Brill’s Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics 1 (2009) 283–310
brill.nl/baall
Hebrew Idioms: The Organization of the Lexical
Component
Julia Horvath and Tal Siloni
Tel Aviv University
[email protected] and
[email protected]
Abstract
The paper argues that the empirical domain of idioms can shed light on the architecture of the
mental lexicon and the nature of its building blocks whether roots or words. A corpus-based
study of the distribution of various diatheses in verb phrase idioms in Hebrew was conducted.
Its results reveal an intriguing discrepancy between the behavior of unaccusatives, transitives and
adjectival passives on the one hand and verbal passives one the other. The findings are straightforwardly accounted for if the lexicon includes actual verbs – words not merely roots – under
which verb phrase idioms are stored as sub-entries.
Keywords
phrasal idioms, idiom storage, mental lexicon, root, verbal passives, unaccusatives, adjectival
passives
1. Introduction
The root and vocalic template structure of Semitic words has long been a focus
of inquiry both in traditional and generative approaches to morphology and
the lexicon. This striking typological property of the structure and meaning of
(content) words has traditionally been taken as evidence for word-formation
rules being root based in Semitic languages and importantly, also for the claim
that entries listed in the mental lexicon are consonantal roots, rather than
words (see e.g. Gesenius 1910, Berman 1978, McCarthy 1979, 1981). In relation to Modern Hebrew, these assumptions have been particularly prevalent
with regard to the verbal system of the language.
Subsequent research of Modern Hebrew derivational morphology, and in
particular the derivation of new verbs, has uncovered evidence implying that
words, not just roots, are able to serve as input in at least a subset of cases of
verb formation. Specifically, evidence for the inadequacy of the traditional
concept of the Semitic root has come from phenomena such as systematic
© Koninklijke Brill NV, Leiden, 2009
DOI 10.1163/187666309X12491131130666
284
J. Horvath and Tal Siloni /
Brill’s Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics 1 (2009) 283–310
consonant cluster preservation, and the transfer of vowel quality from the base
category to derived verbs, as attested in the formation of denominal and deadjectival verbs, and other derivational processes (see e.g., Bolozky 1978,
Horvath 1981, Bat-El 1994, Ussishkin 1999).
Beyond the immediate implications regarding possible inputs to word formation, such morpho-phonological phenomena were also interpreted at the
time as a source of evidence with regard to the structure of the mental
lexicon of Hebrew. If words can serve as inputs to word formation, so the
argument went, then contrary to the traditional consensus on lexical representations in Semitic, words (in the sense of vocalic templates, interleaved
with the consonantal root) must be listed in the lexicon of Hebrew. Thus, the
items stored in the mental lexicon under this alternative view could be
verbs, in various diatheses, as well as nouns and adjectives. But drawing such
a conclusion from the observed morpho-phonological evidence crucially
depended on the additional assumption – inherent in models of morphology
throughout the seventies and eighties – that rules of word formation must all
apply within the lexicon, i.e., that morphology is a subcomponent of the
mental lexicon.
However, subsequent developments have motivated a significantly different
type of architecture of grammar. On this architecture at least some rules of
word formation may apply outside of the lexicon; namely, certain morphological processes take place within the syntactic derivation or must be fed
by output from the syntactic derivation. See for instance Baker’s (1988) theory of incorporation, Borer’s (1988) parallel morphology, Anderson’s (1992)
model of a-morphous morphology, and most recently, Halle and Marantz’s
(1993) Distributed Morphology. Given that in such models phonological
substance can get associated with terminal nodes after the application of syntactic operations (e.g. creation of complex heads by movement), the morphophonological correspondence say between denominal and deadjectival verbs
and the corresponding noun or adjective is no longer evidence with regard to
the kinds of forms listed in the mental lexicon. Within these alternative architectures, these morpho-phonological phenomena have to be accounted for,
but they do not bear on the nature of the entries stored in the lexicon. The
question then remains unanswered: Are there derived entries in the mental
lexicon?
The present study aims to assess the issue of root based versus word based
lexical storage in Modern Hebrew on novel empirical grounds, that are independent of matters of morpho-phonological realization, and bear directly on
the nature of the entries in the Hebrew lexicon. More precisely, the issue we
J. Horvath and Tal Siloni /
Brill’s Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics 1 (2009) 283–310
285
address is whether words are being stored in the lexicon, or it consists (exclusively) of roots.1
We remain agnostic in the paper as to the status of late insertion of the
phonological matrix of words. Although this option seems empirically wellmotivated in some cases, the issue has no direct bearing on the subject matter
of our present investigation. Accordingly, the term “word” (vs. “root”) we use
in the paper in discussing lexically listed items, is not meant necessarily to
include phonological content; rather it refers to an abstract item derived from
a root, i.e., one that has undergone (generative/derivational) processes, such as
valence changing (henceforth, arity) operations or operations determining
categorial status.
Major current developments in the architecture of grammar brought the
role of the root and the issue of lexical operations back into theoretical focus,
in a general, non-Semitic context. The past decade has seen recurring attempts
to eliminate the active (operative) role of the lexicon altogether, and proposals
to replace it by non-computational lists of items that are necessarily minimal
“building blocks”, namely roots (see Marantz 1997, Borer 2005, Ramchand
2006, Pylkkänen 2002, Alexiadou et al. 2004, and with respect to Semitic in
particular, Doron 2003).
Our study targets directly the question of what is listed in the mental lexicon based on Modern Hebrew, and uncovers novel (non-morphological) evidence that a lexicon comprised of roots only is in fact inadequate. Our results
strongly support the conclusion that the mental lexicon must contain words in
the above sense. It is worth noting already here that the evidence we present
ranges over the Hebrew verbal system, the precise empirical domain that has
served as the primary source of motivation for a traditional root-based conception of lexical representations. Therefore, our findings are expected to apply
a fortiori to other languages. Our investigation focuses on voice alternations.
“Voice” is used here in the broad sense of the term, referring to verbal diatheses, subsuming alternations such as the active-passive and transitiveunaccusative (causative-anticausative) alternations. The empirical material
investigated comprises the set of phrasal idioms in Hebrew, specifically, idioms
headed by a verb and including the verb’s internal domain, excluding the
external argument.
1
Thus by “word-based” lexicon we mean a lexicon that must include words. It can in addition
list roots, or code them indirectly, if this turns out to be well motivated on linguistic grounds or
given evidence regarding their psychological reality (see Prunet et. al. 2000), as pointed out by
an anonymous referee.
286
J. Horvath and Tal Siloni /
Brill’s Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics 1 (2009) 283–310
Our hypotheses and the corresponding quantitative, corpus-based study we
conducted have uncovered robust evidence that (a) a non-computational lexicon storing only roots is inadequate and consequently (b) attributing all derivation to the syntax could not be the right architecture. Rather, the grammar
ought to include an active lexicon within which computational mechanisms
can apply, and where the outputs of these operations get stored.
The paper is organized as follows. Section 2 motivates the use of idiom data
as the source of empirical evidence for studying lexical representations and sets
out the two relevant alternative hypotheses of idiom storage, the word based
and the root-based hypotheses. Section 3 introduces some prima facie puzzling discrepancies manifested in the distribution of verb phrase idioms among
a variety of verbal voice alternations and discusses the potential implications
with regard to alternative theories of lexical storage. Section 4 presents the
description and results of our corpus-based quantitative study assessing the
distribution of four voice alternates in Hebrew phrasal idioms: transitive,
unaccusative, verbal passive, and adjectival (stative) passive. Section 5 provides
a detailed discussion and account of the statistically significant results of these
corpus searches. It is demonstrated that they follow straightforwardly under
the word based theory of the mental lexicon. Finally, we discuss the implications of our empirical findings with regard to the availability of derivational
operations in the lexicon.
2. On the Relevance of Idioms
The most profound issue raised by all varieties of idioms within the framework
of generative grammar stems directly from the combination of their two core
characteristics. On the one hand, the choice of their fixed lexical material is
conventionalized and their meaning typically idiosyncratic, i.e., not predictable based on the (independently attested) meaning of their subparts. The
unpredictability of the form-meaning relation of idioms is thus reminiscent of
listed lexical items, such as roots/words. On the other hand, idioms exhibit
internal structure which in the overwhelming majority of cases reflects, i.e.,
is homomorphic with, fully compositional syntactic constructions available
independently in the syntax as a result of Merge. The fundamental challenge
idioms pose then is to develop a theory that can reconcile these two apparently
conflicting facets of idioms. Progress towards meeting the above challenge can
be expected to shed new light on competing conceptions of the architecture of
grammar, and in particular, on the internal organization of the store of basic
building blocks, namely the traditional mental lexicon or its alternatives.
J. Horvath and Tal Siloni /
Brill’s Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics 1 (2009) 283–310
287
The primary characteristic of being conventionalized forms associated with
special, to varying degrees, unpredictable meanings entails that idioms, or at
least the idiosyncratic information they contain, must be stored in mental
representations. This conclusion immediately raises the question of what the
particular place and manner of storage is.
As for where idiom information may be stored, in principle one can entertain two basic possibilities: an extra-grammatical and a grammatical approach.
The extra-grammatical approach would assume that knowledge of the form
and meaning of idioms is stored in the general part of speakers’ memory, along
with memorized non-linguistic knowledge such as facts of history or geography. This contrasts with the grammatical approach, according to which knowledge of idioms is part of linguistic knowledge, and idioms, or at least
specification of their special (idiomatic) meanings, are listed in the mental
lexicon. The choice between these two basic approaches is quite uncontroversial. First, the knowledge of form-meaning associations constituting idioms is
linguistic in nature, and as such clearly distinct from knowledge of facts of
history and other language-independent knowledge stored in the general
memory. Moreover, as argued prominently in recent work by Jackendoff
(1997) and Marantz (1997), there is no empirically motivated way to draw a
sharp distinction between the special meanings of words and the special meanings of bigger (multi-word) expressions such as idioms. Thus, we can reasonably discard the extra-grammatical approach to the storage of idioms.
In spite of the broad consensus that idioms ought to be stored by the language faculty, the actual locus and manner of storage of idiom information
within the grammar is controversial and far from well-understood. Idioms
may be stored as items on a list independently of their subparts. Or idioms
may be stored as subentries of one or more of their subparts.
The empirical domain of our investigation consists of Hebrew phrasal idioms headed (mainly) by a verbal predicate, henceforth, verb phrase idioms.
We present evidence that they are not listed as independent entries on a list of
their own. The question then arises what constitutes an idiom subpart relevant
for idiom storage. Two basic hypotheses come to mind depending on one’s
conception of the nature of the mental lexicon: (i) The relevant subpart is at
the word level (“word” understood in the sense of section 1) (Jackendoff 1997,
Everaert 1990, Williams 2007 and others) or (ii) The relevant subpart is a
root.
Theories underlying hypothesis (ii) represent a currently prevalent trend,
an architecture of grammar that reduces the generative lexicon to noncomputational list(s) of lexical items. These items are necessarily minimal
building blocks, namely, roots, as everything else is formed in the syntax
288
J. Horvath and Tal Siloni /
Brill’s Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics 1 (2009) 283–310
(Marantz 1997, Borer 2005, McGinnis 2002). Obviously, under this view, the
root must be the relevant subpart for idiom storage. Roots have to include
specifications regarding special meanings, as no other element can be listed.
For example, the Distributed Morphology framework (Halle and Marantz
1993) postulates a specific list labeled the Encyclopedia, which relates roots to
their meanings. The Encyclopedia would list the idiom kick the bucket, whose
meaning is roughly ‘die’, as a subentry of the entry for the root kick specifying
that the latter may be interpreted as ‘die’ in the environment of the direct
object the bucket.
In general, if hypothesis (i) turns out to be more adequate, we have solid
evidence that the mental lexicon must include information about actual words,
say nisgar (unaccusative ‘close’) and sagar (the transitive ‘close’) in Hebrew. If
hypothesis (ii) turns out to be on the right track, then the mental lexicon
could be root based; in Hebrew that would most probably mean consonantal
roots, which are characteristic of the morphological paradigm of Semitic languages, e.g., s.g.r for ‘close’.
Our investigation of Hebrew verb phrase idioms has turned out to offer a
novel source of evidence in favor of a word based lexicon. We conducted systematic searches of idiom corpora, dictionaries and on-line inventories in order
to examine the distribution of the various verbal voices in verb phrase idioms.
The results reveal (a) a significant level of independence in the distribution of
the various root-related voices, and (b) a nonarbitrary distribution that can be
straightforwardly accounted for if certain verbal diatheses are listed in the lexicon, while others are not as they are syntactic outputs. As noted in section 1,
if the Hebrew verbal system, which has been considered an important source
of motivation for a purely root-based lexical representation, turns out to provide evidence for word-based representations, then all the more so, for other
languages. More generally, it is unlikely that the internal structure of the lexical
component, just like other properties of the architecture of grammar, would be
subject to (parametric) variation “root based versus word-based”. Hence, our
conclusions with regard to the mental lexicon of Hebrew strongly suggest that
the mental lexicon in general must include actual words.
The data base of our study comprises verb phrase idioms of both the compositional and the non-compositional type. Nunberg, Sag and Wasow (1994)
draw a distinction between (a) non-compositional idioms (such as kick the bucket,
saw logs), whose meaning cannot be distributed among their subparts, and (b)
compositional idioms (such as spill the beans, pull strings, keep track of), which
though conventionalized in form and sometimes opaque in interpretation,
still have meanings whose elements can be seen as corresponding to the various subparts of the idiom.
J. Horvath and Tal Siloni /
Brill’s Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics 1 (2009) 283–310
289
3. Differences in the Distribution of Verbal Voices in Idioms
The distribution of verb phrase idioms across various diatheses has distinct
empirical consequences for the two alternative storage hypotheses: (i) the word
based listing hypothesis, which assumes that idioms are stored as subentries of
words appearing in the mental lexicon and (ii) the root based hypothesis,
which assumes that idioms, i.e., special (idiomatic) meanings, must be specified as properties of roots. As noted in section 2, the root based hypothesis
follows by necessity for theories that reduce the lexicon to non-computational
lists of roots (Marantz 1997, Borer 2005, McGinnis 2002).
Since under the root based hypothesis lexical categories (verbs, nouns, etc.)
are not listed in the mental lexicon at all, only roots are, it is a straightforward
expectation that the existence and special meaning of a verb phrase idiom will
be listed by the root, and should a priori be available for its various diatheses.
In principle, higher functional heads that a given VP merges with in the syntax are not expected to influence the availability of its idiomatic meaning. In
other words, for the same ‘root + VP-internal material’ combination we expect
to have, or not to have, a particular idiomatic meaning available, quite independently of what functional heads the VP appears embedded under. This
expectation is indeed valid for verb phrase idioms with regard to the variety of
inflectional heads such as tense, and modality.2 Yet, even upon casual inspection, the same turns out not to hold for the various voices, which in root based
models are also formed in the syntax via functional heads.
Before illustrating that, it is important to note that we have set aside idioms
having a fixed subject/external argument. For one thing, we wanted to remove
any suspicion that these may be clausal idioms, as the latter are often argued
to be of a distinct nature, which justifies a different storage method (Marantz
1984:27, Nunberg, Sag and Wasow 1994). Moreover, in order to have a common basis for comparison across the different verbal diatheses, it was crucial
to limit the searches to the VP internal domain, as idioms involving the external argument a priori cannot be headed by unaccusative verbs.
Consider first the transitive-unaccusative alternation. Examination of an informally collected set of phrasal idioms headed by verbs participating in this alternation appears to straightforwardly contradict the expectation of the root based
2
As is well-known, verb phrase idioms may exhibit limitations with regard to certain aspectual choices (e.g. no progressive aspect possible for kick the bucket). However the observed restrictions demonstrably follow from the semantic incompatibility between a given choice of Aspect
and the aspectual type of the verb of the idiom (for discussion, see e.g. McGinnis 2002).
290
J. Horvath and Tal Siloni /
Brill’s Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics 1 (2009) 283–310
hypothesis. Let us start with the set of idioms in (1), each headed by the unaccusative (anticausative) alternate. The same idiomatic meaning is not available
for the transitive alternate. The citation form of Hebrew verbs is conventionally
the past tense form; the English glosses are presented in the past tense as well.
I. Unaccusative idioms unavailable for the transitive alternate
For the pair yarad ‘went down’ – horid ‘lowered’:
(1) a. yarad le-omek ha-inyan
went down to-depth the-matter
‘got to the bottom of the matter’
b. horid et x le-omek ha-inyan
lowered acc x to-depth the-matter
nonexisting
For the pair yaca ‘went out’ – hoci ‘took out’:
(2) a. yaca le-x me-ha-af
went out to-x from-the-nose
‘got tired of ’
b. hoci le-x me-ha-af
took out to-x from-the-nose
nonexisting
For the pair nafal ‘fell’ – hipil ‘fell.trans’:
(3) a. nafal al oznayim arelot
fell on ears not+circumcised
‘fell on deaf ears’
b. hipil et x al oznayim arelot 3
fell.trans acc x on ears not+circumcised
nonexisting
For the pair xazar ‘returned’ – hexzir ‘returned.trans’:
(4) a. xazar al arba
returned on four
‘came crawling’
b. hexzir et x al arba
returned.trans acc x on four
nonexisting
Why would the transitive alternate not exhibit the same idiomatic meaning in
these cases? Under the root based lexicon hypothesis, which derives transitive
3
One should not confuse the nonexisting idiom (3b) with the existing idiom hipil et txinat-o
(bifney x) ‘fell.trans/dropped acc his plea (in front of x)’, which means ‘beg, plead’.
J. Horvath and Tal Siloni /
Brill’s Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics 1 (2009) 283–310
291
structures in the syntax, it is far from obvious. Could the unavailability of the
transitive version for these unaccusative idioms be due to the fact that the
addition of the external argument – or the functional voice (little v) head commonly claimed to introduce it – somehow blocks the idiomatic meaning of
the ‘root+VP internal material’ unit? That this could not be the case is shown
by examples where the unaccusative and its transitive alternate do share the
same idiomatic interpretation.
II. Unaccusative and transitive shared idioms
For the pair yaca ‘went out’ – hoci ‘took out’
(5) a. yaca me-ha-kelim
went out from-the-dishes
‘got very angry, got furious’
b. hoci et x me-ha-kelim
took out acc x from-the-dishes
‘made x mad, drove x crazy, made x’s blood boil’
For the pair nafal ‘fell’ – hipil ‘fell.trans’
(6) a. nafal ba-pax
fell in-the-bin
‘x was tricked’
b. hipil et x ba-pax
fell.trans acc x in+the-bin
‘tricked x’
For the pair šav ‘returned’ – hešiv ‘returned.trans’
(7) a. šav le-eytan-o
returns to-strength-his
‘x recuperated’
b. hešiv et x le-eytan-o
returned.trans acc x to-strength-his
‘recuperated x’
For the pair nidlak ‘got lit’ – hidlik ‘lighted’4
4
The unaccusative idiom (8a) has a fixed subject. This may seem to conflict with our decision
not to take into account idioms with a fixed subject in order to remove any chance of having
some clausal idioms among our data. Observe, however, that in the case of (8a), the suspicion of
being clausal does not arise. The attested transitive alternate (8b) is limited to the VP internal
domain, directly establishing that the pair constitutes a phrasal idiom. The same is true for the
(nonexistent) unaccusative idioms (11b) and (12b) below. Unaccusative idioms with a fixed
292
J. Horvath and Tal Siloni /
Brill’s Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics 1 (2009) 283–310
(8) a. nidleka le-x nura aduma
got-lit to-x bulb red
‘x sensed a warning sign’
b. hidlik le-x nura aduma
lighted to-x bulb red
‘was a warning sign for x’
As seen above, “blocking” does not hold systematically, and retaining such an
account would mean having to specify whether the transitive alternate shares
or fails to share the idiomatic meaning not just for each individual root, but
for each specific idiom. This is shown by the contrasting behavior of idioms
that are headed by the same root, as exemplified by the roots y.c.a and n.p.l,
for instance, which appear in unaccusative idioms not available for the transitive alternate, (2) and (3) respectively, as well as in idioms shared by the unaccusative and the transitive alternate, (5) and (6) respectively.
Even more troublesome for the root based lexicon hypothesis is the existence
of the third possible type: transitive idioms not available for their unaccusative
alternate, as exemplified in (9)-(12). These idioms are headed by a transitive
verb which has an unaccusative alternate, yet this unaccusative form fails to
share the idiomatic meaning manifested by the ‘root+VP internal material’ of
the transitive version.
III. Transitive idioms unavailable for the unaccusative alternate
For the pair sovev ‘turned.trans’ – histovev ‘turned.unacc’:
(9) a. sovev et x be-kaxaš
turned acc x in-lie
‘cheated x’
b. histovev be-kaxaš
turned.unacc in-lie
nonexisting
For the pair hidbik ‘glued.trans’ – nidbak ‘glued.unacc’
(10) a. hidbik et x la-kise
glued acc x to+the chair
‘fascinated x’
subject often exhibit a verb subject order. Nonexisting idioms, of course, occur in neither order.
To simplify presentation, however, we give them in subject verb order only.
J. Horvath and Tal Siloni /
Brill’s Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics 1 (2009) 283–310
nidbak la-kise5
got+glued to+the-chair
293
nonexisting
For the pair hixnis ‘entered.trans’ – nixnas ‘entered’
(11) a. hixnis le-x milim la-pe
inserted to-x words to+the-mouth
‘put words in x’s mouth’
b. nixnesu le-x milim la-pe
entered to-x words to+the-mouth
nonexisting
For the pair gamar ‘finished.trans’ – nigmar ‘ended.unacc’
(12) a. gamar omer
finished utterance
‘made up his mind, reached a decision’
b. omer nigmar
utterance ended.unacc
nonexisting
Here even an ad hoc blocking stipulation would be of no use. How could the
unit ‘root + VP internal material’ have an idiomatic meaning that is possible
for the transitive verb and unavailable for its unaccusative counterpart? Note
that the external argument itself is not part of the idiomatic meaning. How
can its presence nonetheless be necessary for obtaining this meaning?
In sum, on a root based theory, one would need to make reference to the
presence or type (feature-content) of particular voice heads as part of the specification of a root’s idiomatic meaning. Moreover, it would have to explain
why voice heads but not other functional heads can be referred to in the listing
of verb phrase idioms.
Could we attribute this discrepancy to the hierarchical structural superiority of
functional heads (such as Tense) in comparison to voice heads? If this were so,
one would expect all voice heads to be able to affect idiomatic meaning this way.
Thus, verb phrase idioms headed for instance by a passive form would a priori be
expected to exist without the active counterpart with the same idiomatic interpretation. Yet it is well known that passive verb idioms do not exist without a
corresponding active counterpart (see e.g. Chomsky 1981, Marantz 1997) unlike
5
The unaccusative form does not bear the idiomatic meaning of the transitive. The form is
homophonous with the reflexive verb, which is associated with an idiomatic meaning, but a different one: ‘held on to his position’. A Google search detected only one instance where nidbak
la-kise ‘got+glued to+the-chair’ is used with the meaning ‘got fascinated’.
294
J. Horvath and Tal Siloni /
Brill’s Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics 1 (2009) 283–310
unaccusative and transitive verbs. This conclusion was confirmed for Hebrew by
the preliminary survey we conducted. This systematic difference between verbal
voice alternations with regard to the distribution of idioms, if empirically solid,
is of obvious theoretical significance and calls for further investigation.6
In order to be able to draw reliable, firmly-grounded conclusions from the
intriguing pattern of distribution of idioms, the above preliminary observations
need to be substantiated by more systematic testing, meeting criteria of statistical
significance. The reason why the investigation of this domain calls for a quantitative, corpus-based study in particular is that speakers’ judgments regarding the
existence/non-existence of various alternates of idioms are notoriously slippery.
The mere presentation of an example for eliciting a judgment may induce the
adoption of a possible but previously non-existent variant of the idiom in question. The spontaneous formation and use of novel idiomatic expressions is a
common manifestation of speakers’ linguistic competence. Thus, in case the
potential alternates of an existing idiom are not blocked by some principle of
grammar, it is possible that speakers will exhibit flexibility and pliability when
judging their actual existence. Judging the existence of particular forms of idioms
is thus in sharp contrast with elicitation of grammaticality judgments in syntax
or other judgments of well-formedness. Accordingly, we have conducted a corpus-based quantitative study to test idiom distribution across different verbal
diatheses. We turn to the presentation of this study in the following section.
4. Idiom Distribution Study
4.1 Methods
In order to systematically test our initial evaluations outlined above, we collected a random sample of sixty predicates of various diatheses (voices), and
examined their distribution in phrasal idioms. Our corpora included seven
Hebrew idiom dictionaries: Avneyon (2002), Cohen (1999), Dayan (2004),
Fruchtman et al. (2001), Levanon (1981), Rosental (2005), Sévenier-Gabriel
6
It is worth noting here that the distinction mentioned in section 2 between compositional
versus non-compositional types of idioms (drawn by Nunberg, Sag and Wasow 1994) is orthogonal to the differences in idiom distribution observed above. Unique unaccusative (1)-(4),
unique transitive (9)-(12), as well as shared unaccusative-transitive (5)-(8) idioms are all attested
both for compositional and for non-compositional idioms (see Appendix). In other words, the
observed differences in idiom distribution among various diatheses arise independently of the
matter of compositionality.
J. Horvath and Tal Siloni /
Brill’s Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics 1 (2009) 283–310
295
(2004). In addition, we conducted online Google searches to test our findings,
and consulted 8 native speakers’ judgments for completeness, not as a primary
source of information. Given the ‘flexible’ nature of the data, i.e., the human
ability to form new metaphors and extend the use of idioms, when conducting
our on-line searches, we did not consider an isolated occurrence as valid
evidence for the existence of a particular idiom form. Such an occurrence
may well be a personal borrowing, inventive novel use, or distortion, rather
than reflection of a listed, i.e., conventionalized, idiom form of the mental
lexicon.7
We have counted the distribution of unique idioms. We use the term unique
idiom to refer to an idiom whose matrix predicate has a transitive alternate (in
the vocabulary of the language) that does not share the same idiomatic meaning. As for transitive verbs, idioms involving them are referred to as unique if
they are unavailable for the corresponding unaccusative verb. As will be clear
below, additional cross-checking has been done to uncover possible sharing of
idiomatic meaning between other diatheses.
Idioms were taken to be nonexisting in three different situations. (i) The
string is grammatical and nonanomalous, but has only the literal, nonidiomatic, reading. For example, Hebrew has the idiom macuc me-ha-ecba literally ‘sucked.adj from-the-finger’, which means ‘invented’. The corresponding
verbal passive sentence nimcac me-ha-ecba has only the literal, nonidiomatic
meaning ‘was sucked from the finger’ (say, referring to some poison). (ii) The
string is grammatical and nonanomalous, and the idiomatic meaning can be
readily understood, but the string is never used that way. This happens only
with idioms that are compositional in the sense of Nunberg, Sag & Wasow
1994 (see section 2). For example, consider the idiom hevi le-x et ha-sa’if, literally ‘brought to-x the-clause’, namely, ‘annoyed him’. Its unaccusative counterpart ba le-x ha-sa’if ‘came to-x the clause’ can be understood, but sounds
weird and was neither found in idiom dictionaries nor detected in online
searches.8 (iii) The string is ungrammatical and/or anomalous. For example,
consider the idiom ba im x xešbon, literally ‘came with x account’, that is,
‘settled matters with x’. Its transitive counterpart hevi et y im x xešbon ‘brought
y with x account’ does not only lack the idiomatic meaning but is actually
ungrammatical as the verb does not allow the addition of xešbon ‘account’.
7
To be on the safe side, we considered an idiom as nonexisting if (i) it was not found in the
idiom dictionaries and (ii) had in most cases no occurrence, and in no case more than two occurrences. Statistically, we could have ignored even more than two instances.
8
A parallel idiom does exist with the verb ala ‘rose’: ala lo ha-se’if ‘rose to-him the-clause’, ‘he
got mad’.
296
J. Horvath and Tal Siloni /
Brill’s Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics 1 (2009) 283–310
We have looked at the existence versus non-existence of unique idioms for
the following three verbal voices: unaccusatives, transitives, and verbal passives, and in addition, adjectival passives. We have sampled the first sixty items
in an alphabetical verb dictionary (Stern 1994). There is no reason to assume
that there is a correlation between the alphabetical position of a verb and
its behavior with regard to participation in idioms. Predicates (i.e., voices/
diatheses) that do not appear as entries in dictionaries were formed based on
their transitive alternates, which were sampled from the verb dictionary. For
each selected item, it was then checked whether or not it participates in a
unique idiom, or in more than one. As mentioned, this was done by searching
the seven idiom dictionaries, followed by online Google searches. 8 native
speakers were carefully consulted for completeness. We counted the number
of predicates of each type giving rise to unique idioms (not the idioms themselves, which are often numerous for each specific predicate).
Intransitives whose subject qualifies as a Theme were classified as unaccusatives based on converging results of three different diagnostics: (i) licensing of
verb subject order, with no sentence initial trigger typical of stylistic inversion
(which is possible in Hebrew with any verb type)9; (ii) licensing of possessive
datives (a diagnostic originally suggested by Borer and Grodzinsky 1986);10
(iii) existence of a transitive alternate whose external role is a Cause role (indifferent with regard to the mental state of the argument) (Reinhart 2002).
4.2 Results
Table 1 summarizes our findings. Each cell indicates the number of predicates
of the relevant type involved in the formation of unique idioms. As mentioned, in all categories sixty predicates were sampled.
The number of phrasal idioms headed by a verbal passive (0) is significantly
different from those headed by an unaccusative (χ² = 23.088, p < .0001),
9
By Verb Subject order we mean strict VS with no material intervening between the verb and
its subject, as such intervention can in certain cases license “inversion” also with unergatives.
Further, it is important to note that the sole counterexample to the generalization that strict VS
order is impossible with unergatives is the verb tilpen (also as tilfen), which to some extent
licenses VS (see Shlonsky 1987). This, however, seems to be a special use of the verb, as suggested
by the fact that in this environment it does not allow complements; hence the marginality of
tilfen avixa le-dan (‘called your father to Dan’).
10
Modification by possessive datives is limited to verbs whose subject is an internal argument,
in case the subject is an alienable noun and the possessive dative a lexical noun phrase (not a
personal pronoun). Inalienable subjects license possessive datives with unergatives, too. Personal
pronouns can be ethical datives, which are also possible with unergatives. The possessee can be
neither a proper name nor a kinship noun.
J. Horvath and Tal Siloni /
Brill’s Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics 1 (2009) 283–310
297
Table 1. Unique Idioms
Verbal passives
Unaccusatives
Transitives
Adjectival passives
0/60
21/60
23/60
13/60
a transitive (χ² = 26.033, p < .0001), as well as an adjectival passive
(χ² = 12.423, p = .0004). The difference between the number of idioms headed
by unaccusatives, transitives, and adjectival passives is insignificant (χ²(2) =
4.313, p = .116).
The next section puts forth our hypothesis with regard to the storage of
idioms, shows how it accounts for the above results, and discusses the implications of our findings with regard to the organization of the lexicon.
5. The Organization of the Lexicon and the Storage Technique
5.1 The Head Based Storage Hypothesis
Why can unaccusative and transitive verbs as well as adjectival passives give
rise to unique idioms, but passive verbs cannot? We believe that the reason for
that is straightforward. We put forth the hypothesis that phrasal idioms are
listed as subentries of their matrix predicate.11 Such listing is possible only in
a word based lexicon, which allows storage of actual verbal diatheses, that is,
of actual words, meant in the abstract sense (not a phonological word). We
then argue that there are independent reasons to believe that unaccusative and
transitive verbs as well as adjectival (stative) passives are entries in the mental
lexicon, while passive verbs are not. The former can head unique phrasal idioms because they are lexical entries, but the latter cannot as they are not present in the lexicon.
As will become clear below, we believe on independent grounds that unaccusatives and adjectival (stative) passives are formed in the mental lexicon by
universal operations. Following early proposals (e.g., Aronoff 1976, Jackendoff
1975), we take that to mean that they are listed lexical entries, rejecting the
11
In the same vein, noun phrase idioms are stored as subentries of their matrix head, the
noun. The Head Based Storage Hypothesis has often been implicitly assumed in the generative
literature, and is also reflected by the organization of work by lexicographers. Emonds (2006)
explicitly suggests that phrasal idioms are specified in the entries of their lexical heads.
298
J. Horvath and Tal Siloni /
Brill’s Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics 1 (2009) 283–310
option that they are formed anew upon each use of the predicate, for parsimony among other reasons. Crucially, our present findings reinforce this view,
as they provide autonomous evidence that these predicates must be stored.
The lexical component should somehow specify the links between inputs and
outputs (links labeled by Aronoff (1976) Redundancy Rules), but this is not
directly relevant for our purposes.12
Our storage hypothesis then can be stated as follows.
(13) The Head Based Storage Hypothesis
Verb phrase idioms, whether compositional or not, are stored as subentries of their matrix predicate, the lexical verb.
5.2 Discussion of results
Let us first examine the case of verbal passives in light of the Head Based
Storage Hypothesis. Recent studies have specifically argued that passive verbs
are not listed in the lexicon. Independently of one’s conception of the lexicon,
it has been repeatedly argued that passivization does not involve any lexical
procedure. If this is correct, then in the formation of both active and passive
sentences, the same lexical item is inserted into the syntax. Further, given the
choice of functional head or other device (depending on one’s theory), the
external θ-role is assigned either to a null (or affixal) category in the syntax
(Baker, Johnson and Roberts 1989, Collins 2005) or to a variable in the
semantic representation (Chierchia 2004, Reinhart 2002, Horvath and Siloni
2008a), resulting in a passive sentence. For our purposes here, it is irrelevant
what the precise derivation of passives is. It is however crucial that passive
verbs are not entries in the mental lexicon. Given the Head Based Storage
Hypothesis, this immediately accounts for the lack of unique verb phrase idioms whose matrix predicate is a passive verb: since passive verbs are not listed,
an idiomatic meaning specific to them cannot be stored, and consequently
they cannot head unique idioms. We do find passive phrasal idioms if the corresponding transitive (active) shares the idiomatic meaning. This is expected:
since the transitive is listed, an idiom can be stored as its subentry.13
12
Note incidentally that despite the listing of lexical outputs, lexical operations ought to be
operative all along from the acquisition stage to the steady state as shown, for instance, by speakers’ ability to activate them in the production of innovations.
13
As is well known there are transitive (active) idioms that have no verbal passive counterpart.
This is fully consistent with our theory, given that it is the transitive (active) verb that is listed in
the lexicon. The unavailability of a verbal passive alternate with the same idiomatic meaning
J. Horvath and Tal Siloni /
Brill’s Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics 1 (2009) 283–310
299
Why can unaccusative verbs head unique idioms? Views regarding unaccusative verbs diverge. According to numerous studies, however, they are lexical
entries: some studies claim that they are underived (Kratzer 1996, 2000), others argue that they are derived from their transitive alternate by an operation of
decausativization, which reduces the external (Cause) role of the corresponding
transitive entry in the lexicon, thus forming a new, unaccusative entry (Levin
and Rappaport 1995, Reinhart 2002, Reinhart and Siloni 2005, Horvath and
Siloni 2008b). As both views treat unaccusatives as lexical entries, they predict
that they ought to be able to head unique idioms, as is indeed the case.
The next finding to be discussed is the existence of unique transitive idioms,
that is, idioms headed by transitive verbs whose unaccusative counterparts do
not share the same idiomatic meaning. Given our head based storage hypothesis, this indicates that transitives must be listed in the mental lexicon. On the
decausativization approach to unaccusatives, transitives indeed are stored in
the lexicon since they feed a lexical operation (of decausativization). In contrast, if unaccusatives, and more generally, verbs and their internal arguments,
are listed but not their transitive counterparts, which are derived by the addition of the corresponding voice head in the syntax (Kratzer 1996, 2000), the
fact that transitives have unique idioms is unexpected.
Recall that sharing of idiomatic meaning between unaccusatives and their
transitive alternates is also attested. Our preliminary survey (see section 3) has
revealed that certain idioms are common to both diatheses. The existence of
such idioms shows that it cannot be the case that the addition of the voice
head responsible for the external argument (forming the active voice) blocks
the accessibility of the root to the idiomatic meaning. We validated the findings of our preliminary survey on the basis of the 60 pairs of transitive and
unaccusative verbs that we have sampled for the search of unique idioms. For
each pair we checked whether or not its members shared some idiomatic
meaning(s). Out of 60 pairs, 16 pairs exhibits idiomatic meaning common to
both members.
depends on whether or not the transitive verb phrase idiom is able to undergo verbal passivization. In fact, it has been independently argued in the literature that failure to passivize is a systematic property of non-compositional verb phrase idioms, i.e., idioms such as kick the bucket, saw
logs (see e.g. Nunberg, Sag and Wasow 1994). Ruwet (1991) discusses further semantic properties, such as “referential autonomy” required of subjects, which may have an effect on the passivization of idioms. The same seems to hold for Hebrew as well; thus, hexzir ciyud ‘returned.trans
equipment’, namely, ‘died’ cannot passivize. In Hebrew, in addition, certain verbal passive forms
fall outside of current usage, e.g., subav ‘turned’. This may be the reason why sovev be-kaxaš
‘turned.trans in-lie’ (9a) cannot passivize.
300
J. Horvath and Tal Siloni /
Brill’s Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics 1 (2009) 283–310
(14) Transitive-Unaccusative Pairs
Shared Idioms:16/60
It is worth noting that the difference between the number of unique idioms
and shared idioms is insignificant, both in the case of transitives (χ² = 1.368,
p = .242) and in the case of unaccusatives (χ² = .625, p = .429). A word based
lexicon, where both the transitive and unaccusative alternates are listed can
store both unique and nonunique idioms, and a priori does not predict a significant difference in their occurrence.14
Constructionist root based theories, which have become prominent recently,
take both unaccusatives and transitives to be formed in the syntax from the
root by the merger of the appropriate functional heads (Borer 2005, Ramchand
2006, Marantz 2008). On such theories, roots must be able to list specific
idiomatic meaning as depending on the particular voice head they will merge
with in the syntax. But if such listing is allowed in a root based lexicon, why is
it impossible to list the idiomatic meaning the root would have in the syntactic context of the passive voice head? Setting apart the passive voice head this
way without independent evidence seems completely ad hoc. Moreover, recall
that adjectival (stative) passives do form unique idioms. This raises the additional query as to why verbal and adjectival passives should differ this way if
both are represented in the lexicon by the root.15,16
14
As noted by an anonymous referee, idioms shared between the transitive and the unaccusative diatheses of the same root might be taken to suggest that roots are also listed in lexicon, in
addition to words. As we point out in section 1, this is consistent with our conclusion that the
conception of the lexicon as a list of roots is inadequate. More importantly, the fact that some
transitive-unaccusative shared idioms are attested by no means constitutes sufficient evidence
that roots are also listed. If these idioms were subentries of a listed root, one would predict that
other things being equal they would appear also with other available diatheses, such as adjectival
passives, for instance.
15
None of the unique idioms headed by an adjectival passive was available for the corresponding verbal passive. This is, of course, expected as verbal passives can head idioms only if the
idiomatic meaning is available for the transitive alternate.
16
Constructionist single generative engine approaches typically propose to recapture the
empirical differences between lexical outputs (i.e., items listed) and items derived in the syntax
by appealing to a distinction between two domains: the locality domain (phase) of the root
delimited by the category-determining functional head merged directly with the latter (such as
little v and a heads) vs. the domain of other functional heads, which merge above the categorydetermining head, i.e., outside of the root domain. Marantz (1997, 2008), a prominent representative of this approach, suggests that special meanings, including phrasal idioms, arise only
within the root-phase, as the idiosyncratic information associated with the root is not accessible
higher. Such an account would have to stipulate that transitives, unaccusatives and adjectival
passives are formed by merging with the root domain, while verbal passives are formed higher.
Within these approaches, the point of merger of the external argument also is crucially assumed
J. Horvath and Tal Siloni /
Brill’s Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics 1 (2009) 283–310
301
On the Head Based Storage Hypothesis, it is not at all surprising that adjectival, but not verbal, passives give rise to unique idioms, given that numerous
studies argue that the former are formed in the lexical component (Wasow
1977, Dubinsky and Simango 1996, Horvath and Siloni 2008a). If adjectival
passives, in contrast with their verbal counterparts, are lexical entries, they
ought to be able to head unique idioms, as is indeed the case.17
6. Conclusions
Under the Head Based Storage Hypothesis, the results reported in table
(1) and (2) follow straightforwardly. More importantly, our findings have several significant consequences. They provide robust evidence in favor of a word
based approach to the internal organization of the lexicon. More specifically,
they constitute a novel type of evidence in favor of the various studies claiming that unaccusatives, transitives, and adjectival passives are lexical entries,
whereas verbal passive are not. Thus they represent a serious challenge (a) to
strictly lexicalist approaches (HSPG, LFG) arguing that even verbal passives
are the result of a lexical rule (e.g., Bresnan 1982, Pollard and Sag 1994, Van
Valin 1993), and (b) to approaches uniformly deriving the various diatheses in
the syntax (Borer 2005, Ramchand 2006, Pylkkänen (2002), Alexiadou et al.
2004). In light of our results, the mental lexicon cannot be reduced to noncomputational lists of items. It must be an active component, where arity
operations can apply. Finally, our findings reinforce the view that lexical outputs are listed and not formed repeatedly. That is so because there is a striking
correlation between the diatheses claimed on independent grounds to be
formed in the lexicon and those that give rise to unique idioms and must
therefore be stored.
to define a locality domain. Thus it would be expected that the two domains will coincide: the
domain of idiosyncratic meanings and the domain of the external argument. But this turns out
not to be the case: verbal passives, transitives as well as a class of adjectival passives (on the latter,
see Anagnostopoulou 2003, Horvath and Siloni 2008, Meltzer 2006) all involve an external role,
yet transitives and adjectival passives do but verbal passives do not play a role in idiomatic meanings. One could of course insist that verbal passives are nonetheless formed higher than the latter
voices, but we do not see any plausible independent reason for this stipulation.
17
Our finding that adjectival passives must be listed in the lexicon provides evidence also in
support of the assumption that category membership is specified in the lexicon. This in turn
implies that other cross-categorial alternations, such as verb-derived nominal pairs, may exhibit
unique idioms as well. This latter consequence is investigated in work in progress.
302
J. Horvath and Tal Siloni /
Brill’s Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics 1 (2009) 283–310
Acknowledgments
This paper was supported by the israel science foundation – (grant no.
44/05). For comments and discussion we are grateful to: Aviad Albert, Michal
Ben-Shachar, Jenny Birger, Julie Fadlon, David Hron, Lola Karsenti, Lior
Laks, Aya Meltzer, and Hillel Taub-Tabib. Special thanks to Aya Meltzer and
Hillel Taub-Tabib for assistance in designing the study, to Julie Fadlon and
Aya Meltzer for sampling the verbs and collecting the idioms, to Aya Meltzer
for help with the statistical analysis, and Lior Laks for technical assistance.
References
Alexiadou, Artemis, and Elena Anagnostopoulou. 2004. Voice Morphology in the CausativeInchoative Alternation: Evidence for a Non-unified Structural Analysis of Unaccusatives. In
Artemis Alexiadou, Elena Anagnostopoulou, and Martin Everaert (eds.), The Unaccusativity
Puzzle: Explorations of the Syntax-Lexicon Interface, 114-136. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Anagnostopoulou, Elena. 2003. Participles and Voice. In Artemis Alexiadou, Monike Rathert,
and Arnim von Stechow (eds.), Perfect Explorations, 1-36. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Anderson, Stephen R. 1992. A-Morphous Morphology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Aronoff, Mark. 1976. Word Formation in Generative Grammar. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Baker, Mark. 1988. Incorporation. A Theory of Grammatical Function Changing. Chicago: The
University of Chicago Press.
Baker, Mark, Kyle Johnson, and Ian Roberts. 1989. Passive Arguments Raised. Linguistic
Inquiry 20: 219–251.
Bat-El, Outi. 1994. Stem Modification and Cluster Transfer in Modern Hebrew. NLLT 12:
572-596.
Berman, Ruth A. 1978. Modern Hebrew Structure. Tel-Aviv: University Publishing Projects.
Bolozky, Shmuel. 1978. Word Formation Strategies in the MH Verb System: Denominative
Verbs. Afroasiatic Linguistics 5: 1-26.
Borer, Hagit. 1988. On the Morphological Parallelism between Compounds and Constructs.
In Geert E. Booij and Jaap van Marle (eds.), Yearbook of Morphology, 45–65. Dordrecht:
Foris.
Borer, Hagit. 2005. Structuring Sense. Volumes 1 and 2. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Borer, Hagit and Yosef Grodzinsky. 1986. Syntactic vs. Lexical Cliticization: The Case of Hebrew
Dative Clitics. In Hagit Borer (ed.), The Syntax of Pronominal Clitics, 175-217. San
Francisco: Academic Press.
Bresnan, Joan. 1982. The Passive in Lexical Theory. In Joan Bresnan (ed.), The Mental
Representation of Grammatical Relations, 3-86. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Chierchia, Gennaro. 2004. A Semantics for Unaccusatives and its Syntactic Consequences. In
Artemis Alexiadou, Elena Anagnostopolou, and Martin Everaert (eds.), The Unaccusativity
Puzzle: Explorations on the Syntax-Lexicon Interface, 288-331. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Chomsky, Noam. 1981. Lectures on Government and Binding. Dordrecht: Foris.
Collins, Chris. 2005. A Smuggling Approach to the Passive in English. Syntax 8: 81-120.
J. Horvath and Tal Siloni /
Brill’s Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics 1 (2009) 283–310
303
Doron, Edit. 2003. Agency and Voice: The Semantics of the Semitic Templates. Natural
Language Semantics 11: 1-67.
Dubinsky, Stanley and Silvester Ron Simango. 1996. Passive and Stative in Chichewa: Evidence
for Modular Distinctions in Grammar. Language 72: 749-781.
Emonds, Joseph. 2006. Adjectival Passives: The Construction in the Iron Mask. In Martin
Everaert, Henk van Riemsdijk (eds.), The Blackwell Companion to Syntax, 16-60. Malden,
MA.: Blackwell.
Everaert and Martin. 1990. The Lexical Representation of Idioms and the Morphology-Syntax
Interface. Ms., Utrecht Institute of Linguistics.
Gesenius, Wilhelm H.F. 1910. Gesenius’ Hebrew Grammar. E. Kautzsch (ed.), A.E. Cowley
(reviser). Oxford: Clarendon Press. 2nd English ed. (orig. Halle. 1813).
Halle, Morris and Alec Marantz. 1993. Distributed Morphology and the Pieces of Inflection.
In Ken Hale and Samuel Jay Keyser (eds.), The View from Building 20, 111-176. Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press.
Horvath, Julia. 1981. On the Status of Vowel Patterns in Modern Hebrew: Morphological Rules
and Lexical Representations. In Tracy Thomas-Flinders (ed.), Extended Word-and-Paradigm
Theory, 228-261. Los Angeles: UCLA Working Papers.
Horvath, Julia and Tal Siloni. 2008a. Active Lexicon: Adjectival and Verbal Passives. In Sharon
Armon Lotem, Gabi Danon, and Susan Rothstein (eds.), Current Issues in Generative
Hebrew Linguistics, 105-136. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Horvath, Julia and Tal Siloni. 2008b. Causatives across Components. Ms., Tel Aviv University.
Jackendoff, Ray. 1975. Morphological and Semantic Regularities in the Lexicon. Language
51: 639-671.
Jackendoff, Ray. 1997. The Architecture of the Language Faculty. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Kratzer, Angelika. 1996. Severing the External Argument from Its Verb. In Johan Rooryck and
Laurie Zaring (eds.), Phrase Structure and the Lexicon, 109-137. Dordrecht: Kluwer.
Kratzer, Angelika. 2000. Building Statives. In Lisa J. Conathan, Jeff Good, Darya Kavitskaya,
Alyssa B. Wulf , and Alan C.L. Yu (eds.), Proceedings of the Twenty-sixth Annual Meeting of
the Berkeley Linguistic Society, 385–399. Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistic Society.
Laks, Lior. 2007. Two Types of Morpho-phonology: Lexical and Syntactic Operations in Semitic
Languages. In Fabio Montermini, Gilles Boye and Nabil Hathout (eds.), Selected Proceedings
of the 5th Decembrettes: Morphology in Toulouse, 68-78. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla
Proceedings Project.
Levin, Beth and Malka Hovav-Rappaport. 1995. Unaccusativity. At the Syntax-Lexical Semantics
Interface. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Marantz, Alec. 1984. On the Nature of Grammatical Relations. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Marantz, Alec. 1997. No Escape from Syntax: Don’t Try Morphological Analysis in the
Privacy of Your Own Lexicon. In Artemis Dimitriadis and Laura Siegel (eds.), Proceedings
of the 21st Annual Penn Linguistics Colloquium, 201-225. Philadelphia: University of
Pennsylvania.
Marantz, Alec. 2008. Phases and Words. Ms., NYU.
McCarthy, John. 1979. Formal Problems in Semitic Phonology and Morphology. Doctoral
dissertation, MIT.
McCarthy, John. 1981. A Prosodic Theory of Nonconcatenative Morphology. Linguistic Inquiry
12: 373-418.
McGinnis, Martha. 2002. On the Systematic Aspect of Idioms. Linguistic Inquiry 33: 665-672.
Nunberg, Geoffrey, Ivan A. Sag and Thomas Wasow. 1994. Idioms. Language 70: 491-538.
Pollard, Carl, and Ivan A. Sag. 1994. Head-driven Phrase Structure Grammar. Chicago, IL and
Stanford, CA: The University of Chicago Press and CSLI Publications.
304
J. Horvath and Tal Siloni /
Brill’s Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics 1 (2009) 283–310
Prunet, Jean-François, Renée Béland, and Ali Idrissi. 2000. The Mental Representation of
Semitic Words. Linguistic Inquiry 31: 609-648.
Pylkkänen, Liina. 2002. Introducing Arguments. Doctoral dissertation, MIT.
Ramchand, Gillian. 2006. Verb Meaning and the Lexicon: A First Phase Syntax. Ms., Universitetet
i Tromsø.
Reinhart, Tanya. 2002. The Theta System: An Overview. Theoretical Linguistics 28: 229-290.
Reinhart, Tanya and Tal Siloni. 2005. The Lexicon-Syntax Parameter: Reflexivization and other
Arity Operations. Linguistic Inquiry 36: 389-436.
Ruwet, Nicolas. 1991. Syntax and Human Experience. Chicago: The University of Chicago
Press.
Shlonsky, Ur. 1987. Null and Displaced Subjects. Doctoral dissertation, MIT.
Ussishkin, Adam. 1999. The Inadequacy of the Consonantal Root: Modern Hebrew Denominal
Verbs and Output-Output Correspondence. Phonology 16: 401-442.
Van Valin, Robert D. Jr. 1993. A Synopsis of Role and Reference Grammar. In Robert D.
Jr. Van Valin (ed.), Advances in Role and Reference Grammar, 1-164. Amsterdam: John
Benjamins.
Wasow, Thomas. 1977. Transformations and the Lexicon. In Peter W. Culicover, Thomas
Wasow, and Adrian Akmajian (eds.), Formal Syntax, 327-360. New York: Academic
Press.
Williams, Edwin. 2007. Dumping Lexicalism. In Gillian Ramchand and Charles Reiss (eds.),
The Oxford Handbook of Linguistic Interfaces, 353-382. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Sources
Avneyon, Eitan. 2002. lašon rišon – milon nivim u-mixtamim al yesod ha-mekorot (‘First tongue – a
dictionary of idioms from the Rabbinic literature’), Eitav Publishing House (in Hebrew).
Cohen, Tuvya. 1999. nivon ivri xadaš (‘New anthology of sayings in Hebrew’). Yavneh Publishing
House (in Hebrew).
Dayan, Rami. 2004. niv sfatayim ve-imrey šefer (‘Idioms and sayings’), Dani Hafaca Publishing
House (in Hebrew).
Fruchtman, Maya, Orna Ben-Nathan, and Niva Shani. 2001. nivon ariel (‘Ariel dictionary of
idioms’), Korim Publishing House (in Hebrew).
Levanon, Moshe. 1981. lexikon ivri le-nivim ve-le-matbe’ot lašon (‘Hebrew lexicon for idioms and
idiomatic phrases’), Zak Publishing House (in Hebrew).
Rosenthal, Rubik. 2005. milon ha-sleng ha-makif (‘The comprehensive slang dictionary’), Keter
Publishing House (in Hebrew).
Sévenier-Gabriel, Neri. 2004. Thesaurus of Idioms and Phrases: Hebrew – English – Hebrew,
Yavneh Publishing House.
Stern, Naftali. 1994. milon hapo’al (‘The verb dictionary’). Bar Ilan University Press.
J. Horvath and Tal Siloni /
Brill’s Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics 1 (2009) 283–310
305
Appendix
The appendix includes the samples of predicates used for the study. Each category of predicates is followed by examples of idioms of the relevant sort.
The predicates heading these idioms are marked in boldface in the preceding
list of predicates. The list of idioms does not include all the idioms collected
for each predicate but just one example. Usually, there are several to numerous
occurrences of the same predicate in various idioms. In the list of idioms the
gloss of existing idioms appears in parentheses, a nonexisting idiom and its
gloss appear between square brackets.
1. Unaccusative Verbs (with a Transitive Counterpart)
hit’azen ‘became balanced’; hit’amet ‘was verified’; hit’afšer ‘became possible’; ba
‘came’; hitbala ‘became worn out’; hitbarex ‘was blessed with’; nig’al ‘was freed’;
hitgabeš -‘became consolidated’; hitgaber ‘overcame, became stronger’; nigmar ‘ended, was over’; nidlak ‘got ignited, got lit’; hit’arex ‘became longer’;
hivri ‘became healthy’; nidbak ‘was stuck, was glued’; gadal ‘grew up,
increased’; hitgameš ‘got more flexible’; hitdarder ‘roll down’; nolad ‘was born’;
nosaf ‘was added to’; yaca ‘went outside, exited’; yarad ‘descended, went
down’; zinek ‘pounced, leaped forth’; xadar ‘penetrated’; xazar ‘returned’;
hitxil ‘started’; hexmir ‘worsened’; nexnak ‘was strangled’; hexrif ‘worsened’;
hitxareš; ‘became deaf ’; hitaltel ‘wandered, was tossed from side to side’; hutav
‘got improved’; nixnas ‘enter’; huxpal ‘was doubled’; nixšal ‘failed’; hitmotet
‘collapsed’; met ‘died’; na ‘moved’; histovev ‘turned around’; avar ‘passed’;
amad ‘stood’; ne’eram ‘piled up’; nafal ‘fell’; hithapex ‘turned over’; nifsak
‘stopped’; nifrad ‘separated from’; hifšir ‘melted’; nical ‘got saved’; hitkaša
‘hardened’; hikšiax ‘hardened’; neherag ‘got killed’; hitraxek ‘drew away from’;
hura ‘worsened’; niš’ar ‘stayed, remained’; hišxir -‘blackened’ šav ‘returned’;
hitpocec ‘exploded’; hištabeš ‘got disrupted’; hištaxrer ‘was released’; hištalev ‘fit
together’; hištaper ‘improved’
2. Unaccusative Verbs: Unique Idioms
ba al onšo (came on his+punishement); ‘was punished, got his just deserts’
[hevi et x al onšo ‘brought acc x on his+punishment’]; hitgaber ka-’ari (became
stronger like-lion) ‘became stronger in order to perform a task’ [higbir et x
306
J. Horvath and Tal Siloni /
Brill’s Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics 1 (2009) 283–310
ka-ari ‘strengthened acc x like-lion]; gadal pere (grew.unacc wild) ‘grew wild’
[gidel et x pere ‘brought up acc x wild’]; yaca le-x mi-kol ha-xorim (went out
to-x from-all the-holes) ‘had enough, had it up to here’ [hoci acc y le-x mi-kol
ha-xorim ‘took-out acc y to-x from-all the-holes’]; yarad le-omek ha-’inyan
(went down to-depth the-matter) ‘got to the bottom of matter’ [horid et x leomek ha-’inyan ‘lowered acc x to-depth the-matter’]; xazar al arba (returned
on four) ‘came crawling’ [hexzir et x al arba ‘returned.trans acc x on four’];
nixnas be-ovi ha-kora (entered in-thickness the-beam) ‘studied something well,
delved into something’ [hixnis et x be-ovi ha-kora ’ inserted acc x in-thickness
the-beam]; nixšal bi-lšono (failed in-his+tongue) ‘said the wrong thing’ [hixšil
et x bi-lšono ‘failed.trans acc x in-his+tongue’]; met al x (dies on x) ‘is crazy
about x’ [hemit et y al x ‘killed acc y on x’]; na va-nad (moved and-wandered)
‘a vagabond.adj’ [heni’a ve-henid ‘moved.trans and shook.trans]; histovev
sviv ha-zanav šel acmo (turned around the-tail of himself ) ‘became entangled,
encountered difficulties with regard to some issue’ [sovev et x sviv ha-zanav šel
acmo ‘turned.trans acc x around the-tail of himself ]; avar le-seder ha-yom
(passed to-the agenda) ‘returned to routine, ignored’ [he’evir et x le-seder hayom ‘pass.trans acc x to-the agenda]; ’amad al ha-mekax (stood on thepurchase) ‘bargained, negotiated a price’ [he’emid et x al ha-mekax ‘placed acc
x on the-purchase]; nafal beyn ha-kis’ot (fell between the-chairs) ‘was ignored,
fell between two stools’ [hipil et x beyn ha-kis’ot ‘dropped acc x between thechairs]; hithapex be-kivr-o (turned over in-grave-his) ‘turned over in his grave’
[hafax et x be-kivr-o ‘turned.trans acc x in-grave-his’]; nical be-or šin-av (was
saved in-skin teeth-his) ‘had a close call, had a close shave’ [hicil et x be-or
šin-av ‘saved acc x in-skin teeth-his’]; neherag al paxot mi-šve pruta (was+killed
on less than-worth cent) ‘miser’ [harag et x al paxot mi-šve pruta ‘killed acc x
on less than-worth cent]; niš’ar al til-o (stayed on mound-its) ‘remained standing, was not destroyed’ [hiš’ir et x al til-o ‘left acc x on mound-its’]; hišxiru
pan-av ke-šuley ha-kdera (blackened face-his like-edge pot) ‘he looked bad due
to sickness, sorrow, shame’ [hišxir et pan-av ke-šuley ha-kdera ‘blackened acc
face-his like-edge pot’] šav al akev-av (returned on heels-his) ‘turned back,
retraced his steps’ [hešiv et x al akev-av ‘returned.trans acc x on heels-his’];
hitpocec le-x ba-panim (exploded to-x in+the-face) ‘x’s hopes or plans were
shattered’ [pocec le-x et y ba-panim blew up to-x acc y in+the-face].
3. Transitive Verbs (with an Unaccusative Counterpart)
’izen ‘balanced’; ‘imet ‘verified’; ‘ifšer ‘made possible’; hevi ‘brought’; bila ‘wore
out’; berex ‘blessed’; ga’al ‘freed’; gibeš ‘consolidated’; higbir ‘strengthened’;
gamar ‘finished’; hidlik ‘lit’; he’erix ‘lengthened’; hivri ‘made healthy’; hidbik
J. Horvath and Tal Siloni /
Brill’s Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics 1 (2009) 283–310
307
‘glued’; higdil ‘enlarged’; higmiš -‘moderated’; dirder ‘caused to roll down a
slope’; holid ‘fathered’; hosif ‘added’; hoci ‘took out’; horid ‘lowered’; hiznik ‘advanced’; hexdir ‘inserted’; hexzir ‘returned’; hitxil ‘began’; hexmir ‘worsened’;
xanak ‘suffocated’; hexrif ‘worsened’; hexriš ‘silenced’; tiltel ‘rocked, tossed’;
heitiv ‘improved’; hixnis ‘inserted’; hixpil ‘doubled, multiplied’; hixšil ‘caused
to fail’; motet ‘destroyed, collapsed’; hemit ‘killed’; heni’a ‘caused to move’; sovev
‘turned’; he’evir ‘removed, transferred’; he’emid ‘placed, positioned’; he’erim
‘piled up’; hipil ‘dropped’; hafax ‘turned over’; hifsik ‘stopped’; hifrid ‘separated’; hifšir ‘melted’; hicil ‘rescued, saved’; hikša ‘hardened’; hikšiax ‘hardened’; harag ‘killed’; hirxik ‘distanced, moved away’; here’a ‘made worse’; hiš’ir
‘left, kept’; hišxir ‘blackened’; hešiv ‘returned’; pocec ‘destroyed, blew up’;
šibeš ‘disrupted’; šixrer ‘freed’; šilev ‘integrated’; šiper ‘improved’.
4. Transitive Verbs: Unique Idioms
hevi le- x et ha-sa’if (brought to-x acc clause) ‘annoyed, upset’ [ha-sa’if ba le-x
‘the clause came to x]; berex al ha-mugmar (blessed on the-completed)
‘rejoiced at the completion of ’ [hitbarex al ha-mugmar ‘was blessed with the
completion of ’]; gamar omer (finished utterance) ‘made up his mind, reached
a decision’ [omer nigmar ‘utterance finished.unacc’]; hidbik et x la-kise (glued
acc x to+the chair ‘‘fascinated x’ [nidbak la-kise ‘got+glued to+the-chair’ (see
note 5)]; higdil roš (enlarged head) ‘‘took initiative and responsibilities more
than expected or required’ [rošo gadal his+head enlarged.unacc’]; hosif šemen
la-medura (added oil to+the-fire) ‘added fuel to the fire, aggravated the situation’ [šemen nosaf/hitvasef la-medura ‘oil got added to+the-fire’]; hoci bišvil x
et ha-armonim min ha-eš (took-out for x acc the-chestnuts from the-fire ‘did
a difficult or unpleasant job for x’ [ha-armonim yac’u le-x min ha-eš ‘thechestnuts went-out to x from the-fire’]; horid bifney x et ha-kova (lowered in
the presence of x acc the-hat) ‘took his hat off to x’ [ha-kova yarad bifney x
‘the-hat lowered.unacc in the presence of x’]; hexzir le-x (returned to-x)
‘repayed x in kind’ [xazar le-x ‘returend.unacc to x’]; heytiv et libo (improved
acc heart-his) ‘enjoyed himself ’ [libo hutav ‘heart-his improved]; hixnis le-x
milim la-pe (inserted to-x words to+the-mouth) ‘put words in x’s mouth’ [nixnesu le-x milim la-pe ‘entered to-x words to+the-mouth’]; sovev et x be-kaxaš
(turned acc x in-lie) ‘cheated x’ [histovev be-kaxaš ‘turned.unacc in-lie’];
he’evir et x al da’at-o (transferred acc x on mind-his) ‘drove x mad’ [avar al
da’a-to ‘passed. unacc on mind-his’]; he’emid panim (placed face) ‘pretend’
[pan-av amdu ‘face-his stood’]; hipil xitit-o al x (drop fear-his on x) ‘frighten’
[xitit-o nafla al x ‘fear-his fell on x’]; hafax šulxanot (turned over tables)
‘threatened violence’ [šulxanot hithapxu ‘tables turned-over.unacc’]; hicil et
308
J. Horvath and Tal Siloni /
Brill’s Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics 1 (2009) 283–310
ha-macav (saved acc the-situation) ‘salvaged a situation, saved the day’ [hamacav nical ‘the-situation was+saved’]; hikša et orp-o (hardened acc nape-his)
‘became stubborn’ [orp-o hitkaša ‘nape-his hardened.unacc’]; hikšiax et lib-o
(hardened acc heart-his) ‘became stubborn’ [lib-o hitkašeax/kašax ‘heart-his
hardened.unacc’]; harag zman (killed time) ‘killed time’ [ha-zman neherag
‘the-time was+killed’]; hiš’xir et pan-av (blackened acc face-his) ‘ruined his
reputation’ [hiš’xiru pan-av ‘had a ruined reputation’]; hešiv le-x ki-gmul-o
(returned to+x as-reward-his ‘repayed x in kind’ [šav le-x ki-gmul-o ’ returned.
unacc to-x as-reward-his’]; pocec et x be-makot (blew-up acc x in-blows) ‘beat
the hell out of x’ [hitpocec be-makot ‘blew-up.unacc in-blows’]; šixrer kitor
(released steam) ‘let out steam, said what he felt’ [kitor hištaxrer ‘steam
released.unacc].
5. Transitive and Unaccusative Shared Idioms (List of Predicates 1 and 3)
ba la-olam (came to+the-world) ‘was born’, hevi et x la-olam (brought acc x
to+the-world) ‘delivered x’; hidlik le-x nura aduma (lighted to-x bulb red) ‘was
a warning sign for x’, nidleka le-x nura aduma (got-lit to-x bulb red) ‘x sensed
a warning sign’; he’erix yamim (lengthened days) ‘lived long’, arxu yam-av
(lengthened.unacc days-his) ‘lived long’; hoci et x me-ha-kelim (take out acc
x from-the-dishes) ‘made x mad, drove x crazy’, yaca me-ha-kelim (went out
from-the-dishes) ‘got very angry, got furious’; horid et x le-timyon (lowered
acc x to-treasure) ‘threw x down the drain’, yarad le-timyon (went down totreasure) ‘down the drain’; hexzir et x la-mutav (returned acc x to+the-better)
‘made x return to the straight and narrow’; xazar la-mutav (returned to+thebetter) ‘returned to the straight and narrow’; hitxil et x be-regel smol (began
acc x in-foot left) ‘began x poorly / with bad luck’, hitxil be-regel smol (began
in-foot left) ‘began poorly / with bad luck’; hixnis et x la-tmuna (let+in acc x
to+the-picture) ‘brought x into the picture, let x in on a matter’, nixnas latmuna (entered to+the-picture) ‘got into the picture, became involved in the
matter’; sovev le-x et ha-roš (turned to-x acc the-head) ‘confused x’, histovev
le-x ha-roš (turned around to-x the-head) ‘x got confused’; he’emid et x al ta’uto (stood.trans acc x on mistake-his) ‘showed x wrong’, x amad ‘al tau’-to (x
stood on mistake-his) ‘x realized he (x) had made a mistake’; hipil et x ba-pax
(fell.trans acc x in-the-bin) ‘tricked x’, nafal ba-pax (fell in-the-bin) ‘x was
tricked’; hafax et ha-ke’ara ‘al pih-a (turned acc the-bowl on face-its) ‘x changed
the situation completely’, ha-ke’ara hithapxa al pih-a (the-bowl turned on
face-its) ‘the situation has completely changed’; hifšir et ha-kerax (melt.trans
acc the-ice) ‘x broke the ice’, ha-kerax hifšir (the-ice melted) ‘the ice broke’;
J. Horvath and Tal Siloni /
Brill’s Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics 1 (2009) 283–310
309
hiš’ir et-x ba-avir (left acc x in-the-air) ‘left x high and dry’, niš’ar ba-avir
(stayed in-the-air) ‘x was left high and dry’; hešiv et x le-eytan-o (returned.
trans acc x to-strength-his) ‘recuperated x’, šav le-eytan-o (returns to-strengthhis) ‘x recuperated’; pocec le-x et ha-rosh (exploded.trans to-x acc the-head)
‘annoyed/bothered x)’; hitpocec le-x ha-rosh (exploded to-x the-head) ‘x is
annoyed/bothered’.
6. Adjectival Passives
avud ‘lost’; ahuv ‘loved’; ahud ‘admired’; me’uvrar ‘ventilated’; me’uzan ‘balanced’; axuz ‘held’; me’uyaš ‘manned’; me’uxzav ‘disappointed’; axul ‘eaten’;
me’uxlas ‘populated’; me’ulac ‘forced’; me’ultar ‘improvised’; me’uman ‘trained’;
me’umac ‘strenuous’; me’umat ‘verified’; asur ‘forbidden’; afuf ‘immersed’;
me’upar ‘made-up (cosmetics)’; me’urgan ‘organized’; aruz ‘packed’; me’ušpaz
‘hospitalized’; me’ušar ‘confirmed’; mevo’ar ‘annotated’; baduy ‘fabricated’;
baduk ‘checked’; baluy ‘worn out’; banuy ‘built’; mevusas ‘established’; mevucar ‘fortified’; mevukar ‘controlled’; megubaš ‘consolidated’; megudal ‘grown’;
megohac ‘ironed’; meguvan ‘varied’; gazuz ‘cut’; gazur ‘cut’; meguyas ‘conscripted’; galuy ‘revealed’; gamur ‘finished’; ganuv ‘stolen’; megune ‘disgusting’; garus ‘ground’; daxuy ‘rejected’; meduka ‘depressed’; daluk ‘lit’; dafuk
‘defective’ (lost original meaning); medukdak ‘exact’; darus ‘be run over’;
daruš ‘required’; mu’afal ‘darkened’; mu’arax ‘lengthened’; muvtax ‘promised’;
muvan ‘understood’; mugbar ‘increased’; mugdal ‘enlarged’; mugdar ‘defined’;
mudbak ‘glued’; davuk ‘glued’; mudgaš ‘emphasized’; mudxak ‘repressed’.
7. Adjectival Passives: Unique Idioms18
axuz xerev (held sword) ‘wariror.adj’ [axaz xerev ‘held.trans sword’]; axul
ve-šatuy (eaten and drunk) ‘ate and drank to the point of satisfaction’ [axal
ve-šata ‘ate and drank’]; aruz ve-muxan (packed and-prepared) ‘all ready’19
[araz ve-hexin ‘packed.trans and-prepared .trans’]; baduk ve-menuse (checked
and-tried) ‘well-tested, effective’ [badak ve-nisa ‘checked.trans and-tried.
18
Interestingly, most of the idioms collected in this category are idioms headed by an adjectival passive in the pa’ul template. We discuss this phenomenon in work in progress.
19
The use is not specific to luggage or being packaged. The idiom is used with regard to things
which are ready in every detail, e.g., fully cooked food, etc.
310
J. Horvath and Tal Siloni /
Brill’s Annual of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics 1 (2009) 283–310
trans’]; mevucar be-emdato (fortified in-his+position) ‘stubborn’ [bicer et x
be-emdato ‘fortified.trans acc x in-his+position’]; gazur al x (cut on x) ‘loves
x’ [gazar y al x ‘cut.trans y on x’]; galuy ve-yadu’a (revealed and-known) ‘well
known’ [gila ve-yada et x ‘revealed.trans and-knew acc x’]; ganuv al x (stolen
on x) ‘loves x’ [ganav/higniv et y al x (stole/sneaked in acc y on x)];20 daluk al
x (lit on x) ‘has a crush on x’ [hidlik et y al x (lit acc y on x)]; dafuk ba-roš
(knocked in+the-head) ‘stupid’ [dafak et x ba-roš ‘knocked.trans acc x in+thehead’]; drusat ‘iš (be run over.fem man) ‘not a virgin (about a woman)’ [‘iš
daras x.fem ‘man ran over x.fem); muvan me-el-av (understood from-to-x)
‘self evident’ [hevinu et x me-’el-av (understood.trans acc x from-to-x)];
davuk la-kise (glued to+the-chair) ‘holding on to one’s position’ [hidbik et x
la-kise (glued.trans acc x to+the-chair) ‘fascinated him’].
20
Both ganuv al (stolen on x) ‘loves x’ and the following idiom daluk al x (lit on x) ‘has a crush
on x’ have a transitive version excluding the preposition al ‘on’. We take the preposition to be
part of the idiom, and therefore consider these idioms unique.