Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.
To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to upgrade your browser.
…
5 pages
1 file
AI-generated Abstract
This article discusses the application of memory research in higher education settings, arguing for the benefits of utilizing undergraduate students as research participants. It emphasizes how understanding young adults' memory processes can improve educational practices and benefit campuses. The author explores various factors influencing memory, including social dynamics within student groups and the implications of specific contexts, such as the experiences of veterans, suggesting avenues for future research that could enhance both psychological literature and practical applications within academic institutions.
Applied Cognitive Psychology, 2004
Research examining changes in memory and memory awareness during learning suggests that early in the process, students primarily have representations that are episodic in nature and experience 'remember' awareness during recall. However, as learning continues and schematization occurs, students' knowledge is more likely to be dominated by semantic memory representations and 'just know' awareness is experienced during recall. The greater the amount of remembering experienced early in learning, the more likely it is that the shift to knowing will occur in students. In this study, university students studied either material rich in distinctive features that may serve as cues to episodic memory, or material lacking in these features. Students' knowledge was tested after a 2-day and a 5-wk interval. In contrast to students who studied the material lacking distinctive features, students who studied the distinctively rich material showed a predominance of remember awareness on the first test, and on the follow-up test showed a predominance of know awareness and were able to recall more details of the learning material.
Learning refers to the act of integrating information into one's memory systems in its preferred and culturally ingrained ways. The scholarly investigation of such integration necessitates the study of the human memory systems as well as how information is encoded, stored and retrieved by those systems. In that respect, this talk introduces the human memory systems in their neurological and psychological contexts and several techniques such as spaced repetition and interleaved practice which have been established in the neuroscience literature regarding information storage and retention. Based on the literature on those systems, teaching implications are drawn in the conclusions section.
1974
Abstract 1. Asked 30 graduate students to produce a type of semantic information-they named psychologists who satisfied certain restrictions. Not only was the speed of naming a psychologist influenced by the order in which restrictions were given, but the effect of order differed for advanced and beginning students. Advanced-student retrieval resembled the pattern observed for well-learned semantic material, while beginning-student retrieval did not.
BMC Psychology
Background Mainstream psychology is experiencing a crisis of confidence. Many of the methodological solutions offered in response have focused largely on statistical alternatives to null hypothesis statistical testing, ignoring nonstatistical remedies that are readily available within psychology; namely, use of small-N designs. In fact, many classic memory studies that have passed the test of replicability used them. That methodological legacy warranted a retrospective look at nonexperimental data to explore the generality of the reported effects. Method Various classroom demonstrations were conducted over multiple semesters in introductory psychology courses with typical, mostly freshman students from a predominantly white private Catholic university in the US Midwest based on classic memory experiments on immediate memory span, chunking, and depth of processing. Results Students tended to remember 7 ± 2 digits, remembered more digits of π following an attached meaningful story, an...
Journal of experimental psychology. General, 1997
First-year psychology students took multiple-choice examinations following each of 4 lecture courses and 3 laboratory research methods courses. One lecture course was later retested. Students indicated state of memory awareness accompanying each answer: recollective experience (remember), "just know" (know), feeling of familiarity (familiarity), or guess. On the lecture courses, higher performing students differed from other students because they had more remember responses. On research methods, higher performing students differed because they knew more, and in the delayed retest, higher performing students differed because they now knew rather than remembered more. These findings demonstrate a shift from remembering to knowing, dependent upon level attained, type of course, and retention interval, and suggest an underlying shift in knowledge representation from episodic to semantic memory. The authors discuss theoretical and educational implications of the findings.
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 1982
Previous attempts to measure the effects of instruction on students' cognitive structure have produced inconsistent and ambiguous findings. One reason may be that researchers have not distinguished between well-learned, abstracted information and memorized facts or formulas. Using the distinction between episodic and semantic memory as an heuristic, a procedure was designed to increase the likelihood that student performance on structure tasks was indicative of semantic rather than episodic memory. Significant correlations between measures of cognitive structure and performance on achievement test items were observed. In addition, partial correlations revealed that the measures of cognitive structure are correlated with the achievement items tapping higher-order cognitive processes and not with items tapping lower-order processes. These results are interpreted as supporting the argument that related research needs to be guided by theory-based definitions of cognitive structure.
2016
INTRODUCTION TO THE SPECIAL ISSUE Who we are and how we interact with the world around us hinges on long-term memory, the ability to remember past events and experiences. The ability to remember the past improves rapidly in the first years of life (e.g., Schneider & Pressley, 1997). As we grow up, we increasingly remember more details about when or where something happened, and become more sophisticated at using different strategies to facilitate learning and remembering (e.g., Schneider & Pressley, 1997). Many memories are created in school: we can often clearly picture encounters with our teachers and classmates or school field trips that we went to. At the same time, memory is critical for success in school, supporting language comprehension (e.g.,
This article presents an evaluation of research strategy in the psychology of memory. To the extent that a strategy can be discerned, it appears less than optimal in several respects. It relates only weakly to subjective experience, it does not clearly differentiate between structure and strategy, and it is oriented more toward remembering which words were in a list than to the diverse functions that memory serves. This last limitation fosters assumptions about memory that are false: that encoding and retrieval are distinct modes of operation; that the effects of repetition, duration, and recency are interchangeable; and that memory is ahistorical. Theories that parsimoniously explain data from single tasks will never generalize to memory as a whole because their core assumptions are too limited. Instead, memory theory should be based on a broad variety of evidence. Using findings from several memory tasks and observations of everyday memory, I suggest some ways in which involuntary reminding plays a central role in cognition. The evolutionary purpose of memory may have been the construction and maintenance—through reminding—of a spatio-temporal model of the environment. I conclude by recommending ways in which efficiency of the field's research strategy might be improved. Everyone has heard the East Indian fable of the blind philosophers and the elephant. The philosophers' descriptions of the animal are drastically different, because each is feeling a different part of its anatomy. Knowing what they do not know, we find their disagreement mildly amusing, as well as instructive. But now consider a revised version of the story, in which all the blind philosophers are feeling the elephant's tail. There would be good agreement on what the elephant is like, and a correspondingly high degree of confidence, but the mutually accepted description would be seriously wide of the mark. Sometimes it seems that students of human memory have gotten themselves into a similar fix. In what follows, I describe several aspects of the field's dilemma and discuss how we might work our way out of it.
Μαρία Λεοντσίνη, Τα Κύθηρα και αἱ νῆσοι ἑπτὰ Πελοποννήσιοι λεγόμεναι στο «Περί θεμάτων» του Κωνσταντίνου Ζ΄ Πορφυρογέννητου (913‐959), Ιστορίας μέριμνα, Τιμητικός τόμος στον καθηγητή Γεώργιο Ν. Λεοντσίνη, Αθήνα, Εθνικό και Καποδιστριακό Πανεπιστήμιο Αθηνών, τ. Α1, 697–711., 2011
Scripta Theologica
Jeu Revue De Theâtre, 1989
Indo Nordic Authors’ Collective, 2023
in "Kermes 100. Attualità, prospettive e sfide per il futuro", 2017
Acento y longitud vocalica en el tepehuano del sureste , 2015
15TH INTERNATIONAL CONFERENCE ON EDUCATION AND NEW LEARNING TECHNOLOGIES PALMA (SPAIN) 3RD-5TH OF JULY, 2023 ISBN: 978-84-09-52151-7 ISSN: 2340-1117, 2023
Res Publica, 2024
Journal of Experimental Zoology Part B: Molecular and Developmental Evolution, 2016
Oncotarget, 2017
Brazilian Archives of Biology and Technology