a phrase.
Figure 1: A schematic layout of the high definition video cameras
used to capture the interactions between WM and his troupe RD.
Interviews were a further source of insight into the
creative process. Each day, WM was asked to explain his
goals for the day and describe what actually transpired.
The interview was open ended and after the first ten
minutes, the conversation turned to basic questions
concerning choreographic choice, objectives, values,
tasks, imagery, etc. Of the 60 hours of interviews
collected from WM and the dancers, 35 hours were
transcribed, and the process of keywording and indexing
begun. Much of WM’s speech during the day was also
transcribed from cameras and microphones.
Findings
Multi-modality: When creating a dance in the
contemporary tradition, choreographers communicate
with their dancers in diverse physical ways. In table
1 we list seven communicative vehicles we observed
WM using when working with RD. Each carries specific
information for the dancers. Some are obvious: he
To organize and code this data two classes were created
at UCSD to provide the trained labor needed. A master
talked, gestured, used his own body to display what
was to be done, and moved to a position on stage,
vocabulary of keywords was established and used to
produce a single master list of activities, time coded to
facilitate video indexing. This served as a guide to what
or in relation to others, to show the dancers where
to position themselves. But some communicative
mechanisms are non-obvious and uncommon outside
we might find at different moments in the video. The
the dance domain.
Design and semantics of form and movement
189
For instance, touching a dancer can be used to physically
choreographer were invariably in close proximity when
reshape a posture or movement. Its function is more
corrective than denotational. If force is applied to a
body, even gentle force, its purpose may be not so much
to ‘describe’ a structural shape or a body dynamic, as it
vocalizing, the use of vocalization often led to further
interaction. It is usually a move in a sequence of multimodal interactions.
We also observed emergent communicative meaning
is to cause the dancer to change the way s(he) moves,
feels, or even thinks. Several factors operate at once:
the touch must be exactly at the right time and place;
arising because multiple modalities, such as words and
gestures, gestures and vocalization, were used at once.
In table 2, we display a five-minute period of instruction
if it communicates a feeling, such as fatigue, anger, or
physical distress, the touch needs the right dynamics;
and if it communicates a position then the touch must
be appropriately corrective, marking the extension of
a limb or the direction the body should be moved in.
In a physical context such as dance, where the
structures being created are the dynamics of form and
position, it is natural to see touch used as a tool for
sketching, shaping or correcting. But we observed other
less predictable modes of communication, especially
with sound.
and the duration of different modalities in use. Note
how many of the channels overlap. In particular, in the
early phase of this instruction – around the first minute
– we see that WM combines words with gestures,
dancing, touch, and positioning.
Table 2. In a five-minute period WM uses many different
modalities, often at the same time.
Table 1. The choreographer used seven main communicative
devices.
Methods of Instruction: We now describe three
methods of instruction we found WM using and
provide a framework from which to conceptualize their
differences.
1. Show a phrase to the whole troupe or large subset.
WM uses his own body to display the structure and
dynamics of a move or phrase. He has two styles. He
Sound for communicating rhythm is almost universal:
”One, two, three; one, two, three … ”. But sound to
either dances amidst the troupe in the same physical
orientation as everyone else; or he faces the group, as
if teaching. He expects the entire troupe to observe
communicate form, feeling, or ‘quality’ is less familiar.
WM regularly offered corrections, or communicated
some aspect of dynamic form by calling out phrases
and reproduce the move, though, for some moves,
the dancers are expected to execute in a ‘more
perfect’ manner. See figure 2 immediately below.
such as “N’yahh uh oom” or “Tri dah day”. We call this
use of sound ‘vocalization’. The goal of vocalization was
clearly to direct and alter dancer movement. But in
subsequent studies, we were not able to prove that all
dancers interpret the sound the same way. This may be
because its function varied between dancers. To some,
it communicated a dynamic or gestural form; to others,
it communicated a feeling; and to still others, it helped
them to remember the dynamics of a phrase they
Figure 2. When WM shows the troupe a phrase he either
already had mastered. Moreover, because dancer and
stands in front of them, as if teaching in a class (2b).
190
Design and semantics of form and movement
2a.
2b.
operates amidst the group as if dancing with them (2a), or he
2. Make a phrase on a target dancer (solo), or a duo,
a trio or quartet. This method of direction involves
using the bodies of specific dancers as targets on
which to shape the form and dynamics of a move
or phrase. Typically, the entire troupe watches
moving outside it and pushing on it from the outside.
|Using this threefold classification of methods, we
reviewed the video to determine how the methods
were involved in the actual creation of phrases.
these target dancers and later will reproduce those
movements in their own duos, trios, or quartets.
There are also occasions, however, where the
Evolution of Phrases. In table 3, we show the
details concerning the evolution of each phrase. It
is apparent that a phrase is never the outcome of a
point of a ‘make on’ is solely for the target dancers.
See figure 3. A further form of ‘making a phrase
on’ occurs when WM adapts or modifies a phrase
originally created by the dancers. In that case, making
on is more like reshaping.
single instruction method or directive. Sometimes the
choreographer will come to a session with a clear idea
of a movement he wants the dancers to learn. In that
case, he will either Show them all, or Make on a duo,
trio, or quartet. More often, though, he will begin the
creation of a new phrase by assigning a choreographic
problem or task. As can be seen below, nine of the
sixteen phrases started with a Task, five with a Make
On, and two with a Show.
3a.
3b.
Figure 3. WM makes on a duo in 3a, and in 3b, the troupe
observes and copies. WM is taller and shaved. Notice how close
choreographer and target are, and the density of interaction.
3. Task or pose a choreographic problem. In this
third kind of direction, the choreographer assigns
‘choreographic problems’ for the dancers to solve or
choreographic ‘tasks’ for the dancers to complete.
Typically, these problems or tasks require the dancers
to create some sort of mental imagery – a landscape
of Manhattan, the feel of being touched on a certain
part of their body, the dynamic and kinematic feel
Table 3. The evolution of each phrase is shown here as the
outcome of several methods. Final length refers to the usable
material recorded during the final review. A bar indicates that
a day or more passed before the next method was used. An
arrow indicates the same day.
of being a piston moving back and forth. Often, the
way the problem is posed requires the dancers to
invent an image or scenario for themselves. The
choreographic problem is to use this imagery in some
Analysis
way to create a virtual structure that they are then
able to relate to in a ‘choreographically’ interesting
Tasking: Based on the time spent using a method,
the number of times it was used, and the number of
manner. As shown in the pictures on the right and
left, one dancer imagines moving a heavy bell around
another dancer imagines interacting with a barrel –
usable minutes generated, the most important method
is clearly Tasking / Problem solving. As seen in table 4,
62% of instruction time – that is, time dancers were
stepping into it, demarcating its boundaries and then
not practicing – was devoted to working on tasks.
Each of these methods has its individual strengths and
weaknesses, both in terms of productivity, long run
value, and creative potential.
Design and semantics of form and movement
191
This 62% delivered 60% of the final output. Although
tasking sessions were marginally longer, 55 mins. vs.
46 and 44 mins. for making and showing, tasking was
still the method of choice, being called on 58% of the
time. In interview, the choreographer provided several
additional reasons for valuing tasking more highly than
other methods. He mentioned that by assigning the
dancers problems to solve they stretch their repertoire
more effectively – they discover new ways of moving
themselves; he, the choreographer, has the opportunity
to see new things that the dancers can do, and
therefore, he may use those dancers differently in the
future or ‘make on’ them differently; he believed that if
a movement originated as a solution to a problem, the
dancers are likely to imbue it with greater feeling, affect
or quality – what some call greater intentionality; they
will find the phrase easier to remember; and they will
have intellectual ‘anchors’ that can serve as reference
points in the phrase later.
Table 4. This table shows the frequency, average duration, and
fertility of each method of instruction.
Table 5. The fertility of each method can be measured by the
number of usable minutes it generates. We assume that each
method is responsible for a pro-rated share of the minutes in the
There are further reasons to view tasking as of special
interest, particularly for those interested in the nature
of creativity. First, because dancers themselves must
solve tasks, and because each dancer’s imagination is
different, inevitably there will be as many solutions
as dancers. These solutions can be vastly different.
And even if the phrases a dancer makes falls short of
acceptability, it still may engender ideas in WM. We
regularly observed him trying out dancer ideas on
himself, and then later putting these altered ideas to use
in a Show or Make. We also regularly found WM using
a dancer’s solution as a base that could be reworked or
final phrase, even though realistically, some methods contribute
‘massaged’ into a different form.
Second, tasks increase the resources available to
dancers when looking for inspiration. Imaginary
is a mechanism for fostering novelty. In a make on, WM
uses another dancer’s body in place of his own. The
phrase that emerges invariably reflects something of the
structures or feelings can serve as scaffolds for a
dancer. The challenge any dancer faces is to make a
sustained phrase: not just a nice move here or there,
personal style of the dancer and something of the body
style. In interviews, the dancers freely recognized that
the way a phrase turns out, and the authority on how
but something that lasts 40 seconds or a minute. It is
no surprise, then, that people like dancing with other
people. A person makes a nice foil for a partner to
interact with. When a person is absent, a physical
it should be performed always lies with the ‘makee’.
Moreover, since making on involves a close coupling of
choreographer and dancer the phrases that arise must
be the product of a collaboration of sorts, even if the
structure can serve a similar role, though any dynamism
must come from the dancer. On an empty stage,
creative contributions are unequal.
To explore this idea we counted the number and
structures are absent. At such moments imagination,
when guided, can fill the void. This has the effect that
dancers will often be dancing in relation to something
duration of turns that choreographer and dancer take
while engaged in a make-on vs. the number of turns
taken during a show. Our conjecture was that making on
that only they are aware of.
is far more like a conversational dialogue than showing,
192
Design and semantics of form and movement
more to a phrase than others.
A third virtue of tasks is that they breed diversity; the
phrase one dancer comes up with may be stylistically
different than the phrase another comes up with.
Making: As can be seen from table 4, Making is
responsible for 30.5% of WM’s time and 25% of the final
product. Why is Making more prevalent than Showing?
The reason, we speculate, is that making, like tasking,
is a technique that helps WM reach beyond himself. It
so we expected to find more back and forth activity.
usable. But unlike the other methods, Showing is the
That is exactly what we did find. As shown in figure
5 the number of momentary stops, pauses or
opportunities for non-verbal dialogue that we observed
in a randomly chosen ‘make’ far exceeds those we
least collaborative method. The choreographer stands
to learn little from the dancers. In table 6 we display the
average number of seconds yielded by a minute of work
in each of the different instructional methods.
observed in a randomly chosen ‘show’. There were 32
turnovers in ten minutes of making vs. 14 in ten minutes
of showing. This is to be expected given the differences
between making and showing, since in making WM must
be responsive to exactly what the target dancers are doing.
When WM is making he is invariably close to the solo, duo
or trio he is making on and so he will naturally work with
them in a physical, tactile way. As they move so will he.
Because of the physical nature of dancing, its speed and
change in position, we would predict that the level and
frequency of interaction would necessarily be high.
In figure 5 we display dancer and choreographer
mutual activity over five minutes of Showing vs. five
minutes of Making. We analyzed ten minutes but display
only five for greater visibility. The top activity line
displays a showing episode; the bottom activity line is
a making episode. When he Shows, WM, on average,
communicates longer and the dancers practice longer.
This is to be expected because they must copy him. But
when he Makes he works in shorter bursts with the
dancers. Overall, there are more exchanges, however.
Accordingly, Making is more interactive than Showing.
Table 6. Yield (in secs) of a minute of method.
Group Attention, Group Intention: We began
observing what dancers watched most; what they had
in their center of attention. By center of attention,
we mean the person whom the troupe observes,
particularly while WM is instructing. Our prediction was
that when WM is Showing, he is, himself, the center of
attention; when WM is Making on a single dancer, or on
a duo, the center of attention is WM and the makees
jointly if we ask about the rest of the dancers, and it
is WM alone if we ask about the makees (the targets);
and when WM is Tasking, or when the dancers are
themselves solving a choreographic problem, they are
their own center of attention.
And, indeed, the center of attention usually does behave
in this common sense way. But not precisely. In cases
where WM is ‘Making on’ a trio, the remainder of the
troupe will listen to WM but watch the trio. Their
attention is split, as we predicted. But they use the trio,
Figure 5. These two activity lines show that there is more
physical ‘dialogue’ occurring
and not WM, as the reference for the movement. They
listen to WM but watch the target.
The term reference is one we heard the dancers use
to designate the authority, or role model, for the
Showing: Based on time and frequency, Showing is
the least popular, but also, in some ways, it is the most
group when there is a question about the nature of a
given movement. When a choreographer Makes on
potent. Every minute spent Showing yields 4.7 secs of
usable product, compared to 2.4 secs of Tasking and
1.9 secs of Making. This makes sense since WM only
a dancer, or a small group, it is reasonable to assume
that the group evolving the movement, rather than
the choreographer, is the center of attention and the
Shows when he has material that he feels is likely to be
way they perform the movement is the referent. We
Design and semantics of form and movement
193
confirmed this through interview. Once a target makee
A, is the longest standing member of the troupe. In
has mastered a phrase, and often even before, s(he) or
they are taken to be performing the movement in the
definitive way. We observed many times that WM, too,
will rely on makees to recall the movement made on
interview with other dancers, it was reported that A
has the best anticipation of what WM is trying to Show,
and that, accordingly, it is often easier to copy A’s
movements than WM’s. See figure 5.
them, when he needs it again in the future.
So far, this only weakly stretches our preconceived
ideas. If the referent is the person who best knows a
It is tempting to suppose that the movement performed
by the reference dancer, or especially by the surrogate
given phrase, it is natural to look to that person as the
local authority. If a movement is being Shown, however,
shouldn’t WM be the referent? He is the one who
thought it up, he is the one who danced it to Show how
it is to be executed, and he is the one we expect to
remember it perfectly. Our biggest surprise was that
this is not always true.
The reason things sometimes deviate is that when WM
is Showing, there is often another dancer who acts as
a Surrogate Reference. This is a dancer who can be
counted on to precisely master the key aspects of the
movement in near real time. Invariably, a surrogate
reference will be someone who has worked with a
choreographer a long time, or s(he) will be someone
with outlier skills in copying. A surrogate reference will
be someone who reliably interprets what WM is trying
to get the dancers to do, and can display that intent in
a more accessible manner. The result is that there are
cases where WM instructs by Showing, but many of the
troupe, after initially watching WM, will actually watch
the surrogate referent during subsequent re-showings
by WM. In the instance we observed, this other dancer,
Showing a Phrase
reference, is the very movement that all dancers
should learn and memorize. Even this, however, is not
always the case. At each moment, there is an intended
movement – the Platonic ideal of that movement. But
because of body idiosyncrasies, or because the ideal
movement requires considerable practice, the Platonic
movement each member of the troupe is aiming for
may not yet have been displayed. The troupe may know
what they are aiming for but no one has executed it yet.
We call this ideal movement the intended reference
movement.
The idea of a reference movement as an abstraction
is supported by our observations about the surrogate
reference. In some phrases, there are several surrogate
references, each one specializing in particular aspects of
the phrase: One dancer may be counted on to precisely
note the grips or stance; another may be counted on to
precisely note the emotional dynamics. The complete
phrase must incorporate all of these elements. And to
learn that phrase, different dancers may be called on
by WM, during rehearsal, to show off the phrase. This
distributed memory is part of what makes dance creation
a worthy domain for the study of distributed creativity.
Discussion
Making a Phrase on a Trio
We now turn to a more general discussion of the
problematique of dance creation and what we
have learned about the creative value of different
instructional methods.
A major challenge in creating a new piece of choreography
5a.
5b.
Figure 5. In 5a, WM is showing a phrase and so is, himself, a
reference for the dancers. But one of the dancers – a surrogate
is that the work should ideally be both novel and of high
quality (beautiful, interesting, absorbing). Achieving both
attributes is especially difficult because of a trade-off: it
is easier to be novel if one’s work need not meet existing
norms of quality; it is easier to produce recognizably high
quality work if one’s work need not be novel.
they serve as a reference for the onlooking dancers who must
Quality can be thought of as analogous to reliability
in design. For instance, in classical ballet, where the
movements have been refined over years, new works
also learn the trio.
are essentially reliable forms combined in reliable ways.
reference – knows WM’s intentions well and executes them in
ways the others can follow easily. In 5b, WM makes on a trio and
194
Design and semantics of form and movement
6a.
6b.
6c.
Figure 6. 6a shows the basic iso-goodness curve. 6b shows that
Figure 7. Iso-curves can be used to show where the methods of
modern choreographers are biased toward novel work whereas
Show, Make and Task lie on a risk-reward curve.
classical ballet choreographers are biased toward quality.
In figure 6a, we represent this trade-off on a isogoodness curve. It represents the goodness of a
movement. Choreographers with an eye to time and
success will divide their time wisely among all three
choreographic product in terms of its novelty and
quality. A very novel piece may be valued as highly as
a high quality piece. And a piece of exceptional quality
may be valued as highly as a piece of exceptional novelty.
methods.
Historically, modern dance has differed from classical
dance in its norms for what is good. The best modern
work should lie more toward the novel side, using
few tried and true dance forms, and containing
more inventive never-before-seen forms and moves.
Accordingly, the best new creations in modern dance
should lie somewhere near the upper left of the curve.
See figure 6b. Creations in classical ballet, by contrast,
lie somewhere around the lower right. Despite these
different biases or preferences, most work, with the
exception of the best, tends to lie near the middle of
the curve between novelty and classical quality (or
beauty) because it is easier to create a piece in the
middle region than at either extreme. Fig 6c illustrates
that better choreographers live on a better iso-curve.
Their work never follows below a threshold of novelty
and quality.
Great choreographers are noteworthy because they
are able to push their iso-curve outward. See figure 6c.
David Kirsh,
Dafne Muntanyola,
R. Joanne Jao,
Amy Lew
and Matt Sugihara
The authors thank the students of Cognitive Science
classes 160/260W and 160/260S for countless hours
coding and organizing our hundreds of hours of video.
We also thank Scott deLahunta and Phil Barnard for
insightful conversations; and of course, the generous
time and kindness of Wayne McGregor and all the
dancers in Random Dance.
References
1. Banks, M. (2005). Visual methods in social research. London: Sage.
2. Bourdieu, P. (1998). Les règles de l’art The Rules of Art..
Paris: Seuil.
3. Buscatto, M. (2008). L’art et la Manière : Ethnographies du
of novelty. How do they do this? How can they take risks
and still be confident of coming through?
The answer, we suggest, is that different choreographic
4. Carter, A. (1998). The routledge dance studies reader.
methods have different risk reward profiles. Reliability
in design is achieved through a wise distribution of
methods, akin to an asset allocation. In figure 7, we
show our view of the risk reward structure of WM’s
methods of Showing, Making, Tasking. Showing is the
most reliable use of time, but the least generative.
of California,
San Diego, USA
But it is also the most generative in providing novel
Science, University
Acknowledgements
They can ensure that even their most novel, risky pieces
meet a certain acceptable level of recognizable quality, and
even their most safe pieces meet a certain acceptable level
Making is still a highly reliable use of time but leads to a
larger set of novel moves. Tasking is the least reliable,
in the sense that some tasks lead to no usable output.
Dept of Cognitive
Our parting conclusion: The domain of choreography
is a rich arena for research on the nature of distributed
creative cognition, on multi-modal instruction, and
phenomena of group attention, mental imagery and
interactivity. We have just begun our inquiry into these
areas and hope that others, too, will see the value
of close observational and empirical study of artistic
creativity.
Travail Artistique Art and Form: Ethnographies of Artistic
Work.. Ethnologie Française, 1( 37), 5-13.
New York: Routledge.
5. Franzosi, R. (2004). From words to numbers: Narrative, data
and social science. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
6. Gallagher, Sh. (2005). How the body shapes the mind.
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
7. Goodwin, Ch. (1994). Professional vision. American
Anthropologist, 96(3), 606-633.
8. Williams, R. (2006). Using ethnography to study instruction.
In Proceedings of the 7th International Conference of the
Learning Sciences. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Design and semantics of form and movement
View publication stats
195