Study Like A Champ 121-145
Study Like A Champ 121-145
Study Like A Champ 121-145
R E T R I E VA L P R A C T I C E F O R
M O N I TO R I N G A N D M A S T E RY
Jenny, like many students, uses flash cards to help prepare for exams.
She typically uses them to memorize foreign-language translation equivalents
for her vocabulary tests. However, she also realizes that, although using flash
cards helps her sometimes, it is not always so helpful. A difficulty Jenny is
having is that she does not understand that flash cards are just tools that can
be used in many different ways, and some of these ways promote success and
others do not. Moreover, aside from using flash cards to learn foreign-
language equivalents, when used appropriately they can be a much more
valuable tool to master almost any course materials. Most important, when
using flash cards does work, an active ingredient is the incorporation of
retrieval practice into your study routine. In fact, using retrieval practice to
help you monitor and improve your learning is one of the most powerful
ways to study like a champion.
Although testing feels more difficult than just rereading the information you need to
learn, it is a more effective study strategy and a better use of your time.
FIG URE 5. 1. Mean Percentage Performance on a Test,
with 95% Confidence Intervals (Estimated)
Long Description.
It has worked for children, college students, older adults, adults with
aphasia, and adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
It has helped people learn simple associations (e.g., foreign-language
vocabulary), the content of text passages, definitions that are
foundational to introductory courses, and the steps involved in scientific
processes and mathematics.
It has been shown to not only improve people’s memory for the tested
content but also enhance their understanding of it and their ability to
apply what they have learned in new contexts.
Note. From “Testing Encourages Transfer Between Factual and Application Questions
in an Online Learning Environment,” by R. C. Thomas, C. R. Weywadt, J. L. Anderson,
B. Martinez-Papponi, and M. A. McDaniel, 2018, Journal of Applied Research in
Memory and Cognition, 7, p. 255. Copyright 2018 by Elsevier. Reprinted with
permission.
Long Description.
First, exam performance was better when students took a quiz on the
content, whether the quiz was on facts or application (the two shaded bars in
Figure 5.2), than when the content was not reviewed during a quiz (unshaded
bars). Second, and unsurprisingly, students did the best when the kind of
review quiz matched the kind of exam question; that is, reviews involving
fact-based questions led to better performance on exam questions that asked
for facts rather than applications, and vice versa. Finally, even for
mismatches, review quizzes benefited students’ exam performance; that is,
even when they had review questions that covered facts, attempting to
answer those review questions boosted later performance on application
questions (as compared with not being quizzed over the material). This
represents just one of many classroom demonstrations that have shown the
power of retrieval practice for boosting performance for different kinds of
students, classes, and exams (for a review, see Agarwal et al., 2021).
Long Description.
If you don’t want to spend the extra time making flash cards, no
problem. Many flash card programs are available online (many of them free),
which can make this process easier (for a review of these programs with
respect to how well they support successive relearning, see Dunlosky &
O’Brien, 2020). Some programs will even let you share your cards with
others, so why not pool your efforts with your friends and divide the work?
Moreover, when studying in college, John never used flash cards but used
successive relearning to master definitions, lists, and lengthy processes.
Instead of taking the time to make all those cards, he simply put his hand over
the definitions that appeared at the end of a textbook chapter and tested
himself by trying to write down the correct definition on scrap paper.
With a pack of sticky notes, you can place one over each answer you
want to retrieve and leave the word or phrase that you want to use as your
retrieval prompt uncovered. As shown in Figure 5.4, using sticky notes may
be particularly useful if you’re trying to memorize the names of objects on a
figure—in this illustration, the sticky notes are placed over the labels for the
structures of the forebrain, and the student attempted to recall each structure
name and then peeked behind the sticky note to grade their answers. The
dates on the right side of the figure indicate each session in which the student
used successive relearning, and the slash marks by each date reflect how
many times the student needed to restudy until each answer was correctly
recalled (note how the number of attempts dropped dramatically over just a
few sessions). The sticky note approach works well because you can’t
accidentally peek at the correct answer, and you can write down how well
you did (e.g., whether you recalled it correctly) or any other comments to
yourself (e.g., highlighting a particularly tough item to learn) on the note
itself.
FIG URE 5. 4. Using Sticky Notes to Turn Your Textbook
Into Functional Flash Cards
Note. From Psychology: Themes and Variations (9th ed, p. 99), by W. Weiten, 2012,
Wadsworth Cengage Learning. Copyright 2012 by Wadsworth Cengage Learning.
After you have tested yourself, the next step of successive relearning
involves scoring your answer. For instance, while studying complex
definitions for an Introductory Chemistry course, you may test yourself on
redox and recall “transfer of electrons.” Of course, you get credit only if your
answer captures the meaning of the correct definition. The difficulty with
most complex materials is that the first couple of times you try to recall the
information you won’t get it all correct, and you’ll need to check your answer
carefully. Even worse, research has shown that students often give
themselves credit for incorrect answers, even when the students are highly
motivated to score the answers correctly (for a review, see Dunlosky &
Lipko, 2007).
The bottom line is that scoring complex answers can be difficult, but
you can make it easier. In particular, if you simply recall an answer aloud
without writing it down, then you’ll need to keep your answer in mind while
checking it against the correct answer. For long responses this will be very
difficult because you will begin forgetting what you have recalled before you
can check your response. To sidestep this problem, we recommend that you
write (or type) your response so that you can carefully check it against the
correct answer. For instance, the response above for redox has some of the
correct ideas (i.e., transfer of electrons), but it is not entirely complete (e.g.,
a chemical reaction in which one reacting compound gets oxidized, or loses
electrons, and the other compound gets reduced, or gains electrons).
After you score an item, you need to decide what to do next. If your
answer was incorrect, then you should restudy the correct answer and come
back to that item later, to practice retrieving the correct answer. If your
response was correct, then you can decide to try it again later or to put it
aside to practice it in a different session. Then, you repeat the process until
you have correctly recalled all the answers to all the items you decided to
practice during that study session. When you are done, you will need to come
back to those items again during another session several days later. We
recommend you schedule that next practice session right after you finish the
first round of practice. If you give each set of items a name (e.g., “Basic
Chemistry Concepts”), you can label reminders in a calendar showing when
you should return to each set.
There are many variations on the theme of making flash cards as described
thus far. We know various apps to help you create flash cards and different
widgets, gadgets, and gizmos that you could probably use for retrieval
practice. That said, there is something to the act of creating flash cards on
index cards. There is one more neat way to modify your flash card use. In the
flash-cards-plus method you go beyond having the term on one side and the
textbook definition on the other. To help you process the material better,
paraphrase the definition in your own words under the textbook one, and then
jot down an application of the concept to your own life. Adding these two
simple components (your paraphrase and an application) can help increase
the depth of your processing of the material and has been shown in classroom
research to be effective (Senzaki et al., 2017).
HUDDLE UP
At the beginning of this chapter, we emphasized that retrieval practice works
well for two reasons, namely, when you correctly retrieve a question, doing
so actually enhances your memory of the retrieved content. In addition, you
can use the retrieval attempt to monitor your progress, because when you
don’t answer a question correctly you know that you need to restudy.
One study method, successive relearning, is a good way to monitor your
progress and can help indicate whether you’ve done enough studying to retain
what you need to know for a high-stakes exam. In particular, if you schedule
several days between each successive-relearning study session, then how
well you perform will give you a pretty good sense about what you will
remember. For instance, let’s say that you use the technique to study
definitions for your Biological Sciences course, and you scheduled four
sessions, with 3 days in between each session. Given that you eventually
recalled each definition correctly during the first session, if you correctly
recalled a definition during the first retrieval attempt in the second session, it
means that you retained that information for 3 days. If you correctly recalled
the same definition in the third session, then you probably have it down well
enough to retain for at least another 3 days, if not much longer. You will
retrieve the correct answers more quickly each time you return to them,
which will provide even more confidence that you’ve got it down.
Of course, if you continue to fail to correctly retrieve some information
at the beginning of each session, then you have a really great sign that you
will not remember the information later; that is, you may find that you keep
struggling to remember some definitions. This may indicate that you need to
understand the material better. So, you may keep forgetting the definition of
redox because you never understood the meaning of the definition. For pesky
material that you just can’t seem to memorize, you may need to supplement
your studying with other techniques aimed at improving your ability to
understand the content. One way to proceed would be to discuss the material
with a fellow student or your instructor. Other possibilities involve changing
the technique you use to study the to-be-learned material. We discuss some of
those techniques—such as self-explanation, imagery, and so on—in other
chapters.