International
Journal of Human Sciences
ISSN:2458-9489
Volume: 18 Issue: 4 Year: 2021
The wandering mind and performance routine in golf
Paul Christianson1
Brent Hill2
Bradford Strand3
Joe Deutsch4
Abstract
The past decade of research has brought about new understandings in the study of pre-shot
routines, with multiple researchers advancing the field of knowledge surrounding the usage of preshot routines as a performance enhancement mechanism. Across golfers of novice to expert skilllevels, the results of peer-reviewed studies have clearly presented the potential benefits of
incorporating pre-shot routines for all golfers in improving their play. However, with the current
state of research serving as an indicator as to how far we have come in our learning of pre-shot
routines in golf, researchers and practitioners in the field understand that there is still a long way
to go in expanding our knowledge base on pre-shot routines and their role in the golf performance
spectrum. The paper reviews the concept of the wandering mind, attentional control theory,
performance routines in general, and more specifically, pre-shot routines in golf.
Keywords: Golf; preshot routine; golf performance.
Introduction
As an area of research, pre-shot routines in golf have been studied for the past 40 years by
academics and practitioners. The works of research pioneers have made profound impacts on
advancing the knowledge of pre-shot routines and the role they play in improving task-relevant
thought in golfers. As an industry, researchers and practitioners have learned a significant amount
since the first works of Kirschenbaum and Bale (1980), Rotella and Bunker (1981), and Yancey (1977)
regarding the importance of pre-shot routines and the role they play in improved golf performance.
Since this time, observations have been conducted on the pre-shot routines of elite golfers in
competition (Crews & Boutcher, 1986a) and assessments have been made as to their applicability on
behaviors in beginning golf performance (Crews & Boucher, 1986b). Researchers have introduced
cognitive behavioral interventions and assessed their potential benefits on golf performance (Cohn,
Rotella, & Lloyd, 1990) and groundbreaking research has been conducted on attentional control
methods related to golf performance (Boutcher, 1990). A great amount has also been discovered
1
Assistant Prof., Barton College, Sports Management,
[email protected]
Associate Prof., North Dakota State University, Educational and Organizational Leadership,
[email protected]
3 Prof., North Dakota State University, Physical Education Teacher & Coaching Education, Health, Nutrition, and
Exercise Sciences,
[email protected]
4 Prof., North Dakota State University, Physical Education Teacher & Coaching Education, Health, Nutrition, and
Exercise Sciences,
[email protected]
Orcid ID: 0000-0003-3094-6641
2
Submitted: 19/05/2021 Published: 07/11/2021
537
Christianson, P., Hill, B., Strand, B., & Deutsch, J. (2021). The wandering mind and performance routine in golf. Journal
of Human Sciences, 18(4), 536-549. doi: 10.14687/jhs.v18i4.6189
about the focus of attention in golfers and the task-salient cues they utilize during pre-shot routines
(Boutcher
&
Crews,
1987).
The last 30 years of research have brought upon new systems of achieving peak performance in golf
(Cohn, 1991) and provided a better understanding of the attentional focus patterns that exist amongst
golfers of a high level of skill (Crews & Landers, 1993). Pre-shot routine frameworks have been
developed for researchers and practitioners to build upon (Moore & Stevenson, 1994) and pre-shot
routines have even been integrated into a total performance management model for golf performance
(Murphy, 1994). As an area of study, researchers and practitioners have been able to better understand
the importance of consistency in pre-shot routines (Kingston & Hardy, 2001) and conducted studies
confirming the previous findings of researchers who blazed a trail of understanding (Beauchamp,
Halliwell,
Fournier,
&
Koestner,
1996).
With the emergence and adoption of information technologies like the internet over the last 20 years,
a flurry of research has been conducted with the new ease and speed of gathering information. During
this emerging era, advances in a golfer’s focus of attention have been discovered (Wulf, Lauterbach,
& Toole, 1999) and quickly been utilized by researchers in the field (McCann, Lavallee, & Lavallee,
2001). These rapidly adopted advances in a golfer’s focus of attention have produced results of
mitigating levels of anxiety and arousal (Tenenbaum, 2003) and provided confirmatory evidence that
an external focus of attention is superior to an internal focus of attention during the pre-shot routines
of both novice and elite golf performers (Wulf & Su, 2007). These advances have asserted that a preshot routine is a prerequisite for superior golf performance (Jackson, 2001) and allowed researchers
to propose that golfers need to remain committed to performing a pre-shot routine on every shot in
order to reach elite levels of golf performance (Douglas & Fox, 2002).
The past decade of research has brought about new understandings in the study of pre-shot routines,
with multiple researchers advancing the field of knowledge surrounding the usage of pre-shot
routines as a performance enhancement mechanism (Cotterill, 2008; Cotterill, Sanders & Collins,
2010a; Van der Lei, 2010). Across golfers of novice to expert skill-levels, the results of peer-reviewed
studies have clearly presented the potential benefits of incorporating pre-shot routines for all golfers
in improving their play (Bernier, Codron, Thienot, & Fournier, 2011; Thomas, 2010). However, with
all of the advances that have been made up until this point, golfers are still experiencing task irrelevant
thoughts that can be detrimental to golf performance.
The Wandering Mind
It has been suggested that the human mind spends a great deal of time in a state of stimulusindependent thought—otherwise known as mind wandering—as the default mode of operation
(Killingsworth & Gilbert, 2010; Mason et al., 2007). Killingsworth and Gilbert (2010) found that
46.9% of people reported being aware of their minds wandering throughout the course of a day. This
type of mind wandering occurs throughout a human’s life based upon the tasks at hand and brain
function of each human being. In modern society, mind wandering has obvious problematic
implications regarding such things as a student’s ability to effectively learn, an employee’s
productivity, and a golfer’s ability to focus their attention on the task at hand to accomplish their
goals.
It has been reported that a typical weekday 18-hole round of golf has remained consistent at
four hours year over year, with the average weekend round increasing to four and a half hours (Last,
2014). Some competitive rounds can run even longer. With golfers found to spend roughly 14 to 20
seconds of time actually hitting a golf shot while averaging 81 shots per round, it’s possible that many
golfers only spend roughly 19 to 27 minutes of time focused on their actual golf shots during the
course of a round of golf (Zienius, Skarbalius, Zuoza, & Pukenas, 2015). This leaves a significant
amount of time for a golfer’s mind to wander over the course of a golf round.
538
Christianson, P., Hill, B., Strand, B., & Deutsch, J. (2021). The wandering mind and performance routine in golf. Journal
of Human Sciences, 18(4), 536-549. doi: 10.14687/jhs.v18i4.6189
The Attentional Control Theory (ACT) suggests that mind wandering can lead to impaired
attentional control and task performance (Coombes et al., 2009; Eysenck et al, 2007). In addition,
ACT further contends that impaired attentional control on the task at hand can lead to deficits in
task performance and feelings of anxiety (Eysenck et al., 2007). To further understand how
educational practitioners may be able to limit the detrimental impacts of task-irrelevant thought, it is
important to first understand that under ACT human attention is believed to be regulated by both a
stimulus-driven attentional system and a goal-directed attentional system (Corbetta, Shulman, &
Corbetta, 2002). Coombes et al. (2009) suggested that the stimulus-driven attentional system is
sensitive to salient stimuli (such as when a teacher calls a student’s name in class) and the goal-directed
attentional system is responsive to a focused attention on accomplishing a specific goal (like trying to
hit a golf shot over water). Anxiety inducing situations that are stimulated by task-irrelevant thought
have often been found to prevent athletes in particular from focusing on the task at hand and may
result in a phenomenon known as choking (Bernier et al., 2011).
Choking is a suboptimal performance outcome resulting from situations in which an
individual is experiencing anxiety and has been found to happen in competitive situations that are
directly related to a considerable desire to perform well (Hardy, Mullen, & Jones, 1996). Situations of
choking can happen both in the classroom and on the golf course. Students failing large examinations
due to test anxiety and golfers losing competitions as a result of thinking about prize money during
competition are just two examples of where task-irrelevant thought can cause instances of choking.
Emerging studies have found that anxiety demands a large allocation of cognitive resources and that
pressure situations inducing anxiety prevent athletes from focusing on the task at hand (Bernier et
al., 2011). As a result of this potential for choking, athletic researchers and practitioners have begun
to integrate attentional focus cues (working on the stimulus-driven attentional system) into
performance routines (working on the goal-directed attentional system) as a means of bringing
athletes from a state of mind wandering into a state of sustained focus on the task at hand (Connor,
2010).
The primary rationale for the adoption of performance routines in the field of athletic
education is to ensure that athletes have the ability to attain consistency in their performance and
ensure that positive actions are being implemented to improve sporting performance (Taylor &
Wilson, 2005). In a review of the performance routine literature, Cotterill (2010) found that past
performance routine studies have been conducted in the sports of basketball, bowling, dance, diving,
football, golf, gymnastics, rugby union, skiing, skating, tennis, volleyball, track and field athletics,
water polo and wrestling. Multiple types of performance routines have also been shown to be
effective in improving the performance of skilled athletes across a variety of sports (Boutcher &
Crews, 1987; Cohn et al., 1990; Crews & Boutcher, 1986a; Crews & Boutcher, 1986b; Lobmeyer &
Wasserman, 1986; Lonsdale & Tam, 2008; Mack, 2001; Price, Gill, Etnier, & Kornatz, 2009). In
addition, some evidence even indicates that routines may benefit athletes of not only high-skill levels,
but low-skill levels as well (Beauchamp et al., 1996; Boutcher & Rotella, 1987; Cohn et al., 1990). As
we dig deeper into the current research that has been conducted in relation to performance routines
in athletic education, it’s important to first gain a better understanding of what performance routines
are and how they can be utilized as an ACT informed methodology by practitioners in the field.
An Overview of Performance Routines
Routines are behaviors and thoughts that have automatically been ingrained in our daily lives
and utilize a combination of physical, technical, and psychological strategies to enhance performance
(Hackfort, Duda, & Lidor, 2005; Thomas, 2010). Performance routines (as they are commonly described
in literature when related to performance outcomes) are meant to ensure that the positive influences
of performance are supported and negative influences of performance are minimized (Thomas,
2010). Cotterill (2010) found that there are two common terms that are referred to in research when
looking at performance routines. The previously mentioned term of performance routines has been
539
Christianson, P., Hill, B., Strand, B., & Deutsch, J. (2021). The wandering mind and performance routine in golf. Journal
of Human Sciences, 18(4), 536-549. doi: 10.14687/jhs.v18i4.6189
referred to in a multitude of studies (Cotterill, 2008; Holder, 2003; Jackson, 2001, 2003; Lonsdale &
Tam, 2008; Marlow, Bull, Heath & Shambrook, 1998; McCann et al., 2001; Moran, 1996, 2004;
Singer, 2002). Also commonly found in the golf literature has been the term pre-shot routines (Cohn et
al., 1990; Crews & Boutcher, 1986b; Douglas & Fox, 2002; Gayton, Cielinski, Francis-Keniston, &
Hearns, 1989; Harle & Vickers, 2001; Mack, 2001; Moore & Stevenson, 1994; Shaw, 2002).
In a further dissection of performance routines, Thomas (2010) suggested that performance
routines can be broken down even further and conceptualized in three ways: pre-performance,
between-performance, and post-performance routines. Pre-performance routines are described as
enabling athletes to prepare for competition by consistently integrating the same strategies and by
minimizing distractions. This would be akin to a golfer starting their practice routine on the driving
range the same way every morning before the start of the actual competition. Between-performance
routines are described as routines that can be utilized in sports with a series of period breaks (like
hockey and volleyball) that enable athletes to remain consistent throughout their performance. One
example of a between-performance routine would be a basketball player warming up prior to the
start of the second half the same way that they warmed up prior to the start of the first half. Lastly,
post-competition routines are described as those that allow for time to reflect on one’s performance,
evaluate performance, consider lessons learned, and plan for future training (Taylor & Wilson, 2005).
An example of this type of routine would be a football quarterback watching film every Monday
morning at 8:00am during the football season to enhance his on-field performance.
When describing performance routines and their usage in golf, pre-shot routines have been
found to be the most frequently adopted intervention in golf literature attributed to mind wandering
and are based on the assumption that a key aspect of successful task performance is the ability to
attend to the task-relevant cues, processes, and behaviors during the actual process of hitting a golf
shot (Wilson, 2008). In activities such as golf, the automatic nature of the skill creates ample
opportunities for attention to focus on other areas such as negative thoughts and irrelevant stimuli
(Niedeffer, 1976). The implementation of ACT informed methodology suggests that the integration
of a pre-shot routine can assist athletes in filtering out distractions and maintaining their focus
(Boutcher, 1990; Boutcher & Crews, 1987). Taylor and Wilson (2005) have also suggested that the
importance of pre-shot routines is to assist athletes in the preparation of their next shot and help
them refocus with Schmidt (1982) finding that a pre-shot routine may provide a way of reactivating
the appropriate physiological and mental state before each shot.
In order to gain a better understanding of pre-shot routines and their usage in the field of
golf education, it’s important to first gain a better understanding of how past research has informed
our current understanding of pre-shot routines in golf. Utilizing a chronological review of the
literature, one is able to further understand how past research over certain time periods may have
informed future research and knowledge construction. In adding to previous research in the field, a
chronological review may also better allow future researchers to identify research trends that were
taking place during different eras based upon the knowledge and beliefs at that time.
Pre-Shot Routines in Golf: A Chronological Review
Pre-shot routines have been described in golf as a sequence of task-relevant thoughts and
actions that an athlete engages in systematically prior to his or her performance in a sport (Moran,
1996). Prior to the groundbreaking studies of pre-shot routines on golf performance by Crews and
Boucher (1986a) and Boucher and Crews (1987), little empirical research had been undertaken on
the integration of pre-shot routines on golf performance. Kirschenbaum and Bale (1980) had
previously conducted research on the positive performance benefits of cognitive behavioral skills
training in golf and trailblazers such as Yancey (1977) and Rotella and Bunker (1981) had published
magazine articles related to pre-shot routines in golf, but no additional empirical research can be
identified prior to that point.
540
Christianson, P., Hill, B., Strand, B., & Deutsch, J. (2021). The wandering mind and performance routine in golf. Journal
of Human Sciences, 18(4), 536-549. doi: 10.14687/jhs.v18i4.6189
In one of the first empirical research studies on pre-shot routines in golf, Crews and Boucher
(1986a) used trained observers to record pre-shot routines in female golfers in tournament play. This
study revealed that the pre-shot routines of players at this level were remarkably consistent during
the course of a competitive round of golf and that the consistency of the pre-shot routines could
suggest that competitive golfers playing at a higher level spend less time mind wandering and more
time focused on the task at hand. From that research, Boutcher and Crews (1987) went on to add
that the task facing golfers is to efficiently focus attention on the most task-relevant cues while
simultaneously screening out and ignoring irrelevant information during pre-shot routines in golf. In
this study, the subjects participated in a six-week putting protocol that found attentional routines
effectively control mental and physiological states associated with a closed-skill performance. The
findings within both Crews and Boucher (1986a) and Boutcher and Crews (1987) studies set the
foundation of the empirical understanding of how pre-shot routines could be an effective method of
curbing the negative impacts of mind wandering on golf performance through the utilizing of preshot routines.
Cohn et al. (1990) followed up the work of Crews and Boutcher (1986a) and Boutcher and
Crews (1987) by introducing a cognitive behavioral intervention via the adherence of pre-shot
routines in collegiate golfers. Although immediate improvements in performance did not occur as a
result of the intervention, it was found via post intervention interviews that the golfers felt the
implementation of a pre-shot routine had a positive effect upon performance. This finding may have
suggested that a properly adhered to pre-shot routine may have a statistically significant impact on
the performance of golfers if adopted over a longer period of time and provided direction for future
research.
During this time frame, Crampton (1989) also conducted a study attempting to establish preshot routines in golf and suggested that pre-shot routines are essentially an ordered collection of
thoughts and behaviors. The study of Crampton (1989) held significance in that it allowed researchers
to better begin to operationalize the impactful thoughts and behaviors that provide the foundation
for effective pre-shot routines in golf. In a further investigation during this time frame, Boutcher
(1990) also researched the role of performance routines in sport and introduced the potential benefits
of ACT informed methods on golf performance by contending that the value of performance
routines was to aid athletes in maintaining focus and eliminating distractions. Furthermore, Boutcher
(1990) introduced the importance of cue words (falling under ACT’s stimulus-driven attentional
system) and breathing as part of performance routines and suggested that they could have a stabilizing
effect on performance.
In a study introducing the experience of flow states, Csikszentmihalyi (1990) addressed the
importance of gaining control over one’s mental processes in an effort to obtain optimal experience
or a “zone-like” state of mind. In his groundbreaking study, Csikszentmihalyi (1990) found that one
of the most frequently mentioned dimensions of flow experience (and potentially expert
performance) was having the ability to focus one’s concentration on the task at hand and eliminate
the wandering mind. The importance of this study introduced the prospect that golfers may be able
to attain a state of flow (or total concentration on the task at hand) via the adoption of effective preshot routines that allow golfers to achieve this level of performance on a consistent basis. States of
“flow” can adequately be described as a state of optimal concentration and performance in which
pre-shot routines seek to achieve on a consistent basis. Golfers may experience a state of flow during
a specific golf shot; however, the overall objective is to attain this state of flow during the course of
an entire round and fall into a “zone-like” state of total concentration on the task at hand.
As researchers embarked on the early nineties, Cohn (1991) conducted an exploratory study
on the aspects of peak performance in golf and found that certain psychological qualities of peak
performance in elite golfers exist. Directly related to characteristics still included in common pre-shot
routines today, Cohn (1991) found that golfers were a) highly focused and immersed in the task at
hand, b) performed the tasks effortlessly and automatically, c) felt physically relaxed and mentally
calm, and d) felt in control of themselves during peak performance. Additionally, the golfers had no
541
Christianson, P., Hill, B., Strand, B., & Deutsch, J. (2021). The wandering mind and performance routine in golf. Journal
of Human Sciences, 18(4), 536-549. doi: 10.14687/jhs.v18i4.6189
fear of negative consequences, maintained high self-confidence, and experienced fun and enjoyment.
These findings helped researchers to understand the important aspects that could be integrated into
the development and activation of effective pre-shot routines in golf.
In an attempt to try and activate a state of flow, Murphy (1994) followed up with a
‘performance management model’ where a four-point model of 1) practice, 2) preparation, 3)
performance, and 4) analysis was advocated. Murphy (1994) suggested this approach could lead to
peak performance given that athletes have control over the internal environment in which the game
is played, but not the external environment that can cause athletes to fall out of their flow state. An
internal environment suggested that a more top-down approach (which would fall in-line with ACT’s
goal-directed attentional system) allows athletes to better control their thoughts based upon the goals
that are set forth in the task at hand. In addition, the impact of the external environment (which
would fall further in line with ACT’s stimulus-driven attentional system) was also noted to have a
potentially damaging impact on a golfer’s performance. In that same year, Kingston and Hardy (1994)
also found that the consistency of routines can be compromised in situations when the golfer over
internalizes thoughts and feelings that can be brought on by salient cues. This type of breakdown
would also be found to be working on a golfer’s stimulus-driven attentional system.
As researchers began to build upon the work of previous studies, Beauchamp et al. (1996)
later adapted the cognitive behavioral program utilized by Boutcher and Rotella (1987) and compared
it to a physical skills training and control group. The results of the study supported the conclusion
drawn by Whelan, Myers, Berman, Bryant, & Mellon (1988) that cognitive behavioral approaches
(such as pre-shot routines in golf) are effective for performance enhancement. The results of these
compiled studies further fall in-line with the suggestions of ACT in that golfers may better be able to
control their behaviors on a golf course by having a goal-directed control over their cognitive
thoughts. This also may support the constructs presented by cognitive behavioral theorists in that
cognitive thoughts can have a major impact on controlling actions and behaviors.
As the field of research entered the late nineties, researchers such as Wulf et al. (1999) found
that an external focus of attention had the ability to enhance golf shot accuracy in both beginners
and experts. To gather this information, the researchers studied the pitch shot performance of 22
subjects without experience in golf. One group was told to focus their efforts internally (focusing on
the arm swing) whereas the other group was instructed to focus externally on the club swing. It was
found that the external-focus condition was more effective for performance during both practice and
retention for novice performers. These findings were important in that they provided researchers
with a better idea of exactly where to direct a golfer’s attention during the pre-shot routine process.
Entering a new decade of study, Jackson (2001) began to study whether or not a pre-shot
routine is a prerequisite for superior performance. In a review of the experimental evidence at the
time linking routine consistency and performance, Jackson (2001) found that pre-shot routines did
have functional significance and concluded that golfers may benefit from their utilization. However,
Jackson (2001) did find that there was no experimental evidence to conclude that a golfer’s
performance will improve by simply increasing their cognitive behavioral consistency.
The findings of Jackson (2001) were followed later that year by McCann et al. (2001) as they
attempted to further examine the effects of pre-shot routines on the wedge shot performance of nongolfers and golfers of low skill. Utilizing intervention strategies in both physical skill and cognitive
behavioral routine programs, participant performance was measured on a pre- and post-intervention
test with groups of non-golfers and golfers of low skill being grouped together. The findings suggest
that non-golfers had the greatest level of performance improvements when a cognitive behavioral
routine was integrated into their practice. In addition, golfers of low skill also attained performance
improvements via the utilization of a cognitive behavioral routine, but the findings did not reach
levels of statistical significance. Although the findings were not statistically significant, the study by
McCann et al. (2001) was important in that it was one of the first of its kind in examining the proven
performance outcomes of pre-shot routines utilizing a cross sectional design.
542
Christianson, P., Hill, B., Strand, B., & Deutsch, J. (2021). The wandering mind and performance routine in golf. Journal
of Human Sciences, 18(4), 536-549. doi: 10.14687/jhs.v18i4.6189
Douglas and Fox (2002) followed up on the pre-shot routine studies that had been conducted
to date and further argued that in order to reach very peak levels of performance, professional golfers
must remain committed to performing a pre-shot routine on every shot, regardless of the situation.
These findings further supported the construct that golfers should seek to adopt a repetitive pre-shot
routine in order to build habits of mind that lead to optimal performance outcomes. Although preshot routines often vary slightly based upon the shot that is required, the premise is that a repetitive
routine should be built that incorporates important elements to achieve a desired performance
outcome.
In further support of the utilization of ACT informed methodology, Tenenbaum (2003)
proposed that during golf routines, task-relevant information is systematically processed by the golfer
utilizing different dimensions of attention and through a continuous interaction between working
memory and long-term memory. As such, high levels of anxiety and arousal could impede the
attentional aspects required to process task-relevant cues and thus compromise both the quantity and
quality of information that is being processed during a pre-shot routine.
As research began to build upon itself and in a follow up study to the work of Wulf et al.
(1999), Wulf and Su (2007) conducted a study assessing a golfer’s external focus of attention and how
it enhances golf shot accuracy in both beginners and experts. The replicated findings reiterated the
performance benefits of having an external (relative to internal) focus of attention and provided
converging evidence that adopting an external focus of attention enhances learning and promotes a
more automatic type of movement control. The overall premise is that golfers may achieve better
performance outcomes if they focus on external thoughts (such as the golf club head location in
space) as opposed to internal thoughts (like the rotation of a golfer’s elbow during the swing).
As the field of research entered a new decade, Thomas (2010) examined the efficacy of preshot routines on improving competitive youth golf putting and approach shot performance. The first
focus of the study was to determine the impact of learning mental skills components related to the
pre-shot routine in golf and the second focus of the study was to examine the impact of learning the
pre-shot routine on competitive performance from the subjects’ perspective. In an assessment of
three youth high school golfers, performance improvements were observed by the investigator
utilizing performance scorecards, mental skills scorecards, adherence logs, thought sampling, and
interviews. The results of the study found putting and approach shot performance occurred in two
of the subjects. In addition, subjects found the intervention to be helpful and agreed that their
performances (ball striking, overall performance, iron, wood, and sand shots) improved. Although
the design of the study presented challenges in attaining statistical significance in its results, the study
presented valuable investigative tools that future researchers may be able to utilize in longitudinal
studies with youth and low skill level golf performers.
In the same year, Van der Lei (2010) studied the attentional and affective states of golfers
during performance. The study implemented a multi-modal assessment approach where the
relationship between affective states of both performance process and performance outcome
measures were determined. The researchers studied three collegiate golfers and revealed
distinguishable and idiosyncratic individual affect-related performance zones associated with
physiological and introspective measures for each golfer. In addition, the patterns during the preshot routines were found to be highly consistent (Van der Lei, 2010). As found previously by Crews
and Boucher (1986a), the development of patterns in pre-shot routines may assist in the development
of consistent pre-shot routines that can have a positive and repeatable impact on golf performance.
One year later, Bernier et al. (2011) conducted a study working on the ACT informed
stimulus-driven attentional system and explored the attentional focus of expert golfers. Within the
study, eight male professional golfers were filmed in both training and competitive contexts. The
results of the study found that golfers adapt their attentional foci depending on the context, which
would further reiterate findings presented within ACT in relation to the potential negative
ramifications of salient cues on golf performance. In addition, Bernier et al. (2011) also found that
sequences of attentional foci were utilized when golfers prepared, executed, and evaluated their shots.
543
Christianson, P., Hill, B., Strand, B., & Deutsch, J. (2021). The wandering mind and performance routine in golf. Journal
of Human Sciences, 18(4), 536-549. doi: 10.14687/jhs.v18i4.6189
This finding further reiterates the importance of having repeatable patterns and behaviors in
developing a consistent pre-shot routine.
In that same year, Yancey, Czech, Joyner, Zwald, and Genter (2011) expanded the knowledge
base of pre-shot routines in golf and conducted an existential phenomenological investigation of the
experiences of professional golfers when conducting pre-shot routines. In the study, eight
professional golfers were interviewed to assess the “lived experience” of golfers during their pre-shot
routines. The three major themes that Yancey et al. (2011) identified were maintaining focus, the
physical aspects of the routine, and shot type. Sub themes were also identified for each major theme
that included focusing on the specific target at hand, internal imagery, and maintaining consistency.
Where golfers stood during the routine, the amount of practice swings, consistency of use, and
external visualization were also identified under important physical aspects of a routine along with
differing shot types ranging from full swing shots, chipping, and putting (Yancey et al., 2011). The
results of this study were important in that they allowed researchers to grasp a better understanding
of what was happening inside the minds of golfers as they are progressing through a pre-shot routine
process as opposed to just observing their physical behaviors.
In an attempt to “fill the gap” between the psychological skills applied in pre-tournament
planning and those applied in the shorter-term pre-shot routines, Davies et al. (2014) conducted a
study that examined the optimal use of the time between shots and holes. In this study, Davies et al.
(2014) were able to identify an area of macro (pre-tournament) planning where professional golfers
focus on things such as imagery, technical changes, refinements, and tactical plans. The researchers
also introduced the concept of a meso-shot cycle that involves planning, response and clearing
strategies such as what happens before the pre-shot routine (known as the pre-pre-shot preparation
phase), and post-shot routine. As previously mentioned, studies (such as Thomas, 2010) have
identified multiple types of performance routines that golfers may adhere to that are not directly
related to hitting the shot at hand such as pre-performance, between performance, and postperformance routines. Although the construct of these themes appears to have general support in
relation to the potential performance benefits of pre-shot routines, very little research has actually
been conducted to validate their impact on successful golf performance outcomes.
Zienius, Skarbalius, Zuoza, and Pukenas (2015) recently assessed the total time taken and
heart rate changes of youth golfers during pre-shot routines utilized on the golf course. The findings
revealed that pre-shot routines for tee shots were significantly longer than those of approach shots.
In addition, the findings also revealed that heart rates increased significantly from the start to end of
pre-shot routines for tee and fairway shots with the number of practice swings having the strongest
relationship. Although the Zienius et al. (2015) is one of the first of its kind in relation to the heart
rates experienced by golfers during the pre-shot routine process, one of the most important findings
was that the time differentiation that was observed across different shots. This further reiterates the
proposed importance of golfers having developed pre-shot routines for different shot types to ensure
their consistency.
Furthermore, and in relation to the findings related to consistency by Zienius et al. (2015),
Van Der Lei, Tenenbaum, and Land (2016) sought to examine changes in routine consistency with
respect to optimal and non-optimal arousal states. Studying three highly skilled golfers during
competitive rounds of golf, Van Der Lei et al. (2016) found idiosyncratic differences in routine
patterns both behaviorally and temporally and that the consistency of routines for each golfer varied
under different arousal-related performance zones. These findings further suggest the potential
importance of controlling for salient cues that can cause alterations in a golfer’s arousal and result in
an activation of a golfer’s stimulus-driven attentional system.
Most recently, the Bernier et al. (2011) study on attentional focus was followed up by
Diekfuss and Raisbeck (2017) where the researchers studied the attentional focus of collegiate golfers
during practice and competition. The results revealed two major themes pertaining to the focus of
attention adopted by collegiate golfers. Situational focus was the first theme that referred to the focus
used within a specific context. One example that was presented was the differences of internal focus
544
Christianson, P., Hill, B., Strand, B., & Deutsch, J. (2021). The wandering mind and performance routine in golf. Journal
of Human Sciences, 18(4), 536-549. doi: 10.14687/jhs.v18i4.6189
between golfers hitting drives and putts. In addition, reactivity focus was found as a second theme
that referred to the focus golfers adopt as a result of a psychological state such as performing poorly.
Within this theme, it is suggested that a golfer’s internal focus may change based upon their prior
performance and reactive feelings toward that performance.
Based upon the findings of this chronological literature review, it was found that very little
research has been published to date that focuses specifically on the development of effective preshot routines for golf. As the body of research has developed over the past several decades, it is
important that we understand how pre-shot routines have been developed and informed by past
research. As such, the remainder of this chronological review focuses on the historical development
of pre-shot routines and the similarities and disparities of what has been developed to date.
Development of Pre-Shot Routines in Golf
Crews and Boutcher (1986b) introduced a six step routine that included 1) two continuous
practice swings from behind the ball followed by visualizing an imaginary line from the target to the
ball 2) standing beside the ball, setting the clubface square, and glancing once at the target, 3) setting
the grip, 4) setting the feet, 5) glancing at the target once more, and 6) swinging. The results of the
study revealed that there was a significant improvement in golf performance when utilizing a preshot routine, but only in more skillful golfers and suggested that pre-shot routines may only positively
affect performance when golfers reach a particular skill level. During this time frame, Crews and
Boutcher (1986a) also presented a behavioral sequence that included 1) standing behind the ball, 2)
moving beside the ball, 3) setting the club behind the ball with one glance at the target, 4) setting the
feet, 5) a combination of three waggles with two glances towards the target, and 6) swing. Although
these are some of the first known studies, the information provided primarily spoke to outward
observations of the physical behaviors of golfers during their pre-shot routines and less to their
inward thoughts that may have been directing these behaviors.
Additional efforts were made by Singer (1988) to further understand what was happening
inside a golfer’s mind during the pre-shot routine. As a result of these efforts, Singer (1988) presented
a five-step approach that included steps of 1) readying, 2) imaging, 3) focusing attention, 4) executing,
and 5) evaluation. This study was later backed by Cotterill (2008) who proposed that the first four
steps could be utilized as a global template for routines. It was suggested that this global template
would allow the flexibility for the routine to be modified regarding behaviors and timing, but still
fulfill the requirements of the individual golfer (Cotterill, 2010). However, beyond its recognition in
the work of Cotterill (2010) which we will discuss in further depth in this review, there still remains
limited publications that have explored the application of this approach.
In an effort to help better understand the effects of a cognitive behavioral intervention on
pre-shot routines, Cohn et al. (1990) developed a representative example of one subject’s preliminary
mental and behavioral routines at the beginning of the study. The mental routine for shots followed
a pattern of 1) selecting a club, 2) picking an intermediate target, 3) drawing an imaginary line from
the ball to the target, and 4) trying not to think about swing mechanics. The physical routine for shots
followed a pattern of 1) getting behind the ball and picking a target, 2) taking a practice swing behind
the ball, 3) approaching the ball and putting feet parallel to the target, and 4) waggling the club two
times while taking two glances at the target. Separate routines were established for shots and putting.
The results revealed a cognitive behavioral intervention increased adherence to a pre-shot routine
and helped researchers to better understand that there is both a cognitive (mental) and behavioral
(physical) routine that golfers adhere to within pre-shot routines (Cohn et al., 1990).
During this time frame, Boutcher and Zinsser (1990) also looked at the pattern of preperformance behaviors and found that elite golfers used their predominant pre-putt pattern on 62%
of putts whereas beginners only used them on 35% of putts. The revelations of this study further
promoted the idea within our current study that mid- to elite-level golf performers may be more likely
to adhere to a consistent pre-shot routine. As previously described, an adherence to a consistent
545
Christianson, P., Hill, B., Strand, B., & Deutsch, J. (2021). The wandering mind and performance routine in golf. Journal
of Human Sciences, 18(4), 536-549. doi: 10.14687/jhs.v18i4.6189
pattern has been proposed to have a positive impact on pre-shot routine performance (Crews and
Boucher, 1986a; Van der Lei, 2010).
Further adding to the research, Moore and Stevenson (1994) developed a three-step pre-shot
routine framework for improved golf performance. The first step included the phrase “Check it out”
to ensure that golfers recognized the situation (lie, distance, obstacles), reviewed their game plan (club
choice and flight path), and selected an intermediate target. The second step utilized the phrase “Click
it in” that focused on a golfer’s swing rehearsal of gaining the correct feel, relocating the intermediate
target, addressing the ball, and getting ready to clear. The third and final step in this approach was to
“Let it go” where the golfer clears their breath and initiates the swing. The findings in Moore and
Stevenson (1994) also presented the development of a long-term training program that included an
education, training, and application phase. This comprehensive approach to golfer performance
improvement features the importance of having a pre-shot routine that is easily repeatable and
integrates important mental and physical aspects.
Mccann et al. (2001) later adapted the pre-shot routine presented by Crews & Boutcher
(1986b) and developed a pre-shot routine to assess golf wedge shot performance in low skill novice
golfers that included 13 self-developed steps. These steps were 1) address an imaginary ball next to
the ball to be hit, 2) visualize an imaginary line from the target to the club face, 3) waggle the club, 4)
visualize an imaginary line from the target to the club face, 5) take a deep breath, 6) perform the
swing recalling the word “smooth” on the backswing and the word “swing” on the downswing, 7)
visualize the ball flying from the club face with the correct trajectory and landing at the target, 8)
address ball to be hit, 9) visualize an imaginary line from the target to the club face, 10) waggle the
club, 11) visualize an imaginary line from the target to the club face, 12) take a deep breath, and 13)
perform the swing recalling the word “smooth” on the backswing and the word “swing” on the
downswing. It was found that non-golfers in both intervention groups improved performance
following an acquisition phase and maintained these levels of performance in the retention test.
Although performance improvements were found utilizing this pre-shot routine method, it is our
position that the inclusion of 13 steps may be too many to optimize the long-term recall and
repeatability of a pre-shot routine.
As we have found up until this point, and as a result of such little research being conducted
related to the development of pre-shot routines in golf; Cotterill (2010) set out to explore the nature
of pre-performance routines in golf and the relevant research that had been conducted up until that
point. During that time, it was found that current practices relating to the development of pre-shot
routines were underpinned by the implicit assumption that increasing the consistency of the routine
would lead to enhanced performance (Jackson, 2003). However, this assumption had not been
consistently substantiated and various rationales had been presented with the main function that a
pre-shot routine fulfills. Cotterill (2010) included findings on the performer’s ability to deal with
distractions (Boutcher & Crews, 1987; Gould & Udry, 1994; Maynard, 1998; Weinberg, 1988),
focusing attention (Boutcher, 1992; Harle & Vickers, 2001), acting as a trigger (Boutcher & Crews,
1987; Lonsdale & Tam, 2008; Moran, 1996), enhancing ability to focus and the recall of physiological
and psychological states (Foster, Weigand, & Baines, 2006; Marlow et al., 1998), and reducing the
unraveling of automaticity (Beilock & Carr, 2001; Beilock, Carr, MacMahon, & Starks, 2002).
Although Shaw (2002) completed a study assessing pre-shot routine development with a professional
golfer, the researcher did not focus on the process they utilized to develop the routine nor the golfer’s
perception of the function and composition.
As a result of the lack of research focusing on the development of pre-shot routines from a
subject’s perspective, Cotterill et al. (2010a) conducted a study exploring the subjects’ views on the
nature of the pre-shot routines that they have implemented into their game. The results of this study
found nine superordinate themes that were categorized as 1) allocation of attention, 2) psychological
skills, 3) shot selection, 4) routine mind set, 5) routine composition, 6) compulsive behaviors, 7)
routine evolution/application, 8) top players, and 9) moderating factors. During roughly the same
time as the Cotterill et al. (2010a) study, Thomas (2010) presented a pre-shot routine utilizing three
546
Christianson, P., Hill, B., Strand, B., & Deutsch, J. (2021). The wandering mind and performance routine in golf. Journal
of Human Sciences, 18(4), 536-549. doi: 10.14687/jhs.v18i4.6189
phases of learning: cognitive skill building, behavioral skill building, and relaxation. As with many
other previous studies that both developed and integrated a pre-shot routine in golf, the Thomas
(2010) study assessed the effects of a pre-shot routine on youth (novice) golf performance without a
widely adopted model to utilize in assessing the effects of pre-shot routine on golf performance.
In conducting a thorough review of the relevant literature that exists today, the findings
suggest that there appears to be no widely adopted model or methodology in which to adopt when
seeking to improve golf performance via the utilization of a pre-shot routine. In some of the most
recent findings to date, Cotterill, Collins, and Sanders (2014) followed up their previous research with
an attempt to develop a framework for developing effective pre-performance routines for golf. In
this study, the researchers found that conclusions drawn, and recommendations suggested by a
number of studies in golf (Boutcher & Crews, 1987; Cohn, Rotella, & Lloyd, 1990; Crampton, 1989;
Douglas & Fox, 2002) did not apply to the ‘real’ golf situations in which shot type and preparation
for different shot types appear to vary. As a result, Cotterill et al. (2014) suggested that the future
direction for work exploring and developing pre-shot routines should be to focus on the role they
fulfill and the key components and behaviors that need to be executed. Furthermore, the researchers
suggested the need to develop a template at the psychological level that underpins the routines in golf
and allows golfers to naturally develop their own pre-shot routines within these psychological
constructs.
Conclusion
Although researchers such as Strand and Craw (2019) have developed compelling templates,
no additional pre-shot routine models or methodologies have been located to date that have been
validated in the field to effectively improve golf performance and curtail a golfer’s wandering mind.
It is suggested that future research be conducted on the development of a pre-shot routine template
that researchers and practitioners can utilize. Additionally, it is suggested that any future pre-shot
routine template be validated by researchers and practitioners in the field in direct relation to its
overall usefulness and utility so that a standardized model can be adopted when conducting research
on the effects of pre-shot routines in golf in the future.
REFERENCES
Beauchamp, P. H., Halliwell, W. R., Fournier, J. F., & Koestner, R. (1996). Effects of cognitivebehavioral psychological skills training on the motivation, preparation, and putting
performance of novice golfers. Sport Psychologist, 10, 157-170.
Beilock, S. L., & Carr, T. H. (2001). On the fragility of skilled performance: What governs choking
under pressure? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 701–725.
Beilock, S. L., Carr, T. H., MacMahon, C., & Starkes, J. L. (2002). When paying attention becomes
counterproductive: impact of divided versus skill-focused attention on novice and
experienced performance of sensorimotor skills. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8,
6-16.
Bernier, M., Codron, R., Thienot, E., & Fournier, J. (2011). The attentional focus of expert golfers in
training and competition: A naturalistic investigation. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 23,
326-341.
Boutcher, S. H. (1990) The role of performance routines in sport. In J. G. Jones & Hardy (Eds.),
Stress and Performance in Sport (pp. 221-245). New York: Wiley.
Boutcher, S. H. (1992). Attentional and athletic performance: An integrated approach. In T. S. Horn
(Ed.), Advances in Sport Psychology (pp. 251–266). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Boutcher, S. H., & Crews, D. J. (1987). The effect of a preshot attentional routine on a well-learned
skill. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 18, 30-39.
547
Christianson, P., Hill, B., Strand, B., & Deutsch, J. (2021). The wandering mind and performance routine in golf. Journal
of Human Sciences, 18(4), 536-549. doi: 10.14687/jhs.v18i4.6189
Boutcher, S. H., & Rotella, R. J. (1987). A psychological skills educational program for closed-skill
performance enhancement. Sport Psychologist, 1, 127-137.
Boutcher, S. H., & Zinsser. N. W. (1990). Cardiac deceleration of elite and beginning golfers during
putting. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 12, 37-47.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York: Harper & Row.
Cohn, P. J., Rotella, R. J., & Lloyd, J. W. (1990). Effects of a cognitive-behavioral intervention on the
pre-shot routine and performance in golf. Sport Psychologist, 4, 33-47.
Cohn, P. J. (1991). An Exploratory Study on Peak Performance in Golf. Sport Psychologist, 5, 1-14.
Coombes, S., Higgins, T., Gamble, K., Cauraugh, J., & Janelle, C. (2009). Attentional control theory:
Anxiety, emotion, and motor planning. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23, 1072–1079.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.07.009
Connor, S. (2010, July 19) Psychology of sport: how a red dot swung it for open champion.
Independent.
Retrieved
from
http://www.independent.co.uk/sport/general/others/psychology-of-sport-how-a-red-dotswung-it-for-open-champion-2030349.html
Corbetta, M., Shulman, G., & Corbetta, M. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven
attention in the brain. Nature reviews. Neuroscience. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn755
Cotterill, S. (2008). Developing effective pre-performance routines in golf. Sport & Exercise Psychology
Review, 4(2), 10-16.
Cotterill, S. (2010). Pre-performance routines in sport: Current understanding and future directions.
International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 3, 132-153. 10.1080/1750984X.2010.488269.
Cotterill, S., Sanders, R., & Collins, D. (2010). Developing effective pre-performance routines in golf:
Why don't we ask the golfer? Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 22(1), 51-64.
Cotterill, Stewart & Collins, Dave & Sanders, Ross. (2014). Developing effective pre-performance
routines for golf: Implications for the coach. Athletic Insight, 6, 53-64.
Crampton, J. (1989). Establishing pre-shot routines for tournament golfers: An example of the use
of micro-computers in performance planning. Sports Coach, 12, 9-12.
Crews, D. J., & Boutcher, S. H. (1986a). An exploratory observational behavior analysis of
professional golfers during competition. Journal of Sport Behavior, 9, 51-58.
Crews, D. J., & Boutcher, S. H. (1986b). Effects of structured preshot behaviors on beginning golf
performance. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 62, 291-294.
Crews, D. J., & Landers, D. M. (1993). Electroencephalagraphic measures of attentional patterns
prior to the golf putt. Medicine in Sport and Exercise, 25, 116-126.
Davies, T., Collins, D., & Cruickshank, A. (2014). So what do we do with the rest of the day? Going
beyond the pre-shot routine in elite golf support. International Journal of Golf Science, 2, 163-175.
Diekfuss, J., & Raisbeck, L. (2017). Attentional focus in NCAA division 1 golfers. Journal of Motor
Learning and Development, 5, 240-251. 10.1123/jmld.2016-0025.
Douglas, K., & Fox, K. R. (2002). Performance and practise of elite women european tour golfers
during pressure and non-pressure putting simulation. In E. Thain (Ed.), Science and Golf IV
(pp. 246-256). London: Routledge.
Eysenck, M., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance:
attentional control theory. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 7, 336–353.
Foster, D. J., Weigand, D. A., & Baines, D. (2006). The effect of removing superstitious behavior
and introducing a pre-performance routine on basketball free throw performance. Journal of
Applied Sport Psychology, 18, 167–171.
Gayton, W. F., Cielinski, K. L., Francis-Keniston, W. J., & Heanes, J. F. (1989). Effects of preshot
routine on free-throw shooting. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 68, 317-318.
Gould, D., & Udry, E. (1994). Psychological skills for enhancing performance: arousal regulation
strategies. Medicine and Science in Sport and Exercise, 26, 478–485.
Hackfort, D., Duda, J., & Lidor, R. (2005). Handbook of research in applied sport and exercise psychology:
International perspective. Morgantown, WV: Sheridan Books.
548
Christianson, P., Hill, B., Strand, B., & Deutsch, J. (2021). The wandering mind and performance routine in golf. Journal
of Human Sciences, 18(4), 536-549. doi: 10.14687/jhs.v18i4.6189
Hardy, L., Mullen, R., & Jones, G. (1996). Knowledge and conscious control of motor actions under
stress. British Journal of Psychology, 87, 621–636.
Harle, S.K. & Vickers, J.N. (2001). Training quiet eye improves accuracy in basketball free throw. The
Sport Psychologist, 15, 289–305.
Holder, T. (2003). Concentration Training for Closed Skills. Pre-performance routine. In I.
Greenlees, and A. Moran (Eds.), Concentration Skills Training in Sport (pp.67-75). Leicester: The
British Psychological Society.
Jackson, R. C. (2001). The preshot routine: A prerequisite for successful performance? In P. R.
Thomas (Ed.), Optimising Performance in Golf (pp. 279-288). Brisbane, Australia: Australian
Academic Press.
Jackson, R. C. (2003). Pre-performance routine consistency: temporal analysis of goal kicking in the
Rugby Union World Cup. Journal of Sports Sciences, 21, 803–814.
Killingsworth, M., & Gilbert, D. (2010). A wandering mind is an unhappy mind. Science (New York,
N.Y.), 330(6006), 932.
Kingston, K., & Hardy, L. (1994). Factors affecting the salience of outcome, performance, and
process goals in golf. In A. J. Cochran & M. R. Farrally (Eds.), Science and Golf II: Proceedings of
the 1994 World Scientific Congress of Golf (pp. 144-149). London: E & FN Spon.
Kingston, K. M., & Hardy, L. (2001). Pre-performance routine training using holistic process goals.
In P. R. Thomas (Ed.) Optimizing Performance in Golf (pp.264-278). Brisbane: Australian
Academy Press.
Kirschenbaum, D.S., & Bale, R.M. (1980). Cognitive-behavioral skills in golf: Brain power golf. In
R.M. Suinn (Ed.), Psychology in Sports: Methods and Applications (pp. 334-343). Minneapolis, MN:
Burgess.
Last, J. (2014). Tracking research. Presentation at the 2014 Pace of Play Symposium, Far Hills, NJ.
https://www.usga.org/content/dam/usga/images/pace%20of%20play/trackingresearch.p
df
Lobmeyer, D. L., & Wasserman, E.A. (1986). Preliminaries to free throw shooting: Superstitious
behavior?. Journal of Sports Behavior, 9, 70–78.
Lonsdale, C., & Tam, J. (2008). On the temporal and behavioural consistency of pre-performance
routines: An intra-individual analysis of elite basketball players’ free throw shooting accuracy.
Journal of Sports Sciences, 26(3), 259–266.
Mack, M.G., (2001). Effects of time and movements of the preshot routine on free throw shooting.
Perceptual and Motor Skills, 93, 567–573.
Marlow, C., Bull, S., Heath, B., & Shambrook, C. (1998). The use of a single case design to investigate
the effect of a pre-performance routine on the water polo penalty shot. Journal of Science and
Medicine in Sport, 1, 143–155.
Mason, M., Norton, M., Van Horn, J., Wegner, D., Grafton, S., & Macrae, C. (2007). Wandering
minds: The default network and stimulus-independent thought. Science (New York, N.Y.),
315(5810), 393-395.
Maynard, I. W. (1998). Improving concentration. Leeds, UK: National Coaching Foundation.
McCann, P., Lavallee, D., & Lavallee, R. M. (2001). The effect of pre-shot routines on golf wedge
shot performance. European Journal of Sport Science, 1, 231-240.
Moore, W. E., & Stevenson, J. R. (1994). Training a pre-shot routine for golf. Applied Research in
Coaching and Athletics Annual, 161-167.
Moran, A. P. (1996). The psychology of concentration in sports performers: A cognitive analysis. Hove, UK:
Psychology Press.
Moran, A. (2004). Sport and exercise psychology: A critical introduction. Hove: Routledge.
Murphy, S. (1994). Mental preparation for golf: achieving optimal performance. In A. J. Cochrane
and M. R. Farrally (Eds.) Science and Golf II: Proceedings of the World Scientific Congress of Golf.
London: E and FN SPON.
549
Christianson, P., Hill, B., Strand, B., & Deutsch, J. (2021). The wandering mind and performance routine in golf. Journal
of Human Sciences, 18(4), 536-549. doi: 10.14687/jhs.v18i4.6189
Nideffer, R.M. (1976). The inner athlete: Mind plus muscle for winning. San Diego: Enhanced Performance
Associates.
Price, J., Gill, D., Etnier, J., & Kornatz, K. (2009). Free-throw shooting during dual-task performance.
Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 80, 718-726.
Rotella, R., & Bunker, L. (1981). Mind mastery for winning golf. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Pentice-Hall.
Schmidt, R. A (1982). Motor control and learning. Champaign, IL. Human Kinetics.
Shaw, D. (2002). Confidence and the pre-shot routine in golf: A case study. In Solutions in Sport
Psychology, Edited by: Cockerill, I. 108–119. London: Thomson.
Singer, R. N. (1988). Strategies and meta-strategies in learning and performing self-paced athletic
skills. Sport Psychologist, 2, 49-68.
Singer, R. N. (2002). Pre-performance state, routines, and automaticity: What does it take to realize
expertise in self-paced events? Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 24, 359-375.
doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00495
Strand, B., & Craw, M. (2019). PEER Golf: A Four-Part Model for Teaching and Improving Course
Management. Strategies, 32(6), 18–26.
Taylor, J., & Wilson, G. (2005). Applying sport psychology: Four perspectives. Champaign, IL. Human
Kinetics.
Tenenbaum, G. (2003). Expert athletes: An integrated approach to decision making. In J. L. Starkes
& K. A. Ericsson (Eds.), Expert Performance in Sports. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Thomas, J. A. (2010). Considering intervention efficacy: The effect of a pre-shot routine on competitive youth golf
performance. (Doctoral dissertation: University of North Carolina at Greensboro).
Van der Lei, H. (2010). Applied Golf Research: Affective States During Golf Performance. (Doctoral
dissertation, Florida State University).
Van Der Lei, H., Tenenbaum, G., & Land, W. (2016). Individual arousal-related performance zones
effect on temporal and behavioral patterns in golf routines. Psychology of Sport & Exercise, 26,
52–60.
Weinberg, R. S. (1988). The mental advantage. Champagne, IL: Leisure Press.
Whelan, J., Myers, A., Berman, J., Bryant, V., & Mellon, M. (1988, October). Meta-analysis of cognitivebehavioral interventions for performance enhancement in sports. Paper presented at the 4th annual
conference of the Association for the Advancement of Applied Sport Psychology, Nashua,
NH.
Wilson, Mark. (2008). From processing efficiency to attentional control: A mechanistic account of
the anxiety–performance relationship. International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 1, 184201.
Wulf, G., Lauterbach, B., & Toole, T. (1999). The learning advantages of an external focus of
attention in golf. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 71, 229-239.
Wulf, G., & Su, J. (2007). An external focus of attention enhances golf shot accuracy in beginners
and experts. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 78, 384-389.
Yancey, R. (1977, November). Develop a preshot routine and play better. Golf Digest, 115-117.
Yancey, A., Czech, D., Joyner, B., Zwald, D., and Genter, N. (2011). The experience of preshot
routines among professional golfers: An existential phenomenological investigation. Journal
of Excellence, 14, 48-68.
Zienius, M., Skarbalius, A., Zuoza, A., & Pukenas, K. (2015). Total time taken and heart rate
changes of youth golfers during pre-shot routines in on-course conditions. International Journal
of Performance Analysis in Sport, 15, 560-571.