Depersonalization Syndrome
Depersonalization Syndrome
Depersonalization Syndrome
AN O V E R V I E W
Evan M. Torch, M . D .
There are few disorders in the realm of psychiatric research which have
attracted the attention displayed towards depersonalization as a symptom, a
syndrome or metaphysical phenomenon. T h r o u g h every era, including our
own"biologically centered" age, prominent authors seem to have been highly
interested in examining this complex state from several vantage points, and
writing about the subject in early psychoanalytic studies took on a rich and
multivaried air.
M a n y reasons for this interest would seem to offer themselves up for
analysis, but the chief explanation can perhaps be found in the difficulty which
the occurrence, undeniable as it is in the psychic content of patient and therapies
alike, presents in definition. The therapist must identify as a disorder a group of
facts which the patient relates in an "as if" manner, with both detached and
paradoxically intense affects, an obsessional persistance, and in such terms as to
suggest a close relation to what often seems to be more philosophical perspective
than psychiatric enigma. Indeed, "noticing o n e ' s own actions and thoughts" is
one prerequisite for h u m a n intelligence, so it would seem perhaps predictable
that either everybody should be occasionally depersonalized or odd that anyone
ever would. This is not to deny the certain existence of varied mechanisms of
dissociation; but the question must at least be broached as to why the symptoms
cause the patient such consternation and suffering.
O n e of the greatest difficulties presented by the subject of depersonalization
is found in the attempt to unravel depersonalization as a specific, d o m i n a n t
aspect of a syndrome. This problem seems to be especially prominent in
American diagnostic itemization, as was seen in the preliminary draft of D S M -
I I I 1, where the differentiation was made on the frequency of occurrences in a
specified period of time. This m a y be partly due to Dixon's 2 welt publicized study
where a high proportion o f " n o r m a l " college students experienced the symptom,
or the fact that most textbooks seem to preface discussions of depersonalization
with reminders about its reported concomitant appearance in so m a n y other
Dr. Torch is Unit Director, Georgia Mental Health Institute and Assistant Clinical Professor of
Psychiatry, Emory University School of Psychiatry, Emory University School of Medicine. Reprint
requests should be addressed to Dr. Torch at 2151 Peachford Road, Atlanta, GA 30338.
PSYCHIATRIC QUARTERLY, 53(4) Winter, 1981 249
0033-2720/81/1600-0249500.9501981 Human Sciences Press
250
PSYCHIATRIC QUARTERLY
disorders. It is still not entirely clear to the author why there needs to be such an
arbitrary line drawn between the two presentations, since, for example,
depression is treated as a primary factor when it is the primary presenting factor
in a disorder. Yet depression is an accompanying aspect of m a n y other neurotic
and psychotic syndromes. Depersonalization, then, should be treated as a
primary causative factor in a patient's suffering when it is foremost in a clinical
picture, and it is not obviously undergoing rapid alteration or fluctuation with
other primary symptoms. It is important to note, however, that compulsivity
(especially rumination) and hypochondriacal self-scrutiny are often parts of the
syndrome, and may be mistaken for an obsessional or hypochondriacal disorder.
In a previous article on the subject, the author ~ listed four characteristics
which are important in delineating depersonalization syndrome:
1. A feeling of change throughout, of estrangement from the self, and
usually, though not always, a feeling of total change in the subjective perception
of the external world.
2. There is a distinct feeling of unreality present, which shades most of the
patient's clinical picture. Despite the feelings of unreality present, the feelings are
perceived and described in a non-delusional and ego-dsytonic manner, with an
"as if" quality to them.
3. The patient interprets the feelings as being distinctly unpleasant.
4. There is a perplexing and curious subjective report (usually not verified in
objective testing) of a change, usually a diminution, of affect, though the patient
will remain quite able to experience discomfort in his depersonalization.
Patients, especially those less verbal or educated, seem to have a great deal
of difficulty in describing these feelings, very often leading to extreme
exasperation in patient and physician and physician.
Metaphysical associations are usually put forth:
"There is a veil--I can't seem to get hold of the world--it's as ~ I were dead, but
none of it is concrete."
"None of this makes any sense. I laugh but it's like someone else is laughing. It is as
though there is part of me watching and part of me doing it."
" t can't seem to totally wake up. Everything suddenly changed, but I don't know
how. I hate this. There's no use in being alive if my central flame has gone out. It's
almost like I'm dead."
From these patient descriptions, it quickly becomes apparent that the patient
is experiencing quite severe problems in describing a seemingly polar state of af-
fairs, giving rise to intense anguish and an altered state of experiencing internal
and external equilibrium. Yet these are feelings born of seemingly innocuous
observational events, tbr we are all capable of observing our own activities, be
they psychic or "automatic movements", without experiencing this distress.
Roberts 4 provides us with a dynamic reflection of this point in one of his pub-
lished accounts of students experiencing depersonalization (from a paper bearing
the title, interestingly enough, " N o r m a l and Abnormal Depersonalization").
E.M. TORCH
words I was saying to myself, " I am a human being and my name is S..., but
whereas this would normally have been a joke or a tautology because I could not
imagine anything else, it here took on significance because of having a feeling of
strangeness--that is, that it could be questioned."
A PREFORMED RESPONSE
Doubt or uncertainty is the key word even for patients with depersonalization. It is
also the patient with obsessive doubt and related anancasms who most exhibits the
feeling of unreality visa vis the "self"...Such connections were so common in my
material that it appears difficult to distinguish between anancasms and
252
PSYCHIATRIC QUARTERLY
When one takes into account such characterizations as these and the high
incidence of depersonalization in the normal populace, it is not surprising that
many" authors postulate an organic basis or substrate for depersonalization.
Reed 12, for example, speaks of a "preparatory set" personality, and Davison's 1~
patients seemed to exhibit specific, though minor, EEG changes, (a factor which
is often reported anecdotally in isolated cases): a slight excess of slower rhythms,
increasing with attacks of depersonalization, together with slowed alpha rhythm
(though this fits well, especially in his material, with depressive pictures).
Now let us examine some of the descriptions of the interictal behavior of
many temoral lobe seizure patients, Waxman and Geschwind 14 claim to have
identified a certain set of behavior patterns which, at least on the surface, bear a
striking similarity to the behavior of many patients with depersonalization
syndrome. Their patients seem to be more prone to religiousity, hypergraphia, a
certain cosmic sense of fate and a preoccupation with detail. Bear 15 in postulating
a syndrome of sensory-limbic hyperconnection, finds that while patients with
right focal lesions seemed to be more "externally emotive (aggressive, depressed,
emotionally labile)," patients with left temporal foci "developed an internal,
ideational-suggestively verbal-pattern of behavior traits (religiousity, philo-
sophical interest, personal destiny, hypergrahia). Even more striking is this
profile of the left temporal epileptic's interictal behavior described by Bear and
Fedio: 16 ,, Left temporal patients were identified with a sense of personal destiny
and a concern for meaning and significance behind events. Related items
emphasized powerful forces working with one's life (paranoia) and the need for
sober intellectual and moral self-scrutiny (humorlessness, conscientousness)."
Were it not for the factual and long known association between
depersonalization and temporal lobe epilepsy, these reports would seem to offer
little more than an interesting similarity in symptomatology. But the equal
similarity in the presenting personality descriptions of patients with
depersonalization disorder and temporal lobe epilepsy would lead to at least
wonder about the possibility of a certain peculiarity in temporal construction or
subtle alteration in ability to process external reality. According to VVaxman and
Geschwind: 1~
The available evidence suggests that spike discharges in the temporal lobe and
related structures may have preformed effects on behavior. For example, there is
evidence that amygdaloid activity may modulate the secretion of gonadotrophin and
adrenocorticotrophin (Eleftherious et.al. 17, Zolovick18) and that gonadal hormones
in turn alter neural thresholds invoking the amygdala and rhinencephalic pathways
(Kawakami and Sawyer19). Experimental evidence also suggests differential
facilitating and inhibitory effects on stimulation of various regions within the
amygdala on hypothalamically elited behavior in cats (Egger and Flynn2°). On the
basis of these and similar finds, we might in theory expect temporal lobe stimulation
to alter limbic function in a relatively immediate fashion.
253
E.M. TORCH
The implication of this highly interesting theoretical association is not that there
is a direct correlation between temporal lobe disorder and depersonalization per
se, but that perhaps there is a particular neural substrate which could make one
more reliable to experience these particular symptoms, and that a constant
discharge from a temporal lobe focus into the limbic system could alter perception
of the conjunction between an external event and its "properly quantitative"
elicited emotional reaction. Certainly depersonalization is present in so many
other settings that one would be highly remiss in hssuming a point to point
concurrence; the issue of lateralization, much less than of cerebral lesion focus
and particular corresponding disorder, is still very much open to debate. And the
presentation of temporal lobe epilepsy and depersonalization syndrome is
dissimilar in important respects (Roth and Harper 21) among them lack of
episodes of unconsciousness in the great majority of depersonalization patients,
sex incidence, age of onset, incidence of serious illnesses anf family history. But
on the basis of the close similarity of the reported behavioral pattern of many
temporal lobe epileptics and that of many patients with depersonalization
syndrome, it would seem that there is at least enough evidence to merit the strong
suspicion of an association between a particular temporal substrate and
syndrome occurrence. The term substrate is, of course, not at all encased within
the confines of abnormality, and may reflect a sharpening of classically left
temporal, more "analytical" modes of handling a particular conflict.22
OBSESSIONALISM
PSYCHIATRIC QUARTERLY
All depersonalized patients observe themselves continuously and with great zeal;
they compare their present dividedness-within-themselves with their previous
oneness-with-themselves. Self observation is compulsivewith these patients. The
tendency to self-observation continuously rejects the tendency to live, and we may
say it represents the internal negation of experience.
E.M. TORCH
DEPRESSION
PSYCHIATRIC QUARTERLY
PERSISTENCE
E.M. TORCH
REFERENCES
1. American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual III (Preliminary Draft).
Washington, DC, APA, 1978.
2. Dixon J: Depersonalization phenomena in a sample population of co]lege students. Br J
Psychiatry t09:371-375, 1963.
3. Torch E: Review of the reIationship between obsession and depersonalization. Ac~a Psychiatr
Stand 58:191-198, 1978.
4. Roberts W: Normal and abnormal depersonalization. J ?dent Sci 106:478-493, 1960.
5. Janet P: Les obsessions et la psychosthewie. Paris, Alean, 1903.
6. Mayer-Gross: On depersonalization. BritJMed Psychol 15:t03-126, 1935,
7. Penfield W, Jasper HH: Highest level seizures. Ass Res Nerv ?dent Dis Proc 26:252-27t, t947.
8. Kenna JC, Sedman G: Depersonalization in temporal lobe epilepsy and the organic psychoses.
BrJPsychiat 111:293-299, 1965.
9. Slater E, Beard AW, Glithero E: The schizophrenia-llke psychoses of epilepsy. B r J Psychiat
109:95-150, 1963.
10. Slater E, Roth M: ClinicalPsychiatry. Baltimore, Williams &Wilkins, 1969, p. 121.
11. Skoog G: Onset of anacastic conditions. Acta PsychiatrScand, Supplement 184:67-78, 1965.
12. Reed G: Prepatory set as a factor in the production of sensory deprivation phenomena. ProcRoy
Soc Med 55:1010-1014, 1962.
13. Davison K: Episodic depersonafization. BrJ Psychia~ 1I0:505-513, 1964.
258
PSYCHIATRIC QUARTERLY
14. Waxman S, Geschwind N: The interictal behavior syndrome of temporal lobe epilepsy. Arch Gen
Psychiat 32:1580-1586, 1975.
15. Bear D: Temporal lobe epilepsy--a syndrome of sensory-limbic hyperconnection. In Renewal in
Psychiatry."A CriticalPerspective. Edited by Manschreck T, Kleinman A. Washington, Hemisphere
Publishing, 1977.
16. Bear D, Fedio P: Quantitative analysis of interictal behavior in temporal lobe epilepsy. Arch Neur
34:454-467, 1977.
17. Eteftheriou B, Zolovick A, Norman R: Effects of amygdaloid lesions in plasma and pituitary
levels of leuteinizing hormone in the male deer mouse. J Endocrin 38:469-474, 1967.
18. Zolovick A: Effects of lesions and electrical stimulation of the amygdala on hypothalmic-hypo-
physeal regulation. In Neurology of the Amygdala. Edited by Eleftheriou B., New York, Plenum
Press.
I9. Kawakami M, Sawyer C: Neuroendocrine correlates of danger in brain activity thresholds by
sex steroids and pituitary hormones. Endocrin 65:652-668, 1959.
20. Egger M, Flynn J: Effects of electrical stimulation of the amygdala on hypothalmically elicited
attack behavior on cats. J Neurophysiot 26:705-720, 1963.
21. Roth M, Harper M: Temporal lobe epilepsy and the phobic-anxiety depersonalization syn-
drome II. Compr Psychiat 3:2t5-226, 1962.
22. Galin D: Implications for Psychiatry of left and right cerebral specialization. Arch Gen Psychiat
31:572-583, 1974.
23. Schilder P: The Image and Appearance of the Human Body. New York, International Universities
Press, 1950, pp. 138-141.
24. Klimes K: Die Hemmung als storung des Personlichkeitsbewusstseins. Archivesfor Psychiatricund
Nervenkrankheiten 116:151-162, 1943.
25. Sedman G: An investigation of certain factors concerned in the etiology of depersonalization.
Acta Psychiatr&and 48:191-219, 1972,
26. Shorvon H: The depersonalization syndrome. ProcRoy Soc Medicine 39:779-792, 1946.
27. Roth M: The phobic-anxiety depersonalization syndrome. Proc Roy Soc Medicine 52:587-595,
1959.
28. Sedman G, Reed G: Depersonalization phenomena in obsessional personalities and in depres-
sion. BrJ Psychiat 109:376-379, 1963.
29. Sedman G, Kenna J: Depersonalization and mood changes in schizophrenia. Br J Psychiat
109:669-673, 1963.
30. Videbech T: Psychopathology of anancastic endogenous depression. Acta Psychiatr &and
52:336-373, 1975.
31. Klein D: Delination of two drug responsive anxiety syndromes. Psychopharmacologia(Berl.)
5:397-408, 1964.
32. Lion E: Anancastic depression. J Nerv Ment Dis 95:730-738, 1942.
33. McCranie EJ: The neurotic process, Psychiat Q 44'.422-434, 1970.
34. Brauer R, Harrow M, Tucker G: Depersonalization phenomena in psychiatric patients. BrJ
Psychiat 117:509-515, 1970.