One Step Forward, Two Steps Back: Transitions Between Home, Pre-Primary and Primary Education in Rural India
One Step Forward, Two Steps Back: Transitions Between Home, Pre-Primary and Primary Education in Rural India
One Step Forward, Two Steps Back: Transitions Between Home, Pre-Primary and Primary Education in Rural India
Education
To cite this article: Benjamin Alcott, Manjistha Banerji, Suman Bhattacharjea, Mansi Nanda &
Purnima Ramanujan (2018): One step forward, two steps back: transitions between home, pre-
primary and primary education in rural India, Compare: A Journal of Comparative and International
Education
ABSTRACT KEYWORDS
A growing evidence base highlights the value of high-quality early Early-childhood education;
childhood education (ECE) to children’s cognitive and social devel- participation; India; Assam;
opment. However, far less is known about how families and chil- Rajasthan; Telangana
dren, especially in developing countries, participate in ECE or how
these participation patterns reflect families’ thinking and decision-
making. This paper utilises a mixed-methods approach to analyse
longitudinal household survey and interview data (on 7336 and
180 children, respectively) from the India Early Childhood
Education Impact study. Our results indicate that children’s parti-
cipation trajectories in the early years (age four to eight) do not
reflect the age or grade norms specified by national educational
policies. And, far from being linear, children’s educational path-
ways entail considerable back and forth between home, preschool
and school. The authors argue that these trajectories reflect both
poor implementation of national norms as well as an inadequate
understanding among both parents and service providers of how
best to support young children’s cognitive development.
Introduction
The increasing emphasis on early childhood education (ECE) in educational policy
debates globally (UNESCO 2015, Goal 4, Target 2) is well founded (for a review, see
Woodhead et al. [2014]). Within the fields of neuroscience and psychology, there is
widespread agreement that the developmental period of infancy is crucial to brain
development (Karoly et al. 1998; Young and Richardson 2007) and the subsequent
acquisition of competencies (Kohlberg 1968; Ramey 1998), meaning that additional
learning during these stages can have long-lasting effects on cognitive and academic
development (Campbell et al. 2001; Shore 1997). Within the field of economics, a
common rationale, stemming from human capital theory (Becker 1980), is that early
cognitive development shapes subsequent opportunities throughout the lifespan
(Cunha et al. 2006; Heckman 2011).
These conceptualisations are backed by empirical evidence from developing coun-
tries, which show that high-quality ECE (e.g. through structured sessions delivered by
CONTACT Benjamin Alcott [email protected] Faculty of Education, University of Cambridge, 184 Hills Road,
Cambridge, CB2 8PQ, UK
© 2018 British Association for International and Comparative Education
2 B. ALCOTT ET AL.
trained staff) can boost development not only in children’s cognitive and social skills,
but also long-term educational, health, economic and labour market outcomes (Engle
et al. 2011; Nonoyama Tarumi, Loaiza, and Engle 2009; Rao et al. 2013). But while there
is already clear evidence on the value of good ECE, and policy in many countries
reflects this knowledge, we know relatively little about the pathways young children in
developing contexts actually take between home, ECE and primary education through
the early years, and how these reflect families’ thinking about the importance of ECE.
While policy-makers may mandate clear transitions at specific developmental stages,
the extent to which such norms are manifested in households’ decision-making is less
clear.
This paper aims to inform research and policy debates on early childhood education
by shedding light on whether, when and why households in rural India make use of
ECE opportunities for their young children. We do this with mixed-methods analysis of
a unique longitudinal dataset conducted in three states: Assam; Rajasthan; and
Telangana. With access to both quantitative and qualitative data, we are able to both
identify distinctive patterns between states with regard to the pathways children take as
well as explore how these differences reflect families’ thinking and decision-making.
in some parts of the country (Government of India 2013b, 16). In other words, ICDS
has focused more on establishing elements of wellbeing that are essential for children to
learn, rather than focusing explicitly on providing opportunities to learn.
Policy attention towards the education component of early childhood development
has increased in recent years, partly as the result of efforts from supranational agencies
to highlight its importance (UNESCO 2015). The National Policy on Early Childhood
Care and Education was approved in 2013 and a national curriculum for ECE was
released soon afterwards, although its implementation on the ground has been slow.
The policy framework in the domain of school education largely ignores children below
school-going age: the 2009 Right of Children to Free and Compulsory Education (RTE)
Act guarantees eight years of free education for children in the 6–14 age group,
recommending only that ‘the appropriate Government may make necessary arrange-
ment for providing free pre-school education’ for children until the age of six
(Government of India 2009, Art. 11). Hence, despite the recent policy changes, govern-
ment provision of ECE in India falls into a grey area between a focus on more
immediate health and nutrition priorities in early childhood development and a focus
on education that begins only from primary school entry.
According to data from the Ministry for Women and Child Development for 2014–
2015, ICDS anganwadis (‘courtyard centres’) reached over 100 million children aged
between zero and six (Government of India 2017), of which more than 36 million were
in the ECE age group of three to six years – more than a third of all children in this age
group (Registrar of India 2011). The true extent of ECE in India though is greater still,
as many children are served by a growing number of private preschool providers, many
of which offer English-medium classes imitating formal primary schooling (Streuli,
Vennam, and Woodhead 2011; Singh and Bangay 2014). Even in rural areas, while
most four-year olds attend anganwadis, a fifth attend private pre-primary centres
(ASER Centre 2017).
Despite the government’s stated intention of harmonising services across all ECE
providers (Government of India 2013a), no comprehensive list of all ECE providers
exists in India. Research evidence on Indian children’s participation in ECE and its
value is also limited. Much of the recent evidence available comes from cross-sectional
data from Jammu and Kashmir (Arora et al. 2006, 2007, Samridhi et al. 2011) and
longitudinal data from Andhra Pradesh (Vennam and Komanduri 2009; Streuli,
Vennam, and Woodhead 2011; Woodhead et al. 2009). Findings by Arora et al.
(2007), Samridhi et al. 2011) suggest that children who attend anganwadis in Jammu
and Kashmir may on average have greater levels of cognitive development than those
who do not, although this should not be misconstrued as a causal effect of anganwadi
attendance. Additionally, a few experimental studies have demonstrated the potential
for ECE participation to have substantial benefits for children’s cognitive development,
for example via parenting training (Nair et al. 2009), oracy programmes (Piramal and
Law 2001) or improved staff training and funding (Ade et al. 2010).
Research on conditions in Andhra Pradesh (now Andhra Pradesh and Telangana),
using data from the longitudinal Young Lives survey, has added to our understand-
ing of the non-uniform nature of ECE participation. Perhaps most importantly, they
find that not only ECE participation, but also the type of ECE provider attended, is
socially stratified (Streuli, Vennam, and Woodhead 2011). Simply put, surveyed
4 B. ALCOTT ET AL.
(1) To what extent do children progress into pre-school and then primary school ‘on
time’, as per national policy norms?
(2) In what ways does the timing of these transitions reflect parents’ thinking about
early childhood education?
Methods
We use a mixed-methods approach to answer these research questions. More specifically,
we consider our analysis to represent a blended research design, wherein we afford two
different methods equal status to explore different aspects of the same phenomena (Greene
2007). To identify patterns in children’s progress into pre-primary and then primary school
(research question 1), we analyse a longitudinal household quantitative dataset. To explore
the thinking and decision-making at the household level that underpin these broader
patterns (research question 2), we use qualitative interview data from households purpo-
sively sampled to provide perspectives from a diversity of household conditions. The aim is
that by building on the distinctive strengths of each approach, the methods provide real
complementarity (Small 2011) in developing our knowledge of ECE participation in India.
For research question 1, we undertake descriptive quantitative analysis of 11 waves of
survey data from the India Early Childhood Education Impact (IECEI) study, which
followed a cohort of children in the states of Assam, Rajasthan and Telangana over a
four-year time frame (September 2011–December 2015). Our analysis focuses on the
7336 children for whom we have participation information for each of the 11 survey
waves. Summary statistics for this sample is presented in Table 1.
Our analysis of the survey data focuses on whether children were participating in an
educational institution on the day of each survey wave and, if participating, the level of the
educational institution (i.e. pre-primary or primary). Information on children’s participa-
tion was triangulated from three sources: parental reports; institutional records; and
surveyors’ direct observations. Thus ‘participation’ as defined in this study includes both
‘official’ or ‘formal’ enrolment as well as ‘unofficial’ or ‘informal’ participation because
children were often found to be going to an institution (ECE centre or school) without
being formally enrolled, and/or enrolled in one institution but attending another.
To explore household decision-making processes behind sampled children’s participa-
tion trajectories (research question 2), we analyse parent interviews undertaken within the
IECEI study at the completion of all 11 waves of the quantitative survey. Fieldwork for the
qualitative component was conducted in a total of 12 sampled villages (four per state),
within which semi-structured interviews were conducted with parents of 180 children who
took part in the quantitative survey: 60 in Assam; 58 in Telangana; and 62 in Rajasthan. In
cases where parents of sampled children were unavailable, the available primary caregiver
or guardian was interviewed. Data from the quantitative survey was used to inform the
selection of both villages and children, with the aim of ensuring a diverse sample in terms of
village, household and child characteristics.
Interviews focused largely on the reasons for sending the sampled child to the institution(s)
she or he attended over the previous five years, and what the parents felt the child had gained
from the experience. Given that the interviews addressed actions taken several years in the
past, the analytical approach sought primarily to identify the kinds of considerations parents
discussed – and, equally, those that were not mentioned. Interviews were conducted in either
the state language or the local dialect, as appropriate. All interviews were then recorded and
transcribed verbatim into English before analysis.
The embedding of the qualitative inquiry within preliminary findings from the
survey data allows us to generate insights that sharpen our understanding of household
decision-making and choice. We use the thematic analytical approach (Braun and
Clarke 2006) to analyse data from these interviews, focusing on two major areas of
inquiry: reasons for choice and experience in pre-primary and school. Interviews were
first coded inductively and thereafter relevant codes were clustered into mutually
compatible categories. This process was iterative and involved delimiting both the
themes as well as the codes up to a point of thematic and coding saturation.
Limitations
This study faces several limitations common to longitudinal research designs.
Regarding the quantitative analysis, as with most studies with multiple survey waves
(Allison 2012), IECEI is prone to considerable sample attrition. This occurred primarily
because enumerators could not locate a child during a particular wave, either because
the child was out of the village on the day of the survey or, in some cases, because the
family had migrated from the village for a certain period of time. Consequently,
whereas the first wave sampled 11,828 children, 7336 of these children participated in
all 11 survey waves. Given that our focus is on how children’s participation changed
during the full survey, for our analysis we concentrate on this restricted sample of 7336
children. By definition, it is not possible to know whether those participating in all 11
waves differed on unobservable characteristics. However, analysis on observable char-
acteristics from the first survey wave suggest at least a reasonable degree of compar-
ability between the groups: there was no statistically significant difference (at the 0.05
level) between the two samples in either caste or household wealth, indicating that
children from marginalised economic and social backgrounds were not more likely to
have been left out across survey waves.
Regarding the qualitative analysis, the retrospective nature of the interviews means
that parents could encounter difficulties in recalling their perspectives and decision-
making processes. Since interviews were conducted at the end of the study, in some
instances (i.e. those relating to survey wave 1) parents would have been asked about
their children’s participation four years earlier. In an effort to circumnavigate this
challenge, interviewers drew on information collected during the survey waves in
COMPARE 7
order to prompt parents’ memories and focus the discussion on concrete events during
the past years. For example, in cases where children had jumped ahead a grade,
interviewers asked parents the following questions:
● After being in [institution name], [child] moved to [institution name] and joined
class [number]. Can you tell me about this change?
● Why did [child] shift grades when s/he changed school?
● Why did you select this particular school instead of the other options available to you
in the village?
● What has [child] experience been at the school? Does s/he like going to school? Did s/
he face any difficulties?
Figure 1. Proportion of children who are in the appropriate grade for age, by age and state.
which children turn six, those who are in Grade 1 are in the appropriate grade for age;
those who are not participating in any educational institution, are in anganwadi or
other ECE classes are ‘behind’; and those who are in Grade 2 or higher are ‘ahead’. The
following school year, children who are in Grade 2 are in the appropriate age for grade,
and so on. Survey waves are depicted in the graphs according to average child age at the
given survey point. The charts highlight that in each state a substantial percentage of
children are ‘behind’ or ‘ahead’ of track for most of the pre-primary and primary cycle.
At the first survey wave (average child age 4.2 years), almost 100% of children in
Assam are in the appropriate grade for age, meaning that they are attending ECE
centres, with Telangana also close to 100%. In contrast, nearly a quarter of children in
Rajasthan are ‘behind’ track, meaning they are not participating anywhere. Another
peculiarity is that around 20% of children in Rajasthan are ‘ahead’ of track, meaning
that they are already attending primary school at age four.
COMPARE 9
Despite state laws permitting entry into primary school at age five, at an average age
of 5.2 years, almost all children in Assam remain in the appropriate grade for age with
respect to RTE norms, i.e. participating in ECE. In contrast, close to half of children in
both Telangana and Rajasthan are ‘ahead’, indicating that state norms permitting earlier
entry into primary school matter far more in these states.
The pattern in Assam changes considerably, though, during the purported pri-
mary-school years. At an average age of 6.2 years, when RTE norms expect children
to be in Grade 1, more than a quarter of children in Assam have fallen ‘behind’,
suggesting they are spending additional years in ECE; the proportion of ‘behind’
children increases further still, covering more than half of children by age 8.2 years.
In Telangana and Rajasthan, on the other hand, from age six onwards a substantial
percentage of children are present in each of the three categories: appropriate grade
for age; ‘behind’; and ‘ahead’. At an average child age of 8.2 years, in Rajasthan
approximately a third of children are in each of the three categories and in
Telangana a quarter are ‘behind’ track, roughly 30% ‘on’ track and the remaining
40% ‘ahead’ of track. While state norms of earlier school-entry age can explain why
a large proportion remain ‘ahead’, those who are behind are conforming neither to
national nor to state policy norms.
To summarise, between age four and eight only a minority of children progress
through pre-primary and primary education in the appropriate grade for age as
per national norms. However, the dynamics of these patterns vary across states. In
Assam, while children are in the appropriate grade for age early on, many fall
‘behind’ in later years. In other words, it appears that children in Assam are
attending ECE for longer than stipulated in RTE norms, leading to delayed entry
into primary education. In contrast, Rajasthan and Telangana are characterised by
many children entering primary school at an early age, as indicated by the large
proportion of children who are ‘ahead’ of track even by age five. Still, quite a
number of children in both states fall ‘behind’, suggesting prolonged stays in ECE
or slow movement through the primary grades, such that by age eight in both
states there are near equal shares of children in the appropriate grade for age,
‘behind’ and ‘ahead’.
These observations are confirmed in Table 2, which shows the proportion of
children who have participated in pre-primary and the proportion of children who
have already participated in primary at average age five (wave three). By this age, as
per RTE norms, all children should have had some pre-primary education but
should not yet have entered primary school. As Table 2 shows, these policy
norms are not followed in practice. In Rajasthan, a fifth of children have not
participated in pre-school by age five, while half have already begun primary
schooling. Telangana has a similar pattern, albeit with fewer children not having
Figure 2. First non-linear grade movement after first entry to primary school, by state.
Note: This figure depicts the first instance of non-linear grade movement after children transition to
primary school for the first time. Many of the children may have multiple instances of non-linear
grade movement.
COMPARE 11
The idea that children below primary-school age should attend an educational
institution is almost universally accepted, though not necessarily ECE
Parental interviews across the three states find close to universal acceptance that young
children below primary-school-going age should be attending an educational institution
rather than just staying at home, corroborating our earlier finding on young children’s
participation trajectories. But parents vary considerably in what they consider the best
type of exposure for their young children.
Systematic governmental outreach initiatives emerge as important processes that
shape parental attitudes with regard to the importance of ECE. The majority of parents
interviewed in Telangana described participation in preschool as ‘the usual path for
young children’; all the parents who offered this explanation had sent their young
children to a government anganwadi as the first step in their educational trajectories,
and most explicitly mentioned the role of the ICDS anganwadi workers (AWWs) in
shaping their view that participation in anganwadi was a prerequisite for enrolment in
primary school. For example, outreach by the AWW tipped the balance for one mother
in Telangana, who explained: ‘He was small and I thought what he will do sitting in the
home . . . and teachers also had come to our house asking to send him to the anganwadi
centre, so I sent him.’ A similar pattern is seen in Assam, where about a third of parents
mentioned being informed by AWWs that children ought to be sent to their local
anganwadi centres. These parents often referred to the child’s preschool as the ‘allotted
centre’, indicating their awareness of the existence and purpose of the local anganwadi
centres in the village.
In Rajasthan, in contrast, several parents spoke of how ‘children [here] go directly to
[Grade] 1’, echoing our quantitative finding that many children participated in primary
school even at age four. No parent in Rajasthan mentioned being influenced by AWWs
or other kinds of outreach. On the contrary, many parents articulated a negative
12 B. ALCOTT ET AL.
that parents’ main considerations are practical ones, to do with children’s physical
readiness on the one hand, and institutional willingness to allow their participation
on the other.
While the timing of a child’s first entry into an educational institution is parent-
dependent, progression through the system is not. Non-linear progression is
often the price of perceived ‘better quality’
The concept of ‘age-appropriate’ grades, so central to education policy, had little
traction among interviewed parents. Within the qualitative sub-sample there are
many cases of children going to anganwadi centres from age two onwards, although
these centres are intended to offer ECE services to children only in the three to six year
old age group. Similarly, many parents send their children to primary school even at age
four, either because the school is the most convenient option or because early entry to
school is perceived as getting a head start on learning.
Once in school, education policy mandates annual progress to the next grade with-
out repetition, all the way through to the end of primary school at Grade 8. In practice,
children’s trajectories in the early years of education often entail grade repetitions,
demotions or out-of-turn promotions, suggesting that not only parents but equally
educational institutions fail to follow the ‘age-appropriate grade’ norm.
Parental interviews reveal that in several cases children first progressed to primary
school only to be demoted again to preschool grades. For instance, in one case from
Telangana, a child joined an anganwadi at age three and after one year was enrolled
into a private preschool where he remained for three consecutive grades – nursery,
lower and upper kindergarten – thus only joining Grade 1 at age seven, one year later
than the RTE norm prescribes. In another case, the child first joined an anganwadi at
age three and after two years progressed to a local government primary school where he
studied for three years. However, his parents expressed dissatisfaction with the quality
of education provided in the government school, commenting that ‘although he did not
study at all they used to promote him to a higher class’. The child was subsequently sent
to a private school where he repeated two years of preschool before progressing to the
primary grades in the same school – thus repeating not only two years in preschool but
also the first three grades of primary school.
We find similar cases in the other two states as well. In Assam, the reason many
children remained in preschool for so long was due to changing centres at least once
and repeating preschool grades in successive institutions. Also in Assam, a child who
joined a government primary school was made to repeat Grade 1 for two consecutive
years after failing to pass the annual examinations at school, despite the official no-
detention policy. In Rajasthan, a child without any preschool exposure who joined a
private school at age four was given an out-of-turn promotion to Grade 2; according to
the mother of the child, this was done at the suggestion of the school principal who
believed that the child was a good student.
In several, if not all such cases, parents were aware of these non-linear trajectories
and cognisant of the child’s apparent learning difficulties or advantages; they usually
accepted the judgement of the school or teacher and their decision to hold back, demote
or promote the child. Changing schools, most often from government to private school,
14 B. ALCOTT ET AL.
but also on occasion from one private school to another, often involved grade repetition
for the child – but far from being viewed as a problem, parents often interpreted grade
repetition as an indicator of institutional quality, in the sense that a better-quality
school was providing remedial education to a child who was not performing to the
requisite level.
Discussion
Table 4 summarises our findings on ECE participation patterns and the key motivators
underlying parents’ decision-making in each state. These findings demonstrate the
differing ways in which families across rural India approach young children’s educa-
tional needs and their perspectives on early childhood education. In Telangana, for
instance, there was a positive perception among parents in the sub-sample regarding
the importance of preschool; preschool education was considered important not only to
habituate and socialise young children, but also to teach them critical skills for school.
These articulations are missing in the interviews with parents in Assam and Rajasthan.
In Rajasthan particularly, we find an absence of community awareness with respect to
ECE and its importance for young children as well as a poor perception of anganwadis
in general.
Perhaps the clearest implication of these findings is that federal mandates on participa-
tion in ECE and primary schooling are largely ignored in rural India, both by parents and
by educational institutions. Only a minority of children in our sample follow RTE norms on
the age at which children should move through ECE and early primary grades. This should
not be misconstrued, though, as households simply following state-policy mandates
instead: in each state, there are both many children behind and many children ahead of
RTE norms, meaning that most are not following state norms either.
In terms of influencing household practices, a factor that appears more impactful
than central government mandates is outreach by local service providers. A strong
community awareness programme, stressing the importance and appropriateness of
early education for young children, can go a long way in creating understanding of and
‘demand’ for ECE among parents, whether in the government or private sector.
Telangana shows that this can be especially influential when the focus is not just
normalising ECE participation, but also disseminating awareness of its value to chil-
dren’s development. Of course, outreach alone is not sufficient: it is equally important
that anganwadis, the largest preschool network in India, are functional with staff and
appropriate infrastructure so that parents are convinced of the value of sending their
children to these institutions.
As shown, the causes of non-linear progress through the school grades are manifold.
Perhaps the most pertinent factor for policy and practice is that instances of non-linear
progress tend to be instigated by schools rather than parents. A common reason is the
reportedly higher standard followed by private schools that often require children to
repeat grades that they have already completed in a government institution. This raises
important questions about policy implementation on the ground, given that these
practices are not permitted by law. In most such cases, when parents accept the decision
of the schools and teachers, this is partly a reflection of the power imbalance between
parents and schools, but often also an expression of parents’ belief in the superiority of
private schools and consequent willingness to adhere to their decisions.
Elements of ECE programmes that are distinctive from primary-school education,
deemed by both international research and national policy to be important indicators of
high-quality ECE, are completely absent from parents’ conceptualisations of quality.
Moreover, the fact that in two of the three states large numbers of underage children
were participating in primary schools suggests that schools themselves may be know-
ingly or unknowingly complicit in contributing to parental perceptions that ECE is
merely a downward extension of primary school. Although mechanisms to regulate
what ECE providers offer to young children are important to put in place, the evidence
presented in this study suggests that without strong efforts to convince both service
providers and parents of the fact that teaching the 3Rs to young children is detrimental
to the latter’s development, regulation alone is unlikely to transform practice on the
ground.
Acknowledgements
All data used in this paper were collected as part of the India Early Childhood Education Impact
study (IECEI), which was conducted by ASER Centre, New Delhi, and the Centre for Early
Childhood Education and Development at Ambedkar University Delhi in partnership with
UNICEF. We are grateful to our reviewing editor, Germ Janmaat, and the three anonymous
referees whose comments helped improve this manuscript. We would like to thank the partners,
field investigators, families and children who made this study possible.
16 B. ALCOTT ET AL.
Disclosure statement
No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.
Funding
The writing of this paper was supported by the British Association for International and
Comparative Education’s (BAICE) Seedcorn Fund.
References
Ade, A., S. S. Gupta, C. Maliye, P. R. Deshmukh, and B. S. Garg. 2010. “Effect of
Improvement of Pre-School Education through Anganwadi Center on Intelligence and
Development Quotient of Children.” The Indian Journal of Pediatrics 77 (5): 541–546.
doi:10.1007/s12098-010-0056-7.
Alcott, B., and P. Rose. 2015. “Schools and Learning in Rural India and Pakistan: Who Goes
Where, and How Much are They Learning?” Prospects 45 (3): 345–363. doi:10.1007/s11125-
015-9350-5.
Ali, S. L. 2015. “Right to Education (RTE) in Context with Pre-Schooling (ECCE).” International
Journal of Innovative Research and Development 4: 10.
Allison, P. 2012. Missing Data. Thousand Oaks: Sage.
Arora, S., S. Bharti, and S. Sharma. 2007. “Comparative Study of Cognitive Development of ICDS
and Non-ICDS Children (3–6 Years)”. Journal of Human Ecology 22 (3): 201–204.
Arora, S., S. Bharti, and A. Mahajan. 2006. “Evaluation of Non-formal Pre-school Educational
Services provided at Anganwadi Centres (Urban Slums of Jammu City).” Journal of Social
Sciences 12 (2): 135–137.
ASER Centre. 2015. Trends over Time 2006–2014; a Supplement to the Annual Status of
Education Report. New Delhi: ASER Centre.
ASER Centre. 2017. Annual Status of Education Report (Rural) 2016. New Delhi: ASER Centre.
Ashley, L. D., C. Mcloughlin, M. Aslam, J. Engel, J. Wales, S. Rawal, R. Batley, G. Kingdon, S.
Nicolai, and P. Rose. 2014. “The Role and Impact of Private Schools in Developing Countries.”
Rigorous Literature Review. London: UK Department for International Development.
Becker, G. 1980. Human Capital: A Theoretical and Empirical Analysis, with Special Reference to
Education (2nd ed.). Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press.
Bhattacharjea, S., R. Banerji, and W. Wadhwa. 2011. Inside Primary Schools: Teaching and
Learning in Rural India. New Delhi: ASER Centre.
Borooah, V. K. 2012. “Social Identity and Educational Attainment: The Role of Caste and
Religion in Explaining Differences between Children in India.” Journal of Development
Studies 48 (7): 887–903. doi:10.1080/00220388.2011.621945.
Braun, V., and V. Clarke. 2006. “Using Thematic Analysis in Psychology.” Qualitative Research
in Psychology 3 (2): 77–101. doi:10.1191/1478088706qp063oa.
Campbell, F. A., E. P. Pungello, S. Miller-Johnson, M. Burchinal, and C. T. Ramey. 2001. “The
Development of Cognitive and Academic Abilities: Growth Curves from an Early Childhood
Educational Experiment.” Developmental Psychology 37 (2): 231. doi:10.1037/0012-
1649.37.2.231.
Cunha, F., J. J. Heckman, L. Lochner, and D. V. Masterov. 2006. “Interpreting the Evidence on
Life Cycle Skill Formation.” Handbook of the Economics of Education 1: 697–812.
Educational Initiatives. 2010. Student Learning Study: Status of Student Learning across 18 States
of India in Urban and Rural Schools. Ahmedabad: Education Initiatives.
Engle, P. L., L. C. H. Fernald, H. Alderman, J. Behrman, C. O’Gara, A. Yousafzai, M. C. de Mello, M.
Hidrobo, N. Ulkuer, and I. Ertem. 2011. “Strategies for Reducing Inequalities and Improving
COMPARE 17
Rao, N., J. Sun, J. Wong, B. Weekes, P. Ip, S. Shaeffer, M. Young, M. Bray, E. Chen, and D. Lee.
2013. Early Childhood Development and Cognitive Development in Developing Countries: A
Rigorous Literature Review. London: DFID.
Reddy, A. N., and S. Sinha. 2010. School Dropouts or Pushouts? Overcoming Barriers for the
Right to Education. New Delhi: National University of Educational Planning and
Administration.
Registrar of India. 2011. “Census of India 2011.” Registrar of India. Accessed 29 June 2018. http://
censusindia.gov.in/
Rolleston, C., and Z. James. 2015. “After Access: Divergent Learning Profiles in Vietnam and
India.” Prospects 45 (3): 285–303. doi:10.1007/s11125-015-9361-2.
Rose, P. M., R. Sabates, B. M. Alcott, and I. S. Ilie. 2017. “Overcoming Inequalities within
Countries to Achieve Global Convergence in Learning.” Background paper for The
International Commission on Financing Global Education Opportunity. http://report.education
commission.org/download/829/
Samridhi, A., B. Madhu, and D. Jyothi. 2011. “An Assessment of Readiness among Children
Attending Anganwadi Centres in Kathua District (J&K).” Asian Journal of Home Science 6 (2):
127–130.
Shore, R. 1997. Rethinking the Brain: New Insights into Early development. New York, NY:
Families and Work Institute.
Singh, R., and C. Bangay. 2014. “Low Fee Private Schooling in India–More Questions than
Answers? Observations from the Young Lives Longitudinal Research in Andhra Pradesh.”
International Journal of Educational Development 39: 132–140. doi:10.1016/j.
ijedudev.2014.08.004.
Small, M. L. 2011. “How to Conduct a Mixed Methods Study: Recent Trends in a Rapidly
Growing Literature.” Annual Review of Sociology 37: 57–86.
Streuli, N., U. Vennam, and M. Woodhead. 2011. Increasing Choice or Inequality? Pathways
through Early Education in Andhra Pradesh, India. The Hague: Bernard van Leer.
UNESCO. 2015. Thematic Indicators to Monitor the Education 2030 Agenda. Paris: UNESCO.
Vennam, U., and A. Komanduri. 2009. Young Lives Qualitative Research: Round 1–India.
Technical Note 21, Oxford: Young Lives.
Woodhead, M., I. Feathersone, L. Bolton, and P. Robertson. 2014. Early Childhood Development:
Delivering Inter-Sectoral Policies, Programmes and Services in Low-Resource Settings. Oxford:
Health & Education Advice & Resource Team (HEART).
Woodhead, M., M. Frost, and Z. James. 2013. “Does Growth in Private Schooling Contribute to
Education for All? Evidence from a Longitudinal, Two Cohort Study in Andhra Pradesh,
India.” International Journal of Educational Development 33 (1): 65–73. doi:10.1016/j.
ijedudev.2012.02.005.
Woodhead, M., P. Ames, U. Vennam, W. Abebe, and N. Streuli. 2009. Equity and Quality?
Challenges for Early Childhood and Primary Education in Ethiopia, India and Peru. The
Hague: Bernard van Leer.
Woodhead, M., P. Dornan, and H. Murray. 2013. What Inequality Means for Children: Evidence
from Young Lives. Oxford: Young Lives.
Xinyi, Y. D., and W. Tze-Peng. 2015. “Bridging the Research and Cultural-Practice Gap in Early
Language Intervention in Malaysia.” Jurnal Sains Kesihatan Malaysia (Malaysian Journal of
Health Sciences) 13 (2): 11–18.
Young, M. E., and L. M. Richardson. 2007. Early Child Development from Measurement to
Action: A Priority for Growth and Equity. Washington, DC: World Bank Publications.