Planning: Speech: Levelt's Model of L1 Production
Planning: Speech: Levelt's Model of L1 Production
Planning: Speech: Levelt's Model of L1 Production
1-Message conceptualization
The first component in Levelt’s (1989, 1993) production system is the
conceptualizer. This component is responsible for generating the communicative
intention3 and for encoding it into some kind of coherent conceptual plan. In Addition,
the conceptualizer monitors what is about to be said as well as what has been said and
how. In order to generate a message, declarative knowledge is accessed. Declarative
knowledge includes encyclopedic knowledge (about the person’s general experience of
the world), knowledge about the situation (e.g. the interlocutor/s and the communicative
context, among others), as well as information about the discourse record, that is, what
has already been said.
➢ macro planning and micro planning.
Macro Planning breaks the communicative goal into a series of subgoals and retrieves
the information necessary to realize these goals.In other words, it involves generating
speech act intentions, like to narrate an event or express an opinion. In Levelt’s terms
(1993, p. 3): “The speaker’s planning of a speech act, his selection of information to be
expressed, and his linearization of that information are called ‘macro planning’”.
Example;
● Getting the various ideas organized in a way that is going to best suit the
communication is part of macro planning,
● Deciding how to achieve an intended communicative goal using relevant speech
acts
➢spreading activation
Levelt presents Dell’s (1986) spreading activation theory as the most promising one to
account for how lexical access takes place during real time performance.a chunk in the
preverbal plan activates a number of lemmas in the lexicon. The lemmas which receive
the highest activation because their semantic specifications match the concepts in the
preverbal plan will be selected.To illustrate .Competition between words is often
represented in terms of this activation. Prompted by a particular string of letters or
sounds, people access a number of possible word matches. They are activated to
different degrees – with the more likely ones (those that are most frequent and those
that form the closest match to what is in the input) receiving more activation than the
others. Activation level can change as the language user reads or hears more of the
word – so some candidate words may have their activation boosted by late-arriving
information while others may have their activation depressed.
Example 1;, if a speaker wants to produce the sentence ‘The man gave the
woman the money”, out of 30,000 words average speakers have active in their
lexicon the four content words ‘man’, ‘give’, ‘woman’ and ‘money’ will receive
the highest activation because they best match the pre-verbal plan. This does
not mean that other items do not get activated. Together with ‘man’, other
entries which share similar conceptual specifications get activated, but it is
‘man’ that gets the highest activation .
man —--------woman—---child—---------person
Example 2; After encountering a word such as doctor, they automatically
activate closely linked words such as nurse or patient, recognising them more
readily if and when they occur.Researchers investigate lexical connections of
this kind by means of a method known as priming which measures how much
faster words are recognised when preceded by a word that appears to be
associated with them.As soon as a lemma is retrieved, its syntactic properties
become available .Each lemma requires its own specific syntactic environment
or "frame". Syntactic planning is like solving a set of simultaneous equations.
- Grammatical encoding;
3-Articulation
This is the third and very important stage of speech production, that is, the physical
stage of sound speech, where words are translated into the sounds and syllables of
actual speech. Additionally, in this stage our thoughts and linguistic plan are sent from
the brain to the speech systems in order to execute the required movements and
produce the desired sounds. Therefore, this is however the starting point for the speech
comprehension.
To sum UP
❖ planning An utterances
Conceptualisation is pre-linguistic. That is, it does not involve forms of
language, but is all done ‘in the head’ in abstract terms. The speaker
needs to make some very general decisions about what to say, taking
into account facts about the situation that they are in, and relying on
their general knowledge as well as on their understanding of how
communica-tion (including conversation) works. The result of the
process of conceptualisation is a pre-verbal message, i.e. it is still not
language. It consists of a set of ideas ,which are ideas form part of the
mental model ) of what the speaker wants to say.Although speech appears
to be spontaneous, it requires a planning process in which the components
(clauses, words, phonemes) are assembled. Critical to the process are pauses
in the flow of speech, which enable a speaker to construct a new chunk of
language. When experimenters force speakers to suppress pausing, it results
in confused and sometimes incoherent discourse.
Evidence of planning
Evidence for a unit of planning has been sought in pausing, in speech errors,
in intonation patterns and in the gestures which accompany speech. Pauses
tend to come at or near clause boundaries, suggesting that the clause is a
major unit of planning. This is supported by evidence from Slips of the
Tongue, in which most word misplacements take place within a single clause.
To illustrate more, A key source of information about the processes of
conceptualisation and planning in producing spoken output is the pattern of
pausing that speakers produce. The argument is that planning involves mental
activity that competes for our attention resources with the actual process of
speaking. The more planning we need to do, the less easy it is for us to
continue speaking and the more likely it is that we will hesitate.Studies of the
pausing and hesitation patterns of speakers provide insights into the way
speech is planned and executed. The planning of speech takes place under
enormous pressures of time. A degree of thinking ahead occurs while the
speaker is actually articulating, but brief pauses seconds are normally
essential for planning the form of the next utterance. If the opportunity to
pause is suppressed, speech production is adversely affected. Juncture
pauses of this type occur mainly at syntactic boundaries.The clause
appears to form an important unit of planning, while hesitation pauses (filled
and unfilled) can occur anywhere within an utterance and may result from a
failure to retrieve a word or a failure of planning. Slips of the Tongue research
also suggests that a syntactic frame is prepared in advance of lexical items
being slotted into it.
Examples
The type of speech we utter in every speech situation bring many differences
in the amount and types of planning involved.Let us take the differences
between read speech and unprepared speech ,which reflect the equally
obvious differences in the planning involved in the two tasks.
-When the task is reading aloud, most of the planning has already been done
in preparing the text in the first place. As a consequence, when fluent readers
speak aloud from a prepared manuscript, they do not need to pause for
plan-ning purposes but instead pause almost exclusively at points marked
punctuation.Planning in spontaneous speech involves deciding what to say
and which words and sentences to use. Spontaneous speech also contains
more self-interruptions and false starts, and people continuously monitor what
they are saying to convey their message effectively.
❖ Type of planning
Articulatory pauses: has to do with pauses within words due to a phoneme or
syllable.Example: the phoneme /p/ in the word space.
-Delimitative pauses can occur at places where a written text might have
punctuation,breaking utterances into constituent parts, possibly to help the
listener.Intonation and other aspects of prosody can also contribute to this
fonction.
-Physiological pauses are those that help speakers to regulate
their breathing while speaking. In practice, speakers mostly breathe at
points where they might have to pause for some other reason.
NOTES!!!!We can include fillers to pause or show that one is not done with what they
have to say.Fillers are words or sounds that are used to fill pauses in speech, such as
"uh", "um", "like", "you know", "so", and "well". These fillers are often used to signal
to the listener that the speaker is not finished speaking and needs more time to
formulate their thoughts or find the right words to express themselves. Fillers can also
indicate hesitation, uncertainty, or a lack of confidence in what the speaker is saying.
While fillers are a common aspect of everyday speech, excessive use of fillers can make a
speaker appear unprepared, nervous, or lacking in credibility.
Side note: •Planning pauses are longer than physiological pauses but shorter than
delimitative pauses. •Delimitative pauses are longer than physiological pauses
1-The lexicon
The nature of lexical knowledge has been a major area of psycholinguistic enquiry. A
language user is envisaged as possessing a vocabulary store in the mind (a mental
lexicon).the lexicon is The system of vocabulary which is stored in the mind in the form
of a lexical entry for each item. linguists use the term LEXICON to refer to the
collection of all the words (or meaningful elements) in the language.” Each individual
word is referred to as a LEXICAL ITEM. For each lexical item, the lexicon must specify
how it is pronounced, what it means, and how it patterns in the grammar.All of the
phonological, semantic, and grammatical information which is specific to a particular
word is included in its LEXICAL ENTRY.
➢ LEXICAL ENTRY
The information that is stored in the mind concerning a particular lexical item. Levelt
(1989) represents a lexical entry ,consisting of two parts, one related to form and one
(the lemma) related to meaning and use.
➔ ‘Form’ includes:
Mental representations of the item which enable it to be identified when it is encountered.Form
includes phonological and orthographic representations that allow for variation. the fact
that a speaker may have any one of a number of accents or that a written text may appear in
any one o a number of different typefaces. It also includes information on the morphology of the
item – both inflectional (providing a plural for a noun or a past tense form for a verb) and
derivational (indicating the component parts of a word such as UN-HAPPI-NESS).
❖Lexicalization process
The sketch of the lexicalisation process broke lexicalisation down into two stages –
finding words and building words.
-Lemma ACCESS : the first stage of lexical access in Speaking. In the first stage we
select the word that corresponds to the chosen concept. In the view of Levelt et al.
(1999), the speaker first selects a lemma, or syntactic word unit. Lemmas specify the
syntactic class of the word and often additional syntactic information, such as whether a
verb is intransitive (e.g., sleep) or transitive (e.g., eat) and, if transitive, what arguments
it takes. Lemma selection is a competitive process. Several lemmas may be activated at
once because several concepts are more or less suitable to express the message, and
because lemmas that correspond to semantically similar concepts activate each other via
links to shared superordinate concepts or conceptual features. A lemma is selected as
soon as its activation level exceeds the summed activation of all competitors.
❖Building words
In order to get deeply immersed in the production process ,it is crucial to state the four
levels of processing ,which include the message level, the functional level, the positional
level, and the phonological level. The message captures features of the speaker's
intended meaning and provides the raw material for the processes of grammatical
encoding. These processes are grouped into two sets, functional and positional. The
primary subcomponents of functional processing are
-lexical selection which involves the identification of lexical concepts that are
suitable for conveying the speaker's meaning)
=The first step, lexical selection, involves identifying the lexical concept and LEMMAS
suitable for conveying the message. Lemmas carry the grammatical information
associated with individual lexical concepts, such as their form class (noun, verb, etc.)
Error:A common type of speech error that appears to reflect a problem of lexical
selection is a SEMANTIC SUBSTITUTION,
Target: Where is my tennis racquet?
Error: Where is the tennis bat?
These substitutions preserve general features of the meaning of the intended word
(Hotopf, 1980) and are nearly always members of the same grammatical form class
(noun, verb, adjective, adverb, or preposition).t.
-The second step is function assignment. This involves assigning syntactic relations or
grammatical functions (e.g., subject-nominative, object-dative). for example, during the
formulation '' She was handing him some broccoli'' the feminine pronoun lemma should
be linked to the nominative (subject) function, the masculin or the argument ''him''
should be assigned Accusative function, and hand to the main verb function.
The next two steps constitute positional processing, so called because iT fixes the order
of the elements in an utterance.We consider constituent assembly first. This is the
creation of a control hierarchy for phrasal constituents that manages the order of word
production and captures dependencies among syntactic functions. In other words, the
basic features of such hierarchies are largely predictable from the types Of syntactic
functions that have to be represented and from the syntactic features of the selected
lemmas;The last of the grammatical encoding processes, inflection, involves the
generation of fine-grained details at the lowest levels of this structure. In English, many
of these details involve elements that carry information about number, tense, and aspect
but are bound to other words.
Error 2 :Another type of error that may arise during inflection is called a
SHIFT (Garrett, 1975) and consists of the mislocation of an affix. Such an error could
lead to the utterance of'' She was hand himming some broccoli by our hypothetical
speaker''. The elements involved in such errors are much more likely to be involved in
errors than the final syllables of word stems, implying that strandings and shifts are not
simplemislocations of syllables but mis locations of pieces of grammatical structure.
Evidence :Evidence for the distinction between functional and positional processes
comes from the finding that some speech errors (e.g., exchanges of words from different
phrases, as in “put the tables on the plate”) can best be explained as errors of functional
encoding. Other errors with different properties (e.g., shifts of morphemes within
phrases, as in “the come homing of the queen”) can best be explained as errors of
positional encoding. The distinction is further supported by the results of structural
priming studies. In such studies, people first hear or say a sentence such as “The woman
shows the man the dress.” They later see a picture that can be described using the same
kind of structure (e.g., “The boy gives the teacher the flowers”) or a different one (“The
boy gives the flowers to the teacher”). Speakers tend to repeat the structure used on
previous trials, even when the words featured in prime and target sentences are
different and even when the events are unrelated. The results of many such studies
strongly suggest that the priming effect arises during the positional encoding processes
(Bock, 1986; Bock & Loebell, 1990; Chang, Dell, Bock, & Griffin, 2000)
Straddling Errors
Morphological Stranding errors show that word stems and word ending are treated
separately during at least some stages of the process of speaking.Accordingly, it is
suggested that the affixed form is constructed during speech production. If this were
not the case, if in fact the complete affixed form is retrieved from the mental lexicon, the
it is normal to predict that the affix remains with the relevant stem.For instance ,
In this case ,the Morphemes remain in place but are attached to the wrong words,which
lead to what we call Morpheme Stranding. The existence of such a speech error can be
taken as evidence that roots (trunk, pack) exist at a separate representational level from
the level which stores both the form and semantics of affixes (-s). Thus, the distribution
of speech errors involving inflectional or derivational morphology (e.g., intended word
grouping → actual word groupment; Melinger, 2003) has become an important evidence
for developing representational models of speech production
Infections
Grammatical marking is part of the syntactic frame with affixes attached during speech
production,including tense and plural markers,which is one of the most frequent
elements involved in stranding errors in English.However ,There are some exception in
the english language ,which is irregularities .those exception refers to deviations from
the logical rules in a few grammatical constructions .In this regard,Not all English
plural and past tense forms involve the simple affixation of endings onto stems. English
has plenty of irregular plurals, like person => people ,mouse => mice and irregular past
tense forms like Buy=> bought and go=>went .
Such forms can- not be predicted by a rule such as the plural rule and so it would
reasonable to expect these words to be stored as complete forms in the lexicon and
accessed as such rather than being constructed as and when
needed. Nevertheless, irregular past tense forms are also involved in
English speech errors. For instance ,
‘’know one if I heard it → I’d hear one if I knew it’’The exchange is clearly of the
underlying morphemes for the stems know and hear .know is inserted with the abstract
past feature associated with the frame at the functional stagE and the subsequent
process of specifying word forms results in the insertion of the correct irregular form,
rather than a regularized knowed .
The distinction between derivational and inflectional affixes is based in part upon
whether they change the grammatical category of the word to which they apply. By this
criterion, the plural affix for nouns and number, tense, and aspect affixes for verbs are
inflectional, whereas derivational affixes change verbs into nouns (e.g., -tion, as in
creation), nouns into verbs (e.g., -ate, as in pulsate), nouns into adjectives (e.g., -ly, as in
princely), and so on. However, not all derivational affixes change form class (e.g., un-,
mis°Errors involving derivational prefixes have also been taken to indicate
that morphological structure is represented in the production lexicon.
Examples (4.12)
admitting entails inserting . . . entails asserting (FSED)
1)the arguments against the idea that all inflected forms are looked up in
the mental lexicon.( the full-listing hypothesis)
°Morpheme-shift errors indicate that the inflection may not be associated with the verb
in the mental lexicon. (e.g Point outed). Affixation is applied to the multi-word unit and
it accommodates to its context of insertion. The allomorph is appropriate to the base to
which it is attached and not to the base it should have attached.
Negative préfixesThe negative prefix is represented as NEG in the mental lexicon at the
functional level. It may be associated with the wrong position at the positional level as in
the example . Instead of surfacing on precise, it is attached to regard.If there was
anything that was unclear vs if there was nothing that was clear
Morphology and the lexiconone can say that the regular morphology and the
irregular lexicon are separate entities; one might imagine the two having very little to
do with one another, since the morphology deals only with potential words and the
lexicon only with existing words. In fact, the two systems do have a great deal to do with
one another, for two simplereasons. The first is that they serve the same role in a
language: both provide words. The second reason is that morphology and the lexicon
are interdependent. Most centrally, the morphology, which forms words from words,
finds the words that it operates on (its bases) in the lexicon.
For example, that nominal -th (as in length) can only attach to a small number of
specific words, but cannot attach to any other words beyond that set. This suffix can
therefore be considered unproductive. Even among affixes that can in principle be used
to coin new words, there seem to be some that are more productive than others. For
example, the suffix -ness (as cuteness,happiness ) gives rise to many more new words.is
suggests that –ness suffixation is a productive derivational process.
Example-2 -s (books) plural and –en (children) plural in English. In this example –s is
productive and –en is unproductive.
Layers of representation:
Phonological encoding has two layers of representation :
• Segmental layer : Phonological segments
• Metrical layer : Syllable structure and stress patterns
• Segments and syllable frames are independently
retrieved and then the segments are associated to
positions in the syllable frames.
• Evidence : The syllable position constraint ,Misplaced segments almost always move
from their target position to corresponding positions in other syllables. (e.g. Onsets
remain onsets.)
Chapter 6: The Use of Gesture
Approaches of gestures
Research into the use of gestures in speaking took two different approaches:
Phases of gesturing
Neurophysiological studies of listening participants who were in front of
speakers who use gestures while speaking resulted in a close link between
speech and gesture for the listener. The hard evidence is that gestures help
speakers to maintain an image of the concepts that they are trying to express
by making the connection between the concepts (lemmas) and the linguistic
expression (lexemes). Gestures help in accessing a word in the mental
dictionary by a mechanism of ‘visual priming’ just as when seeing a picture
helps the speaker Gesture phases:
Functions of Gestures:
Content-related gestures convey the semantic content of the speech
Discourse management gestures are used for managing the conversation that
the speaker is involved in.
1 Symbols: They often replace aGestures that stand for something, sometimes
referred to as emblems. whole utterance and can also be used along with
information conveyed in a sentence. They are used for interpersonal control
(hello, be quiet) or to express personal states ( I agree, I do not know) and for
the evaluation of others ( he is crazy)
1-2 Indices or Indicative gestures are mainly used to direct the listener’s
attention to particular objects, these gestures involve a part of the body , an
instrument ( ex: the index) or a locative action ( ex: to point at something)
depending on cultural differences. Indices accompany speech and coincide
with certain parts of it as in the following example: Example: Can you put the
carpet down here ? (pointing with our index to the place we wish to put the
carpet) The Indice can be of extreme necessity in some utterances and without
the indice comprehension is not possible as in the following example where
the painter is saying that he painted two walls in a room: “I have painted these
two just now” As we can see, there is no possibility for the listener to know
which walls if the speaker does not use the pointing gesture.
Metaphorics, or metaphorical gestures, are true to their name in that they are hand symbols
that represent abstract ideas that are impossible to represent directly.
Example: A fist motion upwards and twisted to the left may stand for the idea of freedom.
Symbols/Emblems:
Emblems, a term first used by researcher, David Efron, describe gestures that
have very precise meanings known within an ethnic, cultural, or sub-cultural
group.Hence, Emblems are culturally specific, have standard forms and
significances, and vary from place to place. Emblems are used to take the
place of speech or to accompany specific words. Emblems are culturally
specific, have standard forms and significance, and vary from place to place.
For example, circling the index finger around at the side of your head says “He
or she is crazy,” or rolling your hands over and over in front of you says “Move
on.”In America someone might ask, “are you OK?” In response, it would be
common for someone to respond by placing their thumb and forefinger
together while raising their other three fingers to form the “OK” sign. However,
people from Germany, Brazil, or Russia might interpret this gesture differently
since it is offensive in their cultures.
There are also Auditory symbols/emblems,which are sounds that carry similar
functions to gestures.For example: Clap =I approve ,Hiss=I disapprove
Symbols as Junctions:
Junctions : gesture that use usually requires more than one person to be
actively Participating and are typically joint physical actions, such as shaking
hands, hugging, kissing (Clark, 1996 )
Crosslinguistic differences
Indeed, some cultures prescribe deixis with the lips (Enfield 2001). Deixis
entails locating entities and actions in space vis-à-vis a reference point, which
Bühler called the origo (Bühler 1982, Haviland 2000).Much of the pointing we
see in adult conversation and storytelling is not pointing at physically present
objects or locations but is abstract pointing, which Bühler referred to as deixis
at phantasma. The emergence of abstract pointing is a milestone in children’s
development.
❖ Discourse management gestures :
Discourse management gestures are used for managing the conversation that
the speaker is involved in.Such gestures perform a number of discourse
management functions.
The results Interestingly, the analysis of their gestures showed that the
Japanese participants were likely to use straight-line gestures to show the
move- ment of the cat from one building to the other, while the
English-speaking participants almost exclusively used arc-like gestures. While
some of the Japanese speakers did use a separate arc-like gesture in addition
to the straight-line gesture, only the latter coincided with the words indicating
the movement.
Verbs expressing motion express the manner of motion (run, walk, climb) or
the path/direction of motion (exit, enter, pass). Manner of motion verbs encode
their path with an adverbial particle (run to the store, run out of the store, run
into the store.Languages tend to fall in one of two categories:
Jacobs & Garnham ( 2007 ) tested the role of conversational hand gestures in
a narrative task to see whether they are meant to serve speech production
(The Production Hypothesis) or the speech comprehension (The
Communication Hypothesis) . here are two main hypotheses of what role
gesture plays in speech,
Results:Condition one (the same strip narrated to one listener) was found to
produce a significantly lower gesture rate than condition two (the same strip
narrated to different listeners) Condition three (different strips related to
informed listeners) was found to produce a significantly lower gesture rate
than condition four (different strips related to different listeners). There was no
significant difference between conditions one and three or between conditions
two and fourWhen the listeners already knew what was being described
(either by having heard it described before or by dint of being able to see the
cartoons themselves), then the speaker used
fewer gestures.These results are as predicted by the communication
hypothesis but not the speech production hypothesis.This suggests that
gestures primarily serve a communicative function