Bowl by 1963
Bowl by 1963
Bowl by 1963
CHILDHOOD hlOURNING i
. T h e substance of this paper was given as the Third Alexander Lecture to the
Chicago Institute of Psychoanalysis on May 4, 1961.
I am much indebted to James Robertson for the observations on which I have
drawn. T h e enquiry was undertaken as part of the work of the Tavistock Child
Development Research Unit, which is supported by the National Health Service
and by grants from the Foundations Fund for Research in Psychiatry and the Ford
Foundation, to each of which our thanks are due.
500
PATHOLOGICAL AND CHILDHOOD MOURNING 501
phase yearning and an angry effort to recover the lost object seem
to be the rule. T h e hypothesis was advanced that, so far from
being pathological, an open expression of sych angry striving to
recover the object is a sign of health and-that it enables the
bereaved gradually to relinquish the object. TVhat seems to char-
acterize much pathological mourning is an inability to accept and
express this striving; instead, it becomes repressed and unconscious
and so, insulated from change, persists.
If grief itself is to be regarded as a disease, as Engel (1 1) has
persuasively argued, how, it may be asked, are the terms “healthy”
and “pathological” best applied? Engel’s own analogies are useful.
If the experience of loss is likened to the experience of being
wounded or being burned, the processes of mourning that follow
loss can be likened to the processes of healing that follow a wound
or a burn. Such healing processes, we know, may take a course
which in time leads to full, or nearly full, function being restored:
or they may, on the contrary, take one of many courses each of
which has as its outcome an impairment of function of greater
or less degree. In the same way, processes of mourning may take
a favorable course that leads in time to restoration of function,
namely, to a renewal of the capacity to make and maintain love
relationships, or they may take a course that leaves this function
impaired in greater or less degree. Just as the terms healthy and
pathological are applicable to the different courses taken by heal-
ing processes, so may they be applied to the different courses run
by mourning processes.
Four main variants of pathological response that are shown
by bereaved adults will be considered. I shall begin with those clin-
ical conditions, often suffused with anxiety and depression, in
which a persistent and unconscious yearning to recover the lost
object is most evident. A main reason for starting here is that an
unconscious urge to recover the lost object is a dynamic that
probably is present or at least latent in all the other pathological
variants of mourning but, because more deeply repressed, is more
difficult to see. Moreover, little attention hitherto seems to have
been directed to it, and much in my thesis rests on a clear under-
standing of its role.
T h e second variant to be considered is intense and persistent
502 JOHN BOIVLBY
other and with reality though these responses appear, and in some
respects indeed are, it is my hypothesis that if mourning is to run
a healthy course, it is necessary that they be experienced and
expressed. TVhen they are not, it is suggested, reality testing is
more likely to fail and the unrealistic demand for the object’s
return to live on at an unconscious level. Certainly this is charac-
teristic of many cases of pathological mourning.
A case of neurosis in an adolescent girl reported by Root
(39) has many features which‘appear to be fairly typical. T h e
condition, he believed, stemmed from unsatisfactory mourning
following the death in an accident of the patient’s mother when
the patient herself was aged ten. T h e main symptoms were those
of anxiety and depression, with inhibition, indecision, nausea and
sickness. “In the analysis,” it is reported, “she could not at first
comprehend, even intelIectually, that she missed her mother.” I n
her dreams and fantasies, on the other hand, mother constantly
appeared. In some of them there was a happy reunion with her
mother; i n others the picture of her mother in a sanatorium, or a
frightening scene of her mother “with the gory signs of head
and face injuries.” From such evidence we may conclude that,
unknown to herself, this girl was still in the first phase of mourn-
ing and still unconsciously striving for reunion with her lost
mother. This view is supported by a remark she made when well
on in the analysis: “she spoke of grieving as ‘letting her mother
die.’ ” Only after she could accomplish the task of mourning her
mother’s death, Root reports, did improvement in her condition
develop.
There are two points in this girl’s history which are of inter-
est. I n the first place her initial reaction to her mother’s death
appears to have been one of incredulity and denial, and there had
been n o expression of grief. At the same time there was much
concern for vicarious figures: “she could shed tears for an
orphaned beggar girl,” and, during the analysis, “often displaced
her sadness onto something else or felt sad for someone else”;
moreover, her marriage at seventeen had been to a man who, like
herself, had lost his mother when he was aged ten.
T h e second point refers to the patient’s early childhood. Her
mother had continued working and the patient had been looked
PATHOLOGICAL AND CHILDHOOD MOURNING 507
after by maids. How many is not stated, but when one to whom
she had been attached left when she was seven, there seems to
have been a “tearful parting” which in the analysis “was easily
recalled with much emotion and weeping.” T h e question there-
fore arises whether perhaps this patient had had experiences in
the earlier years that had predisposed her to respond to her
mother’s death with a pathological form of mourning, amounting
to an “absence of grief.” This is what my theoretical position
leads me to predict.
Two of the three cases reported by Joan Fleming et al. (13)
show many resemblances to this case of Root’s. T h e first is of a
woman of thirty complaining of anxiety and depression precipi-
tated by failure in an examination and disappointment in a love
affair. At fifteen years she had left Germany as a refugee and three
years later had heard of her father’s death: the fate of her mother
remained uncertain. Although she insisted that she “never
mourned them and never seemed to miss them,” in her dreams
“she cried as she rushed to meet them.” Because she was tearful
when reminded of her parents, “she fought against this response
by becoming angry and condemning of the grief responses of
others.” Although in analysis she protested her ability to cope
without help, later she admitted: “Time stopped for me at fif-
teen.” T h e second patient, a man of twenty-three, showed many
of the same features.
I n both these patients the repression of yearning for the
lost object is clear enough, and i n neither does the analyst seem
to have had too great a difficult in enabling the patient once
more to experience it and thereby to get over it. I n other cases
the unconscious yearning is more deeply repressed and therefore
less evident. Thus Helene Deutsch (10) reports the case of a man
whose personality had developed in such a way that, but for a
fantasy about a dog, it would be very difficult to convince a skeptic
that unconscious yearning was really present.
Helene Deutsch’s patient was in his early thirties when,
without apparent neurotic difficulties, he came into analysis for
nontherapeutic reasons. T h e clinical picture was one of a wooden
and aff ectionless character. She describes how “he showed com-
plete blocking of affect without the slightest insight. I n his limit-
508 TOHN BOWLBY
As a result the anger and reproach felt toward the loved object
cease to be conscious and instead persist unconsciously.
Although this hypothesis regarding the origin of pathological
self-reproach has been widely accepted by analysts, there are some
who have attenuated or even at times reversed it; for example,
Melanie Klein and her colleagues and also Weiss (44)have empha-
sized instead the degree to which reproach against the self is
appropriate and reproach against the object not so. T h e position
I am adopting is near to Freud’s; it expands his hypothesis by
putting it into a more generalized form.
As we have seen, depending on the circumstances of each
case and the degree of understanding of the bereaved, blame for
loss of object may reasonably be attributed by him to one or more
of several persons: the object, the self, and third parties. This
being so, in principle reproach directed toward any one person
may be shifted away from that person toward any other. In prac-
tice, of course, such shifts tend to be of particular kinds and are
usually designed to spare either the self or the object. T h e com-
monest are away from the self and toward third parties, and away
from the object and toward the self. XVhen the self is spared
blame, the clinical picture is paranoid; when the object is spared
it is depressive. I n each case, it is held, the pathology lies not i n
the existence of anger and reproach but in their displacement
away from an appropriate object toward an inappropriate one.
As a result angry and reproachful feeling toward the original
object become unconscious.
This leads to a consideration of the place of guilt. Within
the context sketched, guilt over loss is seen to be generated i n at
least three different ways. First, there is guilt that springs from a
more or less realistic appraisal of the part the bereaved himself
has played in the loss, and may be large or small according to
circumstances. This was clearly recognized by Freud: there are,
he remarks in discussing the self-reproaches of melancholics, “a
few genuine self-reproaches . . . scattered among those that have
been transposed back” (19, p. 248). Secondly, there is the rather
less realistic guilt that arises from the bereaved knowing consciously
or unconsciously that, even if he is not in reality responsible for
the loss, he has often wished the lost object away. h4uch neurotic
512 JOHN BOIZILBY
active belief in the living existence of the figure who has been
lost, albeit combined with a public acceptance of the reality of
the loss. Since it is a state of mind that was demonstrated very
vividly by a mother, Mrs. Q., to whom I gave once-a-week treat-
ment over a long period, I shall draw on her case for illustration.4
After making good progress over a period of three years,
during which her very intense ambivalence toward each of her
parents (overtly positive to her mother and overtly negative to
her father) was partially analyzed, Mrs. Q. lost her father unex-
pectedly following an elective operation. Her first intimation of
anything untoward was a message from the hospital that he had
died suddenly in the night. During the ensuing months she was
again intensely depressed, again had ideas of suicide, and described
also symptoms of depersonalization. It was unfortunate that at
the time of the loss treatment was suspended owing to my absence,
and not until the anniversary of the loss was drawing near did
she confess to me certain feelings and ideas she had been hiding
but which throughout seem to have been conscious.
During the weeks following her father’s death, she now
told me, she had lived in the half-held conviction that the hospital
had made a mistake in identity and that any day they would
phone to say he was alive and ready to return home. Furthermore,
she had felt specially angry with me because of a belief that,
had I been available, I would have been able to exert an influence
on the hospital and so enabled her to recover him. Now, twelve
months later, these ideas and feelings persisted. She was still half-
expecting a message from the hospital, and she was still angry
with me for not approaching the authorities there. Secretly, more-
over, she was still making arrangements to greet her father on his
return. This explained why she had been so angry with her mother
4Mrs. Q. had been referred for help with her little boy of eighteen months
because he had been refusing to eat and was seriously underweight, and her own
emotional disturbance had seemed to be contributing to his condition. This had
been confirmed by psychiatric investigation which had found her to be in a sub-
acute agitated depression with ideas of suicide and of killing the baby. a condition
that appeared to have developed during the puerperium. For many years previously,
however, she had had isolated episodes when she went berserk and threw saucepans
and crockery about her flat. Othenvise she was a conscientious housewife, notably
house-proud, excessively shy, and attached to her own mother with pathological
intensity. She was thirty-five a t the time of referral.
518 JOHN BOWLBY
for redecorating the flat in which the old people had lived together
and why too she had continued to postpone having her own flat
redecorated: it was vital, she felt, that when at last her father did
return he should find the places familiar. ,Nevertheless she was
not deluded. Side by side with the dynamic system concerned to
recover her absent father was a recognition that he was dead and
never would return.
Among similar patients described i n the literature, Krupp
(30) reports the case of another mother, also i n her early thirties.
Here again there were difficulties with the children, and it was
on this account that she had been referred to a social agency. At
the age of fifteen years this woman had lost her father overseas.
A few months later she had become promiscuous, but shortly
afterward had married. From what she said it seemed that, from
the first, she had refused to believe her father was dead and so
had experienced no grief. For the next eighteen years, moreover,
she had persisted in the illusion that her father might still be
alive; accordingly she was forever searching for him, for example,
i n restaurants and other public places. For a time she had had an
intensely emotional but platonic extramarital relationship with a
man who reminded her of her father; and at the time of referral
she was still excessively preoccupied in fantasy with him, to the
neglect of her'children. I n the course of casework, she began to
accept the idea that her father was really dead and for the first
1
time began to cry and go through the work of mourning. Follow-
ing this, fantasies of her extramarital relationship diminished and
she developed a more satisfactory relationship with her chiIdren.
I n the method adopted for meeting loss the mental state of
this patient closely resembles that of Mrs. Q.By both of them the
object is recognized to be missing but is judged nevertheless to be
recoverable. Given energy and perseverance, the husband or father
can be found again and reunion attained. There is loss admittedly,
but it is only temporary. I t is the permanence of the loss that is
denied.
A variant of this is when the bereaved develops a fantasy
that, despite everything, he is still in touch with the object. Pol-
lock (35) refers to three such cases, all adults, and each of whom
PATHOLOGICAL AND CHILDHOOD MOURNING 519
between the ages of about six months and three or more years
are removed from their mother figures and placed with strangers.
I n particular I emphasized that yearning for the lost object is
not only common but also that it is both more intense and more
prolonged than is generally supposed. Its relative neglect is no
accident, however. On the one hand, the adults in charge of a
child find this yearning painful to perceive; on the other, it is
subject to processes of distortion and repression which quickly
obscure the scene. Thus Laura, aged two years four months, the
subject of Robertson’s film A Two-Year-Old Goes to HospitaZ,
“mould interpolate without emotion and as if irrelevantly the
words ‘I want my hfummy, where has my Mummy gone?’ into
remarks about something quite different” (37, 38). At other times
the wish was displaced to another object, as when she chanted,
“I want to see the steam roller, I want to see the steam roller,
I want to see my hlummy, I want to see the steam roller.”
Distortions of these kinds start early in the separation period
and progress fairly rapidly to a state in which it is only the sensi-
tive observer who will notice them. For example, it will be recalled
how a small boy, Patrick, aged just over three years, after he had
been i n the Hampstead Nurseries for some weeks, “would stand
somewhere in a corner moving his hands and lips with an abso-
lutely tragic expression on his face.” By this time the movement
of hands and lips had ceased to signify anything to anyone unfa-
miliar with the history of their development. Those who were
familiar with it, however, knew that the lip movements were the
remnants of a formula that earlier he had repeated over and over
again-of how his mother would come for him, would put on
his overcoat, and take him home with her; and that the hand
movements were the fragments of a mime in which he had dem-
onstrated how she would put on his overcoat, zip up the zipper,
and put on his pixie hat.s T h e yearning for the lost object per-
sisted but was in course of undergoing repression and so of becom-
ing unrecognizable. A record of this kind is of special value for
the light it throws on cases i n which the process has already pro-
gressed far, for example, the male patient reported by Helene
6 The material on both Laura and Patrick is reported in (4, pp. 22-23).
PATHOLOGICAL AND CHILDHOOD MOURNIKG 523
Deutsch who had ceased to yearn for his mother and had instead
taken to leaving his bedroom door open in the hope that the large
dog would come to him. In all such cases the yearning persists;
but the object of it becomes obscured and changed, and the yearn-
ing itself may become repressed and unconscious.
JVhen eventually this repressed yearning for the mother comes
into the open again the full force of the affect and of the motiva-
tion of which it is a part becomes apparent. In the adult this will
happen in the course of successful analysis. Angry reproaches for
being deserted will alternate with sorrowful yearning for the
object lost, a sequence that is likely first to be experienced within
the transference. In the case of young children whose separation
has been of limited duration the affect and motivation will become
apparent soon after return home. If he has been away only a few
weeks, the usual sequence is as follows: initially he seems strangely
remote from his mother. In spite of the angry protests at her
departure and tearful yearning for her return that he showed
following separation, now that he has recovered her he seems
unmoved. He may fail to recognize her or simply avoid her;
alternatively he may respond to her as though he were an auto-
maton. Sooner or later, however, his remoteness ceases and is .
replaced by clinging, often accompanied by a torrent of tears and
reproaches. Thenceforward he insists on never letting his mother
out of his sight. Below the falade of remoteness the yearning for
the lost object has lived on as a dynamic, ready for active expres-
sion as soon as external conditions permit. It is a mental state
identical with that seen in the adult patient in all features save
its reversibility: whereas in the adult patient suffering from patho-
logical mourning w e know it to be very slow and difficult to
reverse, in the young child, provided the separation has been
limited to weeks only and has not been repeated, it usually reverses
spontaneously after a few hours or days.
. It is because the yearning for the lost object remains an active
dynamic that I find myself unable to agree with the conception
of the process recently proposed by Anna Freud (15). In her com-
ments on my earlier paper, while agreeing that the responses to
separation can usefully be divided into three phases, protest,
524 JOHN BOWLBY
Review of Literature
I n an early section of the previous paper (5) I discussed seven
themes in the theory of mourning about which there had been
controversy in the psychoanalytic literature.. In considering the
sixth, ways in which pathological mourning is believed to differ
from healthy, I gave reasons for not accepting the criteria for
pathological mourning that have commonly been advanced,
prominent among which has been anger and reproach against the
lost loved object; and in later parts of that paper and earlier
parts of this I have explored other criteria. It is time now to
consider what place has hitherto been given in the psycho-
analytic literature to the first of the new criteria proposed, the
inability to accept and express yearning for and angry reproaches
against the lost object.
A reading of the Iiterature shows that an unconscious effort
to recover the lost object has not previously been singled out as
being, in most pathological variants of mourning, the principal
dynamic at work. Naturally it has not escaped attention altogether,
and there are indeed many references to it. Nevertheless, although
a few are explicit, a majority are only implicit, and it is striking
in how many formulations it is virtually absent.
Among many possible reasons for this neglect three stand
out. One is the overwhelming emphasis given by Freud to identi-
fication with the lost object as the key concept in mourning, an
emphasis that has obscured processes which I believe to be more
fundamental. A second reason is the tendency of much analytic
theorizing to be concerned, not with motivation and with objects
and situations in the external world toward which motivation is
directed, but with quantities of excitation and with affects as con-
ditions that lead these quantities to become excessive. As a result
there are plentiful references to affects, such as unconscious sad-
ness and anger and unresolved grief, and correspondingly few to
unconscious striving to recover a lost object and to unconscious
reproach designed to discourage repetition of loss.
A third reason for this neglect is the weight given by Melanie
Klein and her school to the bereaved’s sense of guilt, arising, it is
held, from his belief that he was the agent of the loss, and to her
PATHOLOGICAL AND CHILDHOOD AIOURNING 529
mon with Helene Deutsch’s case of the man with the dog fantasy.
T h e man reported by Edith Jacobson is described as similarly
detached in all his relationships; in addition he maintained that
his only feeling toward his mother was one of “cold resentment.”
Progress in’his analysis, Edith Jacobson reports, was signaled by
“waves of affection toward his mother [which] were permitted to
appear and to replace his symptoms.” Though Edith Jacobson’s
terminology is one of affect not motivation, the one is easily trans-
lated into the other. I n terms of motivation, it is evident, these
“waves of affection toward his mother” are equivalent to a longing
for her; and it is this longing which had been unconscious until
released during the analysis.
Edith Jacobson’s case follows the pattern of Helene Deutsch‘s
i n another respect also. I n both cases the disturbance was thought
by the analyst to have had its origin in a separation experience
of early childhood. I n commenting on her case Edith Jacobson
expresses the opinion that “the trauma that had broken up his
relationship to his mother” had occurred when the patient was
three and a half years old. At this time his mother had had a rnis-
carriage. His father was absent and, although his mother remained
at home, she “had been physically sick and depressed for many
..
weeks after. . Left in the hands of a maid, he had felt lost and
confused.”
T h e theoretical position taken by Helene Deutsch in her
paper “Absence of Grief” (10) is not dissimilar to that of Edith
Jacobson: once again there is implicit recognition that uncon-
scious longing for the lost mother is at the heart of the psycho-
pathology. For example, when discussing the man with the dog
fantasy, Helene Deutsch remarks: “apart from this one revealing
fantasy there was no trace of longing or mourning for his mother”
(who, it will be remembered, had died when he was five years
old). Despite this reference in the case report to longing as an
unconscious motive, however, in the notes at the end of the paper
theory is cast in the terminology of affect. “I believe that every
unresolved grief is given expression in one form or another,” she
writes. I “am convinced that the unresolved process of mourning
as described by Freud must be expressed in full.” Referring to
“suppressed affect following loss,” she continues: “TVe must as-
532 JOHN BOWLBY
.
sume that the urge to realization succeeds under the impetus of
an unconscious source of affect-energy exactly as in the case of
the criminal who is at the mercy of his guilt feelings. I suspect
that many life stories which seem to be due to a masochistic
attitude are simply the result of such strivings for the realization
of unresolved affects.”
This mode of theorizing, as Helene Deutsch herself points
out, is not unlike that employed by Freud in his reference to the
IYolf Man’s inability to mourn his sister. It is a mode of theorizing,
moreover, that has been followed by both Landauer (31) and
Lindemann (33), each of whom has taken mourning and its
pathological variants as his focus of attention. I n each case a use
of the language of affects has had the result that in their formula-
tions the unconscious longing that is part of the urge to recover
the lost object, although implicit, has tended to be obscured.
More explicit references to the longing for the lost object
are to be found in a contribution by Anna Freud (14) and also
in a number of clinical writings that report on cases of compulsive
wandering, depression, and suicide. I n a paper “About Losing and
Being Lost” Anna Freud discusses the connection between wishes
to possess and tendencies to lose as they refer to parent figures
and to material possessions respectively. In the third part she
discusses dreams of the return of the dead during phases of mourn-
ing and likens them to “the well-known folklore of the ‘lost
souls,’ i.e., dead people who are supposed to find no rest and [to]
wander at night in search of their former loved ones. I n both
cases,” she comments, “a libidinal process in the mourner, i.e.
longing, appears as projected into the image of the lost object.”
So long as the ghost is believed searching for his lost loved ones
the bereaved continues mourning. I n other words, mourning ends
when conscious or unconscious searching stops.
T h e truth of this generalization is well illustrated in the
papers by Stengel on compulsive wandering, a symptom, he found,
that is commonly associated with pseudologia, episodic depression,
and impulses to commit suicide. In his reports of thirty-six cases
(42, 43) Stengel draws special attention to two closely connected
features. The first is the high frequency of serious disturbance
in the patients’ relation to their parents in childhood, in par-
PATHOLOGICAL AND CHILDHOOD MOURNIXG 533
illness also refers to the yearning for a former love object. I n his
view, however, it is not so much the lost mother who is longed
for but “the parent or sibling of the opposite sex.” Felix Deutsch
(8) reasons on the same lines.
I n Melanie Klein’s writing and that of many of her followers
the emphasis is so firmly on the sense of guilt and the need to
make reparation that the sense of loss and the striving to recover
the lost object tend to be obscured. Indeed in his comprehensive
discussion of the literature on the theory of depression, particu-
larly as it supports hlelanie Klein’s views, Rosenfeld (40) omits
all reference to it. Nevertheless it is not missing from Melanie
Klein’s own work. For example, she describes how a male patient,
unable to experience sorrow, during his analysis “increasingly
experienced the grief and longing for [his mother] which he had
repressed and denied from his early days onwards.” Earlier in the
same paper she refers to the need for the ego “to develop methods
of defence which are essentially directed against the ‘pining’ for
the lost object” (28).
These same concepts underlie her interpretation of the per-
sonality of Fabian, the hero of a story by the French novelist
Julian Green (29). Fabian is represented as an unhappy dissatis-
fied man, full of resentment, incapable of sympathy, able to make
only brief and frustrating liaisons with girls, full of insatiable
desire for wealth and success and intense envy for those who
possess more than himself. I n the course of the story Fabian is
enabled to assume the personalities of other people. I n one such
personality he murders the girl, Berthe, whom the man he has
now become was trying, unsuccessfully, to win. I n another he
becomes fascinated by the eyes of a second girl, Elise, who has for
long been hopelessly in love with the man he now is. Her eyes, he
finds, have “in them all the tragedy of a longing that can never
be satisfied”; and at that moment he realizes they are the eyes of
Fabian. Finally, he becomes himself again and, with his mother
sitting by his bedside, is overcome by longing to be loved by her
and to be able to express his love for her, which he can never do.’
In the light of her experience with patients like Fabian and
drawing on the other dramatic details of the story Melanie Klein ’
of his mother’s capacity to have it, than on the real loss that Philip
had suffered. Philip’s hallucination of his mother cooking, it can
be inferred, was the result of a split in the ego of the kind
described in the section, Denial That the Object Is Permanently
Lost.
Although striving to recover the lost object has never been
at the center of attention in theoretical formulations regarding
pathological mourning, it is not without interest to note the place
that yearning of other kinds has been given in psychopathology
generally. Many analysts from Abraham (1) and Rado (36) to
Melanie Klein have emphasized a fixation on the mother’s breast
and a longing to recover it. Others, including Freud (22, 23),
Helene Deutsch (9), and Anny Angel (3) have emphasized the
longing in female patients for a penis. Another line of theorizing,
favored among others by Rado (36) and Weiss (44),has explored
the notion that the missing object is wanted not so much for itself
but for its value in restocking the ego with “narcissistic supplies.”
A review of these hypotheses in the light of further knowledge
of object relations and of processes of mourning, healthy and
pathological, may lead to their being reappraised.
T h e resemblance that the mourning processes of childhood
bear to pathological variants of mourning in the adult is discussed
in the literature of only one main school of psychoanalytic the-
orizing, that of Melanie Klein. Inevitably it is missing from the
literature of those schools which either overlook the possibility
or reject the idea that mourning occurs in infancy and childhood.
I n the work of Melanie Klein and her colleagues, on the other
hand, the reality of childhood mourning and of its relatedness
to adult mourning is not only recognized but made a central pillar
of theory construction. T h e way in which hlelanie Klein treats
the connection’is at many points very different from the one pre-
sented here, however. I n the first place, her ideas rest not on the
empirical studies of the actual behavior of young children but on
reconstructions. I n the second, the picture she has of the dynamics
of mourning, whether healthy or pathological, in the adult or
the child, derives from a theoretical schema that is based on the
death instinct, the primacy of persecutory and depressive anxiety,
and the overwhelming importance of orality. Since in earlier
538 .TOHN BOWLBY