Goetzmann Et Al 2014
Goetzmann Et Al 2014
Goetzmann Et Al 2014
Dasol Kim
Alok Kumar
University of Miami
Qin Wang
University of Michigan at Dearborn
This study shows that weather-based indicators of mood impact perceptions of mispricing
and trading decisions of institutional investors. Using survey and disaggregated trade data,
we show that relatively cloudier days increase perceived overpricing in individual stocks
and the Dow Jones Industrial Index and increase selling propensities of institutions. We
introduce stock-level measures of investor mood; investor optimism positively impacts
stock returns among stocks with higher arbitrage costs, and stocks experiencing similar
investor mood exhibit return comovement. These findings complement existing studies on
how weather impacts stock index returns and identify another channel through which it can
manifest. (JEL D84, G11, G12, G14, G23)
A number of recent studies show that weather patterns in major financial centers
influence stock index returns and suggest that investor mood influences asset
prices. For example, using data from international stock exchanges, Hirshleifer
and Shumway (2003) show that stock market returns are higher on days when
the weather is sunny, which is presumably when market participants are in
a good mood.1 The findings from this finance literature are consistent with
evidence in the psychology literature, in that individuals misattribute mood
We thank two anonymous referees, Jawad Addoum, David Hirshleifer (the editor), George Korniotis, and Lisa
Kramer for helpful discussions and valuable comments. We thank seminar participants at the University of
Miami, Case Western Reserve University, University of Arkansas, and Western Finance Association. We are
responsible for all remaining errors and omissions. Supplementary data can be found on The Review of Financial
Studies web site. Send correspondence to Dasol Kim, Department of Banking and Finance, Weatherhead School of
Management, Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland, OH 44106-7235, USA. E-mail: [email protected].
1 Other studies that examine the impact of cloud cover on stock market index returns include Saunders (1993) and
Goetzmann and Zhu (2005). See Section 1 for additional details.
© The Author 2014. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of The Society for Financial Studies.
All rights reserved. For Permissions, please e-mail: [email protected].
doi:10.1093/rfs/hhu063 Advance Access publication September 23, 2014
2 These studies show that individuals misattribute their affective states for information, resulting in mood-congruent
judgments. Supportive evidence using cloud cover as a mood priming device has been found in a variety of
contexts, including tipping (Cunningham 1979), life satisfaction (Schwarz and Clore 1983), and responsiveness
to persuasion (Clore, Schwarz, and Conway 1994).
3 For example, Coval and Shumway (2005) document trading patterns that are consistent with loss aversion among
traders in the Chicago Board of Trade. Frazzini (2006) finds evidence of the disposition effect among U.S. mutual
fund managers, whereas Barber et al. (2007), using international data, show consistent evidence in a broader
set of investors. More closely related to our study, Goetzmann and Zhu (2005) present evidence suggesting that
New York City weather patterns affect New York Stock Exchange market makers, while showing little effect on
retail investors.
74
4 Edmans, Garcia, and Norli (2007) and Agarwal, Duchin, and Sosyura (2012) examine sporting and singing
competition outcomes. Dougal et al. (2012) and Garcia (2013) examine the impact of sentiment reflected in
financial news articles on stock market activity.
5 Schwartz (1990), Clore and Parrott (1991), Wilson and Schooler (1991), and Clore, Schwarz, and Conway (1994)
discuss the general role of mood and emotion in decision making. Loewenstein (2000) and Loewenstein et al.
(2001) discuss the role of emotion in judgment.
6 Mehra and Sah (2002) demonstrate that even small perturbations in preference parameters related to mood can
have a measurable impact on stock returns and volatility. Bodoh-Creed (2013) shows that informed market
participants can be more susceptible to mood-related biases. Motivated by the empirical regularities linking
seasonal forms of depression to equity and Treasury returns, Kamstra et al. (2014) construct a model that
incorporates seasonal dependence in both risk aversion and the intertemporal elasticity of substitution, showing
that it can match features of the data.
75
7 For example, see Camerer et al. (1997) for violations of the law of supply on wage data for New York taxi drivers.
8 Linnainmaa and Rosu (2009) show similar evidence using weather conditions around the location of the Helsinki
Stock Exchange. In contrast with Goetzmann and Zhu (2005), their evidence cannot be explained by the role of
market makers, suggesting that their results are driven by investors.
76
1. Theoretical Motivation
A number of studies document a robust relationship between stock index
returns and local weather patterns, referred to here as the “weather effect.”
Saunders (1993) shows that sky cloud cover over New York City has a strong,
negative association with New York Stock Exchange (NYSE) index returns.
Because his findings appear to relate to the local weather conditions around
the stock exchange, the results are plausibly attributable to factors related to
investor mood rather than to direct effects on firm fundamentals. Hirshleifer and
Shumway (2003) provide additional evidence using an international sample
9 The effect of sunshine as an antidepressant has been well documented, though its effect on clinically depressed
individuals is thought to influence individual decision making in different ways (Wyer, Clore, and Isbell 1999).
Light therapy, which simulates sunshine, has been shown to have antidepressant benefits in individuals suffering
from seasonal (Rosenthal et al. 1984) and nonseasonal (Kripke 1998) forms of clinical depression. However,
these effects are distinct from mood attribution. In their seminal study, Schwarz and Clore (1983) find that
individuals generally respond more positively when asked about life satisfaction on sunnier days. Their key
finding is that the effect weakens when attention is drawn to the weather or their current mood before the start of
the interview. They find similar evidence in a different experiment within the same study that uses a nonweather
priming device. On the other hand, clinical depression is arguably a condition that is generally recognized by
the patient.
77
10 Fishbein (1963) argues that the composite effect of impressions formed on various components of the object
over time influences judgments. Adaval (2001) provides supportive, experimental evidence, demonstrating that
affective reactions to distinguishable aspects of a product together influence purchasing decisions, even when
those reactions may be unrelated to the object of assessment. Additionally, Kida, Smith, and Maletta (1998)
provide similar experimental evidence using a subject pool of experienced managers. The subjects are given
information at an earlier period and are asked to describe that information at a later point in time. They show that
subjects are better able to recall affective reactions at the initial exposure than the actual information presented.
11 Johnson and Tversky (1983) show that individuals attribute greater likelihood of particular events congruently
to mood, even when the event is unrelated to the cause of their mood. Clore, Schwarz, and Conway (1994) show
that the influence of affective states on judgment is pronounced in assessments of objects that are abstract or
when faced with limited information.
12 For examples, see Goel et al. (2005) for nonseasonal depression and Eastman et al. (1998) for seasonal depression.
78
13 A similar mechanism can be found related to daylight savings due to sleep deprivation. Kamstra, Kramer, and
Levi (2000) document anomalous stock index returns patterns around daylight saving weekends in the United
States, the United Kingdom, and Germany.
14 Adetailed overview of the survey is described by Shiller, Kon-Ya and Tsutsui (1996) and Shiller (2000).Additional
information can be found at the International Center for Finance web site (icf.som.yale.edu) under “Stock Market
Confidence Indices.”
15 Institutional investor respondents are randomly sampled from a directory of institutional investors found in the
investment managers section of the “Money Market Directory of Pension Funds and Their Investment Managers.”
The survey is also conducted in the Japanese and Chinese markets.
79
16 See Puckett and Yan (2011) for a detailed description of the ANcerno data.
80
Figure 1
Geographical dispersion of the survey and trade datasets
This figure presents the geographical distribution of the survey in the United States (panel A), institutional
investor trade (panel B), and institutional investor holding (panel C) data on the county level. Black areas denote
the existence of at least one investor in the corresponding dataset at the county level.
81
Figure 1
Continued
82
sunlight is a decreasing, convex function in sky cloud cover, the sky cloud cover
measure used in the analysis (SKC) is defined as the natural logarithm of one
plus the rolling average of the ZIP code-level sky cloud cover.19 Additionally,
we require that each institutional investor has at least one matched weather
station included in the analysis. We note that measurement error in observed
cloud cover is likely to bias our tests from finding any effect.
To adjust for seasonality in the SKC measure, we consider two approaches.
First, we calculate seasonal cloud cover as the average daily cloud cover
for the same month over the entire sample period. Seasonal SKC is the
19 The logarithmic transformation is motivated by the non-linearity in the relationship between illuminance and
sky cloud cover. However, similar results are obtained without the transformation.
20 The procedure to seasonally adjust the cloud cover measure is similar to that of Goetzmann and Zhu (2005) and
Hirshleifer and Shumway (2003). We find similar results when using the average cloud cover in the same month
in the previous year for the seasonal adjustment.
21 Specifically, the question asks, “What do you think would be a sensible level for the Dow Jones Industrial Average
based on your assessment of U.S. corporate strength (fundamental)?”
83
past seven days. Lower values of %DJIAMisPrc are associated with relatively
greater overpricing.22
In the sample of investors examined, we conjecture that pessimism should be
positively related to perceived overpricing, as most of the survey respondents
are more likely to be limited in their ability to short stocks. However, there
are several key limitations affecting inferences with the TooHigh and TooLow
variables. Tests on those measures must assume that the respondents apply
similar criteria to assess mispricing. However, this assumption is likely to be
too strong. To address this issue, we construct similar binary variables from the
22 The %DJIAMisPrc variable is expected to be negatively related to TooHigh, and indeed a paired t -test on the
difference between the group means of %DJIAMisPrc across values of TooHigh is statistically significant (t -value
= 28.81).
84
23 Using weighted averages based on investor position size does not qualitatively alter the results.
24 Kamstra, Kramer, and Levi (2003) also include a fall dummy variable in their tests to distinguish the impact
of SAD during the fall and winter. Because our tests are focused on isolating nonseasonal effects, we instead
include time fixed effects in some of the specifications.
85
Table 1
Summary statistics on survey, trade, and holdings data
25th 75th
Variable Mean SD percentile Median percentile
Panel A: DSKC by estimation window (survey data)
1 day −0.204 0.695 −0.755 −0.086 0.376
3 days −0.085 0.444 −0.362 −0.032 0.234
1 week −0.067 0.346 −0.267 −0.044 0.173
2 weeks −0.049 0.263 −0.190 −0.041 0.124
4 weeks −0.045 0.223 −0.183 −0.039 0.097
Panel B: Survey data sample
This table reports summary statistics for the average sky-cloud coverage variables that are matched to the survey
dataset (panel A), other characteristics linked to or from the survey dataset (panel B), variables from and linked to
the ZIP code-level trade dataset (panel C), and variables from and linked to the ZIP code-level holding and stock
returns dataset (panel D). Please refer to Section 3 for descriptions of each dataset. SKC is the natural logarithm
of one plus the daily average sky-cloud coverage over 1 day to 4 weeks before the response or trade date. DSKC
is the difference between SKC and the seasonal value forSKC, calculated as the average cloud cover for the
same month over the entire sample. Portfolio size is the sum of the capital across asset classes reported by the
investor respondent. Population is the county-level population for the response year. Income is the county-level
median income for the response year. Earnings Growth is the percentage long-term growth rate estimate of the
respondent. Adjusted nighttime hours per day (SAD) is calculated using the procedure outlined by Kamstra,
Kramer, and Levi (2003), based upon the investors coordinate locations. %DJIA MisPrc is the natural logarithm
of the ratio of the respondent estimated, intrinsic level of the DJIA to the average, actual DJIA level over the
previous week of the response date. TooHigh(TooLow) takes the value one if the respondent answers that actual
prices in the stock market are too high (too low) relative to fundamentals. DJIA TooHigh (DJIA TooLow) takes
the value one if the %DJIA MisPrc for a respondent is above the 75th percentile (below the 25th percentile) of
the overall sample. Investor BSI is the daily total, net buy minus sell dollar volume, scaled by the total dollar
trading volume within the same ZIP code. StockInvestor BSI is the daily total, buy minus sell dollar volume for
the same ZIP code and stock, scaled by the total dollar trading volume within the same stock. StockDSKC is the
difference between the average SKC of institutional investors holding the same stock, less its seasonal value.
%IO is the proportion of shares in the stock held by institutional investors. PosRetis an indicator variable that
takes the value one if the stock experiences positive returns for that date and zero otherwise.
the two-week window (0.263) and is relatively stable when using a four-week
window (0.223).
Panel B describes the primary variables used in the survey-based tests.
The binary response variables are displayed in the first four rows. In our
sample, slightly more investors indicate that stock prices are generally too high
86
25 The results are similar using nonparametric tests, such as rank-order correlations.
87
Table 2
Pearson correlation matrix of survey and weather variables
Panel A: Survey responses with conditioning variables
%DJIA DJIA DJIA
TooHigh TooLow MisPrc TooHigh TooLow
TooLow −20.55%
(0.000)
%DJIAMisPrc −58.55% 27.21%
(0.000) (0.000)
DJIATooHigh 67.80% −19.93% −72.42%
(0.000) (0.000) (0.000)
DJIATooLow −25.22% 41.96% 53.71% −33.84%
The table presents the Pearson correlation coefficients between the mispricing measures and the conditioning
variables in the regression models of Table 3 (panel A) and between the mispricing measures and the
deseasonalized cloud cover (DSKC) measures across the estimation windows (panel B). The mispricing measures
are TooHigh, TooLow, %DJIA MisPrc, DJIA TooHigh, and DJIA TooLow. The estimation windows for the DSKC
measures are 1 day, 3 days, 1 week, 2 weeks, and 1 month. p−values on the correlation coefficients are reported
in parentheses.
88
89
90
26 The standard error adjustments are performed using Jonah Gelbach and Douglas Miller’s STATA code
(cgmreg.ado), which is based on the procedures described by Cameron, Gelbach, and Miller (2011). Our estimates
are identical for the two-way clustering cases in which we use Mitchell Petersen’s STATA code (cluster2.ado).
This procedure is described by Petersen (2009).
91
DJIA Vol 0.002 −0.003 0.055 0.053 0.031 0.034∗ −0.011 −0.018 0.204∗∗∗ 0.205∗∗∗
(0.056) (0.055) (0.052) (0.052) (0.020) (0.020) (0.065) (0.065) (0.074) (0.075)
Portfolio size 0.001 0.001 0.000 0.000 0.001 0.001 −0.001 −0.001 0.002 0.002
(0.002) (0.003) (0.002) (0.002) (0.001) (0.001) (0.002) (0.002) (0.002) (0.002)
Growth rate −5.894∗∗∗ −5.908∗∗∗ 1.889 1.883 0.696 0.703 −2.731 −2.749 3.123∗ 3.124∗
(1.441) (1.453) (1.412) (1.415) (0.807) (0.811) (1.837) (1.854) (1.875) (1.876)
Population 0.011 0.010 0.008 0.008 −0.001 −0.001 0.010 0.009 −0.002 −0.001
(0.009) (0.010) (0.008) (0.008) (0.003) (0.003) (0.009) (0.009) (0.010) (0.009)
Income −0.004 −0.014 −0.050 −0.054 −0.005 0.000 0.010 −0.002 −0.049 −0.048
(0.047) (0.048) (0.038) (0.039) (0.015) (0.015) (0.053) (0.053) (0.042) (0.044)
Population change −0.121 0.001 0.222 0.274 −0.007 −0.065 −0.279 −0.117 0.012 0.006
(0.393) (0.385) (0.336) (0.356) (0.123) (0.132) (0.441) (0.434) (0.395) (0.414)
Income change −0.270 −0.174 0.334 0.375 −0.020 −0.065 −0.736 −0.610 −0.377 −0.382
(0.514) (0.520) (0.403) (0.413) (0.161) (0.170) (0.568) (0.569) (0.496) (0.521)
SAD 0.003 −0.001 −0.006 −0.008 −0.000 0.002 −0.003 −0.009 0.007 0.007
(0.012) (0.013) (0.011) (0.012) (0.004) (0.004) (0.014) (0.016) (0.020) (0.021)
N 1,505 1,505 1,505 1,505 1,505 1,505 1,505 1,505 1,505 1,505
Adjusted R 2 1.374% 1.463% 0.629% 0.649% 1.074% 1.290% 1.020% 1.149% 1.917% 1.917%
Page: 92
(continued)
73–111
Downloaded from http://rfs.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of California, San Francisco on April 7, 2015
Table 3
Continued
Panel B: Probit regressions
Dependent variable
TooHigh TooLow TooHigh DJIA TooLow DJIA
(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8)
Yi,t = b0 +b1 DSKCi,t +b2 DJ I AV olt +b3 P ortf olioSizei.t +b4 GrowthRatei,t +b5 P opulationi,t +b6 P opulationChangei,t +b7 I ncomei,t +b8 I ncomeChangei,t +b9 SADi,t +εi,t ,
where Zi,t = b0 +b1 DSKCi,t +b2 DJ I AV olt +b3 P ortf olioSizei.t +b4 GrowthRatei,t +b5 P opulationi,t +b6 P opulationChangei,t +b7 I ncomei,t +b8 I ncomeChangei,t +b9 SADi,t .
Page: 93
Odd number models replace DSKC with SKC and Seasonal SKC. Panel B reports marginal effects from the probit models. The dependent variables (Y) in the models are reported in the top of
93
each column and include a binary variable associated with whether individual stocks are perceived to be overpriced (TooHigh), a binary variable associated with whether individual stocks are
perceived to be underpriced (TooLow), the percentage difference between the perceived DJIA level relative to a 7-day trailing average of the actual DJIA level (%DJIA MisPrc), a binary variable
associated with whether %DJIA MisPrc is in the bottom sample quartile (DJIA TooHigh), and a binary variable associated with whether %DJIA MisPrc is in the top sample quartile (DJIA
73–111
TooLow). Only binary dependent variable models are estimated in panel B. Robust standard errors clustered on ZIP code and date levels are reported in parentheses below. ***, **, and * denote
statistical significance at the 1%, 5%, and 10% levels, respectively.
Downloaded from http://rfs.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of California, San Francisco on April 7, 2015
The Review of Financial Studies / v 28 n 1 2015
94
Yi,t = b0 +b1 DSKCi,t +b2 DJ I AV olt +b3 P ortf olioSizei.t +b4 GrowthRatei,t +b5 P opulationi,t +b6 P opulationChangei,t +b7 I ncomei,t +b8 I ncomeChangei,t +b9 SADi,t +ςi +τt +εi,t .
The dependent variables, Y , include TooHigh, TooLow, %DJIA MisPrc, DJIA TooHigh, and DJIA TooLow. Models 6 through 10 of each panel replace DSKC with SKC and Seasonal SKC. The
regression models in panel A include state fixed effects, and in Panel B, they include year-quarter fixed effects. The estimates on the conditioning variables are not shown to conserve space.
Page: 95
Robust standard errors clustered on the ZIP code and date levels are reported in parentheses below. ***, **, and * denote statistical significance at the 1%, 5%, and 10% levels, respectively.
95
73–111
Downloaded from http://rfs.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of California, San Francisco on April 7, 2015
The Review of Financial Studies / v 28 n 1 2015
the 10% level, and we posit that the lack of variation in nighttime hours in our
data sample may be the explanation.
To evaluate these views, we include DSKC and SAD interaction terms in the
OLS models from Table 3, and the results are displayed in Table A.1 in the
Online Appendix. The DSKC coefficient remains statistically significant for
the TooHigh, %DJIA MisPrc, and DJIA TooHigh models. In these models, the
interaction term coefficients are of consistent sign as our conjecture, though
only the one in the TooHigh model is statistically significant at the 10% level.27
We conclude, as before, that the tests may not have sufficient statistical power
27 The results are similar with the inclusion of a dummy variable corresponding with fall months.
96
Table 5
Univariate analysis of high-low DSKC on investor BSI
High DSKC Low DSKC High DSKC - Low DSKC
50th percentile split 0.021 0.034 −0.013∗∗∗ (0.003)
33rd percentile split 0.018 0.037 −0.018∗∗∗ (0.004)
25th percentile split 0.016 0.036 −0.020∗∗∗ (0.005)
10th percentile split 0.013 0.059 −0.046∗∗∗ (0.008)
Group sample averages of the Investor BSI are reported for high and low DSKC investors. Investor BSI is defined
as the total dollar buy less sell volumes, scaled by the total trading volume for ZIP-code i and date t . Investors
are ranked based on DSKC for each date. High (low) DSKC investor classification is based on whether the DSKC
ranking is in the top (bottom) 50th, 33rd, 25th, or 10th percentile of that date. Standard errors on the difference
When defining the groups by the top (bottom) 50th percentile on DSKC for
each sample date, the difference in Investor BSI is negative and statistically
significant (estimate = −0.013; t-value = −3.71), as expected. Tightening
the threshold serves to increase the group differences and appears to be
primarily driven by increasing values of average Investor BSI in the sunny
groups. For cloudy (sunny) groups based on the top (bottom) 10th percentile
on DSKC, the difference in the group means is −0.046 (t-value = −5.48),
which is approximately 9.2% of the sample standard deviation for Investor
BSI. Altogether, these univariate tests show that DSKC has a negative impact
on investor propensities to buy, as predicted. Additionally, the effect becomes
pronounced when tightening the thresholds for the DSKC groupings.
97
Table 6
Regression analysis on daily ZIP code-level investor BSI
Dependent variable: Investor BSI ratio
(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6)
DSKC −0.024∗∗ −0.021∗∗ −0.031∗∗
(0.011) (0.010) (0.014)
SKC −0.023∗∗ −0.020∗∗ −0.031∗∗
(0.010) (0.010) (0.014)
Seasonal SKC −0.029 −0.059∗∗ 0.017
(0.057) (0.026) (0.070)
Monday −0.014∗∗ −0.014∗∗ −0.014∗∗ −0.014∗∗
(0.006) (0.006) (0.006) (0.006)
I nvestorBSIz,t = d0 +d1 DSKCz,t +d2 Mondayt +d3 J anuaryt +d4 P opulationz,t +d5 P opulationChangez,t
Investor trades are aggregated to the date-ZIP code level. The explanatory variables of interest are the natural
logarithm of one plus the average SKC over the past 14 days (SKC), the natural logarithm of one plus the average
SKC over the same month over the entire sample period (Seasonal SKC), and the deseasonalized SKC defined
as the difference between SKC and Seasonal SKC (DSKC). Though not displayed, some of the models include
additional explanatory variables, as indicated. Monday takes the value of one if the observation date is a Monday
and zero otherwise. January takes the value of one if the observation month is January and zero otherwise.
County economic condition variables are as follows. Population is the logarithm of the county-level population.
PopulationChange is the change over the previous year. Income is the logarithm of the county-level median
household income. IncomeChange is the change in Income over the previous year. Adjusted nighttime hours per
day (SAD) is calculated using the construction outlined by Kamstra, Kramer, and Levi (2003), based upon the
investors coordinate locations. Models 3 and 4 include ZIP code fixed effects (ξ ), and models 5 and 6 include
date fixed effects (η). Coefficients on the fixed effects are not reported for viewing ease. Robust standard errors
clustered on date and ZIP code levels are reported in parentheses. ***, **, and * denote statistical significance
at the 1%, 5%, and 10% levels, respectively.
not reveal the investor’s daily positions, the tests are conditional on whether
the investor trades (e.g., either buys or sells) in the stock for a particular date.
The advantage of the three-level panel is that we can now account for stock-
level factors. Specifically, we include in the regression models individual stock
characteristics related to size, which is measured as the natural logarithm
of the stock’s market capitalization, and liquidity, which is measured as the
inverse of the stock’s share price. We also examine another specification that
includes fixed effects for each stock-date pair, which is expected to significantly
98
This table reports OLS regression model estimates using the daily stock/investor-level BSI ratio (StockInvestor BSI) as the dependent variable:
StockI nvestorBSIz,j,t = f0 +f1 DSKCz,t +f2 Mondayt +f3 J anuaryt +f4 P opulationz,t +f5 P opulationChangez,t +f6 I ncomez,t +f7 I ncomeChangez,t
Investor trades are aggregated to the stock-date-ZIP code level. Though not displayed, some of the models include additional explanatory variables, as indicated. Models (1) through (4)
include fixed effects based on date groups. Models (5) through (6) include fixed effects based on date-stock grouping pairs (φ). Robust standard errors clustered on the stock, date, and ZIP
code levels are reported in parentheses. ***, **, and * denote statistical significance at the 1%, 5%, and 10% levels, respectively.
Page: 99
99
73–111
Downloaded from http://rfs.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of California, San Francisco on April 7, 2015
The Review of Financial Studies / v 28 n 1 2015
decrease the statistical power of the tests. The fixed effects models limit the
variation in DSKC to within-stock-date groupings, purging the estimators of
any unobservable factors associated with a particular stock at each point in time.
The fixed effects model described above is specified as follows:
StockInvestor BSIz,j,t = f0 +f1 ∗DSKCz,t +f2 ∗CountyCharacteristcsz,t
+f3 ∗SADz,t + fj,t ∗D(Stock = j Date = t)+εz,j,t .
j t
The fixed effects model does not include a number of variables that are
100
101
5.2 Investor mood and stock prices: Stock return regression estimates
Table 8 displays the OLS results from the PosRet models. Panel A reports the
estimates when we include only the weather variables in the models, whereas
panel B reports the estimates when all the conditioning variables are included.
The coefficient estimates for the conditioning variables are not reported to
conserve space.
The first two models report the pooled model estimates. The remaining eight
columns report the model estimates on sample splits based on the ArbCosts
rankings. Models 3 and 4 are estimated for the sample in which ArbCosts is
the lowest 50th sample percentile. Models 5 and 6 are estimated on the sample
in which ArbCosts is between the 50th and 80th sample percentiles. Models 7
and 8 are estimated on the sample in which ArbCosts is between the 80th and
95th sample percentiles. Models 9 and 10 are estimated on the sample in which
ArbCosts is between the 95th and 100th sample percentiles.
The first row displays the estimates on the StockDSKC variable. In panel
A, the StockDSKC coefficient in the pooled sample is statistically insignificant
(estimate = −0.003; t-value = −0.14) with only the weather variables. However,
the results from the regression models using subsamples based on ArbCosts
rankings suggest that the weather effect is concentrated among stocks with
higher arbitrage costs. For stocks with ArbCosts in the 95th to 100th sample
percentile range, StockDSKC is negative and statistically significant (estimate
102
−0.061∗∗∗
(0.053) (0.086) (0.054) (0.032) (0.018)
Conditioning variables yes yes yes yes yes yes yes yes yes yes
N 8,590,220 8,590,220 4,295,101 4,295,101 2,577,086 2,577,086 1,288,516 1,288,516 429,517 429,517
Adjusted R 2 0.235% 0.239% 0.098% 0.108% 0.160% 0.167% 0.166% 0.166% 0.205% 0.234%
The dependent variable in the OLS model below is an indicator variable (PosRet) taking the value of one for positive return days and 0 otherwise.
P osReti,t = g0 +g1 StockDSKCi,t +g2 Mondayt +g3 J anuaryt +g4 StockSADi,t +g5 MktCapi,t +g6 StockLiqi,t +εi,t
The top matter of the table presents the subsample definition based on the ArbCosts rankings. The first two models present the pooled estimates. The remaining columns present the subsample
estimates based on ArbCosts rankings for the entire 1999–2010 sample period: [0%,50%] in models 3 and 4; (50%,80%] in models 5 and 6; (80%,95%] in models 7 and 8; and (95%,100%]
in models 9 and 10. We define ArbCosts as the inverse of the proportion of shares held by institutional investors in the stock. Panel A presents estimates from the models that only include the
weather variables, whereas panel B presents estimates from the models that also include the conditioning variables. The estimates on the conditioning variables are not reported to conserve
space. Deseasonalized stock-level SKC (StockDSKC) is defined as the difference in StockSKC and Seasonal StockSKC. Refer to the text for further details on construction. Robust standard
Page: 103
errors clustered on the stock and date levels are displayed in parentheses. ***, **, and * denote statistical significance at the 1%, 5%, and 10% levels, respectively.
103
73–111
Downloaded from http://rfs.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of California, San Francisco on April 7, 2015
The Review of Financial Studies / v 28 n 1 2015
= −0.029; t-value = −3.03). The coefficients before that point are relatively
higher but mostly statistically insignificant. Using StockSKC and Seasonal
StockSKC instead yields similar results. When we include the conditioning
variables, panel B shows that the StockDSKC coefficients attenuate somewhat
but remain statistically significant.
The StockDSKC coefficient is statistically significant in stocks within the top
sample quintile based on ArbCosts. The average proportion of shares held by
institutional investors within those stocks ranges from 2% to 20%. Although
we cannot construct mood proxies for stocks not represented in the 13(f)
104
Conditioning variables yes yes yes yes yes yes yes yes yes yes
N 8,590,220 8,590,220 4,295,101 4,295,101 2,577,086 2,577,086 1,288,516 1,288,516 429,517 429,517
Pseudo R 2 0.235% 0.239% 0.098% 0.108% 0.160% 0.167% 0.166% 0.166% 0.205% 0.234%
The dependent variable in the probit model below is an indicator variable (PosRet) taking the value of one for positive return days and 0 otherwise.
P(P osReti,t |Zi,t ) = Zi,t ,
where Zi,t = h0 +h1 StockDSKCi,t +h2 Mondayt +h3 J anuaryt +h4 StockSADi,t +h5 MktCapi,t +h6 StockLiqi,t .
Only marginal effects are reported. The top matter of the table presents the subsample definition based upon the ArbCosts rankings. The first two models present the pooled estimates. The
remaining columns present the subsample estimates based on ArbCosts rankings for the entire 1999–2010 sample period: [0%,50%] in models 3 and 4; (50%,80%] in models 5 and 6;
(80%,95%] in models 7 and 8; and (95%,100%] in models 9 and 10. We define ArbCosts as the inverse of the proportion of shares held by institutional investors in the stock. Panel A presents
estimates from the models that only include the weather variables, whereas panel B presents estimates from the models that also include the conditioning variables. The estimates on the
conditioning variables are not reported to conserve space. Deseasonalized stock-level SKC (StockDSKC) is defined as the difference in StockSKC and Seasonal StockSKC. Refer to the text
Page: 105
for further details on construction. Robust standard errors clustered on the stock and date levels are displayed in parentheses. ***, **, and * denote statistical significance at the 1%, 5%, and
105
10% levels, respectively.
73–111
Downloaded from http://rfs.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of California, San Francisco on April 7, 2015
The Review of Financial Studies / v 28 n 1 2015
29 Specifically, we require that the estimate use at least 10 and 30 days of nonmissing returns data for the 30- and
90-day estimation procedures, respectively.
30 Lag selection for each specification is based upon the procedure of Newey and West (1994). We also consider
alternative kernels to compute bandwidths and use the ones that provide the most conservative standard error
estimates.
106
0.25
0.25
0.15
0.15
0.05
0.05
-0.05 -0.05
-0.15 -0.15
LO 2 MD 4 HI HI-LO LO 2 MD 4 HI HI-LO
Panel A: 30-day estimation window
0.15 0.15
0.05 0.05
-0.05 -0.05
-0.15 -0.15
LO 2 MD 4 HI HI-LO LO 2 MD 4 HI HI-LO
Figure 2
Comovement estimates on StockDSKC portfolios
This figure displays the comovement estimates on the factor portfolios associated with Optimistic (light bars,
left) and Pessimistic (dark bars, right) Mood portfolios. For each bar graph, the sample average over the daily,
value-weighted comovement estimates for each StockDSKC quintile group is reported, from the lowest (LO) to
the highest (HI) daily StockDSKC rankings. If a stock also belongs to either the Optimistic or the Pessimistic
Mood portfolio, the stock is removed from the factor portfolio before calculating the comovement estimate.
The differences between the high and low quintile group estimates (HI-LO) are reported to the right. Panel A
displays the graphs for the 30-day estimation window. Panel B displays the graphs for the 90-day estimation
window. Ninety percent confidence intervals are overlaid on each series based on the Newey-West standard error
estimates.
107
the differences remain positive but are also statistically insignificant (value =
0.014, t-value = 0.58).
In summary, the results provide evidence of return comovement attributable
to the investor mood proxy. They also suggest that the weather effect is not long
lived, given that weather patterns fluctuate more over longer time horizons.
Consequently, the mood effect related to weather is unlikely to persist for long
periods of time.
31 One notable exception is Loughran and Schultz (2004), who indirectly examine the effect of localized trading
by firm headquarter locations.
108
References
Adaval, R. 2001. Sometimes it just feels right: The differential weighting of affect-consistent and affect-
inconsistent product information. Journal of Consumer Research 28:1–17.
Agarwal, S., R. Duchin, and D. Sosyura. 2012. In the mood for a loan: The causal effect of sentiment on credit
origination. Working Paper, University of Michigan.
Barber, B., Y. Lee, J. Liu, and T. Odean. 2007. Is the aggregate investor reluctant to realize losses? European
Financial Management 13:423–47.
Bassi, A., R. Colacito, and P. Fulghieri. 2013. ’O sole mio: An experimental analysis of weather and risk attitudes
in financial decisions. Review of Financial Studies 26:1824–52.
Camerer, C., L. Babcock, G. Loewenstein, and R. Thaler. 1997. Labor supply of New York City cabdrivers: One
day at a time. Quarterly Journal of Economics 112:407–41.
Cameron, A., J. Gelbach, and D. Miller. 2011. Robust inference with multiway clustering. Journal of Business
& Economic Statistics 29:238–49.
Clore, G., and W. Parrott. 1991. In Emotion and social judgments. Ed. J. P. Forgas. Oxford: Pergamon Press.
Clore, G., N. Schwarz, and M. Conway. 1994. In Handbook of social cognition, vol. 1, pp. 107–23. Eds. R. S. Wyer
and T. K. Srull. New York: Psychology Press.
Coval, J., and T. Moskowitz. 1999. Home bias at home: Local equity preference in domestic portfolios. Journal
of Finance 54:2045–73.
Coval, J., and T. Shumway. 2005. Do behavioral biases affect prices? Journal of Finance 60:1–34.
Cunningham, M. 1979. Weather, mood, and helping behavior: Quasi-experiments with the sunshine Samaritan.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 37:1947–56.
Dougal, C., J. Engelberg, D. García, and C. Parsons. 2012. Journalists and the stock market. Review of Financial
Studies 25:639–79.
Driscoll, J. C., and A. C. Kraay. 1998. Consistent covariance matrix estimation with spatially dependent panel
data. Review of Economics and Statistics 80:549–60.
Eastman, C. I., M. A. Young, L. F. Fogg, L. Liu, and P. M. Meaden. 1998. Bright light treatment of winter
depression: a placebo-controlled trial. Archives of General Psychiatry 55:883–9.
Edmans, A., D. Garcia, and Ø. Norli. 2007. Sports sentiment and stock returns. Journal of Finance 62:1967–98.
Fama, E., and K. French. 1993. Common risk factors in returns on stocks and bonds. Journal of Financial
Economics 33:3–56.
Fishbein, M. 1963. An investigation of the relationships between beliefs about an object and the attitude toward
that object. Human Relations 16:233–40.
Frazzini, A. 2006. The disposition effect and under-reaction to news. Journal of Finance 61:2017–46.
Goel, N., M. Terman, J. Su Terman, M. M. Macchi, and J. W. Stewart. 2005. Controlled trial of bright light and
negative air ions for chronic depression. Psychological Medicine 35:945–56.
Goetzmann, W., and N. Zhu. 2005. Rain or shine: Where is the weather effect? European Financial Management
11:559–78.
Green, C., and B. Hwang. 2009. Price-based return comovement. Journal of Financial Economics 93:37–50.
109
Hirshleifer, D., and T. Shumway. 2003. Good day sunshine: Stock returns and the weather. Journal of
Finance 58:1009–32.
Johnson, E., and A. Tversky. 1983. Affect, generalization, and the perception of risk. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology 45:20–31.
Kamstra, M., L. Kramer, and M. Levi. 2000. Losing sleep at the market: The daylight saving anomaly. American
Economic Review 90:1005–11.
———. 2003. Winter blues: A SAD stock market cycle. American Economic Review 93:324–43.
Kamstra, M., L. Kramer, M. Levi, and T. Wang. 2014. Seasonally varying preferences: Theoretical foundations
for an empirical regularity. Review of Asset Pricing Studies 4:39–77.
Kramer, L., and J. Weber. 2012. This is your portfolio on winter seasonal affective disorder and risk aversion in
financial decision making. Social Psychological and Personality Science 3:193–9.
Kripke, D. F. 1998. Light treatment for nonseasonal depression: speed, efficacy, and combined treatment. Journal
of Affective Disorders 49:109–17.
Kumar, A., J. Page, and O. Spalt. 2013. Investor sentiment and return comovements: Evidence from stock splits
and headquarters changes. Review of Finance 17:921–53.
Lee, C., and M. Ready. 1991. Inferring trade direction from intraday data. Journal of Finance 46:733–46.
Linnainmaa, J., and I. Rosu. 2009. Weather and time series determinants of liquidity in a limit order market.
Working Paper, University of Chicago.
Lo, K. and S. Wu. 2011. The impact of seasonal affective disorder on financial analysts and equity market returns.
Working Paper, University of British Columbia.
Loewenstein, G. 2000. Emotions in economic theory and economic behavior. American Economic Review
90:426–32.
Loewenstein, G., E. Weber, C. Hsee, and N. Welch. 2001. Risk as feelings. Psychological Bulletin
127:267–86.
Loughran, T., and P. Schultz. 2004. Weather, stock returns, and the impact of localized trading behavior. Journal
of Financial and Quantitative Analysis 39:343–64.
Mehra, R. and R. Sah. 2002. Mood fluctuations, projection bias, and volatility of equity prices. Journal of
Economic Dynamics and Control 26:869–87.
Nagel, S. 2005. Short sales, institutional investors and the cross-section of stock returns. Journal of Financial
Economics 78:277–309.
Newey, W., and K. West. 1994. Automatic lag selection in covariance matrix estimation. Review of Economic
Studies 61:631–54.
Petersen, M. 2009. Estimating standard errors in finance panel data sets: Comparing approaches. Review of
Financial Studies 22:435–80.
Petty, R., F. Gleicher, and S. Baker. 1991. In Emotion and social judgements, pp. 181–200. Ed. J. P. Forgas.
New York: Pergamon Press.
Puckett, A., and X. Yan. 2011. The interim trading skills of institutional investors. Journal of Finance 66:601–33.
Rind, B. 1996. Effect of beliefs about weather conditions on tipping. Journal of Applied Social Psychology
26:137–47.
110
Saunders, E. 1993. Stock prices and wall street weather. American Economic Review 83:1337–45.
Schwartz, G. 1990. In Repression and dissociation: Implications for personality theory, psychopathology, and
health, pp. 405–34. Ed. J. Singer. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Schwarz, N., and G. Clore. 1983. Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive
functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 45:513–23.
Shiller, R. J., F. Kon-Ya, and Y. Tsutsui. 1996. Why did the Nikkei crash? Expanding the scope of expectations
data collection. Review of Economics and Statistics 78:156–64.
Shiller, R. J. 2000. Measuring bubble expectations and investor confidence. Journal of Psychology and Financial
Markets 1:49–60.
Wyer, R. S. Jr., G. L. Clore, and L. M. Isbell. 1999. Affect and information processing. Advances in Experimental
Social Psychology 31:1–77.
111