John Benjamins Publishing Company
his is a contribution from Narrative Inquiry 22:1
© 2012. John Benjamins Publishing Company
his electronic ile may not be altered in any way.
he author(s) of this article is/are permitted to use this PDF ile to generate printed copies to
be used by way of ofprints, for their personal use only.
Permission is granted by the publishers to post this ile on a closed server which is accessible
to members (students and staf) only of the author’s/s’ institute, it is not permitted to post
this PDF on the open internet.
For any other use of this material prior written permission should be obtained from the
publishers or through the Copyright Clearance Center (for USA: www.copyright.com).
Please contact
[email protected] or consult our website: www.benjamins.com
Tables of Contents, abstracts and guidelines are available at www.benjamins.com
Narrativity and involvement
in online consumer reviews
he case of TripAdvisor
Camilla Vásquez
University of South Florida
Drawing on recent work on digital narratives of personal experience in online genres such as email, social networking sites, and blogs, the present study
explores narrative features in 100 online consumer reviews of hotels. Focusing
on negative reviews, or “Rants,” from popular consumer travel platform,
TripAdvisor, the article examines both canonical and genre-speciic structural features of narratives, as well as some of the discursive resources used by
online narrators to engage their audiences, and to draw them into their stories.
Speciically, the study explores the use of story prefaces and related forms of second person address, represented speech and mental states, and deictic shits, and
suggests that narrative features such as these are useful in attracting the attention
of an audience amidst a vast universe of online information.
Keywords: digital narratives, narratives of personal experience, consumer
reviews, TripAdvisor, CMC, eWOM
Introduction
he past decade has seen a major shit in narrative studies, particularly with respect to a growing recognition of the diversity of narrative types and narrative
activities. Several scholars have turned their attention from “large,” autobiographical narratives — of the type that are oten elicited in research interviews — to
“small stories,” which occur spontaneously in quotidian contexts, and in virtually every domain of “non-interviewed life” (Freeman, 2007, p. 156). While the
Requests for further information should be directed to: Camilla Vásquez, Department of World
Languages, University of South Florida, 4202 E. Fowler Avenue, CPR 419, Tampa, FL 33620.
Email:
[email protected]
Narrative Inquiry 22:1 (2012), 105–121. doi 10.1075/ni.22.1.07vas
issn 1387–6740 / e-issn 1569–9935 © John Benjamins Publishing Company
106 Camilla Vásquez
canonical Labovian oral narrative of personal experience (i.e., irst-person, pastfocused stories of non-shared events) is still alive and continuing to thrive in
narrative research, some have argued that it is time for narrative researchers to
consider a much broader range of narrative activities and types (Bamberg, 2007;
Georgakopoulou, 2007a, 2007b), which may include “tellings of ongoing events,
future or hypothetical events, shared (known) events” (Georgakopoulou, 2007b,
p. 146) among others. Coincidentally, this analytic focus on small stories comes
at a moment in history when more narrative activities are taking place in online
contexts than ever before.
he ield of narrative analysis has only recently begun to catch up with
trends and developments in information and communication technologies.
Georgakopoulou (2006, 2007a) was one of the irst narrative scholars to examine
narratives in a previously unexplored digital genre: email. Research on digital narratives of personal experience is continuing to grow in other online genres as well.
For example, Myers (2010) has examined narrative features in blogs and wikis.
And Page (2010), in her analysis of narratives in Facebook status updates, has
challenged us to consider how newer forms of social media and digital communication are reshaping key concepts in narrative. For instance, both Myers and Page
have identiied the digital timestamp as a key element in constructing temporality
in online narratives. Furthermore, they have shown how digital narrators in these
CMC contexts share a preoccupation with “nowness” (Myers, p. 69) and recency,
as opposed to pastness and relection. More recently, Page (2012), has examined
aspects of narrativity in various digital genres, ranging from identity on microblogging sites to the construction of place in a digital narratives project.
One online genre in which digital narratives of personal experience have
not yet been explored is that of online consumer reviews. Online consumer reviews, sometimes referred to by marketing scholars as eWOM (electronic word
of mouth), is a genre that continues to grow in both popularity and inluence.
Narratives in online consumer reviews have been previously attested by Pollach
(2006). Pollach, in her study, which examined the generic features of online reviews, found that the 358 reviews of digital cameras she analyzed were comprised
of “comments, and evaluations, and personal stories (e.g., weddings, vacations,
christenings) involving the products reviewed” (p. 4, my emphasis). he present
study focuses on narratives found in online reviews of a diferent type: reviews of
hotels featured on travel website, TripAdvisor. More speciically, this study examines the ways in which reviewers address, reach out to, and engage with the readers
of their narratives. his issue is a relevant one, given the overwhelming amount
of information confronting today’s Internet users, as they participate in an online
environment consisting of millions of posts, tweets, uploads, updates, and so on.
In such an environment, where the trend of increased interactivity has been on the
© 2012. John Benjamins Publishing Company
All rights reserved
Narrativity and involvement in online consumer reviews 107
rise for several years (i.e., Web 2.0), digital narrators must ind ways of engaging
and connecting with their unknown audience. herefore, because my focus here
is on what authors do to engage their audience in their narratives, I discuss the
following features that are commonly associated with involvement in discourse:
story prefaces, reported speech and mental states, and deictic (i.e., pronoun and
tense) shits.
he notion of involvement has interested discourse analysts for nearly three
decades (e.g., Chafe, 1982; Lakof, 1990; Tannen, 1999). Early treatments of involvement (Chafe, 1982; Tannen, 1985) tended to approach the topic from the
perspective of diferences between orality and literacy. Spoken discourse was
characterized as featuring more strategies of involvement, as opposed to written
discourse, which was conversely viewed as more “detached” (e.g., Chafe, 1982).
Consequently, involvement strategies — including reported speech, use of second-person address, and so forth — have traditionally been associated more with
speech than with writing (especially written academic registers). Besnier (1994)
critiques the aforementioned body of scholarship for its lack of precision in deining involvement, yet he usefully provides a number of general descriptors, all of
which are perhaps subsumed under this broad construct. hese include:
–
–
–
–
–
–
–
“the attention that speakers pay […] to the needs of their interlocutor” (p. 280)
“the interactional aspects of communication” (p. 280)
“the cooperative construction of discourse and the display of positive afect
toward interlocutors” (p. 287)
“conversationalists willingness and ability to initiate and sustain verbal interaction” (p. 279)
the creation/display of “engagement” (p. 281)
“interpersonal dynamics (e.g., the maintenance of a good rapport among participants)” (p. 289)
and, referencing Tannen (1999, p. 12): an “emotional connection individuals
feel” toward others (p. 281)
For the purpose of the present study “involvement” is understood as consisting of
a range of discursive resources that index some type of connection or interaction
among participants — in this case, between authors of online reviews and the
readers of those reviews.
It has also been noted (e.g., Besnier, 1994; Tannen, 1984, 1985) that involvement in discourse relies not only on language, but also on various “nonlinguistic
cues, such as facial expressions, gestures, and intonation” (Besnier, p. 281). Because
these types of non-linguistic resources are unavailable in text-based digital contexts, the phenomenon of involvement may have somewhat diferent realizations
in asynchronous computer-mediated contexts. herefore it is worth considering
© 2012. John Benjamins Publishing Company
All rights reserved
108 Camilla Vásquez
some of the resources that narrators use to establish an interpersonal connection
with their audience throughout their digital narratives of personal experience.
Methods
he data for this study of narratives in online consumer reviews consist of a purposeful sample of 100 negative reviews from TripAdvisor, the most prominent
online travel review platform (Zehrer et al., 2011). According to a recent tourism industry study (Yoo & Gretzel, 2009), more than 80% of travelers are currently consulting sites such as TripAdvisor, which means that the inluence of
such internet-based consumer reviews is potentially powerful and far-reaching
(e.g., Briggs, Sutherland & Drummond, 2007; Chung & Buhalis, 2008; Miguéns,
Baggio & Costa, 2008; O’Connor, 2008; Yoo & Gretzel, 2009). Travel review sites
are especially popular, according to Zehrer et al. (2011), because tourism products
represent a type of “experience good” (i.e., a type of product that cannot be easily
assessed prior to consumption). In order to reduce risk and uncertainty, consumers seek information and advice from those who have already experienced the
tourism product (i.e., hotel) for themselves; oten, this information comes packaged in the form of a irst-person digital narrative of personal experience. In a
world characterized by increased mobility and growing interconnectivity, it has
become common to rely on the eWOM of strangers, which can be freely and easily
accessed on websites comprised of enormous user-generated databases. It is even
possible that these online sources of travel information have surpassed in inluence their more traditional, analogue counterparts (e.g., guidebooks, suggestions
from friends and families).
Negative reviews were selected for analysis because, according to prior research (Ricci and Wietsma, 2006; Sen and Lerman, 2007), online audiences pay
more attention to negative reviews than to positive ones. Each week on its homepage, TripAdvisor showcases approximately ive of “the best” and the same number of “the worst” hotel reviews: this section of the website is labeled “Rants and
Raves.” Over a six month period (November 2008–April 2009), once per week,
the website’s featured “Rants” (i.e., negative reviews) were downloaded and saved,
until a data set of 100 hotel reviews was created. hat these particular reviews have
been selected by the website to be showcased on the “Rants” section indicates that
they have somehow been deemed as highly “tellable” accounts.1
he data set of 100 reviews is comprised of nearly 30,000 words. he average
review is approximately 300 words in length, with the shortest around 50 words
in length and the longest approximately 2,000 words. TripAdvisor ofers reviewers
an opportunity to construct a proile, which allows them to provide demographic
© 2012. John Benjamins Publishing Company
All rights reserved
Narrativity and involvement in online consumer reviews 109
information about themselves; though, of course, not all authors of reviews choose
to provide this information. Nevertheless, from the available demographic proile data, a slight majority of reviewers were female, between the ages of 35–49,
and traveled for leisure as opposed to business. his aggregate proile is consistent
with what was reported in an earlier study of TripAdvisor reviewer demographics
(Gretzel, 2007). Approximately 70% of the reviewers resided in the United States
or the United Kingdom, and the top destinations of hotels included cities in those
same countries, as well as in Italy, Australia, and India. he topics of the negative
reviews ranged from cleanliness, size, and condition of the room; location, price,
security, and customer service in the hotel; and restaurant service and food quality. Most reviews addressed several of these categories.
Online hotel review stories
In the following section, I begin by providing a general description of the discrete
phases of the hotel experience. I then describe these narratives in terms of classic structural features, before turning to a closer analysis of features of audience
involvement.
Description of the structure of the hotel experience
A hotel stay is an extended temporal experience, and one which unfolds over several hours, days, or even weeks. his durative dimension of the experience no
doubt lends itself to a chronological sequencing of events, especially in those reviews which follow a linear narrative structure. he sequence of events in the hotel
Table 1. Stages of the Hotel Experience
1
Research and planning stage: this may include reading other reviews
2
Reserving/booking the room: this can be done online, through a travel agent, or by phone
3
First impression/encounter with hotel/staf: this can happen on-site or of-site
4
Check in
5
First encounter with the room
6
Other hotel-related activities including: further communication with staf; visit to restaurant; use of hotel amenities (such as beach, pool, massage, etc)
7
Check out
8
Follow-up communication with hotel
© 2012. John Benjamins Publishing Company
All rights reserved
110 Camilla Vásquez
stay consists of the following components, and narrative reviews oten follow some
variation of the basic chronological structure presented in Table 1.
It should be noted that not all reviewers will have experienced all of these
events during their stay. Only some of these phases are obligatory components of
the hotel visit experience (i.e., 3, 4, 5, 7). Furthermore, reviewers are also selective
in the information that they choose to include in a review. Whether they have experienced them or not, in their reviews some authors may include a discussion of
only some of these phases and not address other phases — even ones which they
may have experienced.
Cannonical narratives
From a formalist perspective, the reviews featured in the “Rants” section tend
to be quite canonical in their narrative structure. In other words, the majority
of negative reviews are clearly recognizable as narratives. Because the data set is
comprised exclusively of negative reviews, it is perhaps unsurprising that the narrative’s “complicating action” is basically a built-in feature of texts in this genre.
Moreover, in some sense, there is a single “master narrative” that is found in each
of the individual reviews — which is that some part(s) of the actual hotel experience contrasted with the reviewer’s expectations of the experience. his disjunct
between the reviewer’s expectations (Vásquez, 2011a) and the actual hotel experience lies at the core of all of the narratives in the data set.
Below, I use an example of one hotel review from the data set to illustrate what
a canonical narrative of personal experience looks like in this genre. he narrative
has been segmented below, in order to illustrate the traditional Labovian narrative
elements (Labov& Waletzky, 1967). Evaluation, which can appear anywhere within the narrative, is indicated below with unitalicized font. (Additional description
in the review has been deleted here due to space constraints.)
Excerpt 1: Canonical Narrative
Abstract
here were many bad omens that preceded our stay at the [resort
name].
Orientation
Firstly, I tried to arrange for transport from the panama airport
to the resort prior to our arrival. I called the [resort name], long
distance and spoke to seven diferent agents who continuously transferred me to another agent.
Complicating
Action
In the end, I was reassured that I would be met at the Panama City
Airport by a representative from the [resort name]. When my wife
and I arrived to Panama City, there was no [resort name] agent
there.
© 2012. John Benjamins Publishing Company
All rights reserved
Narrativity and involvement in online consumer reviews
hey put myself and my wife in a room with two double beds (not
exactly romantic) instead of a queen or king as we requested. he
shower was demon possessed and luctuated from scalding hot to
freezing cold every ten seconds. It was pretty awful.
Resolution
Ater taxes, my wife and I paid about $320 per night for this place.
his was an exorbitant rip of for what we received.
Coda
he only redeeming quality of this place was the nice pools and
beautiful beach… but you can ind this elsewhere for much much
cheaper.
he abstract is an optional component of narratives and when it appears it takes
the form of a summary statement, which introduces the general topic of the narrative. he above example does begin with an abstract (e.g., here were many bad
omens that preceded our stay at the [resort name]), however the majority of the narratives in the data set tend not to include an abstract. Similar to email narratives,
where the subject line oten functions as the abstract (Georgakopoulou, 2007),
these online reviews are prefaced with a “header,” which also serves as a sort of
abstract, and provides the overall gist or tone of the review.
he orientation section of the narrative, also optional, provides background
information about the story’s who, when, and where. In this genre of narratives,
it is very common to ind an orientation section. his is logical, given that stories about hotel stays are oten as much about the “wheres” as they are about the
“whats.” In this context, orientation takes on a special relevance. he orientation
component of online hotel reviews typically consists of reviewers’ reports of their
reasons for travel –in addition to when, where, and for how long — as well as
reference to their travel companions. (he ways in which these categories simultaneously function to index aspects of reviewers’ identities is discussed in Vásquez,
forthcoming). In addition, as can be seen in the example above, the orientation
segment in online hotel reviews may also consist of reports of planning activities
that took place prior to travel.
In terms of their primary function, online reviews are evaluative before they
are narrative. hat is, the main purpose of online consumer reviews is to rate, evaluate, describe, and, on that basis, to provide recommendations to others for — or
against –a particular product or service. herefore it is not surprising that 100% of
the examples in the dataset include some form of explicit evaluation. In structural
terms, evaluation is one of the two deining features of narratives, and it can occur
in any and all phases of the narrative. As seen in the example above, evaluation
appears frequently, is expressed using a variety of lexico-grammatical forms, and
can be found in every section of the narrative. his pervasiveness of evaluation is
quite typical of all of the narrative reviews in the data set.
© 2012. John Benjamins Publishing Company
All rights reserved
111
112
Camilla Vásquez
Along with evaluation, a complicating action is traditionally the other deining feature of a narrative. he complicating action is normally realized linguistically via a series of past-tense clauses that are sequentially ordered. In this genre,
the most highly narrative accounts tend to include forms of temporal deictic anchoring (e.g., upon arrival…, the next day…, ater that we went…, we were then
told…). Quite frequently, the types of actions that are reported in these narratives
are interactions and communications with hotel staf: a phenomenon that will be
discussed in further detail in a later section. While reports of actions and events
are not atypical, there is also another, more genre-speciic, type of “complicating
action” that appears in some of these narratives — and that difers slightly from the
canonical narrative. In several cases, rather than being comprised of actions and
events (as one would expect), the complicating action is instead built up through
negative descriptions. In other words, in a hotel review story, a simple listing of
unpleasant or negative characteristics (e.g., it is dirty and shabby, it smells, it is
gross, the service is bad) can constitute the complicating action: that these characteristics are in conlict with what the reviewer expected to ind, when let unstated,
remains inferable by the audience. To put it another way, in this particular context
and genre of narratives very oten it is the overall experience of place itself that comprises the complicating action.
he resolution serves as an end to the narrative events. Like the abstract and
orientation, this phase is optional, but when it is included in hotel review narratives it tends to be about leaving or checking out of the hotel — or, in some cases,
about requesting and/or receiving a refund. In the resolution phase of the narrative excerpt above, the reviewer makes indirect reference to checking out of the
hotel by mentioning the total cost of the hotel visit. Although this does represent a
form of closure for the narrative, in reviews that are negative, the resolution (more
oten than not) is not necessarily a positive ending.
he coda, which serves as a bridge from the story world to the current time
of telling, is also an optional element of a narrative. When a coda appears in hotel
review narratives, it nearly always takes the form of some type of advice, suggestion, warning, directive, or admonition. he coda is one of the sections of the narrative in which reviewers appeal directly to readers, commonly through the use of
second person pronouns, as seen in the above example: you can ind this elsewhere
for much much cheaper. I now examine several other resources used by reviewers
to appeal to, and engage with, their audiences: story prefaces, represented speech
and mental states, and deictic shits.
© 2012. John Benjamins Publishing Company
All rights reserved
Narrativity and involvement in online consumer reviews
Story prefaces and other second person references
Conversation analysts have identiied the “story preface” (Sacks, 1974) as a resource for interlocutors to negotiate a longer turn at talk, and to secure an extended holding of the conversational loor. Strictly speaking, this is unnecessary
in an asynchronous CMC context, where there is no conversational loor to be negotiated by participants who are not co-present, and where a participant’s “e-turn”
can be as short or as lengthy as they want it to be. Nevertheless, story prefaces —
which, in this context, index the dynamics of face-to-face interaction — do appear
in some hotel review stories, as can be seen in the examples in excerpts 2:
Excerpts 2: Story Prefaces
We just returned, and let me tell you… <94>
Let me start at the beginning… <59>
Where should I begin?… <19>
he story preface, a convention of oral narratives, is thus carried over into a digital
context. In an asynchronous online narrative, a story preface becomes a symbolic
resource, in which the narrator simulates the negotiation of tellership and “speaking rights” with an unknown, non-present audience. In doing so, the narrator
acknowledges the importance of the audience’s active participation in the reading and reception of the narrative. In the three examples above, narrators open
their narratives by drawing their audiences into their stories through the use of resources such as a second person pronominal address form, a verb with an implied
second person subject, and the posing of a rhetorical question.
he story preface is only one of the points within the narrative where reviewers can use second person address to appeal directly to their audience. Similar
types of features and constructions can also appear later in the narrative. he examples in excerpts 3 below rely on similar features (i.e., second person pronouns,
questions) as those in the story preface, in order to engage the audience and to
create a sense of connection and interaction.
Excerpts 3:
Well, what can I say… <15>
Can you believe that… <10>
Why didn’t we move you may ask… <22>
Reported speech and mental states
Another way in which narrators make their stories come alive to their audiences
is through the use of represented speech as well as represented mental states.2 he
© 2012. John Benjamins Publishing Company
All rights reserved
113
114 Camilla Vásquez
use of such “constructed dialogue” (Tannen, 1989) in narratives — when narrators
perform or illustrate what happened, rather than merely tell or describe it — recruits an audience’s interest by helping to bring about a sense of vividness and
veracity. Besnier (1994) explains that constructed dialogue appeals to the audience
to engage in the meaning-making process:
Making sense of a reported dialogue thus requires the active participation of both
speakers and interlocutors, and hence drats the involvement of all concerned
participants in the process of constructing linguistic and interactional meaning. Reported dialogues exemplify one range of involvement strategies, namely
strategies which depend on the collaboration of interlocutors in the derivation of
meaning from form (p. 280).
Traditionally, constructed dialogue has been closely associated with spoken
genres. However, as can be seen in the examples below, not solely restricted to oral
narratives, constructed dialogue is also a feature that appears in digital narratives
of personal experience.
Excerpt 4a
i asked (nicely) the housekeeping lady for an extra pillow, she glared at me and rudely said: “NO!!! NO PILLOW!!! NO!!!” <90>
Excerpt 4b
he staf were without exception surly, rude and unfriendly. When I greet someone
with “Good Morning”, I don’t expect to have “Room number?” snapped back at me.
<23>
In oral narratives, the tone of a represented utterance (or thought, or emotion) is
oten communicated paralinguistically, by a shit in prosody, or nonlinguistically,
through facial expression and gesture. Due to the absence of these resources in
an asynchronous computer-mediated environment, some reviewers, like those in
excerpts 4a and b, instead comment metadiscursively on the reported speech —
through the use of adverbs (rudely) and descriptive speech act verbs (snapped) —
in order to convey not just what was said during an interaction with a hotel staf
member, but also how it was said. As seen in example 4a, orthographic emphasis
can also be used to indicate non-linguistic information about the tone of the interaction.
When describing their interactions, reviewers oten depict their responses to
the utterances of others via representations of their own thoughts or inner feelings.
In Excerpt 5a, the discursive portrayal of the reviewer’s internal afective state (I
felt like crying out ok? How could this ever be in the vicinity of ok?) occurs as a follow-up response to the inquiry of the hotel staf member, which is itself depicted as
a semi-reported speech construction (e.g., he irst checked that ‘everything was ok.’).
© 2012. John Benjamins Publishing Company
All rights reserved
Narrativity and involvement in online consumer reviews
Excerpt 5a
A silver-suited gentleman brought our bags to the room, he irst checked that ‘everything was ok.’ I felt like crying out ok? How could this ever be in the vicinity of ok?
he stench of dampness hit our nostrils 20 paces before we reached the room. <49>
Excerpt 5b
When I irst walked into the hotel I thought, yeah this isn’t as bad as some of the
reviews I’d read.. So I was quite happy I’d picked it. <67>
A key diference here between representations of speech and representations of
mental states is that whereas reviewers tend to use quotation marks to represent
the former (e.g., in 5a, he irst checked that ‘everything was ok.’), they do not do so
for the latter. However, the syntactic frames (I felt like crying out… and I thought…)
as well as the spatial deictic this in both of the above examples (yeah this isn’t as
bad as some of the reviews I’d read and ok? How could this ever be in the vicinity of
ok?) indicate that these constructions are representations of the reviewers’ mental
states, which ostensibly took place during the same time as the events in the narrative. In this context, communicating one’s cognitive or afective states (which
are normally “hidden” or unavailable to others not experiencing them) serves as
a metaphorical way of granting readers privileged access to the internal workings
of the mind of the reviewer, thus forging another type of relational connection
between narrator and audience.
he example below, a segment from a relatively lengthy review, illustrates how
in this particular narrative, the complicating action is actually built up as a dialogic
exchange, as was mentioned earlier. Excerpt 6 contains six instances of attributed
speech indicated by quotation marks, as they appear in the original text, as well
as 15 instances of speech act verbs (e.g., ask, say, tell) and their related nominalizations (e.g., explanation, response), which are highlighted in bold below. his
particular narrative is constructed as a representation of an extended conversation
that took place between the reviewer, her husband and the hotel staf. he reported
speech has an obviously evaluative function in this narrative, as does the information which the reviewer places in parenthesis.
Excerpt 6
Again my husband went to reception to make them aware and ask for it to be put
right. he girl on reception, to our absolute disbelief, said that this was not possible
and the “solution” to our problem was to speak to the manager on Monday (we were
only staying until Sunday morning and live in the UK so we’re not sure how this was
an appropriate “solution”). Her original explanation was that “they only made the
bed that had been slept in”. As I pointed out even if only one bed had been slept in
and they made this, that would make two fully made beds.
© 2012. John Benjamins Publishing Company
All rights reserved
115
116 Camilla Vásquez
We also asked about the change of room and were told that in order to have done
this, we would have to have checked out of the hotel at 11am “so they could clean and
re-sell our old room” (well I hope the new people wouldn’t have minded one bed not
being made…) and that because we had not done this, we could not change. At no
point when my husband asked on Friday night were we told this.
he girl on reception was incredibly rude and became rather aggressive. We asked to
speak to the manager and were told “you can’t speak to him it’s Saturday aternoon”.
We were both astounded at the way we were being treated as paying guests. Her
constant response to our (very reasonable) requests and questions was “this is not
a ive star hotel”. hat was quite evident and we booked fully aware of this but at a
minimum you would expect the place to be clean. <25>
he agentless passive construction, we were told, which appears a few times in excerpt 6, is a typical way in which this type of information is packaged in the larger
set of hotel review narratives — with the focal emphasis on the reviewer (i.e., we)
and on the message itself, rather than on the speciic representative of the hotel
staf member doing the communicating. Excerpt 6 ends with a deictic shit from
irst person to second person reference (i.e., we booked…but at a minimum you
would expect the place to be clean), which is the topic of the next section.
Deictic shits
Deictic shits — combinations of pronoun and tense shits, more speciically —
function to extend, or to universalize, a particular experience to others. Various
uses of you (generic, referring more speciically to addressee, etc.) have been documented in other online narrative genres (Page, 2012), as the reader is projected
into, and thus included, in the narrator’s experience. In examples 7a and b, the
narrators switch between irst person we to second person you. In both excerpts,
the second person forms co-occur with hypothetical constructions (i.e., if-clauses,
and modals, such as could).
Excerpt 7a
If you are strong swimmers (which we are) you could keep yourself warm by constantly swimming, but the water temperature was not suitable for just having a bit
of fun in. <28>
Excerpt 7b
he main thing that we disliked the most was that the place had no “parlor” or a
place that the guests could congregate. When you came back at night the only thing
there was to do was go up to your room, you could not meet the other guests because
there was no place to meet them, the dining room was closed. heir website is terribly
misleading and I’ve had better times at Days Inn… <26>
© 2012. John Benjamins Publishing Company
All rights reserved
Narrativity and involvement in online consumer reviews
Like 7a and b, examples 8a and b also include shits from irst to second person reference, some of which also co-occur with modals. However, the following
examples are noteworthy because the authors take their shit in perspective one
step further. In example 8a, the second person reference occurs with present tense
verbs (e.g., you are let standing… then are given), which has the efect of adding the reader into a situation that was actually only experienced by the narrator.
Interestingly, the narrator in 8a also uses the passive form of the verbs stand and
give, which further projects the reader as someone to whom things are being done.
In addition, both examples combine second person reference with verb forms expressing futurity (i.e., You will receive one hand towel … in 8a; Your cell phone will
not work there… in 8b). Combined personal and temporal deictic shits, such as
these, project the reader into the future — positioning him/her as an eventual
guest of the hotel — and are interpolated into the otherwise past-time-oriented
events of the irst-person narrative.
Excerpt 8a
When I asked when we would be leaving, he just shrugged and sat there for 15
minutes more. All this for the 5 minute ride to the hotel, which initially looks good.
But the terror continues at check in, where you are let standing ignored for another
half hour, then are given a room as though they were doing you a favor. he rooms
themselves are bare in the extreme. You will receive one hand towel — that’s it. No
printed material informing you of hotel regs, services or anything. I tried repeatedly
to call the front desk for a wake up call and inally had to go down, where again I
was ignored… <42>
Excerpt 8b
he bathroom has a large jetted tub. It was not very clean. It was scummy. You can
see that in the pictures on their website. he other problem with the tub is that there
was not ever enough hot water to ill it. he water pressure is terrible and the water
is heated by a wood burning boiler. It takes two hours to get enough water in the tub
to reach the jets. hat is lukewarm water at best. In the morning you better shower
quickly if you want the warm water, which is hard to do with the low water pressure.
About half the time there was no hot water at all. Your cell phone will not work there.
here is a phone in the room but fairly oten when I picked it up someone else would
be on it. here is supposed to be wireless internet but this would come and go and
was very spotty. he same for the TV. he signal would fade in and out. You couldn’t
really watch anything… <54>
hese four narrative excerpts reveal a continuum of perspective shiting: from
more generic uses of you (which could be substituted by one), co-occuring with
modals or other hypothetical constructions, to those uses of you that appear to
refer to the reader more directly (i.e., those co-occurring with present and future
verbs). he latter create a stronger sense of involvement, with the narrator actually
© 2012. John Benjamins Publishing Company
All rights reserved
117
118
Camilla Vásquez
placing the reader in the midst of events that were only and uniquely experienced
by the narrator. Deictic shits within narratives serve a rhetorical function, as they
move uniquely- and subjectively-experienced events to a broader level of shared
experience. Additionally, in this context, they communicate a warning message
akin to: “his is what happened to me, and if you choose to stay here, this will
also happen to you.” Future and hypothetical narratives have been documented in
other digital genres, such as in blogs (Myers, 2010) and in email (Georgakopoulou,
2007). However, to my knowledge, this placing of the reader in the center of narrated activity — within what is otherwise a relatively canonical, past-tense, nonshared, irst person narrative of personal experience — has not been previously
discussed in work on digital narratives.
Conclusions
It has been argued (Page, 2010) that digital genres and online environments are
introducing new constraints as well as new afordances for narrative activities.
Page (2010, 2012) has shown, for example, how narrative elements such as temporality and sequence are being reworked in social media sites, such as Facebook
and Twitter. In addition, explorations of narratives in other online genres
(Georgakopoulou, 2007; Myers, 2010) have also noted the predominant emphasis
on recency, which clearly contributes to the reshaping of narrative in these CMC
contexts. However, digital stories may take many diferent forms across diverse
online contexts. he TripAdvisor review narratives discussed here can legitimately
be considered “small stories” in the sense that they are unelicited, and they do
not refer to major autobiographical or landmark events. Yet, at the same time, it
is evident that these small stories are “larger”– and certainly more canonical in
terms of their structure — than the small stories told in Facebook status updates
(Page, 2010), or on Twitter (Page, 2012). One limitation of the present study is its
exclusive focus on negative reviews, which (as noted earlier) have a sort of “builtin” complicating action. herefore, the extent to which positive reviews (as well as
online reviews of other kinds of consumer goods and services) are narrative, and
the ways in which they difer in their narrative features from negative reviews,
remain questions awaiting further study.
Georgakopoulou (2007) has observed that interesting forms of intertextuality
can emerge when narrators share personal experiences by telling stories both in
face-to-face conversations as well as in email exchanges. he present study, in contrast, has focused on digital narratives of personal experience in an arena where
narrators are not likely to know the people who read their stories. However, this
unknown audience does not result in an absence of features of involvement that
© 2012. John Benjamins Publishing Company
All rights reserved
Narrativity and involvement in online consumer reviews 119
are typically associated with conversational narratives. On the contrary, the analysis indicates that reviewers are aware of their audience as they write their stories,
and that they use multiple discursive resources to engage their readers, to appeal
to them directly, and to draw them into their narrative — in spite of the fact that,
in this context, narrators and audience are unknown to one another, and are separated by both space and time.
he rise and ubiquity of social media has opened the possibilities for anyone
with internet connectivity to post online their opinions about virtually any topic.
Consumer reviews represent an enormous and steadily growing genre of online
discourse: there are over 50 million reviews on the TripAdvisor website alone. Such
quantities of information are staggering, and authors of reviews are clearly aware
of this reality, and understand that they are competing with many others reviewers for the attention of a potentially vast, though simultaneously indeterminate,
audience. here are many ways that reviewers can appeal to the attention of their
potential audience. One of these is to package their experience in a narrative form.
In addition, they can make their narrative a highly tellable one, through the use
of the discourse features illustrated in this study (story prefaces, constructed dialogue, deictic shits), as well as others, such as humor, vivid detail, and so forth. As
more and more of our time is spent mediated by various interactive technologies,
the more frequently our narratives of personal experience will take digital, rather
than oral, forms — making digital narratives of personal experience the focus of
further anticipated narrative research in the future.
Notes
1. In order to obtain information about how TripAdvisor selects which reviews to showcase on
this section of their website, the company was contacted on numerous occasions, yet chose not
to respond to the author’s requests for this information.
2. See Vásquez and Urzúa (2009) for a discussion about representations of mental/emotional
states as a category distinct from reported speech.
References
Bamberg, M. (2007). Stories: Big or small — why do we care? In M. Bamberg (Ed.) Narrative —
State of the art (pp. 165–174). Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Besnier, N. (1994). Involvement in linguistic practice: An ethnographic appraisal. Journal of
Pragmatics, 22, 279–299.
Briggs, S., Sutherland, J., & Drummond, S. (2007). Are hotels serving quality? An exploratory
study of service quality in the Scottish hotel sector. Tourism Management, 28, 1006–1019.
© 2012. John Benjamins Publishing Company
All rights reserved
120 Camilla Vásquez
Chafe, W. (1982). Integration and involvement in speaking, writing, and oral literature.In D. annen (Ed.) Spoken and written language: Exploring orality and literacy (pp. 35–53). Norwood,
NJ: Ablex.
Chung, J. Y., & Buhalis, D. (2008). Web 2.0: A study of online travel community.In P. ’Connor,
W.Höpken, & U. Gretzel (Eds.), Information and Communication Technologies in Tourism:
Proceedings of the International Conference in Innsbruck, Austria, 2008 (pp. 267–278).
Springer: Vienna.
Freeman, M. (2007). Life “on holiday”? In defense of big stories. In M. Bamberg (Ed.) Narrative
— State of the art (pp. 155–164). Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Georgakopoulou, A. (2007a). Small stories, interaction, and identities. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Georgakopoulou, A. (2007b). hinking big with small stories in narrative and identity analysis.
In M. Bamberg (Ed.) Narrative — State of the art (pp. 145–154). Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Georgakopoulou, A. (2006).he other side of the story: Towards a narrative analysis of narratives-in-interaction. Discourse Studies, 8 (2), 235–257.
Gretzel, U. (2007). Online travel review study: Role & impact of online travel reviews. http://
www.tripadvisor.com/pdfs/OnlineTravelReviewReport.pdf [online report].
Labov, W., &Waletzky, J. (1967). Narrative analysis: Oral versions of personal experiences. In J.
Helm (Ed.), Essays on the verbal and visual arts: Proceedings of the 1966 Annual Ethnological
Society (pp. 12–44). Seattle: University of Washington Press.
Lakof, R. (1990). Talking power: he politics of language in our lives. New York: Basic Book.
Miguéns, J., Baggio, R., &Costa, C. (2008). Social media and tourism destinations: TripAdvisor
case study. In IASK ATR 2008 (Advances in Tourism Research 2008) veiro, Portugal.
Myers, G. (2010). he discourse of blogs and wikis. London: Continuum.
O’Connor, P. (2008). User-generated content and travel: A case-study on Tripadvisor.com. In P.
O’Connor, W. Höpken, & U. Gretzel (Eds.), Information and communication technologies in
tourism: Proceedings of the international conference in Innsbruck, Austria, 2008(pp. 47–58).
Springer, Vienna.
Page, R. (2010). Re-examining narrativity: small stories in status updates. Text & Talk, 30(4),
423–444.
Page, R. (2012).Stories and social media. London: Routledge.
Pollach, I. (2006). Electronic word of mouth: A genre analysis of product reviews on consumer
opinion web sites, in theProceedings of the 39th Hawaii International Conference on System
Sciences. IEEE Computer Society.
Ricci, F., & Wietsma, R. (2006). Product reviews in travel decision-making. In M. Hitz, M.
Sigala& J. Murphy (Eds.), Information and communication technologies in tourism, 2006
(pp. 296–307). Vienna: Springer.
Sacks, H. (1974). An analysis of the course of a joke’s telling in conversation. In R. Bauman &
J. Sherzer (Eds.) Explorations in the ethnography of speaking (pp. 337–353). Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Sen, S.,& Lerman, D. (2007). ‘Why are you telling me this? An examination into negative consumer reviews on the web, Journal of Interactive Marketing, 21, 76–94.
Tannen, D. (1984). Conversational style: Analyzing talk among friends. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Tannen, D. (1985). Relative focus on involvement in oral and written discourse. In D.Olson,
N. Torrance & A. Hildyard (Eds.), Literacy, language, and learning: he nature and consequences of reading and writing (pp. 124–147). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Tannen, D. (1989). Talking voices. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
© 2012. John Benjamins Publishing Company
All rights reserved
Narrativity and involvement in online consumer reviews
Vásquez, C., & Urzúa, A. (2009). Reported speech and reported mental states in mentoring
meetings: Exploring novice teacher identities. Research on Language and Social Interaction,
42 (1), 1–19.
Vásquez, C. (2011). Complaints online: he case of TripAdvisor. Journal of Pragmatics, 43,
1707–1717.
Vásquez, C. (forthcoming). “Usually not one to complain but…”: Constructing identities in online reviews. In C. Tagg & P. Seargeant (Eds.), he language of social media: Communication
and community on the internet. London: Palgrave Macmillan.
Yoo, K.-H., & Gretzel, U.(2009). Comparison of deceptive and truthful travel reviews. In W.
Hopken, U. Gretzel & R. Law (Eds).Information and communication technologies in tourism,
2009 (pp. 37–47). Vienna: Springer.
Zehrer, A., Crotts, J., & Magnini, V. (2011).he perceived usefulness of blog postings: An extension of the expectancy-disconirmation paradigm, Tourism Management, 32 (1), 106–113.
© 2012. John Benjamins Publishing Company
All rights reserved
121