Hypnosis, Suggestion, and Suggestibility: An Integrative Model
Hypnosis, Suggestion, and Suggestibility: An Integrative Model
Hypnosis, Suggestion, and Suggestibility: An Integrative Model
net/publication/273348399
CITATIONS READS
79 3,444
3 authors:
Irving Kirsch
University of Plymouth
163 PUBLICATIONS 14,893 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
All content following this page was uploaded by Steven Jay Lynn on 18 April 2015.
To cite this article: Steven Jay Lynn, Jean-Roch Laurence & Irving Kirsch (2015) Hypnosis, Suggestion,
and Suggestibility: An Integrative Model, American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 57:3, 314-329, DOI:
10.1080/00029157.2014.976783
Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the
“Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis,
our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to
the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions
and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors,
and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content
should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources
of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims,
proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or
howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising
out of the use of the Content.
This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any
substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing,
systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms &
Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-
and-conditions
Downloaded by [steven lynn] at 18:29 20 January 2015
American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 57: 314–329, 2015
Copyright © American Society of Clinical Hypnosis
ISSN: 0002-9157 print / 2160-0562 online
DOI: 10.1080/00029157.2014.976783
Jean-Roch Laurence
Concordia University, Montreal, Quebec, Canada
Downloaded by [steven lynn] at 18:29 20 January 2015
Irving Kirsch
Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts, USA
This article elucidates an integrative model of hypnosis that integrates social, cultural, cognitive,
and neurophysiological variables at play both in and out of hypnosis and considers their dynamic
interaction as determinants of the multifaceted experience of hypnosis. The roles of these variables
are examined in the induction and suggestion stages of hypnosis, including how they are related
to the experience of involuntariness, one of the hallmarks of hypnosis. It is suggested that studies
of the modification of hypnotic suggestibility; cognitive flexibility; response sets and expectancies;
the default-mode network; and the search for the neurophysiological correlates of hypnosis, more
broadly, in conjunction with research on social psychological variables, hold much promise to further
understanding of hypnosis.
Keywords: attention, cognitive, core and process variables in hypnosis, expectancy, hypnosis,
hypnotic suggestibility, induction, response set theory, socio-cognitive models, suggestion
Stretching back to the time of Mesmer, hypnosis has provoked the curiosity of scientists
and the lay public. After all, any intervention that produces hallucinations on command,
amnesia for events, and a blunting of response to ordinarily painful stimuli would call
for an explanation (Pintar & Lynn, 2008). How can we account for the fascinating and
perplexing alterations in consciousness that occur during hypnosis? Is the participant in
hypnosis somehow experiencing a trance state, radically different from waking life, as
the dominant view of hypnosis for more than 200 years proclaims? Or can the appar-
ent shifts in consciousness be readily explained in the same terms as more mundane
phenomena?
Address correspondence to Steven Jay Lynn, Binghamton University, Psychology Department, PO Box 6000,
Binghamton, NY 13902-6000, USA. E-mail: [email protected]
HYPNOSIS, SUGGESTION, AND SUGGESTIBILITY 315
In this article, we favor the latter view and present an integrative model of hypno-
sis and hypnotic phenomena that acknowledges the interaction of social, cultural, and
cognitive variables in producing the multifaceted experience of hypnosis (for earlier
versions of integrative/synergistic models, see Lynn & Rhue, 1991; Nadon, Laurence,
& Perry, 1991). These variables include, but are not limited to, participants’ attitudes,
beliefs, and expectancies about hypnosis; their motivation to respond to the hypnotist
and his or her suggestions; their interpretation of how to respond to suggestions and will-
ingness and ability to imagine experiences consistent with the requirements of diverse
suggestions; and their ability to form response sets in keeping with suggested activities
and respond to nonhypnotic imaginative suggestions. We believe that a truly compre-
hensive account of hypnosis must consider the neurophysiological roots or correlates
of what transpires during hypnosis. We keep an open mind with regard to the possi-
Downloaded by [steven lynn] at 18:29 20 January 2015
ble operation of unique attentional and neurocognitive capabilities that may prove to
distinguish highly responsive from non-responsive participants in and out of hypnosis
(Laurence, Slako, & Le Beau, 1998), and we suggest lines of future research to examine
such differences among participants.
We contend—with most mainstream hypnosis researchers—that hypnosis produces
genuine changes in consciousness, and we wholeheartedly agree with Bloom (2004)
who observed that “words change physiology” a claim that is not in dispute among
proponents of competing theories of hypnosis. In fact, hypnotic experiences can be so
compelling that impressive changes in brain activation can be observed as a product of
suggestion (e.g., visual hallucinations) that are akin to those produced by real percep-
tual experiences (Szechtman, Woody, Bowers, & Nahmias, 1998; Woody & Szechtman,
2000).
To be clear from the outset, we eschew the culturally received belief that hypnosis is best
construed as a special state or “trance,” somehow divorced from the operations of day-to-
day consciousness. Whether one must be in an altered or special state of consciousness
to experience hypnosis becomes a moot issue to the extent that “hypnotic” and ordinary
experiences are not static and instead reflect constant and often transient changes in
our awareness, emotions, thoughts, sensations, and action tendencies that are typically
difficult, if not impossible, to parse into discrete states (Lynn, Green, Elinoff, Baltman,
& Maxwell, in press).
The experience of hypnosis differs both within and among individuals on a moment-
to-moment basis. Differences in what people are told about hypnosis; their attitudes,
beliefs, and expectancies about hypnosis; the suggestions they receive; how they con-
strue particular suggestions; and their feeling of comfort and security with the hypnotist
and the situation overall will ensure the uniqueness and idiosyncratic nature of their
experiences during hypnosis (see Sheehan & McConkey, 1982).
316 LYNN ET AL.
Given the shifting nature of hypnotic experience, we suggest that there is no singular
brain state, physiological signature, or state of consciousness that invariably follows a
hypnotic induction. That said, we are optimistic that research efforts to identify phys-
iological concomitants of both a general receptiveness and readiness to respond to
suggestions and cognitive flexibility may yield valuable findings. Moreover, as we will
illustrate, there are already indications that this is the case.
We define hypnosis as a situation in which imaginative suggestions for changes in
thoughts, feelings, and actions are provided to a person in a context defined as “hyp-
nosis,” with the expectation that the participant will respond to them in a compelling
manner consistent with his or her beliefs about hypnosis, often derived from the broader
sociocultural context. Imaginative suggestions are requests to experience an imaginary
state of affairs as if it were real (Kirsch & Braffman, 2001). They differ from various
other types of suggestion (e.g., the placebo effect, sensory suggestions, and the misin-
formation effect), as indicated by relatively low correlations between responsiveness to
these various types of suggestion. Imaginative suggestions can be given with or without
the induction of hypnosis. When given in a hypnotic context, they can be administered by
a person designated or perceived to be in the role of a “hypnotist” or self-administered,
in which case the situation is construed as “self-hypnosis.”
It can be said that someone is “hypnotized” when he or she responds to imaginative
suggestions that are presented following a hypnotic induction ritual, which may be elab-
orate or as simple as merely defining the situation as “hypnosis.” People who are able
to respond to the imaginative suggestions that are characteristic of hypnosis are often
termed highly hypnotizable or susceptible to hypnosis. However, the very high correla-
tion between responsiveness to these suggestions in hypnosis, and the same suggestions
without the induction of hypnosis (r = .67 for behavioral scores; r = .82 for subjec-
tive scores; Braffman & Kirsch, 1999), indicates that the term highly suggestible is a
more accurate description of these individuals. We propose that the ability to respond
to imaginative suggestions depends on the ability to experience or translate the sug-
gested sensations and imaginings into credible and compelling subjective experiences
and actions.
HYPNOSIS, SUGGESTION, AND SUGGESTIBILITY 317
Hypnotic Induction
To flesh out our perspective, we discuss the two stages in which hypnosis typically
unfolds: induction and suggestion. Historically, hypnosis has been associated with
inductions as varied as eye closure, focusing on an internal stimulus (such as breathing)
or an external stimulus (such as the proverbial “dangling watch” popularized in fiction
and the movies), and perhaps most commonly with instructions to relax and feel at ease.
Regardless of what induction method is used, it is crucial to clearly define the events
as “hypnotic” in nature, to distinguish the social interaction from everyday communica-
tions, and to mark the special occasion as one in which consciousness or capabilities will
be optimally and radically expanded beyond the mundane. That is, what is paramount
is that the hypnotist presents communications in such a way that they are deemed to be
Downloaded by [steven lynn] at 18:29 20 January 2015
associated with the executive system, which in everyday life are associated with focused
imagination, absorption, focused attention, and reduced analytical thought, also come
on-line during hypnosis (Oakley & Halligan, 2010).
Defining the situation as hypnosis activates sociocultural schemas and expectan-
cies regarding hypnosis, including the idea that hypnosis produces an altered state of
consciousness that enhances suggestibility. Accordingly, it is not surprising that many
participants experience alterations in consciousness, particularly when the suggestions
provided explicitly call for such alterations in consciousness. Still, most individuals do
not, in fact, experience hypnosis as a “trance,” and virtually all of the effects partici-
pants’ experience during hypnosis can be experienced with motivating instructions in
the absence of defining the situation as hypnosis (Braffman & Kirsch, 1999; McConkey,
1986).
Downloaded by [steven lynn] at 18:29 20 January 2015
The broad skein of cultural beliefs and expectations in which hypnosis is embedded
coalesces into a loosely woven script that specifies how events will unfold and what
is expected from the participant during hypnosis (Lynn & Green, 2011). In this ini-
tial stage, the hypnotist may define hypnosis in various ways, ranging from a state of
absorption in suggestions, much like being absorbed in a movie; to an altered state of
consciousness; a state of dissociation; and thinking and imagining with suggestions. The
particular way in which hypnosis is defined is less important than casting the situation
as “hypnotic,” boosting response expectancies (i.e., anticipations of automatic subjec-
tive and behavioral responses to particular situational cues) for successful responding
and providing a rationale for administering suggestions that are delivered in the next
phase. According to Kirsch, hypnotic inductions produce hypnotic responses, much like
placebos, by the power of the expectancies they induce and produce alterations in a wide
variety of responses, including sexual arousal, anxiety, depression, and pain (Kirsch,
1985, 1991, 1994). When hypnosis is described as a trance, participants are not as
responsive and experience diminished subjective effects compared to when hypnosis is
defined as merely involving cooperation (Lynn, Vanderhoff, Shindler, & Stafford, 2002).
This finding is not particularly surprising, given that many participants experience trepi-
dations regarding what they (mistakenly) believe to be succumbing to a radically altered
state of consciousness in which they relinquish control to the hypnotist. Indeed, this
widely prevalent belief may predictably engender uneasiness and even outright anxi-
ety and reluctance or resistance to participate fully. Accordingly, early on, the hypnotist
often confronts cultural myths and misconceptions head on, informing individuals that
they will not lose control, that they can resist suggestions if they choose to do so, and
that they will not lose touch with their surroundings.
In this initial information-giving or “preinduction” stage of the hypnotic proceedings,
rapport and positive response expectations about hypnosis are established, participant
fears are minimized as myths about hypnosis are debunked, and an unspoken “contract”
is established that the hypnotist will facilitate the responses of the participant who is
willing, motivated, and hopefully able to experience whatever is suggested. This first
HYPNOSIS, SUGGESTION, AND SUGGESTIBILITY 319
stage also often involves setting an agenda for hypnosis and removing barriers to optimal
responding in which (a) suggestions are tied to goals of research or therapy; (b) moti-
vation is enhanced by emphasizing the appealing features of hypnosis (e.g., relaxation,
calm); and (c) expectancies about hypnosis are clarified, often by way of discussion
that reveals and addresses lingering concerns about full immersion in the experience of
hypnosis.
Hypnotic Suggestions
In the suggestion stage, suggestions serve to organize mental contents, while the hyp-
notic context facilitates motivation to get absorbed in imaginative suggestions and
Downloaded by [steven lynn] at 18:29 20 January 2015
respond in keeping with the sensations, imaginings, and implicit and explicit directives
of the suggestions. Suggestions vary in terms of their generality (e.g., full-body relax-
ation) versus specificity (e.g., rehearse in imagination a specific future event) and in
their wording (e.g., permissive versus authoritative tone). Typically, the way suggestions
are worded exerts little or no effect on participants’ subjective or behavioral responses,
beyond whether the suggestions are experienced as involuntary. For example, (a) per-
missive suggestions are experienced as more involuntary than authoritative suggestions
(Lynn, Neufeld, & Matyi, 1987) and (b) suggestions that contain imagery (so-called
goal-directed fantasies [GDFs]; Spanos, 1971) that would be expected to lead to the
involuntary occurrence of a motor response (e.g., imagining a helium balloon as part of
a hand levitation suggestion) produce more involuntary responses than suggestions that
contain no GDFs. What matters more than minor variations in suggestion wording is
that the suggestions convey the expected response clearly. Indeed, as noted earlier, the
identical suggestions delivered in a nonhypnotic context are highly correlated with the
response to the same suggestions delivered in a hypnotic context (Braffman & Kirsch,
1999; Hilgard & Tart, 1966). Further, the addition of a hypnotic induction appears to
add little to suggestibility (approximately 1.5 suggestions on a 12-point scale; Kirsch
& Lynn, 1995), as the imaginative suggestions themselves provide clear direction for
responding and establish expectancies regarding how to respond (Kirsch, Mazzoni, &
Montgomery, 2007; Meyer & Lynn, 2011; Polito, Barnier, Woody, & Connors, 2014).
The small difference between hypnotic and identical nonhypnotic suggestions, and the
small increment in hypnotic responsiveness that follows an induction, imply that an
altered state is not necessary to explain hypnotic responses. The small advantage for
hypnosis in increasing suggestibility may be a function of enhanced motivation and
expectancies that often accompany the induction of hypnosis (Lynn et al., 2008).
Popular depictions aside, people do not respond to suggestions robotically. In fact,
one of the most striking features of hypnosis is that there are pronounced individual
differences in responsiveness to suggestions. For the past 50 years or so, researchers
have struggled, largely in vain, to identify meaningful personality correlates and markers
320 LYNN ET AL.
The current account represents our most concerted attempt to integrate social-
psychological, cognitive, and neurophysiological variables associated with hypnotic
responding. Accordingly, in this section we present a selective review of studies that
provide support for the neurophysiological underpinnings of cognitive flexibility, atten-
tion, and the activation of response sets during hypnosis, with important caveats noted
as we proceed.
At this point, we may say, quite accurately, that there is no one “hypnosis,” no one
“state of hypnosis,” and no one or essential “ability” necessary to respond to hypnosis.
Rather than representing a fixed capacity to experience a special state of conscious-
ness, the experience of hypnosis may require a modicum of mental flexibility and the
ability to regulate attention and emotion in keeping with a variety of suggestions and
their attendant demands (Davidson & Goleman, 1977; Gruzelier, Gray, & Horn, 2002;
MacLeod, 2011). As Erickson, Rossi, and Rossi (1976) stated so cogently, therapeutic
hypnosis occurs when “the limitations of one’s usual conscious sets and belief systems
are temporarily altered so that one can be receptive to an experience of other patterns
322 LYNN ET AL.
studies are needed that control for imagery use, relaxation, motivation, and expectancy
across both hypnotic and nonhypnotic inductions and suggestions before firm conclu-
sions can be drawn regarding the specificity of involvement of the ACC in hypnotic
responding. Indeed, the ACC has been associated with many cognitive functions outside
the context of hypnosis (e.g., monitoring the degree of response conflict, over-riding
prepotent response tendencies, and reappraising the relation between internal states and
events, see Lynn et al., 2006).
McGeown et al.’s (2009) research implicates the default network in hypnotic respond-
ing. This network refers to cortical areas active when goal-directed mental activity is
absent in states such as mind wandering and daydreaming. Conversely, decreases in
the default mode signal goal-directed activity. McGeown and colleagues (2009) found
evidence for decreases in the default network among highly hypnotizable individuals—
but not low hypnotizables—following a hypnotic induction. Decreases in default-mode
activity are associated with increased goal-directed activity in everyday life and are
therefore also consistent with our hypothesis that goal-directed, strategic, and possibly
nonconscious mental activity can play a role in hypnotic responding, much as it does
on a day-to-day basis. We contend that (a) readiness to respond, (b) the propensity to
focus attention as directed—engendered by the hypnotic context, (c) the properties of the
hypnotic induction described earlier, and (d) physical and mental changes experienced
during the induction create a situation in which response sets can form readily with little
awareness and bolster a positive response set in a recursive, feed-forward manner.
Research on the modification of hypnotic responsiveness holds a key to expand
the integrative model, examine the role of the default mode in hypnotic responding,
and identify potentially unique abilities or capabilities of highly responsive hypnotic
participants. Nearly three decades ago, Gorassini and Spanos (1986) developed the
Carleton Skills Training Program (CSTP) to enhance hypnotic responsiveness. The
CSTP includes the following components: (a) information to instill positive attitudes
about hypnosis and motivation to respond, (b) instructions regarding how to use imaginal
strategies to promote successful responding and instructions regarding how to interpret
HYPNOSIS, SUGGESTION, AND SUGGESTIBILITY 323
suggestions (e.g., one must actually lift the hand while imagining that it is rising by
itself), (c) exposure to a video-taped model who enacts successful responses to sugges-
tions and verbalizes imagery-based strategies to facilitate subjective response, and (d)
practice in responding to test suggestions.
Researchers have documented impressive increases on behavioral and subjective mea-
sures of suggestibility following hypnotic suggestibility modification training. In more
than 15 studies, researchers (Gorassini & Spanos, 1999; Spanos, 1986) determined that
more than 50% of initially low hypnotizable subjects who participated in the CSTP
scored as high hypnotizables at posttesting. Importantly, large magnitude CSTP treat-
ment gains endure for 2.5 years (Spanos, Cross, Menary, & Smith, 1988), and research
in Spain (Cangas & Pérez, 1998), Poland (Niedzwieñska, 2000), and England (Fellows
& Ragg, 1992) has provided cross-cultural support for the effectiveness of the CSTP.
Downloaded by [steven lynn] at 18:29 20 January 2015
Although this research indicates that hypnotic suggestibility is modifiable for many
participants via the manipulation of social and cognitive variables, it is still the case
that a sizable minority of individuals does not show increases in responsiveness post-
intervention. We hypothesize that one reason for the failure of the CSTP to produce
hypnotic suggestibility gains is that low-suggestible individuals lack key hard-wired
abilities compared with highly suggestible participants, pertinent to automatic informa-
tion processing and attentional abilities. Notably, this contention represents an expansion
of previous sociocognitive models of hypnosis to better represent the possible influence
of trait-like attributes on hypnotic responding.
Generally speaking, highly suggestible individuals exhibit greater Stroop conflict than
low-suggestible individuals, regardless of whether or not they are hypnotized (Blum
& Graef, 1971; Dixon, Brunet, & Laurence, 1990; Dixon & Laurence, 1992; for a
contradictory finding, see Rubichi, Ricci, Padovani, & Scaglietti, 2005). Researchers
have interpreted the suggestibility findings alternately as indications that highly sug-
gestible individuals process information more automatically (Dixon & Laurence, 1992)
or that their executive attention networks are more efficient (Raz et al., 2002) than
their low-suggestible counterparts. Both explanations imply that trait-like attributes
(neuro-cognitive factors) can modulate Stroop performance. Further evidence for spe-
cific neuro-cognitive factors comes from a neurophysiological investigation of evoked
potentials during a Stroop-like task (Baribeau, Le Beau, Roth, & Laurence, 1994;
Laurence et al., 1998) in which highs displayed significantly shorter latencies of a
pre-300 negativity at the frontal site for the word and neutral conditions.
These results provide further support for the automaticity hypothesis and suggest that
the greater automaticity exhibited by highly hypnotizable subjects is not exclusively ver-
bal but rather perceptual in nature. In a recent case study of brain oscillations during an
electroencephalogram (EEG) procedure, Fingelkurts, Fingelkurts, Kallio, and Revonsuo
(2007) suggested that neutral hypnosis was characterized by an increase in alertness
and heightened attention, reflected as neuronal activation in the pre-frontal cortex. Their
results, although based on a case study, provide support for the hypothesis that in very
324 LYNN ET AL.
1988). MacLeod and Sheehan (2003) reported a case study in which a highly sug-
gestible participant succeeded in eliminating the Stroop interference effect. Laurence
et al. (2008) concluded that the preponderance of the evidence suggests that “Hypnotic
inductions increase interference in highs but not in lows. . . . However, when given
suggestions to reduce interference either in hypnosis or post-hypnotically, highs can
reduce interference more than lows” (p. 242). The authors conclude that this implies that
“highs attentional capacities are more ‘flexible’ or at least more responsive to hypnotic
demands/suggestions than those of lows” (p. 242). We suggest that the combination of
a propensity to highly automatized cognition, which facilitates the seamless operation
of response sets and the recruitment of suggestion-related imaginings, combined with
cognitive flexibility and the use of cognitive strategies deployed in response to a variety
of suggestions, may be essential to optimal hypnotic responsiveness.
Default mode and other neural activity and responses to attentional measures may
be assessed in low-hypnotizable individuals who do not respond to the CSTP to deter-
mine whether these individuals can be distinguished on measures of such activity and
attentional processes from individuals who are highly suggestible with and without ben-
efit of CSTP intervention. If reliable differences can be demonstrated, suggesting that
those individuals who fail to show gains post-CSTP show unique patterns of neurocor-
tical functioning, it would strongly implicate some role for identified unique hypnotic
abilities or capacities in determining hypnotic responsiveness, in addition to social and
cognitive variables.
Still, even if reliable neurological correlations of hypnosis (measured pre- and post-
induction) can be identified, they may well be produced by the social and cognitive
variables we have discussed. Moreover, it will also be important to determine whether
any neurocognitive differences identified with this research paradigm stand in a causal
relation with hypnotic responsiveness, or whether hypnotic responsiveness produces
neurocognitive changes. In either case, our model is open to the inclusion or exclu-
sion of variables that future research will—we are confident—demonstrate to interact in
interesting, complex, and highly individualized ways among participants.
HYPNOSIS, SUGGESTION, AND SUGGESTIBILITY 325
This brief overview of our model points to a number of important issues for
future research. When it comes to investigating hypnosis, cognitively and neuro-
physiologically, social-psychological variables are continuously at play modulating the
hypnotic response. The extrinsic and intrinsic influences of these variables are part and
parcel of what the experience of hypnosis is. It thus becomes quite important to com-
pare what happens during hypnosis to what happens in contexts removed from hypnosis.
As far as suggestibility is concerned, either high suggestibles process information differ-
ently out of hypnosis and bring these differences to the hypnotic context (as the relation
between imaginative suggestibility and hypnotic suggestibility suggests) or the demands
of the hypnotic situation triggers in them cognitive processes that may or may not interact
apart from this specific context.
We are confident that hypnosis research, driven by competing theories, will reveal
Downloaded by [steven lynn] at 18:29 20 January 2015
much about human consciousness. After all, hypnosis will continue to captivate the
attention of the psychological community, as hypnosis and hypnotic phenomena pro-
vide a potent demonstration of the impressive plasticity of human consciousness; the
powerful impact of mere words on thoughts, feelings, and actions; and how suggestions
can be used to structure or restructure our imaginings, experiences, and narratives of the
world and ourselves.
References
Barabasz, A. F., & Barabasz, M. (2009). Hypnosis and the brain. In M. R. Nash & A. J. Barnier (Eds.), The
Oxford handbook of hypnosis (pp. 337–364). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Barabasz, A. F., Barabasz, M., Jensen, S. M., Calvin, S., Trevisan, M., & Warner, D. (1999). Cortical event-
related potentials show the structure of hypnotic suggestions is crucial. International Journal of Clinical
and Experimental Hypnosis, 47, 5–22. doi:10.1080/00207149908410019
Baribeau, J., Le Beau, M., Roth, R. M., & Laurence, J. R. (1994, September–October) Hypnotizability and
perceptual automaticity: An “out of context” event related potential investigation. Paper presented at the
Seventh International Congress of Psychophysiology (I.O.P.), Thessaloniki, Greece.
Bloom, P. (2004). Advances in neuroscience relevant to the clinical practice of hypnosis: A clini-
cian’s perspective. Keynote address to the 16th International Congress of Hypnosis and Hynotherapy,
Singapore.
Blum, G. S., & Graef, J. R. (1971). The detection over time of subjects simulating hypnosis. International
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 19, 211–224. doi:10.1080/00207147108407168
Braffman, W., & Kirsch, I. (1999). Imaginative suggestibility and hypnotizability: An empirical analysis.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 578–587. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.3.578
Cangas, A. J., & Pérez, M. (1998). The effect of two procedures on hypnotic susceptibility modification.
Contemporary Hypnosis, 15, 212–218. doi:10.1002/ch.137
Cardeña, E., & Terhune, D. (2014). Hypnotizability, personality traits, and the propensity to experi-
ence alterations of consciousness. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 1(3),
292–307.
Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2010). The unconscious will: How the pursuit of goals operates outside of
conscious awareness. Science, 329, 47–50. doi:10.1126/science.1188595
326 LYNN ET AL.
Davidson, R. J., & Goleman, D. J. (1977). The role of attention in meditation and hypnosis: A psychobiolog-
ical perspective on transformations of consciousness. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Hypnosis, 25, 291–308. doi:10.1080/00207147708415986
De Pascalis, V., Magurano, M. R., & Bellusci, A. (1999). Pain perception, somatosensory event-related-
potentials and skin conductance responses to painful stimuli in high, mid, and low hypnotizable subjects:
Effects of differential pain reduction strategies. Pain, 96, 393–402.
Dixon, M., Brunet, A., & Laurence, J. R. (1990). Hypnotizability and automaticity: Toward a parallel
distributed processing model of hypnotic responding. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99(4), 336.
Dixon, M., & Laurence, J. (1992). Two hundred years of hypnosis research: Questions resolved? Questions
unanswered!. In I. E. Fromm & M. Nash (Eds.), Contemporary hypnosis research (pp. 34–66). New York,
NY: Guilford.
Erickson, M. H., Rossi, E. L., & Rossi, S. I. (1976). Hypnotic realities: The induction of clinical hypnosis
and forms of indirect suggestion. Oxford, UK: Irvington.
Fellows, B., & Ragg, L. (1992). The Carleton Skill Training Program: A preliminary British trial.
Downloaded by [steven lynn] at 18:29 20 January 2015
Kirsch, I., & Lynn, S. J. (1995). The altered state of hypnosis: Changes in the theoretical landscape.
American Psychologist, 50, 846–858. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.50.10.846
Kirsch, I., & Lynn, S. J. (1997). Hypnotic involuntariness and the automaticity of everyday life. American
Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 40, 329–348. doi:10.1080/00029157.1997.10403402
Kirsch, I., & Lynn, S. J. (1998). Dissociation theories of hypnosis. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 100–115.
doi:10.1037/0033-2909.123.1.100
Kirsch, I., & Lynn, S. J. (1999). Automaticity in clinical psychology. American Psychologist, 54, 504–515.
doi:10.1037/0003-066X.54.7.504
Kirsch, I., Mazzoni, G., & Montgomery, G. H. (2007). Hypnotic history: A reply to critics. American
Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 49(4), 249–254.
Kirsch, I., Mobayed, C. P., Council, J. R., & Kenny, D. A. (1992). Expert judgments of hypnosis from
subjective state reports. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101(4), 657.
Laurence, J.-R., Beaulieu-Prevost, D., & Duchene, T. (2008). Measuring and understanding individual dif-
ferences in hypnotizability. In M. R. Nash & A. Barnier (Eds.), Oxford handbook of hypnosis (pp.
Downloaded by [steven lynn] at 18:29 20 January 2015
Lynn, S. J., Vanderhoff, H., Shindler, K., & Stafford, J. (2002). Defining hypnosis as a trance vs. cooperation:
Hypnotic inductions, suggestibility, and performance standards. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis,
44, 231–240. doi:10.1080/00029157.2002.10403483
MacLeod, C. M. (2011). Hypnosis and the control of attention: Where to from here? Consciousness and
Cognition, 20, 321–324. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2009.10.007
MacLeod, C. M., & Sheehan, P. W. (2003). Hypnotic control of attention in the Stroop task: A historical
footnote. Consciousness and Cognition, 12, 347–353. doi:10.1016/S1053-8100(03)00025-4
Mazzoni, G., Venneri, A., McGeown, W. J., & Kirsch, I. (2013). Neuroimaging resolution of the altered
state hypothesis. Cortex, 49(2), 400–410.
McConkey, K. M. (1986). Opinions about hypnosis and self-hypnosis before and after hyp-
notic testing. The International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 34, 311–319.
doi:10.1080/00207148608406996
McGeown, W. J., Mazzoni, G., Venneri, A., & Kirsch, I. (2009). Hypnotic induction decreases ante-
rior default mode activity. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 18, 848–855.
Downloaded by [steven lynn] at 18:29 20 January 2015
doi:10.1016/j.concog.2009.09.001
Meyer, E. C., & Lynn, S. J. (2011). Responding to hypnotic and nonhypnotic suggestions: Performance
standards, imaginative suggestibility, and response expectancies. International Journal of Clinical and
Experimental Hypnosis, 59(3), 327–349.
Nadon, R., Laurence, J.-R., & Perry, C. (1991). The two disciplines of scientific hypnosis: A synergistic
model. In S. J. Lynn & J. W. Rhue (Eds.), Theories of hypnosis: Current models and perspectives (pp.
485–518). New York, NY: Guilford.
Niedzwieñska, A. (2000). Goal-directed fantasy does not explain the training effect of the Carleton
Skills Training Package. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 48, 404–417.
doi:10.1080/00207140008410369
Nordby, H., Jasiukaitis, K., & Spiegel, D. (1999). Effects of hypnotizability on performance of a Stroop task
and event-related potentials. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 88, 819–830. doi:10.2466/pms.1999.88.3.819
Oakley, D. A., & Halligan, P. W. (2010). Psychophysiological foundations of hypnosis and suggestion. In S.
J. Lynn, J. W. Rhue, & I. Kirsch (Eds.), Handbook of clinical hypnosis (pp. 79–177). Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
Pintar, J., & Lynn, S. J. (2008). A brief history of hypnosis. New York, NY: Wiley-Blackwell.
Polito, V., Barnier, A. J., Woody, E. Z., & Connors, M. H. (2014). Measuring agency change across the
domain of hypnosis. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 1, 3–19.
Rainville, P., Duncan, G. H., Price, D. D., Carrier, B., & Bushnell, M. C. (1997). Pain affect
encoded in human anterior cingulate but not somatosensory cortex. Science, 277, 968–971.
doi:10.1126/science.277.5328.968
Raz, A., Kirsch, I., Pollard, J., & Nitkin-Kaner, Y. (2006). Suggestion reduces the Stroop effect.
Psychological Science, 17, 91–95. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01669.x
Raz, A., Shapiro, T., Fan, J., & Posner, M. I. (2002). Hypnotic suggestion and the modulation of Stroop
interference. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 1155–1161. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.59.12.1155
Rubichi, S., Ricci, F., Padovani, R., & Scaglietti, L. (2005). Hypnotic susceptibility, base-
line attentional functioning, and the Stroop task. Consciousness and Cognition, 14, 296–303.
doi:10.1016/j.concog.2004.08.003
Sheehan, P. W. (1991). Hypnosis, context, and commitment. In S. J. Lynn & J. W. Rhue (Eds.), Theories of
hypnosis: Current models and perspectives (pp. 520–541). New York, NY: Guilford.
Sheehan, P. W., Donovan, P., & MacLeod, C. M. (1988). Strategy manipulation and the Stroop effect in
hypnosis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 94, 249–255.
Sheehan, P. W., & McConkey, K. M. (1982). Hypnosis and experience: The exploration of phenomena and
process. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
HYPNOSIS, SUGGESTION, AND SUGGESTIBILITY 329
Spanos, N. P. (1971). Goal-directed fantasy and the performance of hypnotic test suggestions. Psychiatry,
34, 86–96.
Spanos, N. P. (1986). Hypnosis and the modification of hypnotic susceptibility: Social psychological
perspective. In P. Naish (Ed.), What is hypnosis? Philadelphia, PA: Open University Press.
Spanos, N. P., Cross, W. P., Menary, E. P., & Smith, J. (1988). Long term effects of cognitive skill training
for the enhancement of hypnotic susceptibility. British Journal of Experimental and Clinical Hypnosis,
5, 73–78.
Szechtman, H., Woody, E., Bowers, K. S., & Nahmias, C. (1998). Where the imaginal appears real: A
positron emission tomography study of auditory hallucinations. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences, 95, 1956–1960. doi:10.1073/pnas.95.4.1956
Tellegen, A. (1981). Practicing the two disciplines for relaxation and enlightenment: Comment on “role of
the feedback signal in electromyograph biofeedback: The relevance of attention” by Qualls and Sheehan.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 110, 217–226. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.110.2.217
Woody, E. Z., & Szechtman, H. (2000). Hypnotic hallucinations: Towards a biology of epistemology.
Downloaded by [steven lynn] at 18:29 20 January 2015