ISSN 1980-4415
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980-4415v37n75a04
Students' self-handicapping strategies in a mathematics course
Estratégias autoprejudiciais dos estudantes em um curso de Matemática
Edmilson Minoru Torisu*
ORCID iD 0000-0001-7383-387X
Evely Boruchovitch**
ORCID iD 0000-0001-7597-6487
Abstract
Self-handicapping strategies can be defined as individuals’ behaviors or statements, which happen before an
important task is performed and work as an excuse to justify a likely poor performance. The use of these strategies
can harm the academic performance of students at all levels. The current article presents a case study about the
use of self-handicapping strategies carried out with twenty undergraduate Mathematics students from a Brazilian
public university. Data collection was based on the application of a Likert-type scale and on a focus group
conducted with students. Scale data were analyzed through descriptive statistics, whereas group discussions were
subjected to qualitative analysis. Results have shown that most students do not significantly engage in selfhandicapping behaviors. Difficulties to manage time and to prepare for academic activities, such as tests, were the
reasons most often presented as justification for such an engagement. Data have also pointed out the need of
rethinking the role played by teachers and by the atmosphere they promote in the classroom as factors that can
contribute to the use, or not, of these strategies by students. Future research may help improving knowledge on
these matters.
Keywords: Self-handicapping strategies. Teacher’s Training. Mathematics Education.
Resumo
Estratégias autoprejudiciais podem ser definidas como comportamentos ou declarações de uma pessoa, que
ocorrem antes da realização de uma tarefa considerada importante e que servem como desculpa para justificar um
possível mau desempenho. O uso dessas estratégias pode prejudicar o rendimento acadêmico de estudantes de
todos os níveis. Neste artigo, apresentamos um estudo de caso sobre o uso de estratégias autoprejudiciais realizado
com vinte estudantes do curso de Matemática de uma universidade pública brasileira. A coleta de dados foi
realizada por meio da aplicação de uma escala do tipo Likert e de um grupo focal com os estudantes. Os dados da
escala foram analisados pela estatística descritiva. Já as discussões no grupo foram examinadas qualitativamente.
Os resultados revelaram que os estudantes, em sua maioria, engajam-se pouco em comportamentos
autoprejudiciais. Dificuldades com o gerenciamento do tempo e com a preparação para as atividades acadêmicas,
incluindo as provas, foram os motivos mais frequentemente apresentados como justificativas para tal engajamento.
Os dados apontam, também, para a necessidade de se repensar o papel do professor e do clima que promove em
sala de aula como fatores que podem contribuir para o uso, ou não, dessas estratégias por parte dos estudantes.
Pesquisas futuras poderão se aprofundar nessas questões.
Keywords: Estratégias autoprejudiciais. Formação de Professores. Educação Matemática.
*
Ph.D in Education, Federal University of Minas Gerais (UFMG). Full Professor. Federal University of Ouro
Preto (UFOP), Ouro Preto, Minas Gerais, Brazil. E-mail:
[email protected]
**
Ph.D in Education, University of Southern California (USC), Los Angeles. Full Professor.State University of
Campinas (Unicamp), Campinas, São Paulo, Brazil. E-mail:
[email protected]
Bolema, Rio Claro (SP), v. 37, n. 75, p. 70-90, abr. 2023
70
ISSN 1980-4415
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980-4415v37n75a04
1 Introduction
Human behavior has always been analyzed by the Psychology field under different
perspectives. The Social Cognitive Theory considers human beings as agents of their behavior.
According to Bandura (2008), being an agent means being able to make things happen
intentionally. The agency ability gives individuals flexibility and enables them to play an active
role in their development and adaptation processes. In other words, human agency allows
individuals to be self-regulated.
Self-regulation can be investigated in different domains (ZIMMERMAN; SCHUNK,
2011; SCHUNK; GREENE, 2018). One of them – which is the object of interest in the present
paper – lies on the academic domain and, within it, on the self-regulated learning. Self-regulated
students monitor, control, and reflect on their own learning process, and it makes them more
autonomous and capable of planning, setting clear goals, employing cognitive and
metacognitive strategies, and activating their motivational and self-efficacy beliefs to favor
their
learning
process
(ZIMMERMAN;
SCHUNK,
2011;
PANADERO,
2017;
BORUCHOVITCH, 2014; VEIGA SIMÃO, 2004; FRISON. BORUCHOVITH, 2020).
Fostering self-regulated learning in students is an essential role played by school and by
teachers, mainly in a society that – on a daily basis – imposes challenges that become
increasingly complex throughout the students’ development process and need to be overcome.
If, on the one hand, individuals strive to overcome these challenges by adopting self-regulated
learning strategies capable of leading them to success, on the other hand, some individuals give
up in advance when they face difficulties and give excuses to previously justify their likely
failure in order to protect their social image, by adopting what the literature calls selfhandicapping strategies (BERGLAS; JONES, 1978; MIDGLEY; ARUNKUMAR; URDAN,
1996; GANDA; BORUCHOVITCH, 201; MIRANDA; BORUCHOVITCH; GANDA, 2017;
MIRANDA; ALMEIDA, 2020).
Scientific interest in investigating self-handicapping strategies is not a recent
phenomenon. Berglas and Jones (1978, p. 406) have defined these strategies as “any action or
choice of performance setting that enhances the opportunity to externalize (or excuse) failure
and to internalize (reasonably accept credit for) success” – they can be behavioral or verbal.
Arkin and Baumgardner (1985) referred to behavioral strategies through the expression
acquired impediments. On the other hand, Leary and Shepperd (1986) have called them
behavioral self-handicapping. Examples of this strategy type comprise lack of sleep, drug use
and alcohol intake, picking debilitating performance circumstances, strategic effort reduction,
Bolema, Rio Claro (SP), v. 37, n. 75, p. 70-90, abr. 2023
71
ISSN 1980-4415
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980-4415v37n75a04
investing little effort, or no effort at all, before or during tasks. Verbal strategies – described in
the literature as claimed self-handicapping (LEONDARI; GONIDA, 2007) or self-reported
handicapping (LEARY; SHEPPERD, 1986) – “involve declaring one, or more, real or fictitious
impediments before a presentation, for example, to previously justify one’s likely failure or to
make one’s success more rewarding for its self-image” (COUDEVYLLE et al., 2020, p. 1).
Social anxiety, anxiety before tests, and physical and psychological symptoms, such as alleged
health issues, are examples of this self-handicapping strategy type (LEONDARI; GONIDA,
2007).
According to Hirt, Deppe and Gordon (1991), self-reported handicapping strategies are
lesser harmful than the behavioral ones, since they do not necessarily diminish one’s chances
of success in a given task. Thus, if individuals only declare to be anxious before a test, for
example, it works as justification for their poor performance, although it does not reduce their
chances of success. On the other hand, behaviors such as alcohol intake before a task function
as justification for one’s poor performance, but they also decrease individuals’ chances of
achieving a successful performance.
Despite this typology, according to Sameer Babu and Selvamari (2018), most scholars
agree that self-handicapping strategies are featured by the behavior of creating obstacles to
successful performance in tasks considered important by individuals. It is so because, since the
obstacle is potentially harmful to one’s good performance, it is used as an excuse for one’s
likely failure.
Individuals who often adopt self-handicapping strategies have similar profiles. They
care about what people think about them based on their performance (GANDA;
BORUCHOVITCH, 2016; URDAN; MIDGLEY, 2001; BERGLAS; JONES, 1978) and use
these strategies as self-protection mechanism, since successful individuals are the ones
acknowledged by society (COVINGTON, 2000; ŞAHIN; ÇOBAN, 2020, COUDELYLLE et
al., 2020). Procrastination is another feature presented by self-handicappers (GANDA;
BORUCHOVITCH, 2015), since they always postpone their tasks as a defensive strategy to be
safe from bad criticism. This behavior is doubly favorable for those who adopt it. If they fail in
tests, for example, they can say that it happened because they postponed their studies up to the
last minute. Conversely, if they succeed, despite having procrastinated, they will be considered
quite skilled in the eyes of others. After all, despite their last-minute study, they succeeded.
Engaging in several activities at the same time to justify poor performance can also be typical
of self-handicappers (COVINGTON, 2000).
Low self-esteem and weak self-efficacy beliefs are also common features of individuals
Bolema, Rio Claro (SP), v. 37, n. 75, p. 70-90, abr. 2023
72
ISSN 1980-4415
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980-4415v37n75a04
who adopt self-handicapping strategies (ZIMMERMAN, 2000; PRAPAVESSIS; GROVE,
1998; CHEN; SUNG; WANG, 2017; YAVUZER, 2015). However, by adopting these strategies,
students can be negatively affected in different aspects, such as their self-esteem and selfefficacy beliefs. This behavior generates a vicious cycle. Low self-esteem leads to the adoption
of self-handicapping strategies, which, in their turn and on a reverse path, contribute to harm
individuals’ self-esteem and self-efficacy beliefs (ZUCKERMAN; KIEFFER; KNEE, 1998;
MIRANDA; BORUCHOVITCH; GANDA, 2017; ZIMMERMAN, 2000; SCHUNK;
ZIMMERMAN, 2008).
2 Self-handicapping strategies and Mathematics
Mathematics has been historically considered a complex school subject mastered by few
students, who are taken as the most intelligent individuals in the classroom. Thus, it is feared
by many and generates feelings such as aversion and anxiety (TORISU; FERREIRA, 2009;
MENDES; CARMO, 2014; SOUZA, 2006). This social representation of Mathematics is
shared by several students and triggers stress levels that make them act in certain ways
(VALONI DE JESUS, 2020). One of them lies on adopting self-handicapping strategies, which
have undesirable outcomes in individuals’ cognitive, motivational, affective, and social aspects.
Thus, understanding the use of these strategies in Mathematics is a rather fruitful research field
for Psychology and Mathematical Education. However, the literature in the field has few studies
focused on approaching this topic, both in Brazil and abroad. Thus, considerations about
research involving the use of self-handicapping strategies in Mathematics will be herein
presented.
Affectivity can be understood from several perspectives: attitudes and values, moral and
ethical behavior, personal and social development, motivation, interest, tenderness,
interrelation, empathy, subjectivity formation, feelings, and emotions (RIBEIRO, 2010). In
addition, it is considered essential to create a favorable learning atmosphere (RIBEIRO, 2010;
ECCIUS-WELLMANN; IBARRA-GONZALEZ, 2020), not only in math classrooms.
However, difficulties faced by students in Mathematics, as well as the poor relationship they
establish with this subject, appear to emphasize the important role played by affectivity in
environments where it is taught. In fact, studies conducted by Dorman, Adams, and Ferguson
(2001), Turner et al. (2002), Turner et al. (2003), Dorman and Ferguson (2004), Şahin and
Çoban (2020) have shown that affectivity should be considered at the time to explain the higher
or lower use of self-handicapping strategies in math classrooms. Most specifically, they
Bolema, Rio Claro (SP), v. 37, n. 75, p. 70-90, abr. 2023
73
ISSN 1980-4415
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980-4415v37n75a04
acknowledged teachers as the ones whose discourses, practices, and motivational support
contribute to decrease the incidence of these strategies. They also showed that students who do
not adopt these strategies tend to have better performance.
Brownlow, Rogers and Jacobi (2000) have investigated the association between sex and
the use of self-handicapping strategies in Mathematics. Results have shown that male
participants were more likely to adopt self-handicapping strategies and tended to blame external
agents for their poor performances. Although Yu and McLellan (2019) did not investigate these
strategies in the Mathematics field, they found results similar to the ones reported in previous
study conducted with English students – i.e., boys most often adopted self-handicapping
strategies to preserve their image in the academic and social spheres. On the other hand, studies
conducted by Dorman and Ferguson (2004) with Canadian students, as well as by Leondari and
Gonida (2007), with Greek students did not find significant differences in the use of selfhandicapping strategies in Mathematics between sexes. Although the research conducted in
Turkey by Yavuzer (2015) was focused on a subject other than Mathematics, results have shown
higher trend to use self-handicapping strategies among women.
On the one hand, the inconsistency observed in results among several studies about sex
and use of self-handicapping strategies points towards the need of further investigating this
subject to help better understanding it, given the relevance of sex and its important
psychoeducational implications. On the other hand, this inconsistency can be likely explained
through the theory by Midgley, Arunkumar and Urdan (2001), according to whom, the use of
self-handicapping strategies is more associated with personal differences among students – such
as their general motivations, attitudes towards learning, self-beliefs, and the environment they
are inserted in – than with sex. With regard to the previously mentioned studies, it is possible
that differences observed between sexes are associated with the fact that the analyzed samples
derived from different countries. This factor presupposes a wide variety of cultures, values, and
beliefs, as well as different ways of valuing the role played by men and women in society, an
issue that certainly affects the way people act.
Another interesting finding about the use of self-handicapping strategies by
Mathematics students is associated with the self-concept construct. According to Mesa (2012),
participants presenting positive self-concept were interested in developing skills, believed in
their ability to deal with challenging tasks within the Mathematics scope, and avoided using
self-handicapping strategies. Efklides and Vlachopoulos (2012) have shown positive
correlation between self-concept in Mathematics and mathematical skills. The aforementioned
study has also shown that students who acknowledged their difficulties in Mathematics tended
Bolema, Rio Claro (SP), v. 37, n. 75, p. 70-90, abr. 2023
74
ISSN 1980-4415
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980-4415v37n75a04
to adopt self-handicapping strategies more often.
In addition to self-concept, self-efficacy beliefs can also play decisive role in the
adoption of self-handicapping strategies. Self-efficacy beliefs refer to individuals’ judgments
about their ability to carry out tasks in a specific domain (BZUNECK, 2001; PAJARES, 1996;
BANDURA, 1997). These beliefs are lesser generic than self-concept. The study by Steinhauer
et al. (1993) – who used two mathematical tests, among data collection instruments – has shown
that students who believed in their mathematical skills took the tests and did not use selfhandicapping strategies, although they chose to answer the easiest questions. According to the
aforementioned authors, this factor suggests that they knew they were lesser able than they had
reported. On the other hand, students who underestimated their math skills have used selfhandicapping strategies to avoid being judged by others.
As self-efficacy beliefs refer to specific tasks, it is possible that the option that some
students made to answer the easiest questions is associated with their weak self-efficacy beliefs
about their ability to answer the most complex questions (BANDURA, 1997; BZUNECK,
2001). In a way, choosing easier questions to ensure success can be understood as selfprotection of one’s image. Studies focused on investigating the association between selfefficacy beliefs and use of self-handicapping strategies in Mathematics can lead to new and
interesting results to help improving academic research on this topic.
Moreover, participants in the majority of the studies analyzed were basic education
students, and it evidenced the scarcity of investigations about the use of self-handicapping
strategies among Higher Education students, mostly among Mathematics students. Some
studies associated with the use of self-handicapping strategies among Higher Education
students were carried out in Brazil, (GANDA; BORUCHOVITCH, 2015; GANDA;
BORUCHOVITCH, 2016; VARGAS et al., 2018), but none of them focused on exclusively
examining students enrolled in Mathematics Licentiate and Bachelor Degree courses.
It is essential to investigate the use of self-handicapping strategies in the Mathematics
context, since studies have shown high failure and/or dropout index in disciplines associated
with Mathematics, such as calculus, analytical geometry, linear algebra, among others, in
Brazilian Higher Education courses belonging to the Exact Sciences field. Most specifically,
the calculus discipline is feared by many due to its complexity and to the large number of
failures (ALVARENGA; DORR; VIEIRA, 2016; ZARPELON; RESENDE; REIS, 2017),
which create a myth around it. This issue is not different among students enrolled in
Mathematics Licentiate and Bachelor Degree courses. There are many failures in disciplines
such as calculus and analytical geometry. Students often fail, more than once, in the same
Bolema, Rio Claro (SP), v. 37, n. 75, p. 70-90, abr. 2023
75
ISSN 1980-4415
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980-4415v37n75a04
discipline, a fact that leads to discouragement and, finally, to students’ withdrawal from the
course (ROSA; SANTOS; MENDES, 2019; PINTO; MOREIRA, 2018).
These results arouse significant concern, give rise to discussions and invite us to reflect
on issues such as: Do Mathematics students with low academic performance use selfhandicapping strategies? If so, what would be the most adopted strategies? Accordingly, the
current study presents the results of a postodoctoral research that aimed at answering these
questions by investigating Mathematics Licentiate and Bachelor Degree students from a
Brazilian public university.
3 Methodology
The present study can be considered as a case study. The whole classroom is taken as a
unit of study (VENTURA, 2007). Although our results cannot be generalized, they certainly
promote reflection on a problem that affects many students around the world and can both
encourage further investigations and shed light on how to help students overcome this problem.
3.1 Students involved in the present study
The sample was composed of twenty students from a night course of Licentiate and
Bachelor’s in Mathematics of a public university in Minas Gerais, Brazil. Table 1 presents the
demographic information of the participants.
Gender
Table 1 – Demographic data of the students
N
Female
9 (45%)
Male
11 (55%)
Age range
N
19 to 23 years
14 (70%)
24 to 44 years
6 (30%)
Type of school attended in high school
N
Attended only public school
15 (75%)
Attended only private school
2 (10%)
Attended both public and private school
3 (15%)
Ethinicity
N
White
12 (60%)
Bolema, Rio Claro (SP), v. 37, n. 75, p. 70-90, abr. 2023
76
ISSN 1980-4415
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980-4415v37n75a04
Brown
6 (30%)
Other
2 (10%)
Source: authors
3.2 Data collection instruments
A Likert-type scale comprising nineteen items associated with the use of selfhandicapping strategies in the academic context was used for data collection purposes.
Responses were distributed into four options, which ranged from 1 (it has nothing to do with
me) to 4 (it describes me really well). The total score may range from 19 to 76 points. The
higher the score, the more frequent the use of self-handicapping strategies by students in the
academic context.
3.3 Data collection procedures and conditions
It is worth emphasizing that the present study is part of a larger research, which was
approved by the Ethics Committee of a public university under CAAE number
81094017.0.0000.8142. Data collection was carried out in April and June 2021– when
classroom lessons in most institutions were still suspended due to the Covid-19 pandemic – in
a Google Meet Virtual Room, a platform that allows synchronous remote meetings with good
sound and image quality.
On the day that had been previously scheduled for data collection purposes, students
received a link to access the room created in Google Meet and receive the necessary instructions
to complete the self-handicapping strategies’ scale, one of the scales is available at Autorregular
platform, created to host the larger research the present study is part of (BORUCHOVITCH et
al., 2019). Data collection was carried out by the first author of the present study and a graduate
student previously trained to do so. Firstly, students were informed about the research aim, as
well as that their responses were confidential, and that collected data would only be used for
the study. Students who agreed to participate in the study were asked to sign the informed
consent form before completing the scale and providing demographic data. Few issues that
emerged during data and scale filling process were all solved. Data collection lasted
approximately 20 minutes; the virtual room was closed after the collection procedure was over.
After students’ responses to the self-handicapping strategies’ scale were analyzed,
students who had mostly selected options 3 and 4 were invited to participate in a focal group
Bolema, Rio Claro (SP), v. 37, n. 75, p. 70-90, abr. 2023
77
ISSN 1980-4415
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980-4415v37n75a04
meeting, aimed at deepening the information about the use of self-handicapping strategies by
these students (DALL’AGNOL; TRENCH, 1999; TRAD, 2009). The focal group meeting was
also held in a Google Meet room and lasted approximately 1h 10min.
3.4 Data analysis
Quantitative data associated with responses to the self-handicapping strategies’ scale
were subjected to descriptive statistics, whereas data collected in the focal group were examined
by qualitative analysis. Discussions held in the focus group about the use of self-handicapping
strategies shed light into the reasons why some students of the present study chose to use some
strategies rather than others. Students’ speeches were fully recorded, based on two procedures,
namely: notes taken by the first author and audio-recordings.
4 Results and Discussion
4.1 Responses to the scale
The analysis applied to students’ responses to the self-handicapping strategies’ scale has
shown quite interesting results. For example, some self-handicapping strategies were virtually
not adopted by the investigated group. Among them, four strategies were associated with
classroom behaviors that, based on the analyzed data, are rarely, or virtually not, adopted by
most students. The rate of responses to each of the four items in the scale is presented next in
Table 2.
Table 2 - Percentage of self-handicapping strategies less employed by students in the classroom
Item in the scale
(8) Some students postpone some important tasks until the
deadline set by the professor. If they get a bad grade, they say
the assignment was done in the last minute.
(19) Some students read entertainment magazines during class.
If they fail, they say it was because they did not understand the
subject.
(4) Some students often leave the classroom. If they get low
score in the test, they say it was because they have lost
important content.
(3) Some students play with their cell phones during class. If
they do not get good results, they say it was because they did
not understand the teacher’s explanation.
Source: authors
Bolema, Rio Claro (SP), v. 37, n. 75, p. 70-90, abr. 2023
It has
nothing
to do
with me
95%
It has
little to
do with
me
5%
It
describes
me well
0%
It
describes
me really
well
0%
95%
5%
0%
0%
90%
10%
0%
0%
80%
15%
5%
0%
78
ISSN 1980-4415
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980-4415v37n75a04
It is worth mentioning that the self-handicapping strategies’ scale does not have any
item, other than these four, to measure other self-handicapping behaviors specifically observed
in the classroom. Thus, these results appear to evidence that virtually all investigated students
(80% to 95%) did not report to adopt self-handicapping behaviors in the classroom
environment, such as talking to their classmates, reading magazines, playing with their cell
phones, or even often leaving the classroom, which can hinder their focus on the teacher’s
explanations. They seem to control their attention and concentration in the classroom
environment. This feature is quite important for the self-regulated learning process (VEIGA
SIMÃO; FRISON, 2013; BORUCHOVITCH, 2014; ZIMMERMAN, 2000; GANDA;
BORUCHOVITCH, 2016), mainly for future teachers, who will play key role in creating
situations to foster the self-regulated learning of their future students. Accordingly,
Boruchovitch (2014) has emphasized the importance and need of investigating and
understanding how future teachers deal with and manage their own learning in teacher
education programs. Moreover, two other items of the scale were also considered as little
representative of students’ behavior (Table 3).
Table 3 - Percentage of self-handicapping strategies less employed by students outside the
classroom
Item in the scale
(12) Some students report that they need to stay with their
friends and/or boy/girlfriend. If they fail, they say they did not
have time to invest in their studies.
(14) Some students study the wrong content for the test. If they
do not perform well, they say that was the reason.
Source: authors
It has
nothing
to do
with me
It has
little to
do with
me
It
describes
me well
80%
15%
0%
It
describ
es me
really
well
5%
80%
20%
0%
0%
Item 12 appears to be associated with lack of time management. Individuals can meet
their friends and boyfriend/girlfriend without hindering their studies. However, they must
manage their time in order to save an ideal period-of-time for each activity, without overlapping
them. Item 14 indicates inadequate preparation for academic activities. Only three (15%) of the
twenty students appeared to be partly represented by item 12, whereas four (20%) of the twenty
students were partly represented by item 14. The response option it has little to do with me
suggests that they admit this behavior, although it is not very recurrent. Only one of the twenty
students was well represented by the statement in item 12, which means that this participant
often gives priority to their friends and boyfriend/girlfriend to the detriment of their studies.
With respect to the third scale response option it describes me well, nine (45%) of the
Bolema, Rio Claro (SP), v. 37, n. 75, p. 70-90, abr. 2023
79
ISSN 1980-4415
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980-4415v37n75a04
respondents used this option to evaluate of their behaviors described by some items on the scale.
Of the total number of items, four (4) were more often selected (Table 4).
Table 4 – Self-handicapping strategies mentioned by students as describing them well
Item in the scale
(15) Some students postpone some important tasks
until the deadline set by the professor. If they get a bad
grade, they say the assignment was done in the last
minute.
(16) Some students do not organize their time very
well, so they need to stay awake for several nights in a
row to do an important assignment. If they get a bad
grade, they say they were too sleepy.
(17) Some students are focused on non-essential
details of an important activity and do not dedicate
time to its content. If the grade is not what they
expected, they say they had no time to do a good
assignment.
(18) Some students do not dedicate enough time to an
important assignment. If the result is not good, they
say they were not engaged.
Source: authors
It describes me well
15%
25%
15%
20%
Table 4 shows that three of the twenty students (15%) in the group tend to postpone
important tasks until the deadline set and, if they do not do well, they justify their poor
performance by saying that they left it to the last minute (item 15). Another four students (20%)
do not reserve enough time to carry out an important activity. Thus, the justification for a poor
performance in the task is related with short time dedicated to their studies (item 18). The
tendency of postponing tasks is called academic procrastination in the literature (GRECCO,
1984) and is a hallmark of people who adopt self-handicapping strategies (GANDA;
BORUCHOVITCH, 2015; COVINGTON, 2000). The little time reserved for studies,
mentioned in item 18 of the scale, is, according to Akça (2012), a characteristic of students who
procrastinate.
Moreover, Table 4 shows that 15% of the students surveyed tend to dedicate most of the
time to complete a task to its unimportant details, rather than to the whole task content and, if
the result is not good, they say that the time was not enough to do a good job (item 17). A total
of five students (25%) do not manage their time and must stay up nights in a row to complete a
task requested by the teacher. If the result is not satisfactory, tiredness and sleep are to blame
(item 16). It can be noted that in all these cases, self-handicapping strategies mentioned by
participants are related to poor time management for study.
It is worth mentioning that the answer option this describes me well was selected 26
times and, of that total, in 22 times this option was the answer for a group of only five students.
This suggests that they adopt self-handicapping strategies more often than the other surveyed
Bolema, Rio Claro (SP), v. 37, n. 75, p. 70-90, abr. 2023
80
ISSN 1980-4415
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980-4415v37n75a04
students. Let us call these five students S1, S2, S3, S4, and S5. We will revisit their behavior
further in this paper.
The fourth answer option: describes me very well was selected only seven times. Its
highest frequency was 10% (two out of twenty), for item 6 of the scale: Some students
intentionally engage in too many activities. If they don't do well on the course, they say they
were too busy with other things.
It is worth mentioning some important findings about students S1, S2, S3, S4, and S5.
S1 selected option 3 (describes me well) for four items of the scale and option 4 (describes me
very well) also for four items. S2, S3, and S5 selected option 3 out of three items and did not
select option 4. On the other hand, S4 selected option 3 out of nine items and did not check
option 4. If we consider that when selecting options 3 and four students admit that their behavior
is well represented by the item on the scale, 2 students in the sample stood out for adopting selfdisadvantage strategies, namely: S1, who adopts seven of the nineteen strategies; and S4, which
adopts nine of them. As it can be seen, participants’ selection of response options 3 and 4 was
associated with time management, with emphasis on procrastination, most of the time.
Of these five students, three (60%) were men and two (40%) were women. Since the
total number of students comprised eleven men (55%) and nine women (45%), the difference
in the adoption of self-handicapping strategies between sexes seems to be not significant in this
group. This result is congruent with those reported by Dorman and Ferguson (2004) and
Leondari and Gonida (2007), who did not find significant differences in the use of selfhandicapping strategies in Mathematics between genders. However, it differs from the results
of a study carried out by Brownlow, Rogers, and Jacobi (2000), which revealed that males were
more likely to adopt self-handicapping strategies with a tendency to blame external agents for
poor performance than females. Gender differences in the use of self- handicapping strategies
need to be explored by further research since they can have important educational implications.
.
4.2 Focal Group
To better understand the reasons why S1, S2, S3, S4, and S5 report using selfhandicapping strategies, they were invited to a focus group.
Before reflecting on and discussing with students about the reasons leading them to use
self-handicapping strategies, they were asked about the reasons for not adopting some of them.
Table 2, described previously in this paper, presents four self-handicapping strategies almost
never adopted by students, all related to classroom behaviors.
Bolema, Rio Claro (SP), v. 37, n. 75, p. 70-90, abr. 2023
81
ISSN 1980-4415
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980-4415v37n75a04
When students were questioned about their answers, they were unanimous in saying that
they avoid behaviors such as leaving the room, playing with their cell phone, talking to
classmates, reading magazines, in respect to their teacher. Some responses made it quite clear:
S5: In respect to the teacher, because it is very annoying, when someone is explaining something,
and people are playing with their cell phone, talking [...].
S1: It is directly linked to the fact that we must respect the person standing in front of us, doing
their work. [...] even if it is the most boring subject, the least I must do is respect that person.
(Responses from some students during the focal group - 2021).
Some authors have concluded that affective factors can contribute to the use or not of
self-handicapping strategies in the classroom (DORMAN; ADAMS; FERGUSON, 2001;
TURNER et al., 2002; TURNER et al., 2003; DORMAN; FERGUSON, 2004). According to
Ribeiro (2010), respect is an important value and part of an ethical behavior which can be
considered as also related to affectivity. Therefore, feeling respect for the teacher in the present
study may have assumed the role of an affective factor that had contributed to the non-adoption
of self-handicapping strategies.
Student S3 considers that feeling respect for the teacher makes it difficult for him to
adopt self-handicapping strategies, such as leaving the classroom, reading magazines, and using
the cell phone. However, this student, unlike most of his peers, works full time and has a child
to care for. In fact, the only time he has for learning is during class. If he does not pay attention
to the teacher’s explanations, he will not have another chance to learn. This is interesting data
because it shows that a sense of responsibility can prevent the use of some self-handicapping
strategies (MINUTTI; SANTOS; FERRAZ, 2021).
However, although S1, S2, S3, S4, and S5 do not adopt self-handicapping strategies in
the classroom, they indeed use them in other contexts which are mostly associated with time
management and preparation for activities. Students’ speeches enable us to understand why
they adopt certain strategies. The following responses were obtained from students who were
questioned, for example, about the reasons leading them to study only on the eve of the test day,
or to leave activities to the last minute:
S4: Too much content. (Response during the focal group, 2021)
S1: I often have a hard time managing my schedules, even in face-to-face lessons, but now it got
worse due to the pandemic. This question hit me like a bullet in the chest. [...] Sometimes it is
not lack of time. One believes it’s lack of time. [...] procrastination is something that handicaps
me a lot. (Response during the focal group, 2021).
S2: I found out that I did not manage the time in the remote learning. Before, in my mind, I did
not really have time. In the remote learning, I'm at home full time. So, there is enough time.
(Responses from some students during the focal group, 2021).
The overload of activities (too much content), in association with the complexity of
Bolema, Rio Claro (SP), v. 37, n. 75, p. 70-90, abr. 2023
82
ISSN 1980-4415
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980-4415v37n75a04
disciplines in the mathematics course, can compromise the organization of time by several
students and lead them to use self-handicapping strategies. Masola and Allevato (2016) have
pointed out the need of implementing a didactic update in the process of teaching these
mathematical disciplines in Higher Education institutions. Procrastination (word used by S1 to
translate their behavior towards activities) is also used in the literature as one of the features
presented by students who adopt self-handicapping strategies (GANDA; BORUCHOVITCH,
2016; MIDGLEY; ARUNKUMAR; URDAN, 1996). It is worth mentioning that the content of
item 15 of the scale, showed previously in Table 3, deals with procrastination and well
represented the behavior of four of the five students. S1 and S5 have highlighted the Covid-19
pandemic time – which forced institutions to implement remote teaching – as something that
enhanced their time organization difficulties or contributed to their perception that, although
they have time, they procrastinate.
With regard to anxiety, S4 and S5 have said they feel anxious before tests they did not
prepare themselves for. According to S5:
S5: There were times when I could not take the test. I did not prepare for it and when the time
came to take the test, my head started to throb. [...] I have anxiety and I get very agitated before
any test. It ends up handicapping me. [...] I get anxious thinking about my score. If I don’t do
well, I'll have to take the final test
(S5 student response during focal group, 2021).
Although the future is not something that S5 can know, she cognitively represents it in
the present as something bad (to get a low score) and this is the cause of her anxiety before the
tests. S5 admits occasions when she did not prepare for the test, but she blames anxiety for her
likely poor performance. Leondari and Gonida (2007) have classified anxiety claim before tests
as claimed self-handicapping (COUDEVYLLE et al., 2020) or self-reported handicapping
(LEARY; SHEPPERD, 1986).
An important point to be discussed, raised by S5’s response, lies on the value given by
students to the test scores. It seems to reflect the highest value given by society to the most
successful individuals (COVINGTON, 2000; ŞAHIN; ÇOBAN, 2020, COUDELYLLE et al.,
2020). The anticipation of a probably low score generates anxiety, which is used to justify one’s
likely poor performance. Excessive valuation of scores in tests is something that needs to be
debated in academic research, since scores have been used as the only parameter to classify
individuals as successful or unsuccessful students. Thus, in the case of S5, low score can
represent the undesirable label of unsuccessful student, which is something that she does not
want for herself. Self-handicappers care about the judgment of others due to their performance
(GANDA; BORUCHOVITCH, 2016; URDAN; MIDGLEY, 2001; BERGLAS; JONES, 1978)
Bolema, Rio Claro (SP), v. 37, n. 75, p. 70-90, abr. 2023
83
ISSN 1980-4415
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980-4415v37n75a04
and it may be the reason why they use self-handicapping strategies as a self-protection
mechanism.
This discussion leads to new questions: To what extent can the evaluation based on
scores contribute to the adoption of self-handicapping strategies by students? Based on a
broader perspective, to what extent does the teachers’ pedagogical practice collaborate to this
behavior? Several studies have emphasized teachers as the ones whose discourses, practices,
and motivational support contribute to higher or lower adoption of self-handicapping strategies
by students (DORMAN; ADAMS; FERGUSON, 2001; TURNER et al., 2002; TURNER et al.,
2003; ŞAHIN; ÇOBAN, 2020). If one considers these results and S5’s response, which shows
the importance given by several students to scores, it is necessary to think about how teachers
can reverse this scenario by adopting different ways of evaluating their students, without relying
only on tests.
One of the self-handicapping strategies selected by these five students as representative
of their behavior is associated with the poor organization of time to prepare activities or to study
to take tests, a fact that forces them to stay up all night. Tiredness after a long night of studies,
oftentimes fruitless, turns out to be the justification for students’ poor performance. Scholars
such as Berglas and Jones (1978) and Leary and Shepperd (1986) had already considered this
type of behavior as typical of self-handicappers.
In fact, the focal group meeting has encouraged the debate among students and enabled
deepening the discussion about the use of the self-handicapping strategies. Students listened to
the opinions of others and had the opportunity to better consolidate their initial opinion, as
expected by using this technique (DALL’AGNOL; TRENCH, 1999; TRAD, 2009).
5 Final considerations
The aim of the present study was to investigate the self-handicapping strategies adopted
by undergraduate Mathematics students from a Brazilian public university. Results were
encouraging and revealed that, overall, the investigated students do not always resort to these
strategies.
The five students who have mostly felt represented by the statements in the selfhandicapping strategies’ scale - and, therefore, were the ones who mostly selected response
options 3 and 4 – have pointed out that the strategies adopted by them were associated with the
management of time to prepare for activities, such as tests.
Lack of time organization generates some issues in the academic life of these students,
Bolema, Rio Claro (SP), v. 37, n. 75, p. 70-90, abr. 2023
84
ISSN 1980-4415
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980-4415v37n75a04
and it makes them to adopt self-handicapping strategies. Examples of these issues comprise
last-minute studies leading to loss of nights of sleep, procrastination, anxiety before the tests,
among others. However, some responses have generated discomfort in this regard and need to
be addressed in future research, mainly in studies focused on investigating the role played by
Higher Education professors.
Excessive number of activities to be accomplished was one of the justifications for
students’ poor time management. Another justification for students’ anxiety before tests was
associated with their fear of having a poor performance (low score). Despite the alleged good
training in undergraduate courses, these responses can function as warning about structural
issues in these courses and in the training of university professors. Is it true that students get
overwhelmed with many activities? Do teachers give too much importance to tests to the
detriment of other training aspects, which ends up triggering the students’ fear of failure?
It is also recommended conducting further studies focused on identifying selfhandicapping strategies adopted by undergraduate Mathematics students by using larger and
more representative samples, as well as studies including variables such as self-concept, selfefficacy, and academic performance, among others. Future investigations on this theme should
be based on the combination of quantitative and qualitative research procedures to help better
understanding of the complexity involved in the process of adopting self-handicapping
strategies. Assumingly, results of research based on these purposes can reverberate and lead to
changes in the discipline matrix and in the way teachers and students perceive their education
and skills. These results can also be used to guide interventive actions aimed at strengthening
students’ self-regulatory processes to enable better time management and control of their
emotions.
Acknowledgment
Thanks to the state university of Campinas for having received me for the postdoctoral
and to professor Evely Boruchovitch for the supervision. Thanks to Federal University of Ouro
Preto for the license granted to carry out this study and for the financial support for revisions
and translations.
Referências
AKÇA, F. An Investigation into the Self-handicapping Behaviors of Undergraduates in Terms of
Academic Procrastination, the Locus of Control and Academic Success. Journal of Education and
Bolema, Rio Claro (SP), v. 37, n. 75, p. 70-90, abr. 2023
85
ISSN 1980-4415
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980-4415v37n75a04
Learning, Ontário, v. 1, n. 2, p. 288-297, 2012.
ALVARENGA, K. A.; DORR, R. C.; VIEIRA, V. D. O ensino e a aprendizagem de Cálculo
Diferencial e Integral: características e interseções no centro-oeste brasileiro. Revista Brasileira de
Ensino Superior, Passo Fundo, v. 2, n. 4, p. 46-57, 2016.
ARKIN, R. M.; BAUMGARDNER, A. H. Self-handicapping. In: HARVEY, J. H.; WEARY, G. (eds.).
Attribution: Basic issues and applications. New York: Academic Press, 1985. p. 169-202.
BANDURA, A. Self-efficacy: the exercise of control. New York: Freeman, 1997.
BANDURA, A. A teoria social cognitiva na perspectiva da agência. In: BANDURA, A.; AZZI, R. G.;
POLYDORO, S. (eds.). Teoria Social Cognitiva: conceitos básicos. Porto Alegre: Artmed, 2008. p.
69-96.
BERGLAS, S.; JONES, E. E. Drug choice as a self-handicapping strategy in response to
noncontingent success. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Washington, v. 36, n. 4, p.
405-417, 1978.
BORUCHOVITCH, E. Autorregulação da aprendizaegm: contribuições da Psicologia Educacional
para a formação de professores. Revista Quadrimestral da Associação Brasileira de Psicologia
Escolar e Educacional, São Paulo, v. 18, n. 3, p. 401- – 409, set./-dez. 2014.
BORUCHOVITCH, E.; GÓES N.M.; FELICORI, C.M.; ACEE, T.W. Translation and adaptation of
the learning and study strategies inventory-lassi 3rd edition for use in Brazil: methodological
considerations. Educação em Análise, Londrina, v.4, n.1, p. 20-34, 2019.
BROWNLOW, S.; ROGERS, M. I.; JACOBI, T. Science Anxiety as a Function of Personality, Gender
Roles, Experience with Science. In: MEETING OF THE SOUTHEASTERN PSYCHOLOGICAL
ASSOCIATION, 43., 2000, Atlanta. Proceedings ..., Atlanta, 2000. p. 1-46. ERIC Document
Reproduction Service n. ED443733.
BZUNECK, J. A. As crenças de autoeficácia e o seu papel na motivação do aluno. In
BORUCHOVITCH, E.; BZUNECK, J. A. (eds.). A motivação do aluno: contribuições da psicologia
contemporânea. Petrópolis: Vozes, 2001. p. 116-133.
CHEN, Z.; SUNG, K.; WANG, K. Self-esteem, achievement goals, and self-handicapping in college
physical education. Psychological Reports, v. 121, n. 4, p. 690-704, 2017.
COUDEVYLLE, G. R. et al. An experimental investigation of claimed self-handicapping strategies
across motivational climates based on achievement goal and self-determination theories. Educational
Psychology: an international journal of experimental educational psychology, London, v. 40, n. 8, p.
1002-2021. 2020.
COVINGTON, M. V. Goal Theory, Motivation, and School Achievement: An Integrative Review.
Annual Review of Psychology, Los Angeles, v. 51, p. 171-200, 2000.
DALL’AGNOL, C. M.; TRENCH, M. H. Grupos focais como estratégia metodológica em pesquisa na
enfermagem. Rev Gaúcha de Enfermagem, Porto Alegre, v. 20, n. 1, p. 5-25, 1999.
DORMAN, J. P.; ADAMS, J. E.; FERGUSON, J. M. The Relationship between High School
Mathematics Classroom Environment and Student Self-Handicapping. In: ANNUAL MEETING OF
THE AMERICAN EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH ASSOCIATION, 2001, Seattle. Proceedings...
Seattle, 2001. p. 1-22. ERIC Document Reproduction Service n. ED363435.
Bolema, Rio Claro (SP), v. 37, n. 75, p. 70-90, abr. 2023
86
ISSN 1980-4415
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980-4415v37n75a04
DORMAN, J. P.; FERGUSON, J.M. Associations between Students' Perceptions of Mathematics
Classroom Environment and Self-Handicapping in Australian and Canadian High Schools. McGill
Journal of Education, Montreal, v. 39, n.1, p. 69-87, 2004.
ECCIUS-WELLMANN, C.; IBARRA-GONZALEZ, K. P. Dependencia de la calificación de una
evaluación diagnóstica en matemáticas con aspectos afectivos por la comisión de errores. Bolema, Rio
Claro, v. 34, n. 67, p. 544-563, ago., 2020.
EFKLIDES, A.; VLACHOPOULOS, S. P. Measurement of metacognitive knowledge of self, task, and
strategies in mathematics. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, Newburyport, v. 28, n. 3,
p. 227–239. 2012.
FRISON, L. M. B.; BORUCHOVITCH, E. Autorregulação da aprendizagem: modelos teóricos e
reflexões para a prática pedagógica. In: FRISON, L. M. B.; BORUCHOVITCH, E (orgs).
Autorregulação da aprendizagem: cenários, desafios, perspectivas para o contexto educativo.
Petrópolis: Vozes, 2020. p. 18-30.
GANDA, D. R.; BORUCHOVITCH, E. As Atribuições de Causalidade e as Estratégias
Autoprejudiciais de Alunos do Curso de Pedagogia. Revista Psico-USF, Bragança Paulista, v. 21, n. 2,
p. 331-340, 2016.
GANDA, D. R.; BORUCHOVITCH, E. Self-handicapping strategies for leaning of preservice
teachers. Revista Estudos de Psicologia, Campinas, v. 32. n. 3, p. 417- 425, 2015.
GRECCO, P. R. A cognitive-behavioral assessment of problematic academic procrastination:
development of a procrastination self- statement inventory. Unpublished PHD, California School
of Professional. Fresno: Psychology, 1984.
HIRT, E. R.; DEPPE, R. K.; GORDON, L. J. Self-reported versus behavioral self-handicapping:
Empirical evidence for a theoretical distinction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Washington, v. 61, n. 6, p. 981-991, 1991.
LEARY, M. R.; SHEPPERD, J. A. Behavioral self-handicaps versus self-reported handicaps: A
conceptual note. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Washington, v. 51, n. 6, p. 12651268, 1986.
LEONDARI, A.; GONIDA, E. Predicting academic self-handicapping in different age groups: the role
of personal achievement goals and social goals. British Journal of Educational Psychology, Oxford,
v. 77, [s.n.], p. 595-611, 2007.
MASOLA, W.J.; ALLEVATO, N. S. G. Dificuldade de aprendizagem matemática de alunos
ingressantes na educação superior. Revista Brasileira de Ensino Superior, Passo Fundo, v. 2, n. 1, p.
64-74, 2016.
MENDES, A. C.; CARMO, J. S. Atribuições dadas à Matemática e ansiedade ante a Matemática: o
relato de alguns estudantes do ensino fundamental. Bolema, Rio Claro, v. 28, n. 50, p. 1368-1385,
dez., 2014.
MESA, V. Achievement Goal Orientations of Community College Mathematics Students and the
Misalignment of Instructor Perceptions. Community College Review, Thousand Oaks, v. 40, n. 1, p.
46-74, 2012.
MIDGLEY, C.; ARUNKUMAR, R.; URDAN, T. C. If I don´t well tomorrow, there´s a reason:
predictors of adolescents. Use of academic self-handcapping strategies. Journal of Educational
Psychology, Washington, v. 88, n. 3, p. 423-434, 1996.
Bolema, Rio Claro (SP), v. 37, n. 75, p. 70-90, abr. 2023
87
ISSN 1980-4415
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980-4415v37n75a04
MINUTTI, A. L. P. S.; SANTOS, A. A. A.; FERRAZ, A. S. Atribuições de causalidade, estratégias
autoprejudiciais e a autopercepção de desempenho de universitários. Avances en Psicología
Latinoamericana, Rosario, v. 39, n. 2, p. 1-16, 2021.
MIRANDA, L. C.; ALMEIDA, L. S. Aprendizagem autorregulada – o papel das estratégias
autoprejudiciais. In: FRISON, L. M. B.; BORUCHOVITCH, E. (Orgs). Autorregulação da
aprendizagem: cenários, desafios, contextos, perspectivas para o contexto educativo. Petrópolis:
Vozes, 2020. p. 62-84.
MIRANDA, L. C.; BORUCHOVITCH, E.; GANDA, D. R. Revista AMAzônica, Belém, v. 19, n. 2,
p. 8-22, 2017.
PAJARES, F. Self-efficacy beliefs in academic settings. Review of Educational Research, Thousand
Oaks, v. 66, n. 4, p. 543-578, 1996.
PANADERO, E. A review of self-regulated learning: six models and four directions for research.
Frontiers in Psychology, Lausanne, v. 8, [s.n], p. 1-28, 2017.
PINTO, R. P. L. J. M.; MOREIRA, A. M. Análise da Reprovação em Disciplinas do Curso de
Matemática a Distância da Universidade Federal da Paraíba. In: CONGRESSO BRASILEIRO DE
ENSINO SUPERIOR A DISTÂNCIA, 15., 2018, Natal. Anais ... Natal: UniRede, 2018. p. 1-10.
PRAPAVESSIS, H.; GROVE, J. R. Self-Handicapping and Self-Esteem. Journal of applied sport
Psychology, London, v. 10, n.2, p. 175-184, 1998.
RIBEIRO, M. L. A afetividade na relação educativa. Estud. Psico, Campinas, v. 27, n. 3, p. 403-412,
2010.
ROSA, C. DE M.; SANTOS, F. F. T. dos; MENDES, H. C. O desempenho dos estudantes do curso de
Matemática de uma instituição pública de educação superior. Revista Educação Em Questão, Natal,
v. 57, n. 53, p. 1-25, 2019.
ŞAHIN, F.; ÇOBAN, Ö. Effect of School Climate, Students 'Self-Handicapping Behaviors Effect of
School Climate, Students 'Self-Handicapping Behaviors and Demographic Characteristics on Students'
Achievement and Demographic Characteristics on Students' Achievement. Inquiry in Education,
Chicago, v. 12, n. 2, p. 1-20, 2020.
SAMEER BABU; SELVAMARI. How does academic self-handicapping relate o achievement in
mathematics ?: a small scale study among indian school chult. Educational quest: an international
journal of education and applied social Science, New Delhi, v. 9, n. 3, p. 233-238, 2018.
SCHUNK, D.; ZIMMERMAN, B. Motivation and self-regulated learning: theory, research and
applications. New York: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 2008.
SCHUNK, D.; GREENE, J. A. Handbook of self-regulation of learning and performance. 2. ed.
New York: Routledge, 2018.
SOUZA, L. N. I. Crenças de autoeficácia matemática. In: AZZI, R. G.; POLYDORO, S. A. J. (org.).
Autoeficácia em diferentes contextos. Campinas: Alínea, 2006. p. 111-126.
STEINHAUER, A. et al. An Assessment of the Self-Protective Function of Self-Handicapping. In:
BIENNIAL MEETING OF THE SOCIETY FOR RESEARCH IN CHILD DEVELOPMENT, 1993,
New Orleans. Proceedings ... New Orleans. 1993. p. 1-18. ERIC Document Reproduction Service No.
ED363435
Bolema, Rio Claro (SP), v. 37, n. 75, p. 70-90, abr. 2023
88
ISSN 1980-4415
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980-4415v37n75a04
TRAD, L. A. B. Grupos Focais: conceitos, procedimentos e reflexões baseadas em experiências com o
uso da técnica em pesquisa de saúde. Physis: Revista de Saúde Coletiva, Rio de Janeiro, v. 19, n. 3, p.
777-796, 2009.
TURNER, J. C. et al. The classroom environment and students' reports of avoidance strategies in
mathematics: A multimethod study. Journal of Educational Psychology, Washington, v. 94, n. 1, p.
88-106, 2002.
TURNER, J. C. et al. Teacher Discourse and Sixth Graders' Reported Affect and Achievement
Behaviors in Two High-Mastery/High- Performance Mathematics Classrooms. The Elementary
School Journal, Chicago, v. 103, n. 4, p. 357-382, 2003.
URDAN, T.; MIDGLEY, C. Academic Self-Handicapping: What We Know, What more there is to
learn. Educational Psychology Review, New York, v. 13, n. 2, p. 114 -138, 2001.
VALONI DE JESUS, R. Matemática: que tensão é essa? Um estudo sobre as representações sociais
de um grupo de alunos da primeira série do ensino médio de um instituto federal mineiro. 2020. 164f.
Dissertação (Mestrado em Educação Matemática) – Instituto de Ciências Exatas e Biológicas,
Universidade Federal de Ouro Preto, Ouro Preto, 2020.
VARGAS, J. G.; MIRANDA, L. C.; BORUCHOVITCH, E.; ALMEIDA, L. S. Validação da escala de
estratégias autoprejudiciais: um estudo com alunos do ensino superior português. Psicologia,
Educação e Cultura, Vila Nova de Gaia, v. 22, n. 1. p. 348-364, 2018.
VEIGA SIMÃO, A. M. Integrar os princípios da aprendizagem estratégica no processo formativo dos
profesores. In: LOPES da SILVA, A.; DUARTE, A. M.; SÁ, I.; VEIGA SIMÃO, A. M. (orgs).
Aprendizagem autorregulada pelo estudante: perspectivas psicológicas e educacionais. Porto: Porto
Editora, 2004. P. 95-106.
VEIGA SIMÃO, A.M.; FRISON, L.M. Autorregulação da aprendizagem: abordagens teóricas e
desafios para as práticas em contextos educativos. Cadernos de Educação, Pelotas, [s.v.], n. 45, p. 220, 2013.
VENTURA, M. M. O Estudo de Caso como Modalidade de Pesquisa. Revista Socerj, Rio de Janeiro,
v. 20, n. 5, p. 383-386, set./out. 2007.
TORISU, E. M.; FERREIRA, A. C. A teoria social cognitiva e o ensino-aprendizagem da matemática:
considerações sobre as crenças de autoeficácia matemática. Ciências e Cognição, Rio de Janeiro, v.
14, n. 3, p. 168-177, 2009.
YAVUZER, Y. Investigating the Relationship between Self-Handicapping Tendencies, Self-Esteem d
Cognitive Distortions. Educational Sciences: Theory and Practice, v. 15, n. 4, p. 879-890, 2015.
YU, J.; McLELLAN, R. Beyond academic achievement goals: the important of social achievement in
explaining gender differences in self-handicapping. Learning and individual differences,
Washington, v. 69, [s.n], p. 33-44, 2019.
ZARPELON, E.; RESENDE, L. M.M.; REIS, E. F. Análise do desempenho de alunos ingressantes de
engenharia na disciplina de cálculo diferencial e integral I. Interfaces da Educação, Campo Grande,
v. 8, n. 22, p. 303-335, 2017.
ZIMMERMAN, B. J. Attaining self-regulation: A social cognitive perspective. In BOEKARTS, M.;
PINTRICH, P. R.; ZEIDNER, M. (orgs.). Handbook of self-regulation. San Diego: Academic Press,
2000. p. 13-39.
Bolema, Rio Claro (SP), v. 37, n. 75, p. 70-90, abr. 2023
89
ISSN 1980-4415
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1980-4415v37n75a04
ZIMMERMAN, B. J.; SCHUNK, D. H. Handbbok of self-regulation of learning and performance.
New York: Routledge, 2011.
ZUCKERMAN, M.; KIEFFER, S. C.; KNEE, C. R. Consequences of self-handicapping: Effects on
coping, academic performance, and adjustment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Washington, v. 74, n. 6, p. 1619 -1628, 1998.
Submetido em 04 de Setembro de 2021.
Aprovado em 29 de Setembro de 2022.
Bolema, Rio Claro (SP), v. 37, n. 75, p. 70-90, abr. 2023
90