The Psychology of Consciousness: Theory and Practice: Hashim Talib Hashim Athanasios Alexiou Editors
The Psychology of Consciousness: Theory and Practice: Hashim Talib Hashim Athanasios Alexiou Editors
The Psychology of Consciousness: Theory and Practice: Hashim Talib Hashim Athanasios Alexiou Editors
The Psychology
of Consciousness:
Theory and
Practice
The Psychology of Consciousness:
Theory and Practice
Hashim Talib Hashim • Athanasios Alexiou
Editors
The Psychology
of Consciousness:
Theory and Practice
Editors
Hashim Talib Hashim Athanasios Alexiou
University of Baghdad - College of Novel Global Community Education
Medicine Foundation
Baghdad, Iraq Hebersham, NSW, Australia
© The Editor(s) (if applicable) and The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature
Switzerland AG 2022
This work is subject to copyright. All rights are solely and exclusively licensed by the Publisher, whether
the whole or part of the material is concerned, specifically the rights of translation, reprinting, reuse of
illustrations, recitation, broadcasting, reproduction on microfilms or in any other physical way, and
transmission or information storage and retrieval, electronic adaptation, computer software, or by similar
or dissimilar methodology now known or hereafter developed.
The use of general descriptive names, registered names, trademarks, service marks, etc. in this publication
does not imply, even in the absence of a specific statement, that such names are exempt from the relevant
protective laws and regulations and therefore free for general use.
The publisher, the authors and the editors are safe to assume that the advice and information in this book
are believed to be true and accurate at the date of publication. Neither the publisher nor the authors or the
editors give a warranty, expressed or implied, with respect to the material contained herein or for any
errors or omissions that may have been made. The publisher remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional
claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
This Springer imprint is published by the registered company Springer Nature Switzerland AG
The registered company address is: Gewerbestrasse 11, 6330 Cham, Switzerland
I want to dedicate this work to my father
(Talib Hashim Manea) who supported and
cared for me all these years.
To my mother (Jawaher Mutar Mohammed)
for everything she has given me since I was
born till now.
To my grandmothers and grandfathers who
have given us such a great and
inspiring family.
To my brothers and sister who never give up
on me, and they were there all the time to
support me.
To my friends and to everyone said a
supporting word one day that made me
continue in our way.
To my professors and to my teachers and to
everyone who encouraged me to do it,
especially Dr. Samer Hoz who inspired me to
write and edit this book.
Foreword
vii
viii Foreword
consciousness. Data such as these are part and parcel of a broader exploration of the
psychology of consciousness. And there are many other fascinating examples from
normal states of consciousness that are experienced by most people to pathological
states of the brain. Importantly, the authors do not shy away from a full exploration
of the topic of the psychology of consciousness.
They appropriately begin with definitions of consciousness. While this seems
to be a natural starting point, in and of itself, it is no easy task. First, conscious-
ness by its very nature is difficult to define. We can start with historical defini-
tions, but we must include more recent work to more deeply understand what
consciousness is. It is also interesting to consider consciousness from various
scholars including philosophers, theologians, biologists, and even those who pur-
sue artificial intelligence. Each of these different perspectives may provide a
slightly different type of definition for consciousness—some more practical and
some more esoteric. In addition to trying to drive a definition of consciousness,
we also have to consider a more operationalized perspective. In other words, how
does consciousness manifest itself within the brain, the mind, and perhaps even
the soul. These interrelationships are also essential for understanding conscious-
ness and also for pursuing future explorations both theoretical and practical.
Perhaps most important in the discussion of what consciousness is relates to the
relationship between the brain and the mind. The age-old question between the
dualistic approach that separates the brain from consciousness or an integrative
approach that combines them in some way is a fundamental target for this book.
If the brain creates consciousness, that leads to an entirely different kind of
approach than if consciousness creates the brain. These are the fascinating ques-
tions that are explored in the pages that follow.
One of the troublesome problems of consciousness is not just a basic definition
but the recognition of different levels of consciousness. There is the unconscious
mind that influences us, without us ever knowing it. Preconsciousness and subcon-
sciousness have an influence on our awareness. There is the conscious mind which
is the primary target of our understanding. But there is even a post-conscious mind
that helps us look back on our own awareness. Each of these needs to be explored in
great detail and is tackled by the authors of The Psychology of Consciousness.
Altered states of consciousness are an essential target of study when it comes to
understanding the larger concept of consciousness itself. They are also helpful in
bringing these questions to a more practical realm. By this I mean that everyone has
experienced certain states of consciousness throughout their everyday lives. The
most basic state of consciousness is waking consciousness which we use when we
go to work or school, talk with a friend, or watch television at night. This state of
consciousness is contrasted by the other most well-known state of consciousness—
sleep. We have all experienced our sleep consciousness and the different elements
that arise within it, particularly dream states. Dream states can be rather chaotic and
brief or long and complex. It is also interesting to note that when one shift from one
state of consciousness to another, we take different perspectives of those states. By
Foreword ix
this I mean that no matter how real a dream consciousness state may feel, when you
wake up, you immediately relegate that state to an inferior state of consciousness or
reality. This latter term is also an important part of understanding consciousness
since it is through our consciousness that we establish what reality actually is. But
what do we make of altered states of consciousness such as dream states, drug-
induced states, or even mystical states when it comes to which is real? It seems that
each of these states of consciousness carry with them a certain level of “realness,”
which we are able to compare against each other. But no matter how real any of
these states feel, they are all housed within our consciousness.
When considering these different states of consciousness, we also have to tie
everything back to our psychology and our biology. When talking about a drug-
induced state, we can ask the question as to what parts of the brain are affected by
the drug? Many psychedelics affect the serotonin system, for example. So why does
intense stimulation of the serotonin system lead to these powerful experiences and
altered states of consciousness. Is consciousness in the serotonin molecule itself?
Or does serotonin activate the neurons of the brain in such a way that facilitates
these shifts in consciousness? And what can we say about psychopathological states
such as depression or schizophrenia in the context of consciousness? Is there a way
to explore consciousness through the lens of psychopathology, and perhaps even
turn that discussion around to help those with these pathologies? The answers to
such questions drive at the heart of what consciousness actually is and how it works
with respect to the brain.
Moving beyond consciousness associated with human beings and perhaps other
animals, consciousness is within the field of computer-based artificial intelligence.
There has been a long debate as to whether we can produce a computer that has
consciousness. The argument focuses on whether consciousness is derivative of a
system’s degree of complexity or whether there is something beyond a complex set
of connections that leads to consciousness. If consciousness exists within the brain
simply because of its complex interconnected network of neurons, then arguably,
we can produce a complex enough computer that would become conscious itself.
But if consciousness derives from something more fundamental, it may be found
only in biological organisms. Alternatively, if consciousness is something that exists
beyond all physical matter, then it can potentially manifest in any physical system.
These are the types of fascinating questions addressed in the pages to follow.
In those pages, the authors will consider a great many of new findings related to
consciousness and strive to develop an understanding that provides important infor-
mation for many of the interrelated fields. Thus, this information is important for the
neuroscientist, the psychologist, the philosopher, and anyone else in the field of
consciousness studies. This book encapsulates a great many ideas and provides a
background that will help people to understand this incredibly complex topic. The
authors are also not afraid to explore a variety of consciousness-related topics that
may not be as popular as current mainstream ideas. But given the complexity and
the challenges around the understanding of consciousness, it is always important to
x Foreword
be open to the many possibilities, even those that seem unlikely. The famous author
and futurist, Arthur C. Clarke, once said, “The only way of discovering the limits of
the possible is to venture a little past them into the impossible.” This book provides
a little bit of a look at both the possible and the impossible and provides an essential
reading for anyone who wants to learn more about this curious thing called
consciousness.
Andrew Newberg
Department of Integrative Medicine
and Nutritional Sciences
Marcus Institute of Integrative Health,
Thomas Jefferson University, Philadelphia, PA, USA
Department of Radiology
Marcus Institute of Integrative Health,
Thomas Jefferson University, Philadelphia, PA, USA
7 December 2020
Foreword
The concept of consciousness has many meanings that are used depending on the
scientific discipline dealing with this idea. The term is also used in everyday speech.
We can describe a man who can critically evaluate his behavior as a person who is
conscious of his behavior. The same is stated by court experts when examining a
criminal who has premeditatedly committed atrocious acts. A conscious choice is
deciding about an activity after considering the benefits and consequences of such
behavior. Conscious decisions have been confirmed in numerous studies by parents
adopting a child, transsexuals who decide to correct their gender, or as is the case in
some countries, chronically ill people who decide to consciously take their own
lives (euthanasia). Consciousness is therefore intentional (Husserl 2002). It is a cer-
tain type of knowledge about a given topic that leads to making a deliberate choice.
One could also say that consciousness has its scope. We speak of global con-
sciousness in the context of targeted measures to improve the quality of life on our
planet (Elgin 1997).
Environmental consciousness is the understanding of the relationship between
human activities and the world of nature (Bonnett 2017). Social awareness is the
totality of ideas, opinions, beliefs, and views—the way we understand and define
the environment—the totality of scientific, philosophical, ideological, political,
legal, religious, and ethical views that make up the way of thinking and culture of a
given society (Durkheim 2003). Awareness of one’s surroundings allows you to
move safely in a certain area or avoid a place due to the hazards there. Self-
consciousness is the ability to undertake activity adequate to the circumstances,
using cultural capital (collective consciousness), learned skills, and respect for val-
ues and principles.
Consciousness may be disturbed or lost as a result of a sudden event or deteriora-
tion of health. It is also possible not to be aware of something due to the lack of
knowledge in a given area or because of strong emotions that interfere with a ratio-
nal assessment of the situation. Consciousness can be acquired, and by planning or
unintentional actions, one can also deprive someone of awareness. Consciousness
can be deepened, expanded, enriched, and impoverished. Consciousness is there-
fore plastic.
xi
xii Foreword
who lost it as a result of illness or accident, but also to those who had manipulated
it. This is just one example of the possibilities of working on consciousness.
Learning about consciousness is a journey. Cognition may be an aim in itself, but
there may be many ways to obtain this knowledge. The traveler may choose a direc-
tion determined by philosophy, psychology, neurobiology, or other sciences refer-
ring to processes related to the creation, acquisition, consolidation, or change of
consciousness. They can also try to cover each of these domains.
It was an honor for me to write a foreword to this book. Having read the chapters
that were sent to me, I am proud to share my thoughts and encourage you to read
this book.
As I wrote above, learning about consciousness is a journey. Therefore, I invite
readers to the extraordinary journey that this book is. Each chapter can be a supple-
ment to the others, and all of them can certainly contribute to the expansion of our
knowledge (and therefore consciousness as well). The journey begins with an intro-
duction to the understanding of consciousness from ancient cultures, through the
deliberations of philosophers and psychoanalysts, to the contemporary definition of
consciousness and an interesting theory of post-consciousness proposed by the
authors. Colorful illustrations, similar to beautiful landscapes, allow you to stop
during this journey and record the adventure with this book better. The continuation
of the educational journey is getting to know the relationship between the brain and
the mind, exploring the essence of the levels of consciousness, both human and
machine. The next stages of the journey through the land of consciousness are other
interesting places, the exploration of which will certainly result in the acquisition of
new knowledge that can be stored in our personal “storage room.” Hypnosis and
awareness, sleep and dreaming, defense mechanisms and altered states of con-
sciousness are areas that are definitely worth exploring. The chapter describing
changes in consciousness caused by the agents causing such modifications is a par-
ticularly interesting stage in my adventure with this book. This is a difficult but defi-
nitely interesting subject. It is good that it was included in the “journey” proposed
by the authors.
The book is aimed at doctors, psychologists, psychiatrists, philosophers, and
mental health professionals, but in my opinion, representatives of other scientific
disciplines can also use it. Importantly, the accessible narrative makes this book
suitable for students at all levels of education.
The authors deserve special recognition for their holistic approach to the subject
matter, for taking up a topic that is not easy, yet presenting it in an accessible way
while adding something new in this regard. Each chapter encourages you to con-
tinue this intellectual and scientific journey and move on to the next pages. There
xiv Foreword
are no boring clichés and unnecessary duplications of what has already been done
in this area. Instead, there is good scientific text that is fresh, evokes reflections, and
remains in the reader’s memory.
It is a pleasant educational journey.
Let us travel then!
Marek A. Motyka,
University of Rzeszów
Rzeszów, Poland
11 December 2020
References
Al-Imam A, Motyka MA, Sahai A, Konuri VK. The “March of Progress”: from cosmic singularity
to digital singularity. Curr Trends Inf Technol. 2020;10(1):1–8.
Bayne T, Carter O. Dimensions of consciousness and the psychedelic state. Neurosci Conscious.
2018;1:niy008. https://doi.org/10.1093/nc/niy008.
Bonnett M. Environmental consciousness, sustainability, and the character of philosophy of educa-
tion. Stud Philos Educ. 2017;36:333–47.
Durkheim E. Les formes elementaires de la vie religieuse Le systeme totemique en Australie [The
elementary forms of the religious life]. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France; 2003.
Elgin D. Global consciousness change: indicators of an emerging paradigm. 1997. https://duaneel-
gin.com/wp-content/uploads/2010/11/global_consciousness.pdf. Accessed 11 Dec 2020.
Husserl E. Ideen zu einer reinen Phänomenologie und phänomenologischen Philosophie [Ideas
pertaining to a pure phenomenology and to a phenomenological philosophy]. Berlin: De
Gruyter; 2002.
Mellibruda J, Sobolewska-Mellibruda Z. Integracyjna psychoterapia uzależnień [Integrative
addiction psychotherapy]. Warszawa: Instytut Psychologii Zdrowia; 2006.
Motyka M. Pamięć niszcząca, pamięć uzdrawiająca: labirynty wspomnień osób uzależnionych od
środków psychoaktywnych [Destructive memory, healing memory: labyrinths of memories of
people addicted to psychoactive substances]. Problemy Higieny i Epidemiologii.
2015;2(96):321–8.
Woydyłło E. Dobra pamięć, zła pamięć [Good memory, bad memory]. Kraków: Wydawnictwo
Literackie; 2014.
Foreword
Lokesh Babu,
Sneha Mano Vikasa Kendra,
Sejal New Life Foundation
Bangalore, India
xv
Preface
xvii
Acknowledgments
We want to thank those people who helped us during the journey of writing this
book. They are:
Mustafa Ahmed Muhson, Yehya Zia Tuama, Ahmed Dhiya Sagban, Shoaib
ahmed, Mohammed Yasir Essar, Dr. Samer Hoz, Christos Tsagkaris, Ali Mahdi
Mansour, Mojtaba Talib, Mohammed Qasim Mohammed, Ahed El Abed El Rassoul,
Abbas Kamil, Abbas Mutar, Mohammed Abbas, Mariam Alaa, Fatima Abbas, Dr.
Nabeel Al Khateeb, Sarmed Kadhem, Zain AL Saleh, Intsar Jabbar, Dr. Alaa Jameel,
Dr. Hussein Al Zaidy, John Bchara, Morad Yasir Al Mostafa, Abdallah Reda,
Nazmus Sakib Choudhary, Fahtiha Nasreen, Hadi Hussein, and Muslim
Hassan Manea.
xix
Contents
Introduction������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������ 1
Hashim Talib Hashim, Mustafa Ahmed Ramadhan,
and Mehek Cheema
Brain and Mind������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������ 19
Hashim Talib Hashim and Adil Alhaideri
Levels of Consciousness ���������������������������������������������������������������������������������� 27
Hashim Talib Hashim and Mustafa Ahmed Ramadhan
Sleep and Dreaming ���������������������������������������������������������������������������������������� 39
Saleh Abdulkareem Saleh
Defense Mechanisms and Personality Disorders������������������������������������������ 57
Saleh Abdulkareem Saleh
Drugs and Consciousness�������������������������������������������������������������������������������� 81
Mustafa Hayder Kadhim
The Altered States of Consciousness�������������������������������������������������������������� 95
Mustafa Hayder Kadhim
Hypnosis and Consciousness�������������������������������������������������������������������������� 109
Hashim Talib Hashim and Mustafa Ahmed Ramadhan
Artificial Consciousness���������������������������������������������������������������������������������� 119
Hashim Talib Hashim and Mustafa Ahmed Ramadhan
Relativity of the Human Mind������������������������������������������������������������������������ 127
Hashim Talib Hashim and Mustafa Ahmed Ramadhan
Death and Consciousness�������������������������������������������������������������������������������� 131
Hashim Talib Hashim and Mustafa Ahmed Ramadhan
Index������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������ 135
xxi
Introduction
These are the early recognized explanations of consciousness. Despite being primi-
tive, they created a steppingstone for developing consciousness theory (Wilber 2004).
They suggested a sensible organization, a sensible organization, a temporal rela-
tionship between the consciousness levels and the humans. “The awareness of being
aware” is the definition used by Shamans and priests (Mayan Theories) (Fig. 1).
It is recognized as a branch of metacognition (higher order thinking skills,
defined as cognition about cognition, thinking about thinking, and knowing about
knowing). Thus, for example, these levels will be organized in a pyramidal shape
from the cellular level to a universal level as shown in Fig. 2.
These are the steps of the development of consciousness according to Mayan
theories. At the same time, Incan theories related to civility, which means progress-
ing from level to level in relation to other changes and concerns (Fig. 3). Thus, the
third level is recognized as the level of most people, and the fifth level is the sign of
Taripay Pacha (Inca day of judgment).
Fig. 2 Τhe steps of the development of consciousness (Pons and Harris 2001; Morin 2006)
The philosopher, John Locke, was from the earliest interested in consciousness. The
main idea of his theory was “Personal ID” and the “Consciousness after death.” He
thought that the personal human ID is essential for psychology’s continuity. He
considered personal ID or the human’s self as part of consciousness, but it is not
involved in the soul or the body matters (Kriegel 2006).
Personal identity theory is a confrontation in philosophy with the questions
humans ask about their origin and presence and the end of their life, like who is the
Introduction 3
human, why we are here, will live another life after we die, etc. So, it aligns with
consciousness, starting with it and ending with it consequently.
According to Locke’s theory, “Understanding is one of the first model conceptu-
alizations of consciousness as repeated self–identification” (Nimbalkar 2011).
Locke created what’’ called an “Empty Mind” full of nothing, which can be created
by ideas such as the experience, the sensations, and the reflections. He created a
common concept between the body and the soul, which might not be acceptable to
those who identify consciousness as the brain. According to his theory, the brain is
similar to the body and, similarly to any other substance, may alter while conscious-
ness remains identical. Therefore, personal identity cannot be located in the brain
but within consciousness. The issue with this theory is life after death because to
stay alive after death, there should be a person similar to the person who was there
before death.
He suggested that consciousness can move from anyone’s soul to another one’s
soul, and he thought that the Id moves with it.
John thought that God sees differently from others, so people will judge you
about what they see, but God does not. As a result, the human will be responsible
for the actions for which the human is conscious (Butler 1975; Reid 1785).
John concluded that the actions and ideas that the person makes during youth
would shape the future and be the cornerstone for the upcoming actions.
4 H. T. Hashim et al.
Fig. 5 Human consciousness is the result of the interaction between physiology and behavior
6 H. T. Hashim et al.
4 Spinoza Ideas
Spinoza sought to transcend Descartes’ dualism. Rather than matter and mind
becoming independent objects, Spinoza claimed that there is only one concept.
Therefore, mind and extension are the properties of that one substance.
So, this merely changed the dualism of objects to a dualism of qualities. But
unfortunately, it also retained Nature’s mechanical concept presented by Descartes,
rendering humankind exposed to an absolute mechanical fatalism.
Freudian theory has two components, topographical theory and structural theory.
These two theories overlap and interact with each other (Fig. 6).
Topographical Theory of the mind: It consists of three levels, and each of those
three levels is specialized in some regions and thoughts. These three levels are:
1. Unconscious
2. Preconscious
3. Conscious
Introduction 7
Even though the Freudian theory is still the most known explanation for con-
sciousness, many schools and fields of psychology have started to develop new
theories and levels to widen the science of consciousness. But all these theories
are going in the same way, which is to understand the human self and answer the
questions and queries of the consciousness. There are many models developed to
explain the levels of consciousness depending on many criteria. We will discuss
three models of these modern perspectives and the levels that they thought more
suitable:
8 H. T. Hashim et al.
Holder divided the consciousness into three levels that give distinct differences in
how they are reached.
Spontaneous: The mind keeps up the development and the progression of life
regardless of the future and the past events.
Calculated: It depends on the mind’s ideas of what is right and what is wrong.
Imposed: No awareness, and it ends with failure.
Dr. Bob Gibson stated four levels of consciousness: tiers of extrasensory awareness
(Pons and Harris 2001; Tindall 1990). These levels interfere with the previous theo-
ries of the modern insight of consciousness. Gibson’s insight focuses on some
moments of a human’s personal experience.
1. Sleepness: “Unaware of all surroundings, dreams may or may not occur.”
2. Waking sleepness: “Normal tasks can be performed in sleep, but the individual
is not receptive to what is taking place.”
3. The self-awareness: “Able to identify surroundings and observe what is tak-
ing place.”
4. The objective awareness: “Identify surrounding events without opinions or
inputs.”
Gibson’s theory is more acceptable and accurate than the other theories in mod-
ern ideas. It is very descriptive and observable (Pons and Harris 2001; Tindall 1990).
It is an analytical description for imagination and the control of great power such as
God and prophets (Lee 2012).
environment, desires, and purposes, unlike even our closest primate family.
Interestingly, then, one of the main aims of conscious processing at the person’s
level might be to remove the need for himself as far as possible in the future, freeing
himself up for even greater things. Indeed, if the evolved aim of consciousness
turned out to be the development of ever more complex non-aware systems, it would
be ironic, considering the juxtaposition of automatic and aware systems in contem-
porary social psychology.
9 Neuropsychology on Consciousness
the normal brain is still involved. The natural unconsciousness of dreamless sleep is
a physiological state of brain functioning.
We will discuss the neuropsychology of consciousness with details in chapter
“Brain and Mind”, linking the brain to consciousness and discussing the differences
between the mind and the brain (Fig. 11).
2. René Descartes concluded that the theory of the mind contains two domains that
the consciousness resides in, which are:
(a) Res Cogitans and Res extensa.
(b) Preconscious and Postconsious.
(c) Id and Ego.
(d) Imagination and Reality.
3. The relationship between the mind and the body is described as:
(a) A complementary relationship.
(b) There is no relation between them.
(c) How they can be related and how they affect one another. The characteristics
of each one is different from the other.
(d) Integrity relationship.
4. Topographical and structural theories have been described by:
(a) Sigmund Freud
(b) John Locke
(c) Hashim and Mustafa
(d) Spinoza
5. Holder divided the consciousness into three levels that give distinct differences
in how they are reached. These three levels are:
(a) Sleep, Walking, and Self-awareness.
(b) Spontaneous, Calculated, and Imposed.
(c) Id, Ego, and Superego.
(d) Conscious, Postconscious, and Unconcious.
6. Psychoatomic (Quantum Non-Local) is:
(a) Overmind, developed by consciousness maturity.
(b) It is the adult personality and developed from first meeting experiences. It
includes pleasure, reproduction, and nurture.
(c) It is Buddha-Monad Mind, developed by consciousness maturation. It
includes evolutionary consciousness, DNA–RNA brain feedbacks.
(d) It is a Zen-Yoga mind–body connection developed by neurological somatic
feedback and reprogramming. It includes the consciousness of the body.
7. Freud describes consciousness as:
(a) A state of awareness of our external environment.
(b) An imaginary concept to describe our awareness.
(c) An origin for our reality.
(d) A quality and the capacity of transforming experienced activity into uncon-
scious states.
8. A psychiatry branch that describes the relationship between the brain and the
psychology is called:
(a) Neurology
(b) Neurosciences
(c) Neuropsychiatry
(d) Neuropsychology
Introduction 17
Answers
1. (d)
2. (a)
3. (c)
4. (a)
5. (b)
6. (a)
7. (d)
8. (c)
9. (c)
References
Bargh JA. Social psychological approaches to consciousness. In: The Cambridge handbook of
consciousness. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2007. p. 555–69.
Butler J. Of personal identity. In: The analogy of religion. Berkeley: University of California
Press; 1975. p. 100. Original reprinted in Perry, John, 1736, ‘Personal Identity’.
Gennaro R. Consciousness and self-consciousness. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing; 1996.
Hashim H. The role and the spectrum of the post-conscious in the control of behavior by geriatric
subjects. NPG Neurologie - Psychiatrie - Gériatrie. 2020;20(119):297–302.
Hashim H, Ramadhan M. The need for developing a fourth level of awareness in human conscious-
ness: unconsciousness, preconsciousness, consciousness and postconsciousness. J Psychol
Psychother. 2019;9(3)
Hashim HT, Ramadhan MA. The concept of post-consciousness & its role in human behavior. J
Conscious Explor Res. 2020;11:1.
Hughes H, Hoffman S. Consciousness and society. New Brunswick: Transaction Publication; 2008.
Kriegel U. Consciousness, theories of. Philos Compass. 2006;1(1):58–64.
Lee M. The sword: the development of consciousness. J Symbols Sandplay Ther. 2012;3(2):45–65.
Mcleod S. Unconscious mind | simply psychology. Simplypsychologyorg. 2021. <https://www.
simplypsychology.org/unconscious-mind.html> Accessed 16 Dec 2021.
Miller G. What is the biological basis of consciousness? Science. 2005;309(5731):79.
Morin A. Levels of consciousness and self-awareness: a comparison and integration of various
views. Conscious Cogn. 2006;15(2):358–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2005.09.006.
Natsoulas T. The freudian conscious. Conscious Emotion. 2001;2(1):1–28.
Nimbalkar N. John locke on personal identity. Mens Sana Monogr. 2011;9(1):268–75. https://doi.
org/10.4103/0973-1229.77443.
Pons F, Harris P. Piaget’s conception of the development of consciousness: an examination of two
hypotheses. Hum Dev. 2001;44(4):220–7. https://doi.org/10.1159/000057061.
Reid T. Essays on the intellectual powers of man. Edinburg/London: John Bell/G.G.J. and
J. Robinson; 1785. p. 317–8.
18 H. T. Hashim et al.
Tindall SC. Level of consciousness. In: Walker HK, Hall WD, Hurst JW, editors. Clinical meth-
ods: the history, physical, and laboratory examinations (PDF). Boston: Butterworths; 1990.
p. 296–9. ISBN 0-409-90077-X.
Wilber K. Integral psychology: consciousness, spirit, psychology, therapy. Nova Religio.
2004;8(2):125–7.
Wilson RA. Cosmic trigger: final secret of the illuminati. Las Vegas: New Falcon Publications;
2008. p. 1–269. ISBN 978-1561840038
Brain and Mind
1 Brain Regions
The brain region is separated into left and right hemispheres. The corpus callosum
joins these two hemispheres through white matter fibers (He et al. 2017).
The limbic system is responsible for memory and emotions (Fig. 1). In addition,
it enables the hippocampus, the thalamus, the hypothalamus, and the brainstem to
have complex connections. The limbic system does not have precise, specific ana-
tomical limits but contains many substantial structures.
The amygdala is a series of nuclei found inside the brain (Fig. 2). It receives
several sensory data modes as inputs. The outputs pass via the amygdala via the stria
terminalis and ventral amygdalofugal pathway (Fig. 3).
Pathologies in the basal nuclei can be traced to certain movement conditions, the
most notable being Parkinson’s syndrome, which is due to defects in substantia
nigra dopaminergic cells (Wardlaw et al. 2020; Mendez and Hong 1997; Rughani
et al. 2015).
The neocortex is the human brain’s most phylogenetically evolved organ. The
intricate folding pattern causes a smaller cranial volume to be covered by an
expanded cortical surface. The folding sequence that shapes the sulcal and gyral
patterns remains strongly maintained in humans. This makes for a cortical anatomy
nomenclature (Wardlaw et al. 2020; Herbet and Duffau 2020).
Just below the pons, the pyramids and pyramidal decussation are visualized ven-
trally. They are the descending corticospinal tracts (Jhawar et al. 2016).
The cerebellum has two hemispheres, connected via a midline structure called
“the vermis.” The cerebellar cortex has three membranes, in contrast to the neocor-
tex of the cerebrum: molecular, Purkinje, and granular (Siegel et al. 1999). There
are afferent and efferent pathways inside the three cerebellar peduncles to and from
the cerebellum. “The dissertation of neurologist Paul Broca, who researched the
language disorders of stroke survivors, can be traced to some of the earliest contri-
butions to modern language mapping” (Binkofski and Buccino 2004; Stein 2017).
The main sensory cortex, or Brodmann’s areas 1–3, corresponds to the postcen-
tral gyrus. That of the motor cortex corresponds to the homunculus obtained from
awake mapping (Adrián-Ventura et al. 2019).
2 Brain–Mind Relationship
The word “mind” is widely used also within the scientific discourse to denote a
wide spectrum of connotations. The mind can describe the human consciousness
when the mind–brain connection is meant. However, consciousness, which can be
seen as a confounding paradox, is itself impossible to describe, given that it consti-
tutes perhaps the most immediate and intimately available feature of any person’s
existence (Kochiyama et al. 2018; Solomito et al. 2019).
While the brain is the body’s most relevant organ in relation to the mind, more
precise information is available (Duffau 2018; Solomito et al. 2019).
But if it is possible to clarify the mind–brain interaction, what sort of answer is
being sought? It is important to consider previous scientific breakthroughs to
address this key issue, culminating in a satisfactory explanation of interactions pre-
viously though difficult to describe (Forsell et al. 2020).
Although neuroanatomy provides the fundamental blueprint for nervous system
information processing, the neural function is characterized by turbulent processes
characteristic of complex systems. Thus, a complete comprehension of the brain as
it applies to behavioral information would have to include appropriate accounts of
synaptic dynamics and connectivity, as these elements are essential to conscious
brain activity (Forsell et al. 2020).
It is possible to generalize this primary instance to all human contact. How much
of the intended message is successfully conveyed on average among aware indi-
viduals communicating? We speculate that correspondence hovers between 50%
and 85%, except in the most desirable situations of friends addressing their shared
jobs or partners chatting about their family. If this is the case, 10% or less may be
the normal human contact between two people (including casual interactions).
The immature brain has uncommitted, immature associations at birth, formed
from within the primary. As a result, the senses of a newborn baby are overwhelmed
with the outer world’s many colors, noises, smells, tastes, and textures, as well as
certain perceptions and emotions that originate from inside.
Increased activation contributes to increased synaptic complexity in the develop-
ing brain. As each person’s perception is distinct, each brain–mind has different
connectivity patterns. As Greenfield states, “The brain is made up of links between
brain cells.”
22 H. T. Hashim and A. Alhaideri
Implicit memory, which arises in the early years of childhood or out of intense
dissociation produced through a traumatic experience, retains the habits that control
our forms of being, behaving, and relating without conscious consciousness. It is
Released in cases of misunderstanding which unpredictability, which inevitably
provides the models in the consultation room for both the transition and counter-
transfer encounter (Lei et al. 2017).
3 Consciousness in Medicine
There are two popular and useful tools used in assessing the level of conscious-
ness for any patient which are (Opara et al. 2014; Reith et al. 2017):
• Grady Coma Scale (GCS): Starts from 1 to 5 depending on the level of con-
sciousness and the patient’s responses to the stimuli. Grade 1 is active and Grade
5 means that the patient is in a coma.
• Glasgow Coma Scale (GCS): Starts from 1 to 15 depending on specific criteria
and stimuli and measures the responses. Less than 3 means that the patient is in
a coma or brain death, while 9 and above means that the patient’s neurological
condition is good.
Multiple Choice Questions
1. ………… is involved in memory and emotions.
(a) Cerebellum
(b) Limbic system
(c) Thalamus
(d) Hypothalamus
2. Although neuroanatomy provides the fundamental blueprint for nervous sys-
tem information processing, the neural function itself is characterized by:
(a) Turbulent processes characteristic of complex systems.
(b) Mind–brain relationships.
(c) Conscious reactions.
(d) Moving the body parts.
3. The differences between human and other animal brains are:
(a) The neocortex is the human brain’s most phylogenetically evolved organ.
(b) The animals lack cerebellum.
(c) Humans have bigger brains than animals.
(d) Humans have more complicated brains than animals.
4. There are two hemispheres in the cerebellum, connected by a midline struc-
ture called:
(a) The limbic system
(b) The vermis
(c) The sulci
(d) The gyrus
5. The patient is alert and responsive:
(a) Unconscious
(b) Coma
(c) Conscious
(d) Sleepy
6. The patient is not responsive at all and may have reduced brain activity:
(a) Coma
(b) Lethargy
(c) Unconscious
(d) Confusion
24 H. T. Hashim and A. Alhaideri
7. Delirium is:
(a) Extreme drowsiness, listlessness, and apathy followed by decreased alert-
ness are identified.
(b) Acute confessional condition, marked by diminished cognition, in specific
concentration, sleep-wake period modification, hyperactivity (agitation) or
hypo-activity (apathy), cognitive abnormalities such as hallucinations (see-
ing objects that are not there) or illusions (false beliefs), as well as pulse
rate and blood pressure dysfunction.
(c) No response at all.
(d) Insensitive.
8. Obtundation is:
(a) This is a condition of unresponsiveness, including stimulus.
(b) Awakeness and alertness.
(c) With sluggish responses to stimuli, a decline in alertness needs repeated
stimulation to sustain concentration.
(d) Acute confessional condition, marked by diminished cognition, in specific
concentration, sleep-wake period modification, hyperactivity (agitation) or
hypo-activity (apathy), cognitive abnormalities such as hallucinations (see-
ing objects that are not there) or illusions (false beliefs), as well as pulse
rate and blood pressure dysfunction.
9. GCS of 14 is considered as:
(a) Good
(b) Fair
(c) Poor
(d) Excellent
10. In neuropsychology, the localization of psychological roles in the brain has
historically been focused on the finding that there is:
(a) A deficiency or disorder of a certain psychological capacity in patients car-
rying a lesion in a certain cerebral area, whereas other abilities are retained.
(b) A balance between the chemical and physical components of the brain.
(c) A reaction between the neurotransmitters and the body’s status.
(d) A balance between the sympathetic and parasympathetic systems.
Answers
1. (b)
2. (a)
3. (a)
4. (b)
5. (c)
6. (a)
7. (b)
8. (c)
9. (a)
10. (a)
Brain and Mind 25
References
Adrián-Ventura J, et al. Linking personality and brain anatomy: a structural MRI approach to rein-
forcement sensitivity theory. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2019;14(3):329–38.
Binkofski F, Buccino G. Motor functions of the Broca’s region. Brain Lang. 2004;89(2):362–9.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00358-4.
Cecil RL, Goldman L, Schafer AI. Goldman’s Cecil medicine. 24th ed. Philadelphia: Elsevier/
Saunders; 2012.
Daroff R, Fenichel G, Jankovic J, Mazziotta J. Bradley’s neurology in clinical practice. 6th ed.
Philadelphia: Elsevier/Saunders; 2012.
Duffau H. The error of Broca: from the traditional localizationist concept to a connectomal anat-
omy of human brain. J Chem Neuroanat. 2018;89:73–81.
Forsell L, et al. BrainWiki—a wiki-style, user driven, comparative brain anatomy tool. Front
Neuroanat. 2020;14:548172.
He Q, Turel O, Bechara A. Brain anatomy alterations associated with social networking site (SNS)
addiction. Sci Rep. 2017;7(1):1–8.
Herbet G, Duffau H. Revisiting the functional anatomy of the human brain: toward a meta-
networking theory of cerebral functions. Physiol Rev. 2020;100(3):1181–228.
Huntley A. Documenting level of consciousness. Nursing. 2008;38(8):63–4. https://doi.
org/10.1097/01.NURSE.0000327505.69608.35.
Jhawar SS, Nunez M, Pacca P, Voscoboinik DS, Truong H. Craniovertebral junction 360°: a
combined microscopic and endoscopic anatomical study. J Craniovertebral Junction Spine.
2016;7(4):204–16. https://doi.org/10.4103/0974-8237.193270. PMID: 27891029; PMCID:
PMC5111321.
Kochiyama T, et al. Reconstructing the neanderthal brain using computational anatomy. Sci Rep.
2018;8(1):1–9.
Lei Y, et al. The brain interstitial system: anatomy, modeling, in vivo measurement, and applica-
tions. Prog Neurobiol. 2017;157:230–46.
Mendez I, Hong M. Reconstruction of the striato-nigro-striatal circuitry by simultaneous double
dopaminergic grafts: a tracer study using fluorogold and horseradish peroxidase. Brain Res.
1997;778(1):194–205. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0006-8993(97)01055-x.
Opara JA, Małecka E, Szczygiel J. Clinimetric measurement in traumatic brain injuries. J Med
Life. 2014;7(2):124–7.
Rakel RE, Rakel D. Textbook of family medicine. 8th ed. Philadelphia: Elsevier/Saunders; 2011.
Reith FCM, Lingsma HF, Gabbe BJ, Lecky FE, Roberts I, Maas AIR. Differential effects of the
Glasgow coma scale score and its components: an analysis of 54,069 patients with traumatic
brain injury. Injury. 2017;48(9):1932–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.injury.2017.05.038.
Rughani AI, Penar PJ, Gest TR. Brain anatomy. Medscape; 2015.
Siegel GJ, Agranoff BW, Albers RW, Fisher SK, Uhler MD. Basic neurochemistry, molecular, cel-
lular, and medical aspects. 6th ed. Philadelphia: Lippincott, Williams & Wilkins; 1999.
Solomito MJ, Reuman H, Wang DH. Sex differences in concussion: a review of brain anatomy,
function, and biomechanical response to impact. Brain Inj. 2019;33(2):105–10.
Stein J. Sensorimotor control. Reference module in neuroscience and biobehavioral psychology.
Amsterdam: Elsevier; 2017. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-809324-5.06855-3. ISBN
9780128093245.
Wardlaw JM, et al. Perivascular spaces in the brain: anatomy, physiology and pathology. Nat Rev
Neurol. 2020;16(3):137–53.
Levels of Consciousness
1 Introduction
Before we start with the levels, we should explain Id, Ego, and Superego according
to Freud’s theory (Fig. 1):
• Id (Meeting basic needs): This is the most primitive part of the personality; it
contains the most animalistic urges. For example, eating, drinking, and having
sex. It provides gratification for human needs. If humans do not get their needs,
they will go into a disorder like anxiety, tension, and anger. This level is pre-
sented at birth. The examples for that are: when you are hungry, you will enter
the kitchen and search for food, open the food pots, search in the fridge for any-
thing that can be eaten, when you do not find anything to keep you settled, the
situation will be undesired. It is the same situation when your hungry baby is
crying; he will not stop until he gets his needed food.
• Ego (Dealing with reality): This concept tries to meet the desire that Id present
in a socially acceptable way. This can mean delaying the gratification and help-
ing to get rid of disordered thoughts like tension or anger. This level is developed
at birth. For example, when someone is Muslim, during Ramadhan, he fasts dur-
ing the day and should wait for the sunset to eat and drink. So, the Id desires to
eat and drink, and the Ego is to wait until sunset.
• Superego (Adding Morals): It is lastly developed; it depends on morals and
judgments about right and wrong. The Id and Ego can reach the same point as the
Superego does, but Superego depends on morals while the Ego depends on the
thinking and the consequences of the actions. So, losing the Superego makes Ego
dominate, and losing Ego makes Id dominate. It begins to develop at about
6 years of age. For example, when you want to steal from your family and know
Fig. 1 Freud divides the mind into three levels according to his topographical theory: uncon-
scious, preconscious, and conscious. These three levels interfere with structural theory compo-
nents: Id, Ego, and Superego
Postconscious
Conscious
Subconscious
Preconscious
Unconscious
that no one will note or judge you, you don’t do this because you know that steal-
ing is wrong.
We will start in a sequence from the first level: unconscious, preconscious, sub-
conscious, conscious to postconscious (Fig. 2).
Levels of Consciousness 29
2 Preconscious
It refers to the thoughts that humans are not actively thinking of but can be retrieved
easily once the human wants that; it contrasts with the unconscious, whose thoughts
are repressed and cannot be retrieved or remembered easily.
Preconscious was developed in the psychoanalytic theory by Freud. However,
eclectic therapists and others may use the concept in their ideas.
The thoughts at this level are not in our current thinking, but humans can think of
them quickly by triggering them using a stimulus of something that links them.
These thoughts are pulled out of the unconscious to be stored in the preconscious
and then transformed into conscious when needed. Ego and Superego control it only
without Id (Fazekas and Overgaard 2016).
A preconscious mind is a convenient thing to have. It provides several valuable
things that the conscious mind cannot handle alone.
The type of memories stored here is the long-term memories that align with the
unconscious. They are both unconscious, and preconscious includes the memories
that can be retrieved longer than a few minutes. The repressed memories can be
recalled without a potent stimulus or trigger. So, preconscious is the caller one that
connects unconscious with conscious.
Preconscious provides solutions for worries and problems without being aware
of them. The process is going on at this level until a solution comes up. Like it works
behind the scenes (Fazekas and Overgaard 2016).
The preconscious also contains intuitive thoughts. For example, when you meet
someone, your mind will judge it as good or not good. This process occurs in the
preconscious, depending on your brain’s criteria to judge people and other thoughts
and situations (Roulin and Ramelet 2014).
Adults have more active preconscious infants and teenagers because it depends
on experience and knowledge taken throughout life and ages. These ideas support
self-protection and achievements.
Therapies in psychiatry and psychology target preconscious because they need to
change bad thoughts to overcome good ones. The therapists can usually prompt the
patient to remember facts and events that will help him build his own conclusions.
If the psychologists already know your history, they may have some ideas about
what kind of experience he might suffer to explain the current situations (Overgaard
and Overgaard 2010).
There is a type of therapy called “Connecting memories.” In this therapy, the
therapist tries to connect the patient’s memory with another one to use the same
solution for the previous problem in the current one. It is a connection between the
preconscious and conscious. The ability to solve problems and issues in the con-
scious mind depends on using the information and thoughts in the preconscious. If
humans cannot, they will be without benefit.
In conclusion, preconscious is characterized by reality testing, recallable memo-
ries, and links to word presentation—the key distinction from unconscious contents.
30 H. T. Hashim and M. A. Ramadhan
Words presentations are memory traces that were at one-time perception and
therefore can become conscious again.
According to the French psychosomatic school, sound organization of precon-
scious is aligned with sound mentalization, whereas occasional long-term weakness
can lead to “Operative Functioning” according to the French psychosomatic school.
Inversely, the sound functioning of the preconscious guarantees a rich capacity for
fantasizing (Overgaard and Overgaard 2010).
3 Subconscious
It is part of the mind that is not currently in focal awareness. But it is the powerful
secondary system that runs everything in a human’s life. The subconscious is a bank
of data for everything that is not in the conscious; it stores beliefs, thoughts, memo-
ries, and previous experiences. So, everything that humans see, think, and do is
stored here (Fig. 3).
Subconscious is a guidance system that monitors information and actions com-
ing out of senses and opportunities. Thus, the relationship between the subcon-
scious and conscious is bidirectional (Nelson 2005).
Emotions control communication between the subconscious and conscious, so
moving thoughts to another is done under their control. Only thoughts transferred
with guanine emotions make it possible to be back for the mind. And only the emo-
tions that are backed up by solid emotions are stored in this level. Both negative and
positive emotions have a role in this level, and the negative ones are dominant
(Nelson 2005).
Fear and negative talking to self are contributing factors in dominating the nega-
tive emotions, so to be in a good situation, you have to eliminate these thoughts.
Because sometimes, fears and bad thoughts can be real and maybe come true
(Hawkins 2015).
Subconscious
Conscious
Levels of Consciousness 31
There is a solution that can be used to get rid of bad thoughts through countering
techniques. By confronting these ideas with extreme positive counter thoughts. For
example, an employee on his first interview with a human resources manager in a
company, when he thinks that he will not be competitive and efficient for the posi-
tion, immediately face these thoughts with good ones by thinking that you will
convince the manager by his skills. There is another technique called “Delete
Button.” When humans think of bad thoughts, they immediately delete them and
replace them with good ones (Hawkins 2015).
Desire has a role in this level; depending on the desire, the subconscious tries to
do anything to reach the desire that the mind seeks. For example, a very brave com-
mander in a war is facing a weak army. The weak army makes noise and hits the
drums to make the commander think they are not afraid of him; in this battle, the
commander lost because of his thoughts.
The subconscious mind combines what one sees, experiences, and any knowl-
edge that the mind absorbs that it does not otherwise actively interpret to make
coherent sense. The conscious mind cannot necessarily process disconnected
knowledge since it will be abundant information. Still, the subconscious mind
retains this data where the conscious mind can recover it as it has to protect itself for
survival (and for other purposes, such as solving puzzles).
The subconscious mind retains knowledge that the conscious mind does not
automatically process with complete comprehension, but when “reminded” by the
conscious mind, or by an astute psychoanalyst who can pull out the information
contained in the subconscious, taking it to the conscious consciousness of the per-
son, it retains the information for later retrieval.
The subconscious mind retains knowledge that the conscious mind does not
automatically process with complete comprehension, but when “reminded” by the
conscious mind, or by an astute psychoanalyst who can pull out the information
contained in the subconscious, taking it to the conscious consciousness of the per-
son, it retains the information for later retrieval.
De Becker tapped into the victim’s mind about her “prior knowledge of the sub-
conscious mind that prompted her to act unconsciously,” causing her to know that
the attacker would kill her. The analyst took her conscious mind to consider by
eliciting her initial “inner thoughts/voice” HOW her subconscious acted on her con-
scious mind. Via a sequence of events that eventually drove her subconscious mind
to function in such a way as to prevent her from being killed. Gavin was able to
evoke awareness from her subconscious mind of a threatening condition that
prompted her conscious mind to intervene to save her by her simple instinct of sur-
vival, taking to the conscious mind of the survivor that it was the “subtle warning
that alerted her.” The survivor characterized this as an unrecognized terror that
drove her to act, still consciously uncertain of exactly WHY she was afraid. Her
conscious mind had said the words, “I swear I would not harm you, although her
subconscious mind measured the circumstance even more easily than the conscious
mind could make sense of WHY there was terror. The victim said that” the beast
took over inside her. It is impossible to separate the unconscious from the subcon-
scious. Some authors have actually noted that they are used interchangeably in
32 H. T. Hashim and M. A. Ramadhan
common parlance and by many experienced writers. It’s good to think about con-
scious consciousness as the tip of an iceberg, as in discriminating between sup-
pressed and suppressed: it’s so apparent over the sea. Although the unconscious and
subconscious are far greater than what the eye can see together, both remain below
what is readily apparent. Thus, knowing their relative inaccessibility is the only
practical way they can be set apart (Wu et al. 2019).
In brief, you will possibly recognize from where your thought, instinct, or inspi-
ration is subconsciously derived through some introspection. Yet, it would be much
more difficult to identify the source of present-day activity that simply does not
make much sense to you for what is unconscious to you, the bottom-most portion of
the iceberg. In addition, though, it is much more likely that you can easily unveil its
roots with the help of a mental health provider (Wu et al. 2019).
Many ways to manipulate the subconscious mind specifically, including the
following:
• Affirmations
• Autosuggestion
• Binaural beats
• Hypnosis
• Placebo
• Priming
• Subliminal perception
• Subliminal stimulation
• Suggestion
4 Conscious
The conscious mind acts as the current situation for the human. It is simply our cur-
rent behaviors and actions. These two functions can be consciously responsible for
in general:
• Its ability to direct human focus.
• Its ability to create images that are not real.
The conscious mind acts as a scanner for the human self. It perceives events, trig-
gers the need to react, and then depends on the importance of that event.
It can store these events either in the Unconscious or Subconscious area of the
mind where they will be available to human needs, and humans can recall them once
they want (Young and Rund 2010).
The conscious mind includes everything inside of human awareness in the psy-
choanalytic theory. So, it makes the mind think and act rationally by mental
processing.
The conscious mind controls sensations, perceptions, memories, feelings, and
fantasies inside a human’s current awareness. The closer level of awareness to
Levels of Consciousness 33
5 Postconscious
postconsciousness that offers the reason to do the wrong things will overcome this
dispute, and it is essential and will never be repeated. As humans, we have certain
situations and attitudes that other forms of consciousness cannot be clarified since
the four levels of perception in our consciousness influence all our behavior and
reactions. All of these responses are that our attitudes and disposals alter whether
we decide to make a mistake or it has just been done. As a consequence of our
behavior, which is the failures, the explanation of these changes in our reactions is
regulated as we try to approve the fourth stage of consciousness, the postconscious-
ness, in this analysis. As a consequence of our behavior, which is the failures, the
explanation of these changes in our reactions is regulated as we try to approve the
fourth stage of consciousness, the postconsciousness in this analysis (Hashim
and Ramadhan 2019; Hashim and Ramadhan 2020; Hashim 2020).
The postconscious advises you to do these reactions or instruct the body to dem-
onstrate these reactions (in a detailed description) to overlap the doubts and worries
that might exist or reassure you that you will not be discovered or captured. So, it
wants to defend you, and it tries to get you as happy as possible at the same time. In
this phase, age and gender do not influence because it happens to anyone at any age,
regardless of gender. Under the same names in Freud’s principles of consciousness,
we should not accept these emotions because they are distinct.
The postconscious is a small state that in elderly people may have less value and
can cause other effects that vary from their usual roles in their brains. Over the life
of an older person, certain thoughts and perceptions may change: some of them will
vanish, and new ideas will arise, describing odd behaviors at this point. The figures
below compare the four levels of consciousness in the factors that almost control
them and the differences between them (Figs. 4 and 5).
5. ………….. acts as a scanner for the human self. It perceives events, triggers the
need to react, and then depends on the importance of that event.
(a) Conscious
(b) Subconscious
(c) Preconscious
(d) Postconscious
6. ………… is an imaginary (unreal) state of consciousness.
(a) Subconscious
(b) Conscious
(c) Preconscious
(d) Postconscious
7. Subliminal stimulation is one of the ways that manipulate:
(a) Conscious
(b) Subconscious
(c) Preconscious
(d) Postconscious
8. One of the following is present at birth:
(a) Preconscious
(b) Conscious
(c) Unconscious
(d) Postconscious
9. All the following has an Id involvement except:
(a) Preconscious
(b) Conscious
(c) Unconscious
(d) Postconscious
10. Postconscious is:
(a) An area between conscious and unconscious.
(b) A complimentary area for consciousness.
(c) A part of the unconscious.
(d) An integrated Level of consciousness
Answers
1. (c)
2. (b)
3. (a)
4. (d)
5. (a)
6. (d)
7. (b)
8. (c)
9. (c)
10. (a)
Levels of Consciousness 37
References
1 Sleep
The quality of sleep is essential for survival as food and water. Most body systems
enter the anabolic state during sleep and help maintain normal body function
(Sollars and Pickard 2015).
The term circadian means “around the day” in Latin, so circadian rhythms are phys-
ical, mental, and behavioral changes that follow a daily cycle by responding to light
and darkness. They are also present in plants, animals, and microbes. At the same
time, biological clocks are a collection of specific proteins that interact with cells
throughout the body and act as innate timing devices to produce and regulate circa-
dian rhythms (Schupp and Hanning 2003).
The circadian rhythm controls sleep patterns. SCN is triggered by incoming light
from the optic nerve and controls the production of the melatonin
S. A. Saleh (*)
University of Baghdad, College of Medicine, Baghdad, Iraq
hormone—responsible for sleeping. When the incoming light decreases the SCN, it
stimulates the brain to produce more melatonin to make us drowsy and prepared for
sleep (Fig. 1). In this way, the circadian rhythm controls sleep and wakefulness
throughout the day and night to create a stable regular cycle. Disruption to this cir-
cadian system can result in sleep difficulties and disturbances.
Two types of stages for sleep are discussed. They are (Fig. 2):
1. Non-Rapid Eye Movement (NREM).
2. Rapid Eye Movement (REM).
This represents the first part of the sleep cycle, and it is subdivided into three distinct
stages: stage I, stage II, and stage III (also known as N1, N2, N3). Muscle paralysis
does not occur appropriately during NREM sleep; that’s why some people do not
progress to REM sleepwalk during their sleeping because they do not lose their
motor function; also, dreaming rarely occurs in this type (Tubbs et al. 2019).
Sleep and Dreaming 41
Stage I
Sleep starts by a transition from wakefulness to drowsiness state, during wakeful-
ness—when eyes are open—alpha and beta waves are present, and beta waves are
the predominant ones, while during the drowsiness state—when eyes closed—alpha
waves (with frequency 8–12 Hz) become predominant. Stage I starts when these
alpha waves are replaced by theta waves (with frequency 4–7 Hz). During this stage,
a selective arousal threshold will determine if a specific minor stimulus is worth
responding to and trigger the wakefulness state or should ignore it and proceed to
the next stage of sleep. Typically, this stage lasts for 5–10 min only, and it’s the entry
point for stage II.
42 S. A. Saleh
Stage II
It is a short stage in the first two cycles ranging from 10 to 20 min. Then, it becomes
dominant.
Heart rate, body temperature, breathing will decrease in this stage, and muscle
will relax even further (Varga et al. 2018).
Stage III
They are also called slow-wave sleep (SWS), representing the deepest sleep stage
and the most difficult to awaken. Older people spend less time in this stage. That’s
why they have a light sleep and are easily awakened. However, waking during this
stage will cause a transient phase of mental disorientation and a moderate decrease
in mental performance for a short period: body repair tissue growth (Della Monica
et al. 2018).
REM Sleep
This type of sleep is characterized by brain waves similar to the awake state, mus-
cles paralysis—except the diaphragm and upper airway muscles—to prevent acting
out of the dreams by inhibiting motor neurons in the brainstem and rapid jerking eye
movement from side to side. It occurs after 90 min of falling asleep. The average
time of the first REM period after falling asleep is 90 min, and the individual who is
deprived of REM sleep one night has increased REM sleep the next night (REM
rebound). Dreams mainly occur during this type and are free of sensory experi-
ences, visual content, and emotional reasoning. So, REM dreams play a role in
memory consolidation and emotional processing of complex events. Time spent in
REM sleep decreases with typical aging.
1.4.1 Insomnia
Many disorders emerge under this title. The ICSD-3 classifies the circadian rhythm
sleep-wake disorders into seven types which include:
1. Delayed Sleep-Wake Phase Disorder: There is difficulty sleeping and waking
up (more than a 2-h delay in sleep period) and later than normal individuals. This
leads the patient to delay and poor performance at work or school due to daytime
sleepiness.
2. Irregular Sleep-Wake Rhythm Disorder: Characterized by disorganized sleep
and wake patterns. It’s more observed in older adults and patients with neurode-
generative disease (Spicuzza et al. 2015).
3. Non-24-h Sleep-Wake Rhythm Disorder: Also called free-running disorder
and characterized by a gradual delay of sleep-onset time from 1 day to the next,
so the individual begins to sleep during the daytime hours and then drift back
into the night due to failure of the circadian system to entrain to the 24-h day.
Mainly occur in totally blind people (Baumann et al. 2014).
4. Shift Work Disorder: Occurs to individuals who work on night shifts due to
misadjustment of body circadian rhythm to work schedules lead to drowsiness
during shift work difficulty falling asleep during the day.
5. Jet Lag Disorder: Occurs in individuals who travel to regions with different
time zones because the body can’t reset its circadian time to the new time zone
upon arrival, so it takes some time to correct this (Rosenberg and Van Hout 2014).
6. Circadian Sleep-Wake Disorder not Otherwise Specified: Occurs secondary
to medical or neurological disorders, for example, dementia, movement disor-
ders, and blindness (Molaie and Deutsch 1997; Salminen and Winkelmann 2018).
They are abnormalities of respiration that occur during sleep often associated with
a wide variety of comorbidities. The degree of airway narrowing can range from
snoring to complete collapse of the airway and cessation of airflow. According to
ICSD-3, they are classified into four types: obstructive sleep apnea (OSA), central
sleep apnea (CSA), sleep-related hypoventilation, and sleep-related hypoxemia dis-
order (Maurer et al. 2010).
1. Obstructive Sleep Apnea (OSA): One of the diseases commonly found among
patients relates to the respiratory system (Fig. 3). Usually, OSA is accompanied
by snoring and individuals unaware of their breathing difficulty even when
44 S. A. Saleh
alking at night until their sleep partner or others recognize it. These events
w
occur primarily during sleep stages I, II, and REM and are associated with severe
desaturation, while sleep stage III is protective against OSA with less severe
desaturation. In addition, individuals may have symptoms of unexplained day-
time sleepiness, restless sleep, morning headache, and mood changes. Causes of
OSA may be due to old age, traumatic brain injury, decreased muscle tone, and
obesity. The gold standard for OSA treatment is continuous positive airway pres-
sure (CPAP).
2. Central Sleep Apnea (CSA): It means that the origin of the pathology comes
from the brain that stops ventilation repetitively due to lack of brain signals that
drive respiratory muscles to control breathing during sleep, so there is no respira-
tory effort, and this is in contrast to OSA where respiratory signals from the brain
are normal and the problem in the upper airway which obstructs and not open
properly. CSA is divided into two categories:
(a) Hypercapnic type: In this type, the brain fails to send signals to stimulate
respiratory muscles for breathing due to narcotic drugs (e.g., opioids),
stroke, or trauma that affects the brainstem and inhibits respiratory signals,
or due to neuromuscular disease (amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, multiple
sclerosis) which leads to weakness in respiratory muscles then leads to a
buildup of carbon dioxide.
(b) Hypocapnic type: Occurs because of aberrant pacing and control of respira-
tion that leads to quick deep breath. Treatment of CSA depends on the cause
and the patient’s comorbidities; however, we can use CPAP and oxygen
supplementation. Also there is an implantable device that stimulates breath-
ing muscles.
3. Sleep-Related Hypoventilation: Characterized by insufficient ventilation
results in accumulation of carbon dioxide and elevation of PaCO2 during sleep.
Sleep and Dreaming 45
Cataplexy is a sudden loss of muscle tone with full consciousness when exposed to
emotional triggers, and it’s the main feature that differentiates between both types
of narcolepsy sleep fragmentation and can also be seen in narcolepsy type 2; it is
atypical for IH.
The loss of orexin neurons occurs due to genetic and environmental factors like
autoimmune attack and destruction in susceptible individuals. Individuals with nar-
colepsy pass abruptly from waking to REM sleep with a bit of or absent non-
REM period.
Idiopathic hypersomnia (IH) is characterized by long non-refreshing naps with/
without long sleep time. The main mechanism is unknown and usually doesn’t
respond to normal treatment used in narcolepsy.
Treatment includes a combination of pharmacological and behavioral therapy.
Modafinil is the drug of choice for narcolepsy; it improves wakefulness by reducing
the reuptake of dopamine to decrease excessive daytime sleepiness. Venlafaxine is
a serotonin-norepinephrine reuptake inhibitor and antidepressant agent used to
reduce cataplexy. Usually, to get a maximum effect of these medications, we use
them with lifestyle modifications like advising the patient to take scheduled short
naps during the day and maintain a regular bedtime. This will reduce daytime sleep-
iness. However, these naps are ineffective in patients with IH. We also need to treat
other comorbidities that accompany narcolepsy because people with narcolepsy
tend to be obese, which may affect their lives. Safety precautions should be taken
before doing some activities like driving cars and working in jobs that require full
alertness during the day because patients with narcolepsy can have serious injuries
or death if they fall asleep.
1.4.5 Parasomnia
It means abnormal movements and actions occur during sleep (before sleep, during
sleep, or at arousal period after sleep) (Stefani and Högl 2019).
46 S. A. Saleh
This parasomnia group is associated with arousal and occurs during stage III of
NREM sleep. In addition, certain triggers can induce this type of parasomnias like
alcohol, sleep deprivation, physical activity, emotional stress, depression, and cer-
tain medications; this type includes:
1. Confusional Arousal: It’s a brief episode of arousal occurring when sleep is
interrupted, and the individual is awakened during the first half of the night (dur-
ing stage III) and characterized by mental confusion, disorientation, amnesia of
the event; that’s why their sleep bed partner may only note the episode, and the
individual can’t understand what’s going on. Most commonly occur in children
and resolve by age 5, in rare cases may continue to adulthood.
2. Sleepwalking (Somnambulism): A series of complex behaviors that occur dur-
ing sleep and ambulation. The individual looks awake with open eyes and per-
forms inappropriate actions like moving around, but actually, they are asleep.
This may lead to serious injuries because individuals are unaware of what they
are doing due to altered consciousness and impaired judgment. This episode may
terminate when the individual returns to bed or lies down outside and continues
their sleeping. The exact cause of sleepwalking is unknown, but some studies
show it runs in families.
3. Sleep Terrors: Patients wake up suddenly and start screaming and maybe crying
as well, typically lasting between 30 s and 5 min. The affected individual appears
confused, diaphoretic, tachypneic, tachycardiac, and may sleepwalk during
attacks, and usually, they don’t remember the event the following day. It is chal-
lenging to communicate and console the individual during the attack. Individuals
with sleep terrors run or jump around in an attempt to avoid harm, thereby injur-
ing themselves or others.
4. Sleep-Related Eating Disorder: The exact underlying mechanism of sleep-
related eating disorder is unclear, but it is associated with other sleep disorders
like sleepwalking, obstructive sleep apnea, narcolepsy, and periodic limb move-
ment disorder; also some medications (e.g., zolpidem) may trigger it.
It includes REM sleep behavior disorder (RBD), recurrent isolated sleep paralysis,
and nightmare disorder.
1. REM Sleep Behavior Disorder (RBD): Recurrent episodes of verbal or com-
plex motor behavior during REM sleep. Because the dream content is violent,
the patient may exhibit jerky limb movement, punching, shouting, screaming,
hitting, and running, resulting in serious injury to the patient and their bed part-
ner. In addition, the patient may recall the events and dream content after
waking up.
Sleep and Dreaming 47
Other Parasomnias
This category of parasomnia disorders can occur in both NREM and REM sleep as
well as during the transition between sleep and wakefulness. They include:
1. Exploding Head Syndrome: A loud sound heard in the head upon awakening.
2. Sleep-Related Dissociative Disorder: Characterized by dissociative episodes
that occur just before the sleep that last minutes to hours and mimic other para-
somnias; however, it always occurs during wakefulness state. It may be accom-
panied with daytime dissociative symptoms and go away with treatment of a
dissociative disorder. Patients may be agitated and violent during the episode and
result in self-injury.
3. Sleep-Related Hallucinations: In this condition, the patient experiences audi-
tory, visual, tactile, and kinetic hallucinations at sleep onset or upon waking. The
patient wakes up terrified and may jump out of bed and get injured. This halluci-
nation episode may last for a few minutes and go away when the light is turned on.
They are a wide range of disorders explained one by one as in the following:
1. Restless Legs Syndrome (RLS): Also known as Willis-Ekbom disease, which
is a chronic sensorimotor disorder characterized by an irresistible urge to move
the legs accompanied by an unpleasant sensation in the legs. To ensure that the
case is RLS, we must exclude other conditions that explain the symptoms like
sleep-related leg cramps, positions, discomfort, habitual foot tapping, akathisia,
and arthralgia. RLS can be idiopathic or secondary. Idiopathic form usually
begins slowly before 40 years of age, and the patient may have a family history
48 S. A. Saleh
of RLS, while in secondary form, it has a later onset in life and is associated with
other conditions like neurological disorders (e.g., multiple sclerosis), end-stage
kidney disease, iron deficiency, or pregnancy. Dopamine dysfunction may have
an essential role in the pathophysiology of RLS. Although many patients show
significant improvement after administration of dopaminergic agents, recent
studies show hyperdopaminergic states in RLS patients, in contrast to the
hypodopaminergic state, which is thought to be the primary mechanism for this
disorder. Other non-pharmacological treatments like sleep hygiene, pneumatic
pressure therapy, exercise, massage, and hot baths may be helpful to reduce the
symptoms (Stumbrys et al. 2012).
2. Periodic Limb Movement Disorder (PLMD): The contractions last 0.5–10 s,
and each episode consists of at least four consecutive movements, with 5–90 s
intervals in between. PLMD occurs in the first half of the night during NREM
sleep. PLMD is present in about 80–90% of RLS patients. Dopaminergic region
dysfunction within the hypothalamus and impaired iron availability in the brain
may be involved in PLMD.
3. Sleep-Related Leg Cramps: Intense, short-lived painful contractions of calf or
foot muscles relieved by stretching, messages, and heat application on the
affected muscles. Most commonly occur at night or during the sleep-waking of
the patient. Older adults are most commonly affected. Although sleep-related leg
cramps are more prevalent among elderly people, they may result from neuro-
muscular disorders (e.g., radiculopathies, myopathies, and Parkinson disease),
electrolyte disturbances, and medications (e.g., long-acting beta) agonists and
thiazide diuretics) and could be idiopathic with no relation to other diseases.
One-half of patients with sleep-related leg cramps have several episodes per
week or day. This may lead to sleep disturbances, difficulty falling asleep, awak-
ening at night and excess daytime sleeping.
4. Sleep-Related Bruxism: Polysomnography and masseter electromyography are
used to confirm the diagnosis. Sleep bruxism can result in morning jaw muscle
pain, temporal headache, tooth destruction, masticatory muscles hypertrophy,
and temporomandibular joint discomfort. Unfortunately, there is no effective
treatment that cures or stops sleep bruxism. However, oral appliances can be
used to protect the tooth from damage.
5. Sleep-Related Rhythmic Movement Disorder (SRRMD): Rhythmic, stereo-
typed with large-amplitude and low-frequency (0.5–2 Hz) body movements that
involve large muscle groups and occur before falling asleep or during sleep (par-
ticularly stage II NREM sleep). Movement episodes last for a few minutes and
manifest as body rocking, headbanging, and head rolling. Severe cases of
SRRMD can be treated with benzodiazepines such as clonazepam.
6. Benign Sleep Myoclonus of Infancy (BSMI): It is a benign self-limiting disor-
der characterized by repetitive flexion, extension, abduction, and adduction
myoclonic jerks during the NREM sleep period that last for a few seconds and
disappear with arousal or movement. Myoclonus jerks are more prominent in
upper than lower extremities and begin during the first month of life (first
Sleep and Dreaming 49
2 Dreams
Many cultures worldwide suggest that dreams are a gateway to communicate with
other worlds and supernatural entities (Baird et al. 2019).
Throughout their theories, Sigmund Freud and Carl Jung put the first step about
dreams interpretations. Sigmund Freud hypothesizes that our subconscious can
reveal the wishes that used to be repressed by our conscious mind, so, during
dreams, we live out our deepest wishes and desires as he stated that dreams are the
“royal road to the unconscious.” Carl Jung had another view on dreams that, in con-
trast to Freud view he believes dreams are attempts to lead the individual toward
wholeness through dialog between the ego and the self (ego represents our con-
scious mind while self represents the totality of our physical, biological, psycho-
logical, social, and cultural being that involve the conscious as well as the
unconscious).
Dreams mainly occur during REM sleep, so at this stage, circuits in the brain-
stem are activated then trigger areas of the limbic system responsible for emotions,
sensations, and memories, including the amygdala and hippocampus. Hence, the
brain tries to interpret this electrical activity to create meaning from these signals.
In cognitive neuroscience, the present theory assumes that dreams have a role in
memory consolidation and long-term memory enhancement. Other theories suggest
that dreams have a role in controlling emotions and resolving the problems that
occur in our daily lives (Baird et al. 2019).
temporal discontinuity when the scene changes suddenly with little or no transition
in between, for example, when the dream starts at a specific place (e.g., at home)
and suddenly change to an unrelated location (e.g., in a ship). The other is the
unlikely combination where two or more dream elements are unlikely to be com-
bined at the same time according to waking experience (e.g., playing golf on the
plane). These bizarre events are taken seriously as real-life experiences by the
sleeper as they are unaware this is not real and just a dream until waking up (except
in lucid dreams).
Memory recall of dreams at awakening is poor; that’s why we forget the exact
elements of the dream. Some individuals may have difficulty in a verbal description
of their dreams due to inherent bizarreness. Assessment of dreams scientifically is
not easy because the dreamer is the only observer of his dream, so a third observer
can’t access any subjective experience they pass through. The only way to obtain the
dream information depends on personal memory recall (Baird et al. 2019).
This type of dreaming is unique because it belongs to the area between dreaming
and consciousness, defined as awareness of the dream state and environment with
the ability to concentrate and make decisions. The individual can interpret the dream
while dreaming and remember it after awakening and control the sequence of the
events. The cardinal feature of transparency is the cognitive realization of “This is a
dream!” or awakening within the dream. When this realization or awakening occurs,
it is called a lucid dream. Although most lucid dreams occur during REM sleep,
some studies show they can occur during NREM sleep. The same neurobiological
basis of lucid dreams has not been identified yet. However, a hypothesis suggests
that lateral prefrontal cortices (normally inactive during REM sleep) will remain
active during lucid dreaming (Pigeon and Mellman 2017).
For most people, natural lucid dreams occur infrequently and may never occur in
their lifetime. There are two types of lucid dreams; the first, when the individual
becomes conscious and aware of their dream during the dream; this type is called
“Dream-initiated lucid dreams.” The second, when the individual is conscious and
awake, then enter lucid dreams, and this type is called “Wake-initiated lucid dreams”
(Pigeon and Mellman 2017).
Dreaming about one’s future events and thoughts does not take enough time to be
saved in our consciousness. That’s why our postconsciousness releases them to
make them satisfied and comfortable about the frightens. The dreams that cause you
to be comfortable, which I will call good dreams, while others make you uncomfort-
able, and I will call them bad dreams. Postconsciousness creates these dreams to
release the thoughts from our mind and to make us more tolerated and realistic with
our reality, not with our imaginations, so it creates some kind of imaginations (could
be real or not and could be good or bad), to fight our worries and stressing thoughts.
Sleep and Dreaming 53
Answers
1. (b)
2. (e)
3. (a)
4. (e)
5. (a)
6. (b)
7. (c)
8. (c)
9. (c)
10. (e)
References
Stefani A, Högl B. Diagnostic criteria, differential diagnosis, and treatment of minor motor activity
and less well-known movement disorders of sleep. Curr Treat Options Neurol. 2019;21(1):1.
Stumbrys T, Erlacher D, Schädlich M, Schredl M. Induction of lucid dreams: a systematic review
of evidence. Conscious Cogn. 2012;21(3):1456–75.
Tubbs AS, Dollish HK, Fernandez F, Grandner MA. The basics of sleep physiology and behavior.
In: Sleep and health. Cambridge: Academic Press; 2019. p. 3–10.
Van der Salm SM, Erro R, Cordivari C, Edwards MJ, Koelman JH, van den Ende T, Bhatia KP, van
Rootselaar AF, Brown P, Tijssen MA. Propriospinal myoclonus: clinical reappraisal and review
of literature. Neurology. 2014;83(20):1862–70.
Varga B, Gergely A, Galambos Á, Kis A. Heart rate and heart rate variability during sleep in family
dogs (Canis familiaris). Moderate effect of pre-sleep emotions. Animals. 2018;8(7):107.
Defense Mechanisms and Personality
Disorders
1 Defense Mechanism
In the nineteenth century, Sigmund Freud (1856–1939) exhibited his ideas about the
defense mechanisms and their relation to the human mind, which is represented by
the Id (unconscious fundamental and unorganized part of our mind that represent
the main drive of our needs, desires, and impulses that seek for immediate plea-
sures), ego (present in the conscious and unconscious part of our mind responsible
for organization and planning, and manifest by the defensive, reasoning, reality
testing, intellectual and executive functions that regulate between our primitive
demand of Id and the higher moral values of the superego), and superego (present
in the conscious, preconscious, and unconscious part of our mind that symbolize the
higher moral values and principles that associate with the society rules, so it guides
our behavior to what is right and wrong and function by suppressing the unaccept-
able Id impulses). After that, in the twentieth century, his daughter Anna Freud tried
to complete his work by defining, analyzing, and adding more of these defense
mechanisms. Also, she proposed that defense mechanisms depend on the maturity
of the ego. Defense mechanisms are mental processes that act unconsciously to
resolve the conflicts that result from rapid change in internal and external reality
through distortion or manipulation to avoid suffering from anxiety and depression.
These conflicts may interfere with one’s family, culture, or identity values. Which
defense mechanism takes the act is something that we cannot control, but it has
many consequences on our mental health. When the individual uses the defense
mechanisms as a main coping style with life stressors, this may damage the self and
result in irrational, harmful behaviors. So defense mechanisms act as a way of
masking reality and preventing the individual from facing the problem and taking
responsibility. The target of the defenses is to rebalance the psychological state and
S. A. Saleh (*)
University of Baghdad, College of Medicine, Baghdad, Iraq
provide the mind with time to adjust to the new changes. Also, it leads to a decrease
in cognitive, emotional, and physiological stress. The motives of these defenses are
to ward off the instinctual anxiety feeling of guilt and protection of our self-esteem.
Defense mechanisms work through alteration of perception and relationship
between self, others, and the conflicts. Although defense mechanisms lead to psy-
chological adjustment and more adaptation to the internal and external reality,
sometimes it may cause psychological distortion and result in pathology. These
defenses have an important role in psychodynamic therapy; it helps make the indi-
viduals more aware of their behaviors and allow them to understand themselves
more deeply. So it could be a valuable tool to manage people with depression, anxi-
ety, and personality disorders.
There are many theories developed to classify the defense mechanisms of which the
borderline personality organization theory that developed by Otta Friedmann
Kernberg in 1967 and depend on the object relation theory to explain how the per-
sonality develops during childhood using the primitive defense mechanisms (e.g.,
projection, denial, dissociation, and splitting) as a cornerstone of this theory.
Another theory developed by Robert Plutchik (1927–2006) in 1979 depends on the
eight opposite primary emotions to explain our defense mechanisms, for example,
fear represents repression, anger represents displacement, joy represents reaction
formation, sadness represents sublimation, acceptance represents denial, disgust
represents projection, expectation represents intellectualization, and lastly surprise
represents regression, and all the other emotions and defenses are derived from
these. Finally, the last theory developed by the psychiatrist George Eman Vaillant
claims that defenses are a continuation to their psychoanalytic developmental level
and classify the defense mechanisms into a hierarchical model of four main catego-
ries ranging from the pathological level of defenses to the healthiest adaptive level
(Fig. 1). This is the same classification used by the American Psychiatric Association
(ASA) through their (DSM-4) diagnostic and statistical manual for mental disorders
(Fig. 2). We will go through most of the defense mechanisms according to Vaillant’s
Fig. 1 Vaillant
classification of defense
mechanisms level from
least adaptive
(pathological) to highest
adaptive (mature)
Defense Mechanisms and Personality Disorders 59
classification, including more defenses that were discovered after Vaillant’s original
classification:
This level represents a failure of the individual defensive system to arrange the
response to the stressors, so when these kinds of defenses take the actions, they will
result in distortion of external reality to eliminate the individual’s need to deal with
it and make them think inconsequential and behave in irrational ways. Although
they present in some psychiatric disorders (e.g., schizophrenia), they may typically
appear in dreams (Vos and De Haes 2007).
1. Psychotic denial: Characterized by avoiding the awareness or inability of the
individual to accept part of the internal reality (their feeling, thoughts, and expe-
rience) and part of the external reality (the outside events and stressors) as it is
disturbing then make them to deal and act as if they did not exist despite the
overwhelming evidence. For example, an older person who is newly diagnosed
with cancer refuses to acknowledge his disease and start treatment despite all the
signs and symptoms he suffered from, just because the idea of having cancer is
too annoying and very hard to handle. Hence, it is easiest to deny the actual
disease.
2. Delusional projection: Characterized by fixed beliefs about the external reality
or own self that cannot be changed even in the presence of reasonable conflicting
evidence regarding its truth. These beliefs may be based on wrong or misleading
information. Their content includes many different themes: persecutory (the
most common type), referential, somatic, grandiose, and religious. It may occur
either when the individual perceives their feeling in another individual and acts
on that perception, or inversely when they perceive another individual or their
feelings inside themselves. Also, it appears in psychosis and other psychiatric
disorders. For example, an older man attacks a security guard because he believes
the government agency wants to kill him.
60 S. A. Saleh
part of the development. When the individual becomes mature and realizes that
the people can be both good and bad simultaneously, devaluation will decrease.
However, continued devaluation during adulthood may refer to a problem in
personality development due to childhood trauma. For instance, a person who
thinks he does not deserve a good job and a good life (Thomas et al. 2020; Rnic
et al. 2016; Esterberg et al. 2010).
This type of defense manifests as severe anxiety and arises when the individual
experiences emotional distress, instability, and unconscious struggles to alter the
internal feelings, so the individual behaves eccentrically. Due to bad behavior, the
person may relieve the internal conflicts, but it will affect their relationships and
daily life. It commonly occurs in healthy people and individuals with psychiatric
disorders such as schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, borderline personality disorder.
1. Intellectualization: By this defense mechanism, the individual uses overthink-
ing to deal with an annoying situation then isolates oneself from the feeling to
reduce the harm of uncomfortable emotions. So it looks at the event from a fact,
rational and abstract reasoning point of view to avoid provoking anxiety. So, for
example, a young boy whose parents died in a car accident tries to investigate
and understand how the accident happened instead of showing his sad feelings.
2. Reaction formation: This defense mechanism manifests by transforming the
uncomfortable wishes, thoughts, and impulses that may be considered danger-
ous into the opposite behavior unconsciously to relieve anxiety, sexuality, and
internal guilt. It keeps the original individual feelings and impulses out of the
awareness, so they are only recognized when the person shows a behavior dia-
metrically opposite to what was expected for a particular situation. Also, it is
considered transitional defense between the low-level immature (acting out)
defenses and high-level mature (altruism) defense mechanisms. Finally, it may
count as an adaptive defense because it reduces the destructive behaviors and
makes the individual quit the bad habits or do more beneficial behaviors. Some
studies show an association between reaction formation and anxiety-related
disorders such as panic disorder, anxiety disorder, and agoraphobia. In contrast,
others demonstrate that patients with bipolar disorder use reaction formation
less frequently. For instance, a well-known Stockholm phenomenon is when the
kidnap victim falls in love with the perpetrator to build a bond between them
and show affection contrary to what she was feeling at that time.
3. Dissociation: Drastic modification of customarily integrated functions of iden-
tity, memory, consciousness, and behavior to relieve the emotional distress. It
ranges from a minor adjustment in character to complete detachment from real-
ity. Dissociated individuals usually disconnect from time and external environ-
ment, and sense of self. So it will lead to amnesia and fragmentation in
Defense Mechanisms and Personality Disorders 63
sis may persist, and the individual does not believe there is nothing to be con-
cerned about. Also, it considers a nonverbal, interpersonal connection and
help-seeking behavior to cope with stressful life events. Hypochondriasis
involves a wide variety of symptoms and psychiatric disorders like pain, body
appearance, gastrointestinal, cardiopulmonary, sensory-perceptual issues,
personality disorder, neurasthenia, obsessive-compulsive disorder, hysteria,
depression, and paranoid psychosis. For example, a young boy with a minor
cough may think that he has lung cancer, and he is going to die. While conver-
sion disorder (also named a functional neurological symptom disorder in the
DSM-5) is also considered a defense mechanism by itself, it is characterized by
converting psychological harm into physical symptoms that lead to impairment
of sensory and motor functions. Through this process, the individual uncon-
sciously relieves their anxiety—for example, a woman complained of loss of
consciousness after having a fight with her husband.
3 Personality Disorders
The ten different types of personality disorders are categorized into three clusters
that share the same features (Table 1). Each has a distinct pattern of signs and symp-
toms that differentiate from the others and have its consequences on the individual.
We will consider the classification according to the DSM-5.
These types of personality disorders are characterized by odd and eccentric behav-
ior. Individuals with cluster A considered the world overly self-centered; thus, they
cannot develop meaningful social relationships. Also, there is a genetic association
between this category and schizophrenia, and they are more resistant to treatment.
This cluster includes:
1. Paranoid Personality Disorder (PPD): An individual with PPD considers other
people’s actions as threatening and suspicious regardless of evidence of a threat,
so they have pervasive distrust issues. Also, they may show aggressive behavior
without justification because they consider others unfaithful and accuse them of
things that don’t exist. In addition, PPD is characterized by unforgiving, rumina-
tive, jealous, excessive self-importance, and hostility traits. Individuals with
diagnoses of PPD tend to keep observing the environment and other people for
signs of a threat to accuse others of planning to get them out, so they are always
hypervigilant. Due to this mindset, those individuals have difficulty socializing
with people or engaging in romantic relationships because they keep charging
their partners to be disloyal. Usually, individuals with PPD cannot locate a prob-
lem inside themself because their symptoms are ego-syntonic, so they tend to
project their creativity and feelings on other people. That’s why the projection
defense mechanism is considered to have an essential role in the paranoid pro-
cess. PPD occurs more commonly in men than women and especially in people
from minority cultural statuses. To make the diagnosis of PPD reliable, the
DSM-5 proposed two criteria. Criterion A; involve that individual must have a
global distrust and suspicion of others’ motivations which can be concluded in
four of the following features: (1) belief, without sufficient evidence, that other
people intentionally mean to deceive and harm them; (2) unjustified doubt about
the loyalty and trustworthiness of others; (3) avoid to confide other people due to
fearing of unfaithfulness and this information may be used against them; (4)
understanding benign remarks or events as meant to be threatening or demean-
ing; (5) tendency to keep grudges and unforgiving others; (6) recognize attacks
on their character which are hidden to others require a vindictive response; and
(7) presence of recurrent suspicion and accusing their spouse as being disloyal
without justification. At the same time, criterion B suggests that these paranoid
symptoms must not be attributed to psychotic episodes associated with schizo-
phrenia, bipolar disorder, a major depressive disorder with psychotic features.
Treatment of patients with PPD is quite challenging because they have trust dif-
ficulties and fear of being humiliated. However, psychotherapy is worth the try,
but first, the therapist has to build a good rapport with the patient. A new promis-
ing psychotherapy approach called metacognition-oriented therapy is used to
manage those patients by asserting the importance of helping individuals struc-
ture narratives of their lives and raising the reflection and metacognition capaci-
ties to understand themselves and others, making the patient more aware and
improving their interpersonal relationships (Zimmerman and Mattia 1999).
Defense Mechanisms and Personality Disorders 69
2. Schizoid Personality Disorder (SzPD): Individuals with this disorder are char-
acterized by social withdrawal, emotionally cold, carelessness about other peo-
ple’s opinions, avoidance of intimate sexual relationships with their partner,
looking to the world as an observer, not as a part of it, and avoid social situations
or occupations that require them to be engaged in teamwork. Due to this
disconnection and fear from the social world, the individual with SzPD may fall
back into one’s internal fantasy or isolate themselves as a way of defense.
Prevalence of SzPD is low in the general population and tends to range between
1 and 5%, according to the American Psychiatric Association (2013). There is
evidence of a link between genetic susceptibility to SzPD and schizophrenia.
Features of isolation, lack of relationship with peers, poor school performance,
and annoyance by others during childhood may predict the development of
SzPD later in life. Diagnosis of SzPD is applied through DSM-5 criteria, which
involve Criterion A, defined by detachment from a social and familial relation-
ship with a restricted range of emotional expressions in an interpersonal setting
that starts at or after the age of 18. While criterion B proposed the mentioned
features must not be associated with the course of schizophrenia, bipolar disor-
der, a depressive disorder with psychotic symptoms, or autism spectrum disease.
As is the case with most personality disorders, individuals diagnosed with SzPD
rarely look for treatment because their symptoms are ego-syntonic and don’t
interfere with their self-esteem. In fact, there is no specific treatment approved
for the management of SzPD. However, like other personality disorders, using
psychotherapy may improve the symptoms and help individuals with SzPD
develop social skills. In psychotherapy, the therapist tries to make the patient
observe their maladaptive behavior then encourage using new skills to counter-
act this behavior. Group therapy may be used as adjunctive treatment to help
patients interact with others to reduce their fear and identify their positive and
negative emotions. Because the prevalence of anxiety and depression is higher in
people with SzPD, pharmacological treatment may be used in these cases
(Leichsenring et al. 2011).
3. Schizotypal Personality Disorder (SPD): Individuals with this disorder have an
eccentric appearance, unconventional beliefs, distorted or magical thinking, and
interpersonal awkwardness. Also, people with this mental disease complain of
social difficulties and lack of relationships outside their family due to feelings of
pronounced discomfort. Due to a combination of social deficits and cognitive
deterioration (involves primary working memory, verbal learning, context pro-
cessing, and sustained attention), subjects with SPD suffer significantly poor
functioning and reduced quality of life in society. Although this disorder is not
widely investigated, there is some evidence suggesting low prevalence, and it
ranges from less than 1% to 4%, with a slightly higher rate in men than women.
During childhood, symptoms of SPD manifest by excessive social anxiety, sen-
sitivity, and eccentric thoughts and speech. SPD is also considered a prototype
form of schizophrenia spectrum disorders. To deal with unconscious conflicts,
individuals with SPD use immature defense mechanisms predominated by
emerging projection and passive aggression defenses to manage the situation.
70 S. A. Saleh
This personality disorder class has dramatic, emotional, or erratic behavioral fea-
tures. Individuals with disorders in this group have predominantly manifested by
lack of empathy, and they tend to involve themselves in impulsive, promiscuous,
and illegal experiences. Also, the disorders in this cluster have a genetic association
with mood disorders and substance use. This cluster includes:
1. Antisocial Personality Disorder (ASPD): Characterized by a chronic pattern of
violation of other people’s rights with lack of remorse and tendency to criminal-
ity, responsibility, impulsivity, hostility, and manipulation of others for personal
interest. Individuals with ASPD usually fail to develop stable interpersonal rela-
tionships because they are dominated by aggressiveness and violate other peo-
ple’s rules. In addition, they don’t obey the law, so they may have a history of
Defense Mechanisms and Personality Disorders 71
legal problems and fail to sustain consistent employment. ASPD originates dur-
ing childhood or early adolescence due to physical, emotional, and sexual mal-
treatment and continues into adulthood. The criteria of DSM-5 can confirm the
diagnosis of ASPD. Criterion A: consisted of an enduring pattern of disregard for
and violation of other rights that begin at 15 years of age. The effectiveness of
psychotherapy is questionable since the disorder tends to diminish with time.
Some studies conduct the only treatment is a passage of time. Some literature
suggests cognitive-behavioral therapy for ASPD; however, due to the patient’s
impulsivity and aggressiveness, the therapist has difficulty establishing an alli-
ance, so it has limited benefit. Treating conductive disorder during childhood
may be the best approach to minimize ASPD symptoms during adulthood.
Psychopharmacological agents (e.g., antidepressants, antipsychotics, and mood
stabilizers) could be used only to control symptoms of aggression and impulsiv-
ity and manage the concomitant disorder. An approach called mentalization-
based therapy (involving thinking about self mental state and mental state of
others) has emerged in recent years that appears to be effective in minimizing
aggressiveness. However, more data is needed to confirm these findings.
2. Borderline Personality Disorder (BPD): An enduring pattern of instability
prominent in mood, interpersonal relationships, and self-concept perspectives,
and associated with the feeling of emptiness, fear of abandonment, engaging in
impulsive behaviors, and trying to get others attention via self-mutilation and
recurrent suicidal attempt. Individuals with BPD tend to recognize the world into
the extremes of either “all good” or “all bad.” The literature revealed that indi-
viduals with BPD exhibit a higher rate of using immature defense mechanisms
that include acting out, emotional hypochondriasis, and undoing, so they may
use them as screening tools in a clinical setting to recognize patients with
BPD. Most recent studies demonstrate that using splitting, projective identifica-
tion, and affiliation defenses associate with a higher rate of suicide among peo-
ple with BPD. The DSM-5 defines BPD as a pervasive pattern of instability of
interpersonal relationships, self-image, affects and marked impulsivity that starts
early adulthood and exists in various contexts. Many modalities can be used and
proved to be successful such as dialectical behavioral therapy (DBT),
transference-focused psychotherapy (TFP), and mentalization-based therapy
(MBT). Sometimes medications could be used as an adjunct to psychotherapy to
control symptoms of BPD, such as small doses of second-generation antipsy-
chotics for managing perceptual symptoms and selective serotonin reuptake
inhibitors to reduce impulsivity and aggression behaviors (Lewis and Grenyer
2009; Lieb et al. 2004; Vaillant 1992).
3. Narcissistic Personality Disorder (NPD): Characterized by an enduring pattern
of grandiosity that causes impairment in interpersonal relationships, a require-
ment of excessive attention and admiration, exaggerated sense of self-
centeredness, and hypersensitivity to criticism. Individuals with this disorder
also have a significant psychosocial disability and low quality of life. Although
NPD is associated with many other mental health disorders, it can coexist with
substance use disorders and other personality disorders like ASPD, BPD, HPD,
72 S. A. Saleh
SPD. Some studies classified NPD into two subtypes: grandiose NPD and vul-
nerability NPD. Grandiose NPD is characterized by aggression, excessive gran-
diosity, boldness, arrogance, and self-inflated image. DSM-5 defines NPD as a
pattern of grandiosity (in fantasy or acting), constantly seeking admiration, and
lack of empathy, which appears in early adulthood and present in different situ-
ations. For NPD treatment, no data establishes a single model to be highly
effective and efficient, but a nice try of psychotherapy may be beneficial. Some
experts suggest the same psychotherapeutic approach used for BPD, but it is not
confirmed yet and needs more investigations. Severe symptoms of NPD could be
mitigated by mood stabilizer agents, antidepressant, and antipsychotic medica-
tions (Vaillant 1977; Costa 2016, 2017).
4. Histrionic Personality Disorder (HPD): Characterized by patterns of attention-
seeking behaviors associated with dramatic speech and exaggerated theatrical,
emotional expression, and could be easily provocative sexually. People with
HPD always try to be the center of attention by their physical actions because
they may feel disregarded or depressed if they are not. They also tend to appear
fascinating, tempting, lively, enthusiastic, and manipulative to achieve their
desires. To make the diagnosis of HPD feasible, the DSM-5 defines HPD as a
pervasive pattern of excessive emotional dysregulation and attention-seeking
that starts in early adulthood and persists over time in a variety of contexts, then
proposed eight criteria, five or more of them required to ensure the diagnosis.
The subjects with NPD unconsciously being dramatic as a way of dealing with
their internal conflicts and anxiety. For example, they adopt opinions of people
they admire as a fact without considering or thinking about themselves.
Unfortunately, there is no significant data about the best effective treatment for
NPD. However, psychotherapy may be the best option available that the patient
can benefit from in the long term. Supportive psychotherapy may aid people
with NPD to enhance their emotional control, increase their self-esteem, and
better perceive and cope with the environment. Cognitive-behavioral therapy
may also be used in conjunction with psychodynamic therapy to increase the
effectiveness of the treatment. The goal of psychodynamic psychotherapy is to
reduce the unconscious internal conflicts as an attempt to make the patients bet-
ter understand themselves and recognize their behaviors by learning to exchange
dramatic speech into more adaptive behaviors. Also, the same pharmacological
agents that are used in NPD could be used in severe cases to relieve the symp-
tomatic dysregulations.
Anxious and fearful thoughts and behavior characterize this category of personality
disorders. It occurs due to a combination of genetic and environmental etiologies.
Individuals who have a disorder from this cluster suffer from poor relationships and
Defense Mechanisms and Personality Disorders 73
Answers
1. (c)
2. (e)
3. (c)
4. (b)
5. (a)
6. (c)
7. (d)
8. (d)
9. (e)
10. (a)
References
American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th ed.
Washington, DC: Author; 2013.
Caligor E, Levy KN, Yeomans FE. Narcissistic personality disorder: diagnostic and clinical chal-
lenges. Am J Psychiatr. 2015;172(5):415–22.
Clarkin JF, Caligor E, Sowislo J. TFP extended: development and recent advances. Psychodyn
Psychiatry. 2021;49(2):188–214.
Costa RM. Conversion (defense mechanism). In: Encyclopedia of personality and individual dif-
ferences. Cham: Springer International Publishing; 2016. p. 1–4.
Costa RM. Undoing (defense mechanism). In: Encyclopedia of personality and individual differ-
ences. Cham: Springer International Publishing; 2017. p. 1–2.
Esterberg ML, Goulding SM, Walker EF. Cluster a personality disorders: schizotypal, schizoid and
paranoid personality disorders in childhood and adolescence. J Psychopathol Behav Assess.
2010;32(4):515–28.
Fisher KA, Hany M. Antisocial personality disorder. In: StatPearls. Treasure Island: StatPearls
Publishing; 2021.
French JH, Shrestha S. Histrionic personality disorder. In: StatPearls. Treasure Island: StatPearls
Publishing; 2020.
Frick PJ. Early identification and treatment of antisocial behavior. Pediatr Clin. 2016;63(5):861–71.
Iacovino JM, Jackson JJ, Oltmanns TF. The relative impact of socioeconomic status and child-
hood trauma on black-white differences in paranoid personality disorder symptoms. J Abnorm
Psychol. 2014;123(1):225.
Lee RJ. Mistrustful and misunderstood: a review of paranoid personality disorder. Curr Behav
Neurosci Rep. 2017;4(2):151–65.
Lee YJ, Keum MS, Kim HG, Cheon EJ, Cho YC, Koo BH. Defense mechanisms and psychologi-
cal characteristics according to suicide attempts in patients with borderline personality disorder.
Psychiatry Investig. 2020;17(8):840.
Leichsenring F, Leibing E, Kruse J, New AS, Leweke F. Borderline personality disorder. Lancet.
2011;377(9759):74–84.
Lewis KL, Grenyer BF. Borderline personality or complex post-traumatic stress disorder? An
update on the controversy. Harv Rev Psychiatry. 2009;17(5):322–8.
Lieb K, Zanarini MC, Schmahl C, Linehan MM, Bohus M. Borderline personality disorder.
Lancet. 2004;364(9432):453–61.
78 S. A. Saleh
Zanarini MC, Frankenburg FR, Fitzmaurice G. Defense mechanisms reported by patients with
borderline personality disorder and axis II comparison subjects over 16 years of prospective
follow-up: description and prediction of recovery. Am J Psychiatr. 2013;170(1):111–20.
Zimmerman M, Mattia JI. Axis I diagnostic comorbidity and borderline personality disorder.
Compr Psychiatry. 1999;40(4):245–52.
Drugs and Consciousness
1 CNS Stimulants
They stimulate the CNS and. These drugs include caffeine which can be found in
coffee and chocolate (Fig. 1). It mimics adenosine (a natural brain chemical)
(Weil 1998).
Many students who consume many cups of coffee experience suffering from
headaches due to the sudden decrease in the levels of caffeine in their brains (Fig. 2).
Nicotine which is found in cigarettes is a stimulant. Smokers experience an
addictive effect on nicotine, and in the same way, when they suddenly stop smoking
develop a withdrawal effect. Nicotine is an activator, activates neural mechanisms,
and shows similarity to cocaine.
1.1 Amphetamine
Dexedrine and Benzedrine are strong stimulants. They stimulate the central nervous
system and lead to energy and alertness, talkativeness, insomnia, and wakefulness.
They increase concentration and reduce fatigue (Koob and Volkow 2010).
Chemical structure: C9H13N
M. H. Kadhim (*)
University of Kufa, College of Medicine, Najaf, Iraq
Fig. 1 Addiction
Bath salts are also an amphetamine-like stimulant. It can produce euphoria and a
rise in sociability and sex drive, but the side effects can be severe, including para-
noia and agitation (Koob and Volkow 2010).
1.2 Cocaine
2 CNS Depressants
2.1 Alcohol
It is a depressant, and it is used extensively worldwide and among almost all people
(Feldman 2011; Su et al. 1997; Grant 1997).
84 M. H. Kadhim
Its effects are comprehensive (Fig. 3). Addiction comes from the idea of reliev-
ing tension and making the person feel happy and disconnected from the world
(Feldman 2011; Anthenelli and Schuckit 1993).
2.2 Barbiturates
Chemical structure:
3 Narcotics
They are a wide range of drugs that are used to relieve stress and tension and to
change the mood, and it causes addiction very widely (Wang 2017; Clark et al.
2011; Beaudoin et al. 2016).
3.1 Morphine
Nausea &
Myosis
vomiting
Respiratory
depression Morphine Constipation
side effects
Urine
Sedation
retention
3.2 Codeine
3.3 Fentanyl
It is more potent than morphine in analgesic effects and used in anesthesia (Fig. 6)
(National Center for Biotechnology Information 2021).
Chemical structure: C22H28N2O
Hallucinations
Crying spells
Fentanyl
Behavioral changes Suicidal thoughts
side effects
Anxiety Depression
3.4 Methadone
3.5 Meperidine
It is a synthetic opioid with lower potency, and it is considered a weak opioid. It has
no relation to morphine. Meperidine is a highly lipophilic drug and has anticholin-
ergic properties, increasing the incidence of delusions and delirium compared to
other opioids (Baldo 2018).
Pharmacokinetics: The duration of action of meperidine is shorter compared to
morphine and other opioids.
3.6 Oxycodone
4 Hallucinogens
4.1 Marijuana
It is the most common drug used for hallucination (Fig. 7). In addition, it is a com-
mon addictive drug worldwide and is highly used in high schools nowadays (Koob
et al. 2014; Feldman 2011).
Chemical structure: C21H30O2
90 M. H. Kadhim
Tachycardia
Hallucinations
Effects of
Impaired
Increased tetrahydrocannabinol
coordination
appetite
Impaired memory
Conjunctivitis
The properties of marijuana differ from one to another, but they generally cause
euphoria in addition to feeling well (Fig. 8).
There are obvious risks associated with long-term, heavy chronic users of mari-
juana (Volkow et al. 2014). “Although marijuana does not seem to produce addic-
tion by itself, there is a possibility that marijuana has similarities with other drugs
such as cocaine and heroin in how they affect the brain. In addition, there is evi-
dence that heavy use at least temporarily decreases the production of the male sex
hormone testosterone, potentially affecting the sexual activity and sperm count”
(Volkow et al. 2014).
Medical uses: “Marijuana has several medical uses; it can be used to prevent nau-
sea from chemotherapy, treat AIDS symptoms, relieve muscle spasms for people
with spinal cord injuries, and it may be helpful in the treatment of Alzheimer’s dis-
ease” (Bridgeman and Abazia 2017).
Answers
1. (d)
2. (c)
3. (d)
4. (a)
5. (c)
References
Ahmed S, Zhou Z, Zhou J, Chen SQ. Pharmacogenomics of drug metabolizing enzymes and trans-
porters: relevance to precision medicine [published correction appears in Genomics Proteomics
Bioinformatics. 2018 Apr 21]. Genomics Proteomics Bioinformatics. 2016;14(5):298–313.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gpb.2016.03.008.
Anthenelli RM, Schuckit MA. Affective and anxiety disorders and alcohol and drug dependence:
diagnosis and treatment. J Addict Dis. 1993;12:73–87.
Baldo BA. Opioid analgesic drugs and serotonin toxicity (syndrome): mechanisms, animal mod-
els, and links to clinical effects. Arch Toxicol. 2018;92(8):2457–73.
Beaudoin FL, Merchant RC, Clark MA. Prevalence and detection of prescription opioid misuse
and prescription opioid use disorder among emergency department patients 50 years of age and
Drugs and Consciousness 93
older: performance of the prescription drug use questionnaire, patient version. Am J Geriatr
Psychiatry. 2016;24(8):627–36.
Bridgeman MB, Abazia DT. Medicinal cannabis: history, pharmacology, and implications for the
acute care setting. P T. 2017;42(3):180–8.
Cami J, Farré M. Drug addiction. N Engl J Med. 2003;349(10):975–86.
Clark MA, Harvey RA, Finkel R, Rey JA, Whalen K. Pharmacology, Fifth Edition. Philadelphia:
Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 2011.
Feldman RS. Understanding psychology. 10th ed. Amherst: University of Massachusetts; 2011.
Fredheim OM, Moksnes K, Borchgrevink PC, Kaasa S, Dale O. Clinical pharmacology of metha-
done for pain. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand. 2008;52(7):879–89. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1399-65
76.2008.01597.x.
Grant BF. Prevalence and correlates of alcohol use and DSM-IV alcohol dependence in the United
States: results of the National Longitudinal Alcohol Epidemiologic Survey. J Stud Alcohol.
1997;58(5):464–73.
Grissinger M. Keeping patients safe from methadone overdoses. Pharm Therap. 2011;36(8):462–6.
Imani F, Motavaf M, Safari S, Alavian SM. The therapeutic use of analgesics in patients with
liver cirrhosis: a literature review and evidence-based recommendations. Hepat Mon.
2014;14(10):e23539. https://doi.org/10.5812/hepatmon.23539. Published 2014 Oct 11.
Kirkpatrick M, Lee R, Wardle M, et al. Effects of MDMA and intranasal oxytocin on social and
emotional processing. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2014;39:1654–63. https://doi.org/10.1038/
npp.2014.12.
Koob GF, Volkow ND. Neurocircuitry of addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology.
2010;35(1):217–38.
Koob GF, Arends MA, Le Moal M. Drugs, addiction, and the brain. New York: Academic
Press; 2014.
Lindesmith AR. Addiction & opiates. New York: Routledge; 2017.
National Center for Biotechnology Information. PubChem compound summary for CID 5288826,
morphine. 2021. Retrieved October 13, 2021 from https://pubchem.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/
compound/Morphine.
Prus A. Drugs and the neuroscience of behavior: an introduction to psychopharmacology. 2nd ed.
Thousand Oaks: SAGE Publications; 2018.
Schiller EY, Goyal A, Mechanic OJ. Opioid overdose. In: StatPearls. Treasure Island: StatPearls
Publishing; 2021.
Söderberg Löfdal KC, Andersson ML, Gustafsson LL. Cytochrome P450-mediated changes in
oxycodone pharmacokinetics/pharmacodynamics and their clinical implications. Drugs.
2013;73:533–43. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40265-013-0036-0.
Su SS, Larison C, Ghadialy R, et al. Substance use among women in the United States.
SAMHSA analytic series A-3. Rockville: Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services
Administration; 1997.
Volkow ND, Baler RD, Compton WM, Weiss SR. Adverse health effects of marijuana use. N Engl
J Med. 2014;370(23):2219–27. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMra1402309.
Wagner FA, Anthony JC. From first drug use to drug dependence: developmental periods of
risk for dependence upon marijuana, cocaine, and alcohol. Neuropsychopharmacology.
2002;26(4):479–88.
Wang D. Opioid medications in the management of chronic abdominal pain. Curr Pain Headache
Rep. 2017;21(9):40. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11916-017-0640-x. Published 2017 Aug 8.
Weil A. The natural mind: an investigation of drugs and the higher consciousness. Boston:
Houghton Mifflin Harcourt; 1998.
Yilmaz B, Konar V, Kutlu S, et al. Influence of chronic morphine exposure on serum LH, FSH,
testosterone levels, and body and testicular weights in the developing male rat. Arch Androl.
1999;43(3):189–96. https://doi.org/10.1080/014850199262481.
The Altered States of Consciousness
1 Introduction
Thus, there are those times when our mind seems to split into two kinds of
consciousness.
Recently, researchers started studying consciousness varied by hypnosis and
drugs. Thus, psychology was regaining the definition of consciousness
(Fromm 2017).
Now, there are more research to understand the biology of consciousness. For
example, evolutionary psychologists consider that consciousness must offer a seed
and soil advantage. Another theory suggests that consciousness enhances our sur-
vival by anticipating how we live in the future and reading people’s minds.
Even so, the brain leaves us with a lot of complex problems. For example, it cre-
ates the awareness of ourselves, the pain of a headache, the feeling of fight and flight
(Fromm 2017).
2 Cognitive Neuroscience
It is the science that deals with cognition, and it involves many functions as in Fig. 1.
When researchers asked this young woman to imagine playing tennis or going to
university, fMRI scans showed brain activity as healthy volunteers (Fig. 2).
Despite these advancements, much disagreement remains. For example, some
researchers suggest that consciousness arises from specific neuronal activity. Others
suggest that the whole activity of the neurons produces consciousness.
M. H. Kadhim (*)
University of Kufa, College of Medicine, Najaf, Iraq
Perception,
Motor Control Recognition,
Language Attention
Cognitive Neuroscience
Fig. 1 Cognitive neuroscience, the interdisciplinary trial of the brain excitation integrated with
cognition, including memory, language, and thinking
3 Dual Processing
We are aware of just little about our unconscious mind. Perception, memory, think-
ing, language, and attitudes all work based on two levels: (Preller et al. 2019).
–– Conscious (highly initiative mind)
–– Unconscious and automatic (low initiative mind)
That’s what we mean when we say “dual processing.”
A young woman who had brain damage affecting the visual part could not recog-
nize objects and differentiate them visually. But she was only partially blind. So she
was asked to handle an object; she could adequately do that (Fig. 3).
Scientists were wondering how this happens. As all of us know, we have only one
visual system (Cofré et al. 2020).
Also, the researchers have found that the reverse damage leads to the opposite
symptoms. It means a patient may see objects but have difficulty in handling them.
When you sense ten things by your five senses, you will process only a few of them
but not all of them. Yet your unconscious mind will take a process for the other
things that the conscious mind has not focused on.
For example, when reading this theory, you are not aware of the watch on your
hand or wearing a shoe. When you pay attention to these things, you have been
blocking from the awareness coming from the conscious mind (Cofré et al. 2020).
As you have read this example, you may be confused! I know it is genuinely
complicated that we have all of these integrative systems in our brains, but we are
unaware of them.
Another simple example of selective attention is when four people say your
name simultaneously, your mind will focus on one of them and ignore the others
(Fig. 4). Also, imagine hearing two conversations simultaneously and being asked
to remember both of them; you will not perceive what is said in the other conversa-
tion. Considering selective attention and accidents, cell phone use and driving acci-
dents are typical examples. That’s why you are not allowed to use a cell phone while
driving (Weinel 2018).
98 M. H. Kadhim
Out of sight, out of mind. But moreover, change deafness can happen (Fig. 7).
A common form of selective inattention happens in media, politics, and various
aspects of our lives. An astonishing form called choice blindness (Fig. 8) was
invented by a Swedish researcher, “Petter Johansson,” and his assistants. In his
example, he showed 2 female faces for 5 s to 120 volunteers and asked them which
one of those 2 women they found more attractive? Then, the researcher used a sleight
of hand (sleight of hand is part of the experimental method) to switch the photo that
were handed to the volunteers who did not pick. The volunteers were asked to justify
why they had chosen the photo and also justify the choice they haven’t made. They
were in a weird situation, and they did not noticed that (Holoyda 2020) (Fig. 9).
Selective attention and inattention can even affect your sleep (Fig. 10) and what
you have experienced in your dreams.
The other aspects of altered states of consciousness (Fig. 11) can be summarized
in the following topics:
(a) Sleep and dreams
(b) Hypnosis
(c) Drugs and consciousness
(d) Meditation and yoga
100 M. H. Kadhim
Fig. 7 An example of
change deafness
Hypnosis
There are many drugs that have an effect human’s consciousness starting from alter-
ing its states, addiction to changing the mood. More details on this topic have
already been discussed in chapter “Drugs and Consciousness”.
It is defined as a learned technique for refocusing attention and rebooting it, leading
to an altered consciousness (Moreira-Almeida and Lotufo-Neto 2017).
Meditation affects the mind through different techniques, but it has the same goal
of relaxing and refocusing attention. You can do it while exercising, sitting, and
lying down (Fig. 12). It would be best if you think of nothing for a while (Schmidt
and Berkemeyer 2018).
There are various ways to do meditation. Most of it is done sitting in special
postures, such as:
• the full or half lotus
• using relaxed cushions or sitting on a couch and the feet bent underneath.
All these positions have the same goal; to reach a spiritual insight that is to relax
and to be alert. There is nothing distinct about these positions. In meditation, people
need to achieve a certain level of stabilization and avoid two things: either becoming
sleepy or being nervous by distracting thoughts or perceptions. The unique postures
provide a good base, relaxed state, a straight spine, and deep breathing. So, avoiding
these two irregular things is what we have mentioned.
People say that they are feeling fully relaxed after meditation. They claim that it
has relieved their stress life event and made them more patient in facing the prob-
lems. Barnes, Kleinman, and Travis found that oxygen usage decreases, blood pres-
sure and heart rate decrease, and brain activities change during meditation (Chieffi
et al. 2018; Kandeepan et al. 2020).
With more and more training, it is possible to calm our minds and leave the dis-
tractive thoughts off. Furthermore, our brains may adapt to cope with life worries
and problems and help us to reach the inside happiness (Fig. 13). Although some
experiments are surprising, they found that meditation is no more relaxing than the
other ordinary relaxed states. Some argue that it even may be more problematic if
specific unwanted thoughts keep coming up and you cannot control your mind and
emotions. Therefore, they say it is better to do exercise than to meditate.
The Altered States of Consciousness 103
Improves brain
concentration
Improves
Helps with social
breathing
life
efficiently
Reduces
emotional Relaxes the mind
explosions
Induces physical
relaxation
Meditation is practiced in many cultures and religions. It takes various aims and
different goals among cultures. Interestingly, we found that people with different
cultures and religions seek ways to alter their consciousness to achieve spiritual
experience (Fig. 14).
Answers
1. (c)
Meditation helps reduce blood pressure and increases pain threshold, relaxes
the mind, and improves brain concentration.
2. (c)
It has been used as a treatment for asthma. Also, it is used for headaches and
stress-related skin disorders.
3. (a)
Two-track mind is a theory that describes the mind’s two levels, and it is unre-
lated to hypnosis.
4. (b)
Selective attention is the focus of mind and conscious awareness on a particular
stimulus.
5. (d)
It describes as the mind’s two levels.
6. (d)
Selective attention and inattention can even affect your sleep and what you have
experienced in your dreams.
7. (d)
Depending on your cortical function and consciousness, they can activate spe-
cific cortical patterns and see your brain thinking in limited ways.
8. (c)
Meditation is classified as a psychologically induced state of consciousness.
9. (d)
Choice blindness is an example of selective inattention which is used in poli-
tics, media, and various aspects of our lives.
10. (d)
Distinctive brain activity is one of the biological influences of hypnosis.
References
Chieffi IV, et al. Mind, brain and altered states of consciousness. Acta Medica. 2018;34:357.
Cofré R, et al. Whole-brain models to explore altered states of consciousness from the bottom up.
Brain Sci. 2020;10(9):626.
Fromm E. The nature of hypnosis and other altered states of consciousness: an ego psychological
theory 1. In: Hypnosis. New York: Routledge; 2017. p. 81–104.
Holoyda B. Psychedelic psychiatry: preparing for novel treatments involving altered states of con-
sciousness. Psychiatr Serv. 2020;71(12):1297–9.
Johnson EL, Kaplan PW. Clinical neurophysiology of altered states of consciousness: encepha-
lopathy and coma. Handbk Clin Neurol. 2019;161:73–88.
Kandeepan S, et al. Modeling an auditory stimulated brain under altered states of consciousness
using the generalized Ising model. Neuroimage. 2020;223:117367.
Moreira-Almeida A, Lotufo-Neto F. Methodological guidelines to investigate altered states of con-
sciousness and anomalous experiences. Int Rev Psychiatry. 2017;29(3):283–92.
The Altered States of Consciousness 107
Preller KH, et al. Effective connectivity changes in LSD-induced altered states of consciousness in
humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2019;116(7):2743–8.
Schmidt TT, Berkemeyer H. The altered states database: psychometric data of altered states of
consciousness. Front Psychol. 2018;9:1028.
Weinel J. Inner sound: altered states of consciousness in electronic music and audio-visual media.
Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2018.
Wittmann M. Altered states of consciousness: experiences out of time and self. Cambridge: MIT
Press; 2018.
Hypnosis and Consciousness
1 Hypnosis
Overload of
message units
Hypnosis
Disorganizing inhibitory
Hyper process
suggestible state
Triggering fight
and flight
Both encounters are associated with a degree of subjective belief that borders on
delusion and an experience of involuntariness that borders on compulsion in the
classic example.
Subjects respond to recommendations for creative interactions in hypnosis that
may include cognitive perception, memory, and behavior changes. However, among
those incredibly hypnotizable topics, these phenomena arise more intensely
(Valentine et al. 2019).
While most individuals may undergo hypnosis to at least some degree, the most
extreme hypnosis phenomena are typically found in those that count as representing
changes in consciousness. Thus, those “hypnotic virtuosos” make up the top
10–15% of the hypnotizable distribution.
A great deal of hypnosis study thus requires a priori collection of subjects who
are highly hypnotizable, to the exclusion of those with low and intermediate hypno-
tizable. An alternative is a hybrid system in which subjects stratified for hypnotiz-
able are all subjected to the same experimental manipulations, and hypnotizable
subjects’ reactions are compared to others subjected to the same experimental
manipulations. Hypnosis is insusceptible. In either case, hypnotizable estimation is
important for research into hypnosis; there is no point in analyzing hypnosis in
people who can’t feel it.
Any psychiatric clinicians assume that if only the hypnotist follows the correct
path, nearly all can be hypnotized, but no studies favor this point of view. Similarly,
some experts suggest that hypnotizable can be increased by cultivating constructive
hypnosis beliefs, motives, and aspirations although there is also evidence that these
measures are closely related to enforcement.
The multifaceted essence of hypnosis itself is part of the issue. Hypnosis requires
improvements in conscious thought, memory, and actions, to be sure, but these
Hypnosis and Consciousness 111
Fig. 2 Hypnosis can assist people in accepting and experiencing what can be done by them
Hypnosis does not make the impossible possible (Williamson 2019). Instead, it
can assist people in accepting and experiencing what can be done by them. Since
civilization has existed, hypnotic states have been used for treatment (Fig. 2).
It was possible to see the right brain as the most emotional, imaginative part of
us that deals with metaphors and pictures and may be seen as our unconscious mind.
However, it is still impossible to state that we are not angry or nervous because
words are not the right brain’s language.
They work at an emotional rather than logical level while patients are highly
nervous, and one should activate and direct their imaginative creativity to what is
beneficial for them. To construct potential catastrophic situations, nervous people
use their imagination, which creates still more fear and thus more dopamine, which
can spiral into hysteria (Jensen et al. 2017).
Patients can find that their feelings are overloaded. If health providers can stimu-
late their energy, guide their creativity to feel relaxed, or re-encounter a meaningful
experience or behavior and have positive feedback, they will feel calmer and can
cope more (Williamson 2019; Lynn et al. 2020).
There are some similarities shared between hypnosis and meditation. Both are
states of concentrated attention, and both can be used to create a profound feeling of
relief (Keshmiri et al. 2020).
As we said, hypnosis is a social interaction where the hypnotist asks the subject to
experience certain thoughts, behaviors, and feelings.
So, we can say that we are all open to suggestions. For example, when the hyp-
notist asks the hypnotized person to stand and close his or her eyes, and tells he or
she is swinging now; surprisingly, the subject will sway slightly. Some researchers
say that people with or without hypnosis will respond differently.
Hypnosis and Consciousness 113
The hypnotist supposes various experiences ranging from easy to difficult after a
hypnotic induction (ranging from moving your legs to opening your mouth, opening
your eyes and imagining what the hypnotist said). For example, Barnier and
McConkey had said that the people who did not react to the smell of a bottle of
ammonia are those who deeply went into the hypnosis and experienced the imagi-
nary sight.
So, there is a difference between people in hypnosis experience depending on
your hypnotic susceptibility.
In conclusion, anyone can experience hypnosis by themselves when integrating
the inward and outward imaginary ability. If you expect to be hypnotized, you will
experience it. Imagine that you are sitting at the table after hypnotic induction, and
the hypnotist asks you to draw a circle; you will write it without the ability to eject it.
For instance, can hypnosis help elderly people to remember their primary school
classmates, to discover their fears and details of a crime? Can it be used in criminal
investigations (Franz et al. 2020)?
Also, it is an inaccurate statement to say that hypnosis can recall accurate memo-
ries of people since birth. Johnson and Hauck proved this in 1999. Furthermore,
there is no supposed ability to relive your own childhood experiences. They feel like
real children and speak like them, but there is not any change in adult brain waves
in MRI. Now, it is banned that you are making evidence from witnesses who have
been hypnotized because the hypnotist can create pseudo-memory to the hypnotized
subject. For example, by asking “did you feel that you are entering someone’s
home …. stealing their properties …. killing someone,” so you can plant false infor-
mation and create pseudo-memory.
Sometimes, hypnosis may be used in the wrong way that the hypnotist induces
the subject to do a scary deed. For example, asking the subject to stand up, take a
knife or any other thing, and throw it onto a researcher. Surprisingly, some people
do not remember their acts and deny doing such things.
There is a conflict and controversial debate about this theory. However, most hyp-
nosis researchers suggest that regular social and cognitive neuroscience play an
essential part in hypnosis.
114 H. T. Hashim and M. A. Ramadhan
The researcher, Perugini, in 1998, demonstrated that: “For one thing, hypnotized
subjects will sometimes carry out suggested behaviors on cue, even when they
believe no one is watching.”
In an experiment, people whether hypnotizable or not were asked to say the color
of letters (the word GREEN in red color to create a little bit of conflict). Unhypnotized
people were slowed from this conflicting idea. At the same time, hypnotized people
were quickly slowed by this conflict and were able to solve this conflict (Brain areas
involved in decoding words and revealing the conflict remained inactive). Famous
researcher Ernest Hilgard (1986–1992) believes in social influence theory and
divided-consciousness theory. It means that hypnosis involves social influence the-
ory and a defined theory of dissociation—a split between different levels of con-
sciousness. Hilgard believed that splitting consciousness is a part of everyday life
events. For example, sometimes, we paint or write a poem while listening to a
lecture.
Hypnosis also may lead to pain relief. A PET scan supported this. Thus, hypnosis
blocks our attention to painful stimuli, but it cannot block sensory input in the brain
cortex (Shalbaf et al. 2020).
Many clinical and psychological applications exist for this technique in treatment,
research, and diagnosis (Fig. 3). We will mention them in the following points:
Weight
Depression Anxiety
Hypnosis Motivation
Eating disorders
Stress
1.4.1 Hypnotherapy
For the prevention of irritable bowel syndrome, hypnotherapy has been examined.
In the National Institute for Wellbeing and Clinical Excellence guidelines released
by UK health services, hypnosis for IBS has gained modest support. It has been
applied as an aid or substitutes for chemical anesthesia and has been researched as
a means to soothe skin ailments. However, the topic is still under development and
needs more studies and research to be approved (Gruenewald et al. 2017).
Answers
1. (a)
2. (c)
3. (d)
4. (b)
5. (a)
Hypnosis and Consciousness 117
References
Bowers KS, Bowers PG. Hypnosis and creativity: a theoretical and empirical rapprochement. In:
Hypnosis: research developments and perspectives. New York: Routledge; 2017. p. 255–92.
Franz M, et al. Suggested deafness during hypnosis and simulation of hypnosis compared to a
distraction and control condition: a study on subjective experience and cortical brain responses.
PLoS One. 2020;15(10):e0240832.
Gruenewald D, Fromm E, Oberlander MI. Hypnosis and adaptive regression: an ego-psychological
inquiry. In: Hypnosis: research developments and perspectives. New York: Routledge; 2017.
p. 495–510.
Jensen MP, Jamieson GA, Lutz A. New directions in hypnosis research: strategies for advancing
the cognitive and clinical neuroscience of hypnosis. Neurosci Conscious. 2017;3:1–14.
Keshmiri S, Alimardani M, Shiomi M, Sumioka H, Ishiguro H, Hiraki K. Higher hypnotic suggest-
ibility is associated with the lower EEG signal variability in theta, alpha, and beta frequency
bands. PLoS One. 2020;15(4):e0230853. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0230853.
Levitt EE, Chapman RH. Hypnosis as a research method. In: Hypnosis: research developments and
perspectives. New York: Routledge; 2017. p. 85–114.
Lynn SJ, et al. Hypnosis and health psychology. In: The Wiley encyclopedia of health psychology.
New York: Wiley; 2020. p. 257–63.
Orne MT. On the simulating subject as a quasi-control group in hypnosis research: what, why,
and how. In: Hypnosis: research developments and perspectives. New York: Routledge; 2017.
p. 399–444.
Shalbaf A, et al. Monitoring the level of hypnosis using a hierarchical SVM system. J Clin Monit
Comput. 2020;34(2):331–8.
Shor RE. The fundamental problem in hypnosis research as viewed from historic perspectives.
In: Hypnosis: research developments and perspectives. New York: Routledge; 2017. p. 15–40.
Terhune DB, et al. Hypnosis and top-down regulation of consciousness. Neurosci Biobehav Rev.
2017;81:59–74.
Valentine KE, et al. The efficacy of hypnosis as a treatment for anxiety: a meta-analysis. Int J Clin
Exp Hypn. 2019;67(3):336–63.
Williamson A. What is hypnosis and how might it work? Palliat Care. 2019;12:1178224219826581.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1178224219826581.
Artificial Consciousness
1 Introduction
In 1992, the word “artificial intelligence” was used scientifically for the first time.
Since then, scientists have not agreed on when such an accomplishment is feasible.
Every recognized physical structure does not possess the observed properties of the
human mind, including unity, representation, and being in relation to each other
(Hildt 2019).
The Turing test is the most well-known tool for measuring computer intelligence
(Shieber 2004). Therefore, it was also proposed that the suggestion of Alan Turing
to mimic not the consciousness of a human adult, but the consciousness of a human
child should be taken seriously.
2 Digital Consciousness
It means using the computers to deliver and create a machine consciousness among
robots to deliver the same or at least similar functions of the human brain by pro-
cessing data and codes (Argonov 2014; Searle et al. 1980; Oppy and Dowe 2011;
Alexiou et al. 2020) (Fig. 1).
Mathematics and logic may, to a certain degree, describe and evaluate knowl-
edge understood as rationality, and computer science and rtificial intelligence as a
whole are primarily focused on mathematics and logic (Fig. 2). However, when it
comes to consciousness, mathematics terminates, and theory continues. From the
hippocampus down to the genetic code, scholars have sought to locate the origins of
consciousness.
Strong Weak
AI AI
Human Machine
Consciousness Consciousness
• Biological • Mathematical
• Chemical • Logical
Several concerns about artificial consciousness and creating similar mind process-
ing capabilities to humans raise many issues to be solved and controlled (Koch
2019). Channeling the philosopher Thomas Nagel, we might conclude that if there
is anything like that in a machine, a system is alive. Can the machine have feelings
and emotions when dealing with humans? There is no evidence that our intellect
and perceptions, rather than any divine ones, are inevitable products of our brain’s
inherent causal forces.
Therefore, a host of subsidiary mechanisms such as expression, preparation,
compensation circuits, and short-term memory buffer storage have access to the
signal. What makes us aware is the process of broadcasting this information
internationally.
Aware states emerge from the workspace algorithm processing the appropriate
sensory inputs, motor outputs, and internal variables linked to memory, inspiration,
and anticipation. What awareness is about is global production. Consciousness is
just a smart hack away. You can go back to chapters “Brain and Mind” and “Levels
of Consciousness” to understand these ideas extensively (Koch 2019).
Although in ascriptions of consciousness there is broad disagreement, there is
one thing similar to all of the above; superintelligence is not needed. To show this,
the human case itself is necessary. Many hypotheses go even farther, ascribing cog-
nition to beings of rudimentary knowledge or indeed no intelligence in certain
instances. Extrapolation of human intelligence in minimally intelligent artificial
systems might make it possible to replicate consciousness. Our perception of con-
sciousness must keep pace with the development in artificial intelligence and be
extended to it.
As with artificial intelligence, in following artificial consciousness, a distinction
may be made between two linked but separate priorities.
Artificial consciousness is mainly concerned with understanding the processes
underlying consciousness. Therefore, the tools offered by engineering artificial con-
sciousness, however excellent, are considered to be of theoretical significance only
to the degree that they approximate or otherwise illuminate the processes underly-
ing consciousness.
Inside artificial consciousness, more distinctions can be made about the relation-
ship between the hardware used for an artificial consciousness system and the con-
sciousness that it is assumed to hold. Adapting terms from weak artificial
consciousness is a method that does not assert a link between consciousness and
technology. This will be a use of consciousness awareness technologies similar to
computational hurricane simulations in meteorology: such simulations can promote
understanding, but no one believes that this is because hurricanes are themselves
computational in any substantive sense.
On the other end, strong artificial consciousness is any method whose ultimate
purpose is to create structures that are instantiations of consciousness when applied.
122 H. T. Hashim and M. A. Ramadhan
of thought; thus, it is usually possible for a thinker to write down certain steps in a
finite list. At least some people assume that it is not inconceivable that, with regard
to its experiential properties, there may be something that is biologically (and hence
behaviorally) similar to you and still separate from you, even to the extent of not
getting any memories at all.
These two intuitions are in sharp conflict: the naturalistic intuition is that you fix
everything else if you fix the actual problem. On the other hand, the zombic hunch
is that you somehow have not resolved the feeling even if you fix it physically
(Fig. 3). Thus, an unsatisfactory cognitive dissonance is created by the existence of
any of these intuitions.
There are many explanations why one would assume that artificial consciousness
cannot be done in the context of engineering or that it cannot lead to an experience
of consciousness.
Although the notion that phenomenal cognition cannot be realized in computers
seems to be an apparent inference, there are grounds for a more thorough analysis
of this issue. Advances in artificial intelligence are growing with immense speed.
There will be greater connectivity to more efficient computers that can do more
complex computation as software and hardware developments continue to advance
(Hromiak 2020).
There are computer programs to perform identification, target tracking, and face
recognition.
From simple information gathering to extremely dexterous gestures, logic, and
the development of aesthetic experiences, the human mind can perform many amaz-
ing acts and calculations (Haladjian and Montemayor 2016).
Attention is significant because it is how the brain selectively stores information
derived from sensory and memory inputs (Haladjian and Montemayor 2016).
The focus of the universe will also function on objects that show object-like fea-
tures, such as balance, symmetry, and collective destiny. “Object-based focus
involves a two-stage process that starts with the individualization of objects. Then,
to connect object attributes, which are made accessible by feature maps, selective
Naturalistic Intuition:
Everything else will be fixed if the actual thing is fixed.
Zombic Hunch:
Feeling is not resolved even if the physical thing is
fixed.
Fig. 3 The naturalistic intuition is that you fix everything else if you fix the actual problem, while
the zombic hunch is that you somehow have not resolved the feeling even if you fix it physically
124 H. T. Hashim and M. A. Ramadhan
3. For many attention routines, the solid propensity for reducing these characteris-
tics to mechanistic algorithmic routines can work, but not for feelings and
agency, because:
(a) The emotions are separated from attention.
(b) Attending to and accepting emotions must not be correlated with feel-
ing them.
(c) Emotions are involuntary.
(d) Robots have no emotions.
4. The capability to identify and communicate with other objects is necessary:
(a) To enhance computer vision.
(b) To build more interconnecting robots.
(c) To do more research and experiments.
(d) No need for that.
Answers
1. (d)
2. (a)
3. (b)
4. (a)
References
Alexiou A, Simou P, Alexiou F, Chatzichronis S, Ashraf GM. Social and ethical impact of advanced
artificial and biological enhancements. Open Public Health J. 2020;13(1):62–8. https://doi.
org/10.2174/1874944502013010062.
Argonov V. Experimental methods for unraveling the mind-body problem: the phenomenal judg-
ment approach. J Mind Behav. 2014;35:51–70.
Bauer H, Veira J, Weig F. Moore’s law: repeal or renewal? 2013. Retrieved from McKinsey &
Company.
Ernst A, Bertrand JMF, Voltzenlogel V, Souchay C, Moulin CJA. The proust machine: what a pub-
lic science event tells us about autobiographical memory and the five senses. Front Psychol.
2021;11:623910. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.623910. Published 2021 Jan 20.
Haladjian HH, Montemayor C. Artificial consciousness and the consciousness-attention dissocia-
tion. Conscious Cogn. 2016;45:210–25.
Hildt E. Artificial intelligence: does consciousness matter? Front Psychol. 2019;10:1535.
Hromiak M. A new charter of ethics and rights of artificial consciousness in a human world. arXiv
preprint. arXiv:2010.12019; 2020.
Koch C. Will machines ever become conscious? Scientific American; 2019.
Manzotti R, Chella A. Good old-fashioned artificial consciousness and the intermediate level fal-
lacy. Front Robot AI. 2018;5:39.
126 H. T. Hashim and M. A. Ramadhan
Mudrik L, Faivre N, Koch C. Information integration without awareness. Trends Cogn Sci.
2014;18(9):488–96. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.009.
Oppy G, Dowe D. The Turing test. Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Spring 2011
Edition); 2011.
Riedl MO, Harrison B. Using stories to teach human values to artificial agents. Paper presented at
the 2nd international workshop on AI, ethics, and society. 2015. http://www.aaai.org.
Rinesi M. The price of the internet of things will be a vague dread of a malicious world. IEET.org
website. 2015. http://ieet.org/index.php/IEET/more/rinesi20150925.
Searle J, et al. Minds, brains, and programs. Behav Brain Sci. 1980;3(3):417–57.
Shieber S. The Turing test: verbal behavior as the hallmark of intelligence. Cambridge: MIT Press;
2004. ISBN 978-0-262-69293-9.
Treisman A, Gelade G. A feature-integration theory of attention. Cogn Psychol.
1980;12(1):97–136. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(80)90005-5.
Vervoort L, et al. Artificial consciousness, superintelligence and ethics in robotics: how to get
there? Mind Matter. 2021;19(1):7–43.
Relativity of the Human Mind
1 Introduction
Relativity means the matter or anything that will be discussed can differ with the
situations and the circumstances like space and time. So people can do things and
think it is right in some situations, but the same actions will be wrong in other cir-
cumstances. For example, a man can drink alcohol on certain occasions with con-
trollable amount, but this action is wrong when it is done while driving because the
situation is different here and the consequences are massive. Also, I can think that I
am a child and whatever I do is to learn from it, but when I am an adult and do the
same mistakes, its consequences will be unforgivable because the time is different
and mind is different as well.
Humans’ minds are dealing with the actions and giving orders in the same way
of relativity so to keep a balance between our surroundings and our bodies and
believes.
Any conflict or imbalance between our minds and our environment will create an
issue that the mind cannot deal with, and this leads to psychological collapse.
In this chapter, we will discuss this topic from many fields and focus on a new
presentation for the relativity theory in human consciousness as we believe that the
human’s consciousness is relative as well and we should deal with it depending on
this idea.
The lack of ego is an extreme limit to pro-activity and leaves people passively enter-
ing your path. Nowadays, we have the virtual path; it’s quick but shallow even if the
book was there before. Even for some cosmic reason, people cross our path, which
has never changed. Today, we have a wide variety of networks. We can engage with
crowds in Facebook groups, for instance, for the first time in human history, which
makes me believe that conscious evolution can accelerate.
The “teal” campaign, as “green” is the end of life, has also become almost main-
stream. However, if you are still not familiar with the color code of evolution and
world views, please see my article on Spiral Dynamics Integral.
We may witness a significant transformation in our capacity to cope with our sur-
roundings, living beings, and nature in general while we are there. The transforma-
tion will be dramatic, as crucial individuals will get a broader perspective and break
free from the tiny circular circles of interest that have surrounded them up to this
point (Styer 2011).
When they reach the higher levels, it is not necessary for anybody, or even a
plurality of individuals, to acquire this degree of awareness for the connectivity of
all to reoccur to a critical mass of people. As the global view expands, we will take
more outstanding care of social justice, intentionally promote peace, clear up the
trash in the oceans, replant trees, and so on, climbing the rungs of the spiral phases.
This, by the way, would create a few jobs. We shall develop a healthier planet for all
living beings to ensure a safe spiral of evolution.
Language is part of human nature and being. There are compelling grounds to
believe that no other entity possesses a distinctively human talent. Unlike other
animals, we acquire complex linguistic norms rapidly and without purposeful, con-
scious effort in our early years. As our vocabulary expands, we may investigate the
structure of phrases, the function and importance of terminology, and how phrases
and words might be employed for social and practical goals (Psychology
Today 2021).
Social actions become more complex as a result of language. Language improves
reasoning skills since it allows us to express our views explicitly. Furthermore, lan-
guage necessitates the habit of openly asking for explanations and reasons, which
Relativity of the Human Mind 129
Relativity in our theory means seeing things from different perspectives that change
with situations. So, for example, when you see someone has a terrible situation, you
will have your own opinion and behavior toward the issue. In contrast, the opinion
and the behavior will differ when you are in the middle of the fire (Thierry 2016).
So, the relativity in psychology is not related to space and time; it relates to situ-
ations. When the situation differs, the behavior differs too. For example, when we
put people in a bad situation like fighting and ask the other group to behave, they
advise them to calm down and think clearly. When we make the same group fight
and ask them to calm down, they respond aggressively. The same situation and the
same people but different behavior is our relativity. The control factor in these situ-
ations is the mind. Your mind interacts with situations depending on the data it
receives from the visual perspective with feelings and emotions. So, clearness dur-
ing behavior can affect decisions and, thus, the results.
Answers
1. (b)
2. (a)
3. (a)
4. (a)
5. (d)
References
Psychology Today. Consciousness and language. 2021 [cited 2021 Nov 13]. https://www.psychol-
ogytoday.com/us/blog/theory-consciousness/201608/consciousness-and-language.
Sorli A, et al. Advanced relativity: multidimensionality of consciousness and mind, origin of life,
psi phenomena. NeuroQuantology. 2017;15(2):109–17.
Styer DF. Relativity for the questioning mind. Baltimore: JHU Press; 2011.
Thierry G. Neurolinguistic relativity: how language flexes human perception and cognition. Lang
Learn. 2016;66(3):690–713.
Death and Consciousness
1 Introduction
With plenty of parallels and dislikenesses, rest and demise are if you are in yoga,
one school demonstrates to you that contemplation is a specialty of kicking the
bucket (actually what it means is that we totally free body consciousness as reflec-
tion happens to us), cognition achieves a past state psyche, but in any known dialect,
this cannot be reflected in words. As troublesome as the sweet portrayal of any
dialect (Lund 2016).
A state connected with the body and the brain is added to your inquiry about
when one more condition of a presence called otherworldly is included. Profound
rest is near-death as consciousness is permitted to choose whether to backpedal to
mind and body or free itself and choose another desire to be conceived again.
Cognizance joins itself as it leaves the body with the last thought in the brain.
Mercifully take note that cognizance will relate to only one thing at any given
moment. It gets pulled in to alter, too. So those out of the body will lose the progres-
sions realized by the gross detects, hence no more improvements in this direction,
the last idea is clutched.
Advancing fixation and culminating reflection help us, considering the fact that
the consciousness comes to the past brain, to use the embodiment of the gross facul-
ties placed away in the memory. I used pith and not the gross detect feature. The
dialect is not vital along these lines.
3 Near-Death Experiences
It is defined as the moments that come during some life events prior to death or
when the individual is about to lose his or her life (Koch 2021).
These concrete and fantastic encounters have the potential to alter their lives
permanently. NDEs are not fanciful imaginings (Van Lommel 2006).
The media coverage of near-death experiences (NDEs) is likely to have led to
expectations about how people could feel following such experiences (Carter 2010).
Local brain areas fall dark one after the other, like a municipality losing power
one community at a time.
Given the power interruptions, this experience has the potential to yield the
bizarre and unusual accounts that comprise the corpus of NDE reports. The NDE is
as genuine to the person experiencing it as anything manufactured by the imagina-
tion during regular waking hours. The mind and consciousness are extinguished
when the whole brain shuts down due to total power loss (Klein 2020; Koch 2021).
Again, an aura, a special feeling unique to a patient that indicates an impending
assault, will precede them.
Following a seizure, you may experience changes in the apparent proportions of
items, strange tastes, scents, or physical sensations, déjà vu, depersonalization, or
euphoric sentiments.
Death and Consciousness 133
Many beliefs can lead us to the point that the consciousness may remain to exist
after death or at least remains working (Van Lommel 2010).
Pharmacological manipulation is used by various medications that influence
neural activity by interfering with neurotransmission, resulting in vision, emotion,
awareness, cognition, and behavior changes. Psychoactive drugs are classified into
four groups based on their pharmacological effects: euphoriants, stimulants that
induce transient improvements in either mental or physical functions, depressants
that depress or decrease arousal or stimulation, and hallucinogens that may cause
hallucinations, anomalies in perception, and other significant subjective changes in
perceptions, emotions, and consciousness (Metcalf and Huntington 2014).
Death was formerly defined as the cessation of heartbeat and breathing (cardiac
arrest). Nonetheless, the development of CPR and quick defibrillation has rendered
this paradigm obsolete, as it is frequently feasible to resume breathing and heartbeat.
Life may often be preserved without a functional heart or lungs using various life
support systems, organ transplants, and artificial pacemakers; events that used to be
causally associated with death no longer kill under all conditions (Metcalf and
Huntington 2014).
Answers
1. (d)
2. (a)
3. (b)
4. (c)
References
Carter C. Science and the near-death experience: how consciousness survives death. Toronto:
Simon and Schuster; 2010.
Klein A. The death of consciousness? James’s case against psychological unobservables. J Hist
Philos. 2020;58(2):293–323.
Koch C. What near-death experiences reveal about the brain. Sci Am. 2021 [cited 2021 Nov 13].
https://www.scientificamerican.com/article/what-n ear-d eath-experiences-r eveal-a bout-
the-brain/
Lund DH. Death and consciousness. Jefferson: McFarland; 2016.
Metcalf P, Huntington R. Celebrations of death. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2014.
Parnia S. Death and consciousness––an overview of the mental and cognitive experience of death.
Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2014;1330(1):75–93.
Van Lommel P. Near-death experience, consciousness, and the brain: a new concept about the
continuity of our consciousness based on recent scientific research on near-death experience in
survivors of cardiac arrest. World Futures. 2006;62(1–2):134–51.
Van Lommel P. Consciousness beyond life. New York: HarperCollins; 2010.
Index
A Benzedrine, 81
Abuse, 89 Biological influences, 104, 105
Addiction, 82 Borderline Personality Disorder (BPD), 71
Alcohol, 83 Brain–mind relationship
Altered states of consciousness, 99, 101 consciousness, 21
Altruism, 76 neuroanatomy, 21
American Psychiatric Association (ASA), 58 senses, 21
Amphetamine, 81 Brain regions
Amygdala, 19 left and right hemispheres, 19
Analgesics, 86 limbic system, 19
Antisocial personality disorder (ASPD), 70 memory, 20
Artificial consciousness neocortex, 19
attention, 123 pathologies, 19
behavioral processes, 122 sensory cortex, 21
consciousness liberation, 124
generality, 122
human intelligence, 124 C
Lagom, 122 Cataplexy, 45
mind processing capabilities, 121 Central sleep apnea (CSA), 43, 44
naturalistic intuition, 123 Choice blindness, 100
object-based focus, 123 Circadian, 39
strong consciousness, 121 Circadian rhythm sleep-wake disorders, 43
subsidiary mechanisms, 121 Clinical death, 133
tools, 121 CNS depressants
Artificial intelligence, 119, 124 alcohol, 83
Attention, 112, 114 barbiturates, 84
Automatic processes, 111 CNS stimulants, 92
Autosuggestion, 32 amphetamine, 81, 82
Avoidant personality disorder (AVPD), 73 caffeine, 81, 82
cocaine, 83
nicotine, 81
B Cocaine, 83
Barbiturates, 84, 85, 92 Codeine, 86
Benign sleep myoclonus of infancy Cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT), 73, 74
(BSMI), 48 Cognitive perception, 110
Coma, 22 Delirium, 24
Computer intelligence, 119 Dependence, 84
Computer programs, 123 Detritivores, 131
Computer science, 119, 122 Dexedrine, 81
Confusion, 23 Dialectical behavioral therapy (DBT), 71, 74
Confusional arousal, 54 Digital consciousness, 119
Connecting memories, 29 Divided-consciousness theory, 114
Conscious evolution, 128 Dopamine agonist, 92
Consciousness, 51, 85, 109, 120 Dreams
abnormal states, 22 conscious mind, 49
agreement, 10 consciousness, 51, 52, 54
awareness, 6 definition, 49
death, 131 lucid, 51
decisions and actions, 13 meta-awareness, 51, 52
definition, 2 neurobiological perspective, 50
development, 1–3 postconsciousness, 52
developmental psychology, 10, 11 properties, 49, 50
domains, 4 self-awareness, 52
explanations, 1 Dual processing, 97
Freud, S., 6 change deafness, 99, 100
language, 128, 129 choice blindness, 99, 100
levels, 8 selective attention, 97, 98
ego, 27 two-track mind, 97, 98
Id and ego, 27 Dualism, 6
personality, 27
preconscious, 29
Superego, 27 E
medicine, 22 Electromyography (EMG), 49
meditation, 4 Emotions control communication, 30
memory, 132
mind and body interactions, 4
neuropsychological research, 13 F
neuropsychology, 14, 15 Fentanyl, 87
neuroscience, 14 Freudian theory, 6, 7, 15
object, 13
personal ID, 2
psychiatry, 22 G
social psychology, 11 GABA agonist, 85, 92
Consciousness awareness technologies, 121 Gibson’s theory, 9
Glasgow Coma Scale (GCS), 23
Grady Coma Scale (GCS), 23
D
Daydreaming, 100
Death, 133 H
Defense mechanism Habenula, 20
classification, 58, 59 Hallucination
definition, 57 LSD, 91
immature defenses, 60, 61 marijuana, 89, 90
mature defenses, 65, 66 MDMA, 90, 91
mental health, 57 Histrionic personality disorder (HPD), 72
moral values and principles, 57 Human
neurotic defenses, 62–64 vs. machine consciousness, 120
pathological defenses, 59 Human awareness, 120
Delete Button, 31 Human behavior
Index 137
relativity, 129 L
Human complex conscious abilities, 14 Language development, 128
Human consciousness, 5, 122 Laterodorsal tegmentum (LDT), 50
Human intelligence, 124 Lethargy, 22, 23
Human mind Locke’s theory, 3
ego, 128
human’s consciousness, 127
relativity, 127 M
social justice, 128 Machine consciousness, 119
Hypercapnic type, 44 MAOIs selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors
Hypnosis, 102, 105 (SSRIs), 88
calming experience, 111 Marijuana, 90
clinical and psychological effects of tetrahydrocannabinol, 90
applications, 114 hallucination, 89
hypnotherapy, 115 medical uses, 90
pain management and cancer positive and negative effects, 90
supporter, 115 properties, 90
prevention of irritable bowel Meditations, 4, 102–105
syndrome, 115 Mentalization-based therapy (MBT), 71
consciousness, 109 Meperidine, 88
as divided consciousness, 113–114 Metacognition-oriented therapy, 68
emotional, 112 Metacognitive interpersonal therapy (MIT), 74
experiences, 113 Methadone, 88
hypnotic hypermnesia, 111 Methamphetamine, 82
hypnotizable estimation, 110 Morphine, 85–87
hypnotized people, 109
induction process, 110
interactions, 110 N
and meditation, 112 Narcissistic personality disorder (NPD), 71, 75
as meditative condition, 111 Narcotics
multifaceted essence, 110 addiction, 85
posthypnotic suggestion, 111 codeine, 86
radical neurological fentanyl, 87
improvement, 111 meperidine, 88
social interaction, 112 methadone, 88
uses, 114 morphine, 85, 86
Hypnotherapy, 115 oxycodone, 89
Hypnotic induction, 111 Naturalistic intuition, 123
Hypocapnic type, 44 Near-death experiences (NDEs), 132
Hypothalamus, 23 power interruptions, 132
Neurobiological theory, 54
Neuropsychology, 16
I Nightmare disorder, 54
Idiopathic hypersomnia (IH), 45 Non-human artificial consciousness, 122
Implicit memory, 22 Non-Rapid Eye Movement (NREM), 40
Inattention, 99, 101
Inattentional blindness, 99
Insomnia, 43 O
Intellectualization, 76 Obsessive-compulsive personality disorder
Irritable bowel syndrome, 115 (OCPD), 74
Obstructive sleep apnea (OSA), 43
Operative functioning, 30
J Opioids, 86–88, 92
Jet lag disorder, 53 Oxycodone, 89
138 Index
T
Transference-focused psychotherapy (TFP), 71 Z
Turing test, 119 Zombic hunch, 123
U
Unsatisfactory cognitive dissonance, 123