Yoon Hae Jin
Yoon Hae Jin
Yoon Hae Jin
Dissertation
Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy
in the Graduate School of The Ohio State University
By
2009
Dissertation Committee:
Margaret Binkley
Curtis Haugtvedt
Kathryn Stafford
i
Copyright by
2009
i
Abstract
Americans are gaining weight. As such, the issue of obesity has received the
increased attention. One of the most recent concerns is how the food industry is
consumption of food away from home, food which often contains high levels of calories
and fats, may be worsening the problem of obesity. Thus, while restaurants make an
effort to develop and introduce healthy food, the government has also addressed the
problem by introducing The Menu Education and Labeling Act (MELA), which requires
restaurants to present nutritional information on the menu. Such legislation has been
proposed or passed in several cities and states. Accordingly, the importance of and the
the effects of nutritional information disclosure is required in order to achieve the benefits
consumers’ nutritional information processes and food choices. More specifically, this
well as the moderating effects of several factors such as nutritional menu context,
information level, nutritional level of target items, and nutritional level of menu context.
Using the results of two pilot studies, favorable menu items available in restaurants and
six important nutrient contents were selected; levels of nutritional values for the six
Multiple regression analysis was conducted to test the proposed hypotheses, and
significant results were found. First, the presence of nutritional information had a
negative effect on consumer food evaluation and decisions, regardless of the healthiness
of the item at hand. Additionally, the healthiness of other alternatives on the menu
nutritional information disclosure. Thus, it was concluded that the placement of menu
items on the menu is very important in promoting healthy item choices. It is important to
The results of this study provide useful information not only for restaurateurs,
who may be able to develop more efficient marketing strategies using nutritional
information, but also for consumers, who will be able to make better choices when eating
iii
Dedication
iv
Acknowledgements
First of all, I would like to thank my advisor, Dr. Thomas George, whose advice,
support, and patience helped me complete this study. Without his encouragement and
guidance, I would not have been able to complete this dissertation or my Ph.D. I also
thank all my committee members. I am very grateful to Dr. Binkley who strongly
work. Another expression of thanks goes to Dr. Haugtvedt, who helped in conducting the
experiment and provided guidance for my research and dissertation. I also wish to thank
Dr. Stafford whose invaluable suggestions and feedback improved my dissertation and
encouraged me to be critical and analytical. I am also indebted to Dr. H.G. Parsa who
was my former advisor. His invaluable guidance and advice helped me to complete my
Yoon and Jung Rim Ryu, whose love, support, and patience have allowed me to achieve
this position. Very sincere thanks go to my sisters and their families whose emotional
support kept me going through all the difficult periods during my studies in Columbus.
Finally, I am very grateful to my friend, Jungkee Jang, who always supported and
encouraged me. I also thank my friends and colleagues. Without their support, my
graduate studies would not have been the same. Thanks to all.
v
Vita
Publications
1. Yoon, H., Nusair, K., Parsa, H.G., & Naipaul, S. (2010). Pricing Discounts and
Consumers Perceptions: A Comparison Between Hospitality and Non-
Hospitality Industries. Journal of Foodservice Business Research. 13(Dietary
Guidelines for Americans)
2. Yoon, H., Thompson, S.S. & Parsa, H.G. (2009). Bayesian Approach to Assess
Consumers’ Brand Selection Process and Identification of Brand Attributes in
Service Context. International Journal of Hospitality Management, 28 (1), 33-
41.
3. Nusair, K., Yoon, H., & Parsa, H.G. (2008). Effect of Utilitarian and Hedonic
Motivations on Consumer Satisfaction with Travel Websites. Journal of
Information Technology & Tourism, 10 (1), 75-89.
4. Serovich, J.M., Craft, S., & Yoon, H. (2007). Women’s HIV Disclosure to Immediate
Family. AIDS Patient Care and STDs, 21 (12), 970-980.
Fields of Study
vi
Table of Contents
Acknowledgements ..............................................................................................................v
vii
2.2 Nutritional Information on the Menu ...................................................................21
2.2.1 The Influence of Providing Nutritional Information on the Menu ...........21
2.2.2 The Influence of Nutritional Menu Context .............................................26
2.2.3 The Role of Motivation to Process Nutritional Information .....................29
2.2.4 The Role of Nutritional Knowledge .........................................................33
2.3 Summary of Literature Review and Hypotheses .................................................37
CHAPTER 3: METHODOLOGY .....................................................................................41
4.1 Sample..................................................................................................................66
4.2 Reliability and Internal Validity of Measures ......................................................69
4.3 Manipulation Check of Menu Items ....................................................................71
4.4 Hypotheses Test ...................................................................................................73
4.4.1 The Influence of Providing Nutritional Information on the Menu ...........74
viii
4.4.2 The Influence of Nutritional Menu Context .............................................78
4.4.3 The Role of Motivation to Process Nutritional Information .....................85
4.4.4 The Role of Nutritional Knowledge .........................................................91
4.5 Additional Analysis: Logistic Regression Analysis .............................................98
CHAPTER 5: CONCLUSIONS ......................................................................................102
ix
List of Tables
Table 4.5: Mean Values of Nutritional Perception of the Menu Items without
Nutritional Information.......................................................................................72
x
Table 4.9: Predicted Values of Dependent Variables without Interaction Terms b/w
the Nutritional Menu Context and the Nutritional Information Level...............85
xi
List of Figures
Figure 4.2: Interaction Effect between the Presence of Nutritional Information and the
Nutritional Menu Context on Nutritional Perception.......................................83
Figure 4.3: Interaction Effect between the Presence of Nutritional Information and the
Nutritional Menu Context on Overall Food Attitude..................................... ..83
Figure 4.4: Interaction Effect between the Presence of Nutritional Information and the
Nutritional Menu Context on Purchase Intention.............................................84
Figure 4.5: Interaction Effect between the Presence of Nutritional Information and the
Motivation to Process on Nutritional Perception........................................... ..90
Figure 4.6: Interaction Effect between the Presence of Nutritional Information and the
Motivation to Process on Overall Food Attitude..............................................90
xii
CHAPTER 1: INTRODUCTION
1.1 Background
Americans are gaining weight. The rate of obesity in the U.S. has unexpectedly
increased over the past 30 years. According to The National Health and Nutrition
obese in the 1970s; by 2004, this proportion had increased to more than two thirds of
American adults. The growing rate of obesity has received significant attention in recent
years since it is one of the critical causes of several public health problems, including
cardiovascular disease, hypertension, stroke, and diabetes mellitus (Centers for Disease
and Control Prevention (CDC), 2007b). Consequently, the increasing rate of obesity has
led to an increase in medical expenditures. In 1997, the direct costs associated with being
overweight and obese amount to approximately 5.7% of total U.S. medical expenditures;
this figure continues to rise steeply (Wolf & Colditz, 1998). In 2002, the estimated
medical costs associated with obesity reached 92.6 billion dollars, which accounted for
9.1 % of total U.S. medical expenditures (CDC, 2007a). Obesity has therefore become
one of the fastest growing health issues; accordingly, research has been undertaken to
explain the causes of the obesity epidemic and to prevent the increasing rate of obesity.
been considered: the role that technological change plays in explaining trends in obesity,
1
how maternal employment affects obesity rates, the impact obesity has on wages and
instance, the time and effort that women need to prepare food was reduced by a
(Cutler, Glaeser, & Shapiro, 2003). By economizing time and effort, women spend fewer
calories on their work at home, thus creating an imbalance between calorie intake and
Chou, et al. (2004) identified several significant predictors of body mass index and the
probability of being obese: household income, the price of food at home, the price of
quick-service and full-service restaurants, the price of alcohol, as well as the price of
cigarettes.
another explanation for the obesity problem. Due to new technology, physical activities
undertaken both at the workplace and out-of-work have decreased (Philipson & Posner,
2003). People are also more likely to spend their time participating in watching
television or playing games rather than performing housework or other physical activities
(Cutler, et al., 2003). This sedentary lifestyle may contribute to obesity by disrupting the
balance between energy intake and expenditure. Many studies have provided evidence
for this lifestyle based explanation of obesity (Hill, Wyatt, Reed, & Peters, 2003; Hu, Li,
More recently, researchers have become concerned that the food industry itself is
an important contributor to the problem of obesity and its related health problems, since
2
expenditures on food away from home are increasing. This includes all food purchased
and consumed away from home such as at restaurants, schools, or hospitals. According
to the U.S. Department of Agriculture (USDA, 2008), in 2007 money spent on food away
from home represented 49% of total household food expenditures, a figure which had
increased from 33% in 1970. It has been projected that expenditures on food away from
home will reach about 51% of total food expenditures by 2016; thus exceeding the
expenditure on food consumed at home. Additionally, Americans who are over 7 years of
age dined at restaurants on average 218 times per year; people with household incomes
over $75,000 ate at a restaurants more often (4.9 times per week) than did people with
incomes below $15,000 (3.2 times per week) (Ebbin, 2000). A number of studies have
indicated that this increase in food consumption away from home has a significant impact
on obesity rates (Binkley, Eales, & Jekanowski, 2000; Bowman & Vinyard, 2004; Chou,
et al., 2004; French, Story, Neumark-Sztainer, Fulkerson, & Hannan, 2001). This
phenomenon may be explained by the fact that many foods offered by full-service and
quick service restaurants – two main segments of the food away from home market – are
energy dense foods, high in both calories and fat (Glanz, Sallis, Saelens, & Frank, 2005;
Hill & Peters, 1998). For example, a king size Burger King Double Whopper with
cheese contains approximately 2,180 calories, more calories than the total recommended
daily allowance (2,000 calories). Furthermore, it has been shown that children, on
average, consumed more calories (770 calories) at a restaurant than they did when they
ate at home (420 calories) (Zoumas-Morse, Rock, Sobo, & Neuhouser, 2001).
Accordingly, more frequent food consumption at restaurants should tend to increase the
rate of obesity.
3
As one of the suspected parties responsible for obesity, food companies have
been blamed and threatened with fines, restrictions, and legislation (Wansink & Chandon,
2006). Above all, quick service restaurants such as McDonalds and Burger King have
been blamed for the increase in obesity rates. As a result, they have received bad press
and have been sued by their customers. Food companies and legislators have come
together in order to overcome this problem: not only are food companies making an effort
to develop and provide more healthy options, but the U.S. government has also
encouraged consumers to purchase healthy food and has proposed the law referred to as
The and Nutrition Labeling Education Act (NLEA) for packaged foods and The Menu
The NLEA was proposed and implemented by the U.S. Food and Drug
Administration (FDA). According to the NLEA, the labels of packaged food products
must list the nutritional information with daily reference values for the following
nutrients: total calories, total fat, saturated fat, trans fat, cholesterol, sodium, sugars,
carbohydrates, dietary fiber, protein, vitamin A, vitamin C, calcium and iron (FDA, 1994).
The purpose of this law is not only to increase consumers’ use of nutritional information,
but also to help consumers in choosing healthier foods based on the information provided
to them (Burton & Andrews, 1996). Byrd-Bredbenner (2000) found that 29% of
consumers always read and used nutritional labels, while 66% of consumers sometimes
read and used nutritional labels when purchasing food for home consumption. In
addition, in response to the NLEA, food product manufacturers evaluated and modified
existing packaged food and introduced nutritionally improved new products (Center for
4
However, the NLEA applies only to packaged foods because it is easy to evaluate
the point of purchase, such as in restaurants or cafeterias, on airplanes, and from vending
machines (Kozup, Creyer, & Burton, 2003). Although some chain restaurants provide
to present nutritional information of all menu items due to limited space on menus, lack
of standardized foods, and various costs (Almanza, Nelson, & Chai, 1997). As the
expenditure for food away from home and the calorie intake from eating out have
continuously increased, the lack of nutritional information for foods sold or served in
proposed in several states and is under review by Congress; this law is referred to as the
Menu Education and Labeling Act (MELA). The MELA law would require chain and
quick service restaurants to provide nutritional information such as the total amount of
calories, saturated fat, trans fat, carbohydrates, and sodium contained in menu items
(Cranage, Conklin, & Lambert, 2004). Recently, New York City enacted legislation
which requires restaurant chains with 15 or more establishments to list total calories of
items on the menu, while California enacted the California Menu Labeling Bill, requiring
that nutritional information be displayed for fast-food and chain restaurants with 20 or
more outlets. Philadelphia City Council also passed a menu labeling law which requires
restaurants with at least 15 outlets nationwide to provide nutritional information for four
nutrients in addition to total calories count (Nation’s Restaurant News (NRA), 2008).
5
Additionally, San Francisco and King County in Washington have enacted menu labeling
laws, and more than 20 states, cities and counties, including Montgomery County in
Maryland, Nassau and Westchester Counties in New York, and Multnomah County in
As a result, restaurants are reducing calorie content in their recipes and portion
sizes. For instance, Starbucks is saving the calories by swapping whole milk for 2%, and
McDonalds reduced the calorie count of their french fries from 570 to 500 (Kim, 2008,
October 29). Many other restaurants such as Quiznos and Dunkin Donuts have
introduced smaller sized meals or less caloric food. In addition, consumers are given
access to nutritional information and can therefore choose healthier foods (Cranage, et al.,
2004). According to the report released by Center for Science in the Public Interest
(Wootan, 2003), almost 75% of consumers used the nutrition fact label while purchasing
packaged food, and about half of consumers picked healthier over unhealthier food based
on the nutritional information provided. Thus, the menu labeling law has inspired policy
changes nationwide, and restaurants and food companies have noticed the influence that
explore the impact that nutritional information has on consumer food purchase behavior
in restaurants. Several studies found that healthiness of food is one of the important
components consumers consider when making food choices, and that consumers are more
likely to purchase healthy foods with fewer calories or less fat when the nutritional
information is provided on the menu (Burton & Creyer, 2004; Edwards & Meiselman,
2005; Kozup, et al., 2003; Stubenitsky, Aaron, Catt, & Mela, 2007). However, it remains
6
unknown which factors encourage consumers to process the nutritional information, and
how nutritional information can best promote healthy food choices. Therefore, it is
essential to study the impact that nutritional information presented on the menu has on
food choices, and to identify the factors that influence how nutritional information is used
in food evaluation.
This study thus focused on the effect that nutritional information disclosure has on
nutritional information processing and food purchase behavior in the restaurant context.
The results of this study will be useful not only for restaurateurs, who may be able to
develop more efficient marketing strategies using nutritional information, but also for
consumers, who will be able to make better choices when eating foods away from home.
As the concern for health increases, so has the demand for healthy food. In order
to satisfy consumers’ wishes, a variety of healthy menu items have been introduced in
restaurants and the Menu Education and Labeling Act (MELA) for restaurants has been
proposed. A recent Harvard Poll found that 62% of Americans support this regulation
information in making decisions at restaurants than they use when deciding about what
food to consume at home. The information used differs because these two decisions
7
consumers contemplate their self-image or self-regulation if they are accompanied by
others, excluding family and friends, because it is important that consumers favorably
impress others by their choices. There was evidence that consumers tend to select
healthier menu items when dining with colleagues or business partners (Hochradel, 2007).
Thus, the first purpose of this study was to examine the extent to which the nutritional
restaurant.
Prior research has demonstrated that consumers are more likely to have a positive
food attitude and increased consumer purchase intention when provided with healthier
decrease when unfavorable nutritional information is provided (Burton & Creyer, 2004;
underestimate the number of calories or the amount of fat contained in food offered by
expectations of the number of calories, amount of saturated fats, and sodium levels are
significantly different from the actual levels contained in restaurant food; this difference
may be even greater for less healthy foods than for healthier foods. However, when
nutritional information is presented on the menu, consumers tend to make healthier menu
item choices (Burton, Creyer, Kees, & Huggins, 2006). Thus, it is suggested that
has no effect on the use of information, the evaluation of food, or consumer food choices
in a restaurant setting (Droms, 2006; Stubenitsky, et al., 2007). The lack of effect was
8
explained by suggesting that people consider eating out a special event where they are
allowed to eat any food, regardless of health (Stubenitsky, et al., 2007). Dorms (2006)
also suggested that, when making food choices, people consider other important and
influential components besides health benefits, such as the taste of the food or their food
(Moorman, 1990). Among these characteristics, the impacts of nutritional knowledge and
information, have been explored in prior studies targeting packaged food products
(Burton, Garretson, & Velliquette, 1999; Keller, et al., 1997; Moorman & Matulich,
1993). The finding common to the studies was that consumers are more likely to have a
favorable attitude toward healthy food and purchase healthy food when they have high
despite the significant role nutritional knowledge and motivation play in food decisions,
subsequent studies have not investigated how these traits may affect consumer food
Menu context is determined by the set of all available food items on a menu. Consumer
food choice may be affected by the menu context since consumer food preferences and
choices are dependent on all available options at the point of purchase. In evaluating a
9
specific menu item, consumers compare it with alternatives using information such as the
description or price of the menu items (Dhar & Simonson, 1992). For example, if new
consumers usually compare the new price with the prices of all available products (Lynch,
Chakravarti, & Mitra, 1991). This is called the context effect; alternatives influence
consumer choice. Evidence of the context effect has been found in several previous
studies (Huber, Payne, & Puto, 1982; Huber & Puto, 1983; Simonson, 1989; Simonson &
Tversky, 1992). Similarly, the nutritional information associated with each available
alternative on the menu should relatively influence consumer food evaluation and choice
within the menu context. Nonetheless, the influence that menu context has in terms of
nutritional information processing has been examined in only a few studies (Burton &
Creyer, 2004; Kozup, et al., 2003); prior studies were further limited to include only
unhealthy target items or unhealthy contexts. Although the results from previous studies
showed a significant context effect on food attitude and purchase intention, these finding
evaluate nutritional information of available options on the menu and also to determine
whether the context effect exists for both healthy and unhealthy items.
The aim of this study was to investigate how the presence of nutritional
information on a menu affects consumers’ food evaluation and food purchase behavior in
10
will influence consumer food decision making in a restaurant context.
(2) To identify the influence of menu context in which a specific menu item is
knowledge.
reasons. First, the study adds to the limited amount of literature which considers the
restaurant and have mainly focused on the effects of nutritional menu context depending
on the presence of nutritional information. Thus, the results of this study extend the
literature; they also provide support for menu labeling laws and help to create effective
Second, the results of this study provide important insight into the way consumers
choices. Thus, it is critical to educate consumers about nutrition and the importance of
11
health.
This study also identified which menu items consumers choose among several
using nutrition labels and to develop menu items which balance between healthy and
unhealthy foods.
educate consumers such that they increase the use of nutritional information on menus
and control their food consumptions for health. It also aids restaurateurs in understanding
how consumers use nutritional information in the menu decision process, and allows
states the problem and the purpose addressed by the study. It outlines the significance of
the dissertation and discusses the potential contributions to both academic and practical
impacts nutritional information and other factors have on decision making in a restaurant
setting. This chapter also includes the proposed hypotheses and conceptual model based
experimental design and explains the statistical methods used. Chapter 4 presents the
results of data analysis and hypothesis testing. Chapter 5 discusses the findings of the
research and its limitations, and concludes the dissertation with theoretical and
12
managerial implications.
Nutritional Information
Nutritional information includes the amount, in grams, of serving size, and the nutrients
included in that serving, like total calories, calories from fat, total fat, saturated fat, trans
fat, cholesterol, sodium, total carbohydrates, dietary fiber, sugar, protein, vitamin A,
Healthy Food
According to the criteria published by the Food and Drug Administration (FDA),
“healthy food” refers to “a food that is low in fat and saturated fat and that contains
limited amounts of cholesterol and sodium”. The recommended daily values of the above
four nutrients, based on a diet of 2,000 calories a day, are the following: less than 65g of
total fat, less than 20g of saturated fat, less than 300mg of cholesterol, and less than
course meal including entrée and dessert, it must provide at least 10 percent of two or
three of vitamins A or C, iron, calcium, protein or fiber, and the content of sodium can not
13
Motivation to Process Nutritional Information
healthy behaviors including nutrition label information acquisition from media, family, or
friends, life-balancing behaviors, positive diet addition, negative diet restriction, alcohol
moderation, and tobacco nonuse. However, this study focused on the motivation to
process given nutritional information with the goal of healthy eating behavior in a
restaurant setting.
Nutritional Knowledge
health related cognitive structures”. Namely, it is an ability to use the nutrition and health
Bettman, et al. (1991) used the term “context” to refer to a choice set that includes
other characteristics of the choice environment, and Simon and Tversky (1992, p.281)
definitions, menu context can be defined as a group of all available menu items that a
With respect to nutritional information, Kozup, et al. (2003, p.20) used the term
“nutritional context”; that is “the nutritional frame created by other menu items”. On the
14
other hand, Burton and Creyer (2004, p.122) employed the term “nutritional frame”,
which is “the context within which the menu item is presented”. Thus, in this study, the
term “nutritional menu context” was used to refer to a menu context containing the
15
CHAPTER 2: LITERATURE REVIEW
relating to restaurant menu labeling and healthy eating behavior have been reviewed.
The literature review provides the theoretical foundation for this study and the rationale
for the conceptual model and hypotheses. Specifically, this chapter reviews necessary
theories of how nutritional information in restaurants shapes consumer food choice and
There are many theories which seek to explain consumer behavior in the market
place. Among them, two fundamental theories were drawn upon to defend the structure
and ideas of the present study: The Elaboration Likelihood Model (ELM) and Context
Effect. These models provide a method of examining the basic procedures consumers
use in information processing, and help identify the key influential factors involved in
information processing. As such, these models guided the conceptual model of this study.
(1981), was used in this study to account for the process that consumers use when they
16
become aware of nutritional information and use it in making food choices. The ELM is
influence judgment or attitude change in both social and consumer psychology. The
ELM clarifies the process of how consumers build or change their attitudes through two
different routes: the central route and the peripheral route (Petty & Wegener, 1999).
Through the central route, consumers’ attitudes may be stronger or changed by careful
consideration of information, whereas attitude changes through the peripheral route are
based not on diligent consideration of information but rather on a simple inference from
the related cues in the persuasion context (Petty, Cacioppo, & Schumann, 1983).
through the central route than they do through the peripheral route.
in accessing and elaborating information through the central route (Petty & Wegener,
situational factors such as the need for cognition, involvement, or personal relevance
provided to consumers who are very health conscious, they should be more motivated to
use it in making food choices (Chandon & Wansink, 2007b). In addition to motivation,
consumers are distracted, they will be less able to make a cognitive effort; on the other
17
hand, consumers who are knowledgeable in a subject should be more willing to spend
time and effort thinking about it (Petty & Brock, 1981) . When consumers have enough
motivation and ability to process the relevant information or message content, they are
information (Petty & Wegener, 1999). According to the ELM, when consumers have
sufficient motivation and ability to process, they may focus on information itself and
evaluate the information using any one of these criteria. In particular, the quality of
argument, including such traits as the number of arguments and the source’s level of
expertise, is more important for consumer. On the other hand, consumers under the
peripheral route who lack motivation or ability to process the presented information use
evaluation.
central route are expected to engage in cognitive effort; the thoughts and attitudes
obtained through this process should be more reasonable, relatively persistent, and
resistant to opposite persuasion (Haugtvedt & Petty, 1992; Petty, Haugtvedt, & Smith,
1995). Additionally, attitudes created or changed by the elaborated process are more
likely to represent consumers’ behavioral intention and behavior than are attitudes which
18
2.1.2 Context Effect
The ELM says that motivation and ability to process information, along with
examine the ELM have introduced various significant variables that impact the
elaboration process: mood, the recipient of the message, the number of message sources,
and time pressure (Petty & Wegener, 1999). Although the influence of context, which is
defined by all available alternatives in a choice set, has not been examined in prior
studies relating to the ELM, it remains one of the prominent variables in persuading
advertising messages with the intention of making a purchase, they must evaluate and
compare information for all available options within the choice context. Thus, attitude
other alternatives. In this case, it is very important which options are included in the
context.
Context effect, which is the impact of “the set of alternatives under consideration”,
was introduced by Simon and Tversky (1992). According to several researchers, context
effect is explained by the principle of tradeoff contrast. Tradeoff contrast describes the
attractive alternatives than when it has a background containing more attractive options
(Lynch, et al., 1991; Simonson & Tversky, 1992). Thus, depending on the choice set in
which a specific product is presented, the preference for the product should differ. Simon
and Tversky (1992) divided the context effect into two types: background context effect
19
and local context effect. Background context effect describes the case when past options
influence a current choice; local context effect occurs when a target item is compared
with current alternatives in the context. In the example of food selection at a restaurant,
all available menu items should influence the choice of a particular menu item; this is an
example of the local context effect. Thus, if nutritional information, one attribute by
which menu items may be evaluated and judged, is provided on the menu, consumer food
the menu. On the other hand, past experiences of menu selection at other restaurants may
be the reference point by which menu items in the current context are evaluated and
judged. In this case, the information of menu items in past contexts influences the choice
of a menu item in the current context; this is called the background context effect.
Based on the ELM and context effect models this study examined the effects that
nutritional information disclosure on the menu and other significant factors may have on
consumer food evaluation and choice. In this study, three different levels of nutritional
information were provided and explored: (1) food description without nutritional
information; (2) food description with information for the total calorie content only; and
(3) food description with nutritional information including six key nutrition contents.
toward a menu item, their overall attitude toward a menu item, and their purchase
intention are evaluated. This study also examined the roles that motivation to process
Finally, how the context effect affects evaluation and selection of a menu item depending
20
2.2 Nutritional Information on the Menu
The effect that nutrition labels or nutrition claims have on consumers’ perceptions
and purchase behavior for packaged food products consumed at home has been
perspectives: level of nutritional information (Burton & Andrews, 1996; Garretson &
Burton, 2000), package design (Ford, Hastak, Mitra, & Ringold, 1996; Keller, et al.,
1997), nutrition fact format (Kozup, Burton, & Creyer, 2006), message type (Andrews,
Netemeyer, & Burton, 1998; Garretson & Burton, 2000), and demographics (Axelson &
Penfield, 1983; Nayga, Lipinski, & Savur, 1998). They provided evidence that
nutritional labels on packaged food products have significant associations with nutritional
knowledge, attitudes toward food products, and increased use of label information in food
choices (Burton & Andrews, 1996; Burton, et al., 1999; Garretson & Burton, 2000;
Studies targeting restaurant menu choices have been recently completed with
similar results found in the restaurant environment (Burton & Creyer, 2004; Cranage, et
al., 2004; Hwang & Lorenzen, 2008; Kozup, et al., 2003). Cranage, et al. (2004) studied
and healthy food choice. To find the effect of presented nutritional information, they
chose hot entrée menu items in an on-campus café and analyzed the nutritional content of
menu items using the USDA database. They collected sales data for the items over a two-
week period during which nutritional information was not provided. They then provided
nutritional information for the items, including portion size, calories, fat, calories from fat,
21
cholesterol, sodium, and protein, and collected data for another two weeks. The results of
this study revealed that nutritional information disclosure at the point of purchase
increased customer satisfaction for food quality and future purchase intention for the food;
consumers may feel that they have been more responsible in their choice after being
exposed to nutritional information. This research also concluded that consumers were
willing to select more healthy foods when nutritional information was provided.
claims and nutritional information on the menu, Kozup, et al. (2003) conducted three
different experiments. The first two experiments were designed to test the effects of
nutritional information for a packaged food (a frozen lasagna) and a restaurant menu item
measures (food and nutrition attitude, purchase intention, and source credibility) and
disease risk measures (heart disease and stroke) associated with a certain product,
depending on whether a heart healthy claim was presented on the product. Other
(unhealthy) values, and the measures were reevaluated. For both the packaged food and
the menu item, favorable nutritional information positively influenced consumers’ food
evaluation and disease risk perception. In contrast, the heart health claim only affected
consumer attitude toward the nutrition of the packaged food and the perceived source
credibility for the menu item. Consumers’ food evaluation and disease risk perception for
the menu item were also significantly affected by an interaction between the heart healthy
claim and nutritional information provision. Thus, the provision of healthful nutritional
22
information with a heart healthy claim increased attitude toward nutrition, overall food
with a heart healthy claim reduced consumers’ positive attitude toward a food and
purchase intention.
variable was added. This additional variable measured the nutritional level of other
available menu items. The researchers used three different chicken menu items and
manipulated the nutritional values for one target item and the nutritional context, which
included the nutritional information for the other two items. For this study, participants
were recruited at a shopping mall. In contrast to the two prior experiments, in this
experiment providing nutritional information about the target item had a significant
Burton and Creyer (2004) built upon the third experiment conducted by Kozup et
al. (2003). They focused on the effects of nutritional context by providing nutritional
information. Using four different menu items, they manipulated the nutritional values of
both healthy and unhealthy target items, while the nutritional values of all non-target
items were also manipulated such that they appeared unhealthier than the target item.
Consumers perceived an item as increasing their risk for heart disease and cancer in the
case when nutritional information was present on the menu; the evaluation of nutrients
such as fat, saturated fat, and cholesterol significantly differed depending on the presence
23
In recent research, Burton et al. (2006) demonstrated that consumers were not
aware of the exact levels of nutrients in foods offered at a restaurant, and tended to
underestimate the content of nutrients such as calories, fat, saturated fat, and sodium. In
particular, the differences between the nutritional values expected by consumers and the
actual values were greater for unhealthy foods than for healthy foods. Based on this
result, the researchers conducted the experiment to explore how consumers change their
attitudes, purchase intention, and food choices when their expected nutritional values
differ from the provided actual values. For the test, they selected four different food
items, and measured the expected nutritional values for each of the four items.
Consistent with their survey, the expected nutritional values for two unhealthy items
(chef’s salad and hamburger with fires) tended to be inconsistent with the actual
nutritional values, while the expected values for the other two items (grilled chicken
breast with baked potato and turkey sandwich), which are relatively healthy items, tended
to be closer to the actual values. The results also showed that the presence of nutritional
information on the restaurant menu had a significant main effect on food attitude, and
consumers’ food choice was influenced by the level of nutritional information provided.
When only total calorie count was presented, consumers were more likely to select a
healthier menu item (turkey sandwich). On the other hand, the menu item type
influenced all dependent variables: food attitude, perception of associated risk of weight
gain and heart disease, and purchase intention. For the two foods with high calorie
content and high fat content, the hamburger and chef’s salad, consumer purchase
intention decreased when nutritional information was provided on the menu. Consumers
also changed their food choice if actual calorie and fat content was higher than expected.
24
Accordingly, the provision of nutritional information helped consumers evaluate nutrients
of menu items more accurately and led them to choose healthier foods at a restaurant.
Hwang and Lorenzen (2008) have also examined the effects of providing
consumers felt that different nutritional labeling on the menu was helpful in evaluating
information, only total calories, total calories plus macronutrients, total calories and
macronutrients plus fat, and total calories, macronutrients, and fat plus fiber. They then
measured how helpful consumers deemed the nutritional information presented on each
label, as well as the perceived source credibility. As more nutritional information was
added, consumers felt that the provided nutritional information was more helpful and
more credible. Furthermore, consumer preference for a given menu item increased as
compared the nutrition related attitude, overall food attitude, and willingness to pay for
an unhealthy menu item (southwest chicken sandwich) under three different conditions:
nutritional information where the item was advertised as containing only 50% of the
original fat content (low fat). When nutritional information was provided, consumers had
a more unfavorable nutrition attitude, as well as a more negative overall food attitude.
But consumers perceived that when the menu item was presented under the low fat
condition with nutritional information, it was healthier than the item with original fat
content. Thus, consumers recognized the change in fat content. Consumers were also
likely to pay $2 more for the menu item under the low fat condition.
25
In conclusion, previous studies have demonstrated that providing nutritional
information on the menu influences consumer food evaluation and choice. However, the
intention varied depending on other variables such as menu context and individual
item with unfavorable nutritional values. Thus, this study sought to determine the effect
that nutritional information has on consumers’ food decisions in restaurants when other
influential variables are held constant. Based on the literature reviewed herein, the
There is evidence that consumers’ preferences and product choices are affected by
choice context (Dhar, Nowlis, & Sherman, 1999; Simonson & Tversky, 1992). In
comparing the utilities or subjective values associated with each alternative, the consumer
tends to choose the alternative with the highest utility or subjective value. Thus, in
analyzing consumer choice, it is very important to determine not only which products are
in the choice set, but also which products act as comparisons. For instance, consumers
were more likely to choose a product in a context consisting of alternatives with unique
good features than in a context consisting of alternatives with unique bad features (Dhar,
et al., 1999). Also, if consumers perceived that the nutritional values of the available
26
options were similar, they were more likely to search for additional information to
distinguish the products; this search behavior influenced the product evaluation and
decision behavior (Balasubramanian & Cole, 2002). But, the information search intensity
decreased with increasing similarity of nutrition across brands after the Nutrition
Labeling and Education Act legislation had been enacted. As a result, the nature of the
further investigated; similarly, only a few studies have been conducted in the restaurant
setting (Burton & Creyer, 2004; Kozup, et al., 2003). This study addresses these
deficiencies.
alternatives in a choice set influenced consumer food evaluation, purchase intention, and
disease risk perception in the restaurant setting. To test the menu context effect, they
selected three items (slow-roasted chicken, chicken marsala, and grilled chicken fajitas);
of these three items, one was designated the target menu item and two were designated
alternatives. The following three levels of information were provided for the nutrition
levels of both the target item and the menu context: no information, healthy and
unhealthy. The researchers assumed that the nutritional values of the target item would
act as a reference point by which all items on the menu would be evaluated. Thus, item
evaluation and purchase intention would be influenced by the nutritional menu context of
alternatives on the menu. They found that the nutritional context of alternative menu
items had a significant effect on attitude toward a menu item and purchase intention.
Specifically, the nutritional context significantly moderated the effects that health claims
and nutritional information had on consumer attitudes toward a menu item. Therefore,
27
the effect that healthy information about a target item had on consumers’ attitude toward
that item and its nutrition was less pronounced when the nutrient contents of non-target
Burton and Creyer (2004) built upon the experiment conducted by Kozup et al.
(2003) in order to examine and identify the nutritional context effect. According to
Simonson and Tversky (1992), consumers’ decision processes were affected by other
options in a consideration set, which is referred to as the local context effect. Thus,
Burton and Creyer (2004) suggested that consumers’ food evaluations and purchase
the menu. To test their proposal, they chose one target menu item (pot roast) and three
non-target items (lasagna, chicken fajitas, and chicken chow mien). They provided three
different levels of nutritional information for the target item (control, healthy, and
unhealthy information), but only unhealthy information was presented for non-target
risk perceptions were measured for the target item and for one of the non-target menu
items. Compared to non-target menu items, attitudes toward the target item and purchase
intention for the target item were positively evaluated when the nutritional information
was offered. It was demonstrated that the presence of nutritional information for the non-
target menu items significantly influenced the attitude and purchase intention for the
target menu item. There also existed significant interaction effects between nutritional
information and nutritional context on overall food attitude and purchase intention for the
target menu item. The study showed that the nutrition levels of other options on the
28
information for the item itself.
Thus, the nutrition levels of alternatives in a menu context have significant effects
on food evaluation, and also moderate the effects of nutritional information for the item
itself. From the previous literature, the following hypotheses are identified:
H2: The nutritional menu context will significantly influence consumers’ food
H3: The nutritional menu context will significantly moderate the effect that the
decision process. For instance, Burton and Andrews (1996) found that consumers’ age
negatively influenced the ability to understand a nutrition label, and that education level
was positively related to healthy food consumption (Saegert & Young, 1983; Yung,
Contento, & Gussow, 1984). In addition to age and education level, various health
perform healthy behavior, health knowledge, health status, health locus of control,
healthy behavior control, and income (Moorman & Matulich, 1993). Moorman and
healthy behaviors such as health or label information acquisition from media, family, or
friends, life-balancing behaviors, positive diet addition, negative diet restriction, alcohol
29
moderation, and tobacco nonuse. These healthy behaviors were undertaken as preventive
measures, meant to avoid diseases or unhealthy states, and were divided into two groups:
survey, the researchers measured all individual health characteristic variables and healthy
behaviors, and identified the relationship between healthy behavior and individual
behavior, health knowledge, health status, health locus of control, healthy behavior
control, age, education and income) were significant predictors of healthy behaviors.
Motivation to perform healthy behaviors especially moderated the effects of all health
behaviors and the nutritional knowledge variables have been considered in studies meant
to identify the effects of nutritional information on healthy food choices. The reason for
this is that knowledge and motivation to process information are key factors that affect
perception and evaluation of given information (Petty, et al., 1983). Previous research
studies have demonstrated the impact of nutritional knowledge and motivation to process
nutritional information on consumer food decisions (Burton, et al., 1999; Droms, 2006;
influence healthy behaviors (Moorman & Matulich, 1993); this study focused specifically
Moorman (1990) investigated the nutritional information process, and identified the role
that motivation to acquire and process nutrition related information plays in the process.
30
In both nutritional information process models, motivation to process nutritional
evaluations of the nutrition of a product. Thus, the role that motivation to process
restaurant setting.
Keller, et al. (1997) found that nutritional claims, nutritional values, and
for a packaged food product. The researchers developed four types of nutrition claims
(no claim, 99% fat free, low fat, and low calorie) and manipulated the levels of nutritional
values (poor, medium, and good) for a frozen chicken dinner. Utilizing members of a
process information and several dependent variables including attitude toward nutrition,
overall food attitude, perceived credibility of the product marketer, and purchase
intention. The results indicated that consumers’ level of trust for the information provider
significantly differed depending on the type of nutritional claims. It was also found that
nutritional value had significant effects on all dependent variables: perceived credibility
of the product marketer, nutrition attitude, overall product attitude, and purchase intention.
however, moderated the effects of nutritional claims and nutritional value. Consumers
with high motivation to process information had overall more favorable attitudes toward
nutrition as well as toward the product, and were more likely to purchase a product when
31
nutritional information increases the accessibility of nutritional information and the
probability that the information will be processed. It also moderates the relationship
A recent study investigated the tradeoff effect between carbohydrates and fat on
disease risk perception, purchase intention, and nutrition perception when nutritional
claims and nutritional information were provided (Kemp, Burton, Creyer, & Suter, 2007).
Researchers again found that the motivation to acquire and process nutritional
information had a moderating effect on the tradeoff effect. For this study, an experiment
was developed with two manipulated variables, each with three levels; nutritional fact
panel (no, low carbohydrate-high fat, and high carbohydrate-low fat), and nutritional
claim (no, low fat, low carbohydrate). Participants, who were members of a household
research mail panel for a southern state, were presented with one of the nine possible
scenarios created by combining the variables above, and responded to questions meant to
information concerning the carbohydrate and fat contents of the product, impacted risk
perception of heart disease and stroke and nutrition perception, while the nutritional
claim variable only had a main effect on heart disease risk perception. The researchers
also found that motivation to process nutritional information had a significant influence
on the perceived risk of disease, but not on nutrition perception and purchase intention.
In contrast, motivation to process nutritional information moderated the effect that the
nutritional fact panel variable had on purchase intention. Thus, consumers with high
motivation to process nutritional information were likely to have more positive purchase
32
intentions when nutritional information was provided.
nutritional information may moderate the main effects that nutrition level may have on
nutrition perception.
the effect that the presence of nutritional information has on consumers’ nutritional
number of studies focusing on healthy eating behavior and food choice (Moorman, Diehl,
Brinberg, & Kidwell, 2004; Wardle, Parmenter, & Waller, 2000). Nutritional knowledge
refers to the ability to use nutrition and health information accurately (Moorman, 1993).
In particular, there is evidence that nutritional knowledge plays an important role in how
consumers use nutritional labels (Drichoutis, Lazaridis, & Nayga, 2005; Guthrie, Fox,
Cleveland, & Welsh, 1995; Levy & Fein, 1998) as well as in food evaluations and
Andrews, et al. (1998) investigated how the effects of nutritional ad claims were
disclosure type (none, absolute amount of fat in grams, relative to the daily value,
evaluative), and ad claim type (no claim, general claim, and specific claim). From the
pretest, researchers developed new objective measures of nutritional knowledge, and then
33
manipulated disclosure type and ad claim type (“delicious” for no claim, “healthy” for
general claim and “no cholesterol” for specific claim). They recruited participants in a
shopping mall and showed a target advertisement after displaying two clutter
advertisements. The results indicated that ad claim type influenced how respondents
evaluated fat content and healthiness, whereas disclosure type had an impact on how they
evaluated fat content, healthiness and perception of heart disease and cancer risk.
Nutritional knowledge had a significant main effect on how respondents evaluated fat
content, but did not affect the other variables. In the advertisement showing the amount
of fat in grams (absolute type), consumers with high nutritional knowledge more
positively evaluated the fat content, and perceived the risk of cancer accordingly, than did
consumers with low nutritional knowledge. Thus, the moderating effect of nutritional
Andrews, et al. (2000) conducted a similar study to examine the direct effects of
moderating effects on the relationship between disclosure type and evaluation of nutrients.
Similar to the previous study, respondents were presented with one of two different ad
types, general expression (“Now Healthier than Ever”) and specific phrase (“Now 1/3
Less Salt”), and one of four different nutrition interventions (none, absolute, relative,
evaluative) in an advertisement for Campbell’s soup. The researchers collected data from
mall shoppers, and measured their evaluations of sodium content and healthiness, the
perception of high blood pressure and coronary heart disease risks, claim believability,
and attitudes toward the ad and the brand. Nutritional knowledge had a significant main
effect only on the evaluation of healthiness of the food, and moderated the effect that
34
nutrient disclosure type had on the evaluation of sodium content and the risk perception
of high blood pressure. Consumers with high nutritional knowledge more precisely
evaluated the sodium content in evaluative disclosure type ads than they did in relative
and absolute disclosure type ads. In contrast, nutritional knowledge did not have any
The effect of nutritional knowledge was also investigated by Kozup, et al. (2006).
The purpose of this study was to examine how nutritional knowledge and the presence of
trans fat information affected consumer evaluations and perceptions of disease risk. The
researchers focused on trans fat information since many foods such as crackers, snacks,
fried foods, and fast foods contain trans fat that causes long term adverse health effects.
knowledge by providing respondents from a high knowledge group with an article which
included trans fat information. They also manipulated the level of trans fat (no, low (1g),
and high (4g)) as well as the format of the nutrition fact (a per serving basis, both a per
serving and a per package basis). For this study, a box of snack crackers was selected as
the target item, and university students served as subjects. Induced knowledge directly
affected how respondents perceived the product in terms heart disease, stroke, and blood
pressure risk, and also moderated the effect that the actual level of trans fat had on those
three diseases risk perceptions. On the other hand, the level of trans fat had a significant
effect on heart disease and stroke risk perception, while nutrition fact format influenced
only blood pressure risk perception. Although induced knowledge and the level of trans
fat did not directly affect perceived importance of trans fat and nutrition perception, the
interaction effects between induced knowledge and level of trans fat were significant.
35
That is, consumers who were exposed to trans fat information considered trans fat as an
important component of food and perceived food as being less nutritious when trans fat
Most studies to date which focus on nutritional knowledge have been targeted
toward packaged food products. However, Dorms (2006) has examined the role
nutritional knowledge plays in dietary decision making in a restaurant setting. The first
study, which sought to discover the effects nutritional information has on consumer food
choices, did not obtain significant result. In order to identify other influential factors
relating to the nutritional information process, the researcher measured various variables
dietary restraint in food choices served as a dependent variable. The results indicated that
nutritional knowledge had a significant effect on food choice. That is, consumers high in
decisions. Based on the literature reviewed herein, the following hypothesis is offered:
H5: Nutritional knowledge will significantly moderate the effect that the presence
36
Based on the proposed hypotheses, the following conceptual model of this study
was developed.
Nutritional H2
Menu Context
H4 H5
process and nutritional knowledge tend to process the given information and tend to
compare with other alternatives on the menu using their knowledge ability. As a result,
the final evaluation of menu items through this procedure should lead consumers to
37
purchase healthier items in a restaurant. Based on prior studies, following hypotheses are
elicited, and the specific content of previous literature related to nutritional information is
Hypotheses
H1. The presence of nutritional information on the menu will significantly influence
consumer food decision making, especially consumers’ nutritional perception,
overall food evaluation, and purchase intention.
H2. The nutritional menu context will significantly influence consumers’ food decision
making, especially consumers’ nutritional perception, overall food evaluation, and
purchase intention.
H3. The nutritional menu context will significantly moderate the effect that the presence
of nutritional information has on consumers’ nutritional perception, overall food
evaluation, and purchase intention.
H4. The motivation to process nutritional information will significantly moderate the
effect that the presence of nutritional information has on consumers’ nutritional
perception, overall food evaluation, and purchase intention.
H5. Nutritional knowledge will significantly moderate the effect that the presence of
nutritional information has on consumers’ nutritional perception, overall food
evaluation, and purchase intention.
38
Studies Sample Target Food Key Variables Findings
a b
Burton & Consumer Frozen chicken Level of Nutritional Value NA (+), PI (+), Nutrient Accuracy (+), Nutrition
Andrews research panel dinner product (NV) Information Awareness (0)
(1996) Nutrition Label Format NA (0), PI (0), Nutrient Accuracy (+), Nutrition
*Level of NV Information Awareness (0)
Age*Level of NV NA (+), PI (+), Nutrient Accuracy (0), Nutrition
information awareness (0)
Keller, et al. Statewide Frozen chicken Nutrition Content Claim NA (0), FAc (0), Credibility (+), PI (0)
(1997) household dinner product Level of Nutrition Value NA (+), FA (+), Credibility (+), PI (+)
research panel Motivation*Claim NA (+), FA (+), Credibility (0), PI (+)
Andrews, et General Margarine Ad claim type Fat content (+), Healthiness (+)
al. (1998) consumers product Disclosure type Fat content (+), Healthiness (+)
in 3 cities Nutritional knowledge Fat content (+), Healthiness (0)
Kozup, et al. Study 1 Study 1 Study 1
(2003) Consumer Frozen lasagna Heart HCd NA(+), FA(0), Credibility(0), PI(0), Heart (+), stroke(+)
household dinner NIe NA(+), FA(+), Credibility(+),PI(+), Heart(+), stroke(+)
research panel
39
Continued
Kozup, et al. University Snack crackers Level of Trans fat Disease Risk (+), Perceived importance of trans fat (+)
(2006) students Trans fat knowledge Disease Risk (+), Perceived importance of trans fat (0)
Level*Knowledge Disease Risk (+), Perceived importance of trans fat (+)
Kemp, et al. Household Frozen chicken Level of Nutrition NA (+), PI (0), Heart (+), Stroke (+), Weight gain (0)
(2007) research mail dinner Type of HC NA (0), PI (0), Heart (+), Stroke (0), Weight Gain (0)
panel Motivation*Level NA (0), PI (+), Heart (0), Stroke (+), Weight Gain (0)
Hwang & Grocery shoppers Menu item Level of NI NA (+), FA (+), Willingness to pay (+)
Lorenzen
(2008)
40
CHAPTER 3: METHODOLOGY
In the previous chapter, the conceptual model and five hypotheses were developed
based on prior research studies. Their purpose was to examine the impact of nutritional
information. To test the model and hypotheses an experimental design method was
between variables by controlling for other confounding variables. In order to identify the
effect of a certain variable, the treatment variable, it is very important to indentify the
expected sources of variability and also to design an experiment which reduces these
sources of variability (Dean & Voss, 1999). The randomized experimental design method,
in particular, is used to reduce some possible biases such as placebo effects. Thus, this
study employed the randomized design method in order to obtain rigorous internal
validity. This chapter describes the development of the experimental design of this study,
the instruments associated with key variables, and the survey procedure used in this study.
total calories only, and nutritional information of five important nutrients in addition to
41
total calories), two nutritional levels of a target menu item (healthy and unhealthy), and
two nutritional levels of menu context (healthier than the target item and unhealthier than
To test the effect of nutritional information on the menu and find the extent to
which the level of nutritional information provided has an impact on consumer food
information, which is considered a control group (CG), total calories only (TC), and
Cranage et al. (2004), consumers use only a fraction of the nutritional information
provided. In addition, the nutrition labeling regulations passed by several cities and
states demonstrate the variety of nutritional information that can be included on menus.
New York City, for instance, passed a law requiring that fast food outlets include only
total calorie counts for menu items, whereas California requires total calories, saturated
choice rather than simply testing for the effects of including any information at all. For
this study, the specific nutrition content for the NI Group was based on the definition of
healthy food, passed regulations, and consumer evaluation of important nutrients through
the pretest.
Two levels of nutritional value of the target menu item, healthy or unhealthy, were
healthiness of a menu item. In previous studies, selected menu items investigated were
mostly unhealthy items with high calories, fats, and sodium. However, nutritional
42
information may have a different effect depending on the healthiness of a menu item.
Thus, this study tested the effects of provision of nutritional information for items of two
The nutritional level of a menu context was also manipulated by altering the
the target menu item. For this manipulation, two different nutritional levels of the target
menu item and two different nutritional levels of a menu context were used. A total of 12
target item) × 2 (nutritional level of menu context) design were developed. The specific
measured in this study: consumers perceived nutrition, overall attitude toward a menu
43
item, and purchase intention. The measures of these three variables were adopted from
previous studies, but were modified for this study. All responses were measured using a
five-point Likert-scale.
Nutritional Perception
Consumers’ nutritional perception was measured using three items which have
been used in several previous studies (Burton & Creyer, 2004; Garretson & Burton, 2000;
Kozup, et al., 2003). The three items were: “Based on the information provided on the
menu, how important do you think a menu item would be as part of a healthy diet?” rated
by a five-point Likert scale defined as 1= not important at all to 5=very important, “What
level of nutrition do you think is suggested by the information regarding the menu item
on the menu?” measured by a five-point Likert scale defined as 1=not nutritious at all to
5=very nutritious, and “ How healthy do you think the menu item on the menu would be?”
The questions for overall attitude towards a menu item were chosen based on
previous research (Burton & Andrews, 1996; Burton & Creyer, 2004; Garretson &
Burton, 2000; Kozup, et al., 2003). Three questions were asked concerning the
consumer’s overall attitude toward a given menu item. The specific questions were:
“Overall, how much are you favorable toward a menu item?” measured by a five-point
Likert scale defined as follows; 1=very unfavorable to 5=very favorable, “Overall, how
much do you prefer a menu item?” rated by a scale defined by 1=very dislike to 5=very
44
like, “What is your overall feeling toward a menu item?” measured by a five-point scale
Purchase Intention
Three items used in prior studies (Burton & Andrews, 1996; Burton & Creyer,
2004; Garretson & Burton, 2000; Kozup, et al., 2003) were utilized for assessing
purchase intention of a menu item. The three items included measurements of the
a menu item, and were rated by a five-point Likert scale defined as follows: 1=not
probable at all to 5=very probable for the first question, 1=not willingness at all to 5=very
willingness for the second question, and 1=very unlikely to 5= very likely for the third
question.
Nutritional Information
information, total calories only, and nutritional information of six nutrients. These three
categorical groups can be expressed using two dummy variables in data analysis. Thus,
this information was recorded as two indicator variables to distinguish the level of
nutritional information. For example, one dummy variable was assigned a value of 1 if
total calories count was the only information provided; otherwise it was coded as 0. A
45
second dummy variable took on a value of 1 if nutritional information for the six nutrient
was offered on the menu; otherwise it took on a value of 0. The control group, where no
nutritional information was offered on the menu was indentified by assigning values of 0
According to the experimental design, two different menu contexts were tested: a
healthy context and an unhealthy context, depending upon the nutritional values of
alternatives offered on the menu. To test the main effect of menu context, nutritional
menu context was included as an independent variable, but the effects of menu context
were controlled for when other moderating variables such as motivation to process and
nutritional knowledge were tested. Nutritional menu context was also recorded as an
indicator variable which took on a value of 1 if the provided nutritional menu context was
Measures of the following moderating variables were used in this study. They
were adopted from existing scales used in prior studies. Minor modifications were made
such that the scales were appropriate for a restaurant context. Except for the questions
about objective nutritional knowledge, which were measured using a multiple choice
question scale, the moderating variables were evaluated using a five-point Likert scale.
46
Motivation to Process Nutritional Information
1993). Motivation to process nutritional information was measured using five items, all
of which had been used in previous studies (Chandon & Wansink, 2007b; Kemp, et al.,
2007; Moorman, 1990). The following five statements were presented, and respondents
were asked to rate their level of accord with each statement using a five-point Likert scale
looking for nutritional information of menu items in a restaurant”, “In general, I pay
Nutritional Knowledge
objective and subjective knowledge related questions were asked. Subjective nutritional
knowledge was assessed by how participants rated their present level of nutritional
knowledge. Three items developed by Moorman, et al. (2004) were used, and responses
5=strongly agree for the following statements: “Compared to most people, I am quite
47
confident in using nutritional information to make a choice”, and “I feel confident about
questions were selected from a previous study (Andrews, et al., 1998), and from the ten-
question nutrition quiz offered by the North Carolina Department of Agriculture and
Consumer Services (NCDA&CS). Fifteen questions were selected from the two sources.
Each is associated with important nutrients identified by previous studies and labeling
laws; the difficulty level of each question was tested in the two pilot tests.
Table 3.2 displays the summary of study instruments for measuring the suggested
variables.
48
3.2.4 Other Survey Questions
Besides the three main dependent variables, respondents were also asked to report
which menu item they would choose to order. For each menu item, the expected taste
and the credibility of provided nutritional information were assessed using one- and two-
item Likert scales, respectively; the items were drawn from previous studies (Butron &
Creyer, 2004; Kozup, et al., 2003). At the end of the survey, general questions about
restaurant visits, sources of nutritional information, and health status were added.
employment status, monthly income and monthly expenditure for food away from home
The goal of the first pilot test was to identify popular menu items and important
nutrients that consumers consider when choosing a menu item in restaurants. Consumers’
perceived healthiness of menu items was also evaluated when the actual nutritional
values were provided. Based on these results, nutritional values of menu items would be
university as respondents. Students were asked to fill out the pilot study using a paper
and pencil method. A total of 158 students voluntarily participated in the first pilot test,
49
3.3.1 Procedure
In the first pilot study, eight different menu items available at casual restaurants
were provided on the menu, each with a description: Chicken Caesar Salad, Classic
Salmon, Grilled Pork Chops, and California Turkey Club. After reading eight menu
items with descriptions, participants were asked to list their three most preferred items
contents that consumers usually use in making food choices in restaurants using a five-
point Likert scale defined as 1=Not at all important to 5= Very important. Participants
were also asked to rank the eleven nutrient contents in order of importance.
Finally, actual nutritional information of eight menu items was provided, along
with a description of each item. The perceived nutritional level of each menu item was
information was collected. The questionnaire used for the first pilot study is included in
Appendix A.
The collected data were analyzed using SPSS v.17 using various methods of
analysis. Sixty-four percent of the participants were female with the majority of them
between the ages of 19 and 23 (81%). Most were Caucasian (77%). Sixty-three percent
of participants reported their monthly income to be less than $1,000, and half of them
50
(51%) spent between $100 and $200 on monthly food expenditures.
The first objective of this survey was to identify six important nutrients that
should be presented on the menu for the nutritional information (NI) group. According to
the average importance scores for the eleven nutrient contents, total calories (mean=3.54
out of 5) was the most important contributor when choosing a menu item in a restaurant,
then total fat (mean=3.44), protein (mean=3.29), and trans fat (mean=3.28). Similar
results were found in the ranking analysis. Forty-three percent of respondents ranked
total calories as most important, while total fat was placed first or second by thirty-four
percent of respondents. Also, calories from fat, trans fat, protein, and saturated fat were
highly ranked. The top six nutrient contents were therefore: total calories, total fat, trans
fat, calories from fat, saturated fat, and protein. However, several of the six nutrient
contents such as calories from fat and protein are not included in the definition of healthy
food by the Food and Drug Administration (FDA, 1999) and are not included in any
enacted menu labeling laws. According to the definition of healthy food, the amount of
total fat, saturated fat, cholesterol, and sodium are important when deciding if food is
healthy or not. California requires a listing of total calories, saturated fat, carbohydrates,
and sodium. Philadelphia added trans fat to those requirements. In short, total calories,
total fat, saturated fat, and sodium are critical nutrient contents that consumers need to
consider when selecting a menu item. Similarly, cholesterol is also included in the
definition of healthy food. Thus, for this study, the following six nutrient contents were
selected and were placed on the menu for the (NI) group: total calories, total fat,
51
saturated fat, cholesterol, protein, and sodium.
In order to achieve the second objective of pilot testing, the reported perceived
nutritional values for the eight menu items were analyzed. The goal of this analysis was
to identify the levels of healthiness of menu items so that the appropriate nutritional
values used in the main experiment were manipulated. Analysis of variance (ANOVA)
revealed that the perceived nutritional values of the eight menu items were significantly
different from one another, and the results of the Post Hoc test indicated that the eight
items could be categorized into three groups depending upon their perceived nutritional
values (F=97.5, d.f.=(7, 1250), p≤0.001). Among the eight items, the Grilled Salmon
(mean=4.10 out of 5) and the Lemon Herb Roasted Chicken (mean=4.03) were perceived
as healthier items, which included 15% to 20% of the daily recommended amounts of fat,
saturated fat, and sodium based on a 2,000 calories diet. On the other hand, the Chicken
Caesar Salad (mean=2.46), the Chicken Marsala (mean=2.62), and the Chicken Fajitas
contained 70% to 80% of the recommended daily amounts of fat, saturated fat, and
sodium. These results agreed with the manipulated levels used in previous studies
(Burton, et al., 2006; Hwang & Lorenzen, 2008; Kozup, et al., 2003) where the
nutritional values of healthy food were set at 15% to 20% of the daily recommend values,
and the nutritional values of unhealthy foods were set at 50% to 60% of the daily
recommended values. Thus, the nutritional values for the healthy target item were set
such that the food accounted for 20% of the recommended daily values of fat, saturated
52
fat, cholesterol, and sodium. For items in the healthy menu context, the nutritional values
of the two alternatives items were manipulated such that they accounted for 12% and 10%
of recommended daily values, respectively. The unhealthy target item was made to
account for around 60% of recommended daily values, and the unhealthy menu context
included two alternative options containing 65% and 70% of the recommended daily
values, respectively. The amount of protein for each item was set such that it was
inversely related to the amounts of fat, cholesterol and sodium contained in the item. For
instance, healthy items include 60% to 70% of daily recommended values of protein.
Total calories information for the six menu items was set based on the actual total calorie
counts of each menu item, but some modifications were made depending on the
manipulated level of daily values for other provided nutrients. The manipulated
nutritional values of menu items were similar to their actual values so that it was possible
to control for the effects of actual healthiness of menu items. Table 3.3 shows the
53
Nutritional Perception a Preference b Choice
Item
(Mean, S.D.) (Frequency, %) (Frequency, %)
Chicken Caesar Salad 2.46 (0.80) 77 (16.3%) 17 (10.8%)
Grilled Salmon 4.10 (0.72) 72 (15.3%) 35 (22.2%)
Lemon Herb Roasted
4.03 (0.65) 42 (8.9%) 8 (5.1%)
Chicken
Chicken Marsala 2.62 (0.84) 42 (8.9%) 10 (6.3%)
Chicken Fajitas 2.64 (0.71) 76 (16.1%) 28 (17.7%)
Classic Sirloin Steak 3.05 (0.81) 71 (15.1%) 34 (21.5%)
Grilled Pork Chop 3.27 (1.06) 29 (6.2%) 5 (3.2%)
Italian Turkey
3.30 (0.70) 62 (13.2%) 19 (12.0%)
Sandwich
Note: a. The mean value of three items when the actual nutritional values were provided,
which were measured using the a five-point Likert scale, and 5 is the highest
value representing very healthy
b. Multiple Responses (Three out of Eight items)
The third objective of the pilot study was to construct the objective nutritional
analyzed to confirm the measures. The mean score for the nutritional knowledge
questions was 9.8 out of 16 points with standard deviation 2.26 (the median score was
10). There were three difficult questions for which fewer than 22% of respondents
provided correct answers and five relatively easy questions with more than 80% of
answers correct. The rates of correct answers for the other 8 questions were between 50%
and 80%. To better discriminate between levels of objective nutritional knowledge, two
questions related to fat were deleted because for both questions, 53% of answers were
54
correct and 27% of answers were “don’t know”. Instead, one difficult question related to
fat was added that 27% of respondents had correctly answered in a previous study
(Andrews, et al., 1998). All other questions were kept in the second pilot test.
Regarding menu items, Chicken Caesar Salad (16.3%), Chicken Fajitas (16.1%),
Grilled Salmon (15.3%), and Classic Sirloin Steak (15.1%) tended to be included in
respondents top-three items; the top choice among the three preferred items was the
Gilled Salmon (22.2%). But, the Classic Sirloin Steak (21.5%) and the Chicken Fajitas
(17.7%) were also chosen with similar frequencies. Because of this, Grilled Salmon and
Chicken Fajitas were selected as healthy and unhealthy target items (See Table 3.3).
However, to reduce the effects of food preference, the base ingredient of all selected
menu items was changed to chicken. According to Farm Animal Statistics estimated by
The Human Society of the U.S. (HSUS), the most commonly consumed meat in 2004
was chicken (85 pounds per person), which was followed by beef (65 pounds per person)
and pork (51 pounds per person). The amount of chicken consumed in the U.S. has been
continuously increasing, reflecting that Americans enjoy eating more chicken and that it
is becoming a popular food source. Because of this, the Grilled Salmon and the
California Turkey Club Sandwich were replaced with the Grilled Chicken and the Italian
Chicken Sandwich. The Classic Sirloin Steak, one of the popular items, was excluded
from the menu because it was not easy to find a substitutable chicken item. To create a
menu context including two alternative chicken items, the Lemon Herb Roasted Chicken
and the Italian Chicken Sandwich were selected to create the healthy menu context, while
55
the Chicken Caesar Salad and the Chicken Marsala were provided for an unhealthy menu
context. Thus, the following six menu items were selected to be used for the main
experiment: Lemon Herb Roasted Chicken, Grilled Chicken, Italian Chicken Sandwich,
To check the perceived healthiness of the six selected menu items and the
manipulation of nutritional values of each, a second pilot test was conducted. In addition,
The aim of the second pilot study was to check the consumer perceived
healthiness of the selected menu items without nutritional information, as well as the
manipulation of nutritional values of menu items. The reliability of all measures was also
tested. A total of 94 undergraduate students volunteered to take part in the second pilot
3.4.1 Procedure
The questionnaire asked respondents to evaluate six menu items, and to answer
demographic information questions. The six menu items were presented in a random
order, and the order in which the menu items were presented was also rotated to reduce
the order effect. To test the effect of the level of nutritional information, three different
levels of nutritional information were provided with the description of each menu item:
absence of nutritional information, total calories only, and nutritional information of six
56
nutrient contents. Participants were randomly assigned to receive one of the three levels
of nutritional information.
nutrition of each item, their overall attitude toward a menu item, and their purchase
intention for each of six menu items; they were also asked to choose one of six menu
items.
knowledge questions, while the third section asked about consumers’ individual
information, and future behavior intention related to the use of nutritional information.
Lastly, general questions about restaurant visits and demographic information such as
gender, age, income and ethnicity were asked. The questionnaire is attached in Appendix
B.
Caucasian. The majority of respondents (92%) were between the ages of 18 and 23.
Most of the respondents (77%) reported that their monthly income was less than $1,000;
fifty percent of respondents estimated that their monthly expenditures for food away from
home were between $100 and $200. Sixty-two percent of respondents visited restaurants
once or twice during a week, and the major nutritional information sources used were
The perceived nutritional level of the six menu items without nutritional
57
information was checked to manipulate the control group. The results of analysis of
perception among six menu items without nutritional information (F=20.8, d.f.=(5, 234),
p≤0.001). In particular, the Post Hoc test revealed that consumers perceived that the
nutritional levels of the Chicken Fajitas (mean=2.67 out of 5), the Chicken Marsala
(mean=3.04), and the Chicken Caesar Salad (mean=3.10) significantly differed from the
nutritional levels of the Italian Chicken Sandwich (mean= 3.40), the Lemon Herb
expected, the first three menu items, the Chicken Fajitas, the Chicken Marsala, and the
Chicken Caesar Salad, were selected as unhealthier menu items, while the Italian Chicken
Sandwich, the Lemon Herb Roasted Chicken, and the Grilled Chicken were chosen as
healthier items. Table 3.4 displays the mean values of perceived nutritional levels for the
Similar to the results of the first pilot test, the Chicken Fajitas (23%) and the
58
Grilled Chicken (22%) were the most popular menu items. The Chicken Fajitas was
selected as the unhealthy target item, and the Chicken Marsala and the Chicken Caesar
Salad were chosen as the unhealthy alternatives to manipulate the unhealthy menu
context. For the healthy target item, the Grilled Chicken was used since it was the most
popular item among three healthier items; however, the nutritional perception of the
Grilled Chicken became more negative as nutritional information was added (mean=4.24
without nutritional information; mean=3.97 with only total calories; mean=3.78 with
Chicken to be very healthy, once they had been provided with the healthy nutritional
information of the Grilled Chicken, they realized it was not as healthy as expected. As a
result, the Lemon Herb Roasted Chicken, which was chosen by eighteen percent of
respondents, was selected as the healthy target item since the Lemon Herb Roasted
Chicken did not have a large fluctuation in the perceived healthiness. The Grilled
Chicken and the Italian Chicken Sandwich were provided on the menu as healthier
options for the healthy menu context. The fact that the Grilled Chicken appeared less
healthy when nutritional information was provided also influenced the manipulated
nutritional values for both the Grilled Chicken and the Lemon Herb Roasted Chicken. In
order to maintain similar levels (12%) of nutritional values for the Lemon Herb Roasted
Chicken as those presented in the second pilot test, the nutritional values of the healthy
target item were slightly adjusted such that they accounted for 15% of daily
recommended values. This adjustment was necessary because the nutritional values of
the healthy target item had to be higher than the levels of the other healthy alternatives.
On the other hand, the nutritional values of the Grilled Chicken were changed to account
59
for 8% of daily recommended values because it was perceived as the healthiest item
among six menu items and the actual nutritional values (20%) made this item appear too
unhealthy. The nutritional levels of the Italian Chicken Sandwich were maintained at
their prior level in the second pilot test (10% of recommended values). The manipulated
nutritional values of the six menu items were very close or even healthier than the actual
The second pilot study also provided an opportunity to check the modified
objective nutritional knowledge questions. The mean score was 8.7 out of 15 questions
(median=9). The results of the second pilot test revealed that there were four difficult
questions and five easy questions. The reliabilities of all measures were tested and
Cronbach’s alpha of all instruments met the acceptable level; all Cronbach’s alpha values
were larger than 0.85. Thus, without any modifications, all measures were used in the
main survey.
60
Healthy Menu Item Unhealthy Menu Item
Target Item Alternative 1 Alternative 2 Target Item Alternative 1 Alternative 2
Nutrition Lemon Herb Italian Chicken Chicken Caesar Chicken
Grilled Chicken Chicken Fajitas
Content Roasted Chicken Sandwich Salad Marsala
Total calories 420 380 340 830 900 920
9g 6.5g 5.2g 38g 43g 46g
Total Fat
(14% DV) (10% DV) (8% DV) (58% DV) (66% DV) (71% DV)
3g 2g 1.4g 12g 13g 15g
Saturated Fat
(15% DV) (10% DV) (7% DV) (60% DV) (65% DV) (75% DV)
45mg 36mg 24mg 186mg 201mg 216mg
Cholesterol (15% DV) (12% DV) (8% DV) (62% DV) (67% DV) (72% DV)
384mg 264mg 216mg 1464mg 1560mg 1680mg
61
Sodium
(21% DV) (11% DV) (9% DV) (61% DV) (65% DV) (70% DV)
31g 34g 36g 15g 13g 10g
Protein
(62% DV) (68% DV) (72% DV) (30% DV) (26% DV) (20% DV)
Manipulated
15% DV 10% DV 8% DV 60% DV 65% DV 70% DV
Level
Note: The percent daily values are based on a 2,000 calorie diet.
61
3.5 Primary Study
3.5.1 Sample
The sample selected for this study consisted of undergraduate and graduate
students who were enrolled in marketing or hospitality management courses; any student
students, an email was sent to all graduate students in the home department. Potential
students were offered extra points in selected courses, while graduate students were
To assure reasonable power for the various analyses, the sample size was
calculated using the formula suggested by Dean and Voss (1999). Using the Tukey
multiple pair-wise comparisons of main factors, the sample size was calculated based on
differences between the levels of nutritional information. The formula for the confidence
interval is:
⎛ 2 ⎞
Mean difference between two levels ± qa ,v ( x −1),0.05 / 2 × msE ⎜ ⎟
144444244444 ⎝ bcx3⎠
msd
A reasonable estimate of the mean square error (MSE) was obtained from the
data collected in the second pilot study; estimates ranged from 0.47 to 0.88 depending on
the menu item at hand. The largest value, 0.88, was used as the estimate of the MSE in
the sample size calculation. Assuming an equal sample size for each condition, the
acceptable minimum significant difference (msd) was set at 0.2, a value which was based
on the result of the second pilot test. Thus, the following formula was developed:
⎛ 2 ⎞
q3,12( x −1),0.05 / 2 × (0.88)2 ⎜ ⎟ ≤ 0.2 Æ 2
q3,12( x −1),0.05 ≤ 1.037 x
⎝ 4x ⎠
The value of qa,v(x-1),0.05 was chosen from the Tukey’s method table (Dean & Voss,
1999); the appropriate sample size per condition (x) was identified by setting both sides
Thus, each condition required at least 13 participants, which made the total
In addition, the necessary sample size for this study was estimated using the
sample size calculator software, PASS 2008. The software’s method was based on a
multiple regression model that was employed for testing proposed hypotheses. By
providing the desired power, significance level and number of independent variables, the
software is then able to calculate the necessary sample size. It was expected that a power
level of 0.8 was sufficiently strong to detect the size of the effects of the independent
63
variables (Frazier, Tix, & Barron, 2004), and the sample size was generated to assure
estimates significant at the 0.05 significance level. Additionally, the model with the most
possible independent variables was specified. This model would contain five
independent variables, including the two indicator variables indicating the three levels of
nutritional information, the nutritional knowledge variable, and two interaction terms
between nutritional knowledge and nutritional information. It would also contain the
information of control variables: the two dummy variables meant to control for the effects
of menu context and the healthiness of the target item, were also included. This
information with expected overall R2 values was entered into the software. The software
produced a sample size estimate of 250, which would achieve 80% power to detect 0.05
Thus, the sample size objective was 250 participants, with at least 13 participants
per condition.
using the Media Lab computer program. The data were collected over the course of three
weeks, from April 13 to April 30. Participants were randomly assigned to one of the
An introduction page that explained the study and the procedure was presented,
and then participants were shown a menu that included three different items with or
Appendix C. Questions were presented one by one on the screen, and participants were
64
able to see the designed menu while they answered all questions so that they responded to
each question by comparing menu items without being required to guess or remember the
provided information.
Participants were asked to answer questions about the expected taste of a menu
item, nutritional perception, overall attitude toward a menu item, purchase intention, and
followed in sequence.
The data were analyzed using SPSS version 17.0 Windows. For testing the
reliability and internal validity of measures, reliability analysis using Cronbach’s alpha
and exploratory factor analysis (EFA) were utilized. Additionally, analysis of variance
(ANOVA) was executed for checking the manipulation of the nutritional values of the
menu items. For the hypothesis tests, multiple regression analysis was conducted.
process, and nutritional knowledge, a hierarchical multiple regression method was used.
65
CHAPTER 4: RESULTS
4.1 Sample
Approximately 27 participants were assigned to each of the twelve conditions (See Table
4.1).
The sample contained equal numbers of male and female participants, and the
undergraduate students and were an average age of 22 years. The average monthly
income was $743.02 and ranged between $0 and $5,400; twenty-eight percent of
respondents reported their monthly income as $0, a fact which might be due to the
66
student sample. Respondents spent an average $212.72 per month on food away from
home. Ninety-seven percent of respondents were non-vegetarian, and many reported that
their favorite food was beef (34%) or chicken (29%), as was expected. On average,
respondents ate at restaurants three times during a week; the number of weekly restaurant
visits ranged between 0 and 12. Further, participants reported eating at fast food
restaurants on average twice per week. The major sources from which respondents
other websites (22%) like Yahoo or Google. Other information sources included word of
mouth from family or friends and publications, such as books or magazines. Most
respondents (97%) reported that their current health status was good (mean=3.9 out of 5),
and that they were mainly concerned with diets for health reasons (mean=4.24 out of 5)
rather than for the weight loss reasons (mean=3.66). Using the reported respondents’
weight in pounds and height in inches, Body Mass Index (BMI) was calculated using the
following formula: [weight ÷ (height)2] × 703 (CDC, 2009). Most of the respondents
(70%) were normal weight with BMIs between 18.5 and 24.9, while twenty-four percent
of respondents having BMIs greater than 25 were overweight or obese. Table 4.2
67
Frequency Percent (%)
Gender
Male 160 50.2%
Female 159 49.8%
Race
Caucasian 224 70.2%
African American 13 4.1%
Hispanic 2 0.6%
Asian 78 24.5%
Native American 1 0.3%
Others 1 0.3%
Status
Undergraduate 293 91.8%
Graduate 23 7.5%
Staff 2 0.6%
Vegetarian
Yes 9 2.8%
No 310 97.2%
Type of Favorite Food
Beef 109 34.2%
Pork 6 1.9%
Chicken 93 29.2%
Lamb 7 2.2%
Turkey 14 4.4%
Seafood 64 20.1%
Others 26 8.2%
Nutritional Information Source a
TV 93 11.7%
Restaurant Website 191 24.1%
Other Website 173 21.8%
Newspaper 35 4.4%
Menu 197 24.8%
Ask to the server 58 7.3%
Others 46 5.8%
Total 793 100%
Continued
68
Table 4.2 continued
Exploratory factor analysis (EFA) was conducted to check the internal validity of
variables were included in the analysis using a Principle Component method with a
Varimax rotation. According to the result of the EFA, five components were identified
which had eigenvalues greater than 1.0 and which explained 82% of total variance. The
result revealed that measures of overall food attitude and purchase intention were
identified as one dimension, but that the other variables were clearly identified. Table 4.3
summarizes the factor loadings with total explained variances for each factor.
69
Scale Items Factor Loading
Overall Attitude toward a Item Q1 0.88
Overall Attitude toward a Item Q2 0.91
Overall Attitude toward a Item Q3 0.90
Purchase Intention Q1 0.95
Purchase Intention Q2 0.94
Purchase Intention Q3 0.93
Motivation to Process Q1 0.88
Motivation to Process Q2 0.87
Motivation to Process Q3 0.85
Motivation to Process Q4 0.84
Motivation to Process Q5 (R) a 0.80
Nutritional Perception Q1 0.86
Nutritional Perception Q2 0.87
Nutritional Perception Q3 0.89
Subjective Nutritional Knowledge Q1 0.87
Subjective Nutritional Knowledge Q2 0.76
Subjective Nutritional Knowledge Q3 0.82
Source Credibility Q1 0.90
Source Credibility Q2 0.92
Eigenvalue 5.19 3.99 2.46 2.22 1.72
Percent of Total Variance 27.30 20.97 12.94 11.69 9.08
Cumulative Percent of Total Variance 27.30 48.27 61.22 72.92 81.99
Note: a. Reverse Code
For testing the internal consistency of items, the reliability analysis was conducted
utilizing Cronbach’s alpha. For all continuous variables, the reliability analysis was
implemented using the total sample. The values of Cronbach’s alpha for all measures
ranged from 0.82 to 0.96, which indicated an acceptable level of reliability, α=0.70, for
70
all measures. Thus, it was concluded that the instruments utilized for this study had
internally consistent reliability in measuring the defined variables. Table 4.4 displays the
Prior to hypothesis testing, the manipulation check was completed to ensure that
the six menu items were placed in their proper nutritional menu contexts – healthy or
unhealthy – when nutritional information was not provided. For this manipulation check,
analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted using the respondents’ assessed values of
that there were significant differences between the perceived nutritional levels of the six
menu items (F=26.89, df=(5, 357), p≤0.001). Specifically, according to the tukey post
hoc test, the perceived nutritional levels of the Chicken Fajitas (mean=2.64 out of 5),
71
differed from the perceived nutritional levels of the Italian Chicken Sandwich
(mean=4.09). Among the six menu items, the Grilled Chicken was perceived as the
healthiest item, while the Chicken Fajitas was perceived as the least healthy item. The
average perceived nutrition levels of the menu items when nutritional information was
Table 4.5: Mean Values of Nutritional Perception of the Menu Items without Nutritional
Information
Regarding the choice of a menu item, respondents most often selected the
Chicken Fajitas (n=61, 24.4%) or the Lemon Herb Roasted Chicken (n=54, 21.6%); this
result confirms that popular items were selected as the healthy and unhealthy target items.
Thus, the six menu items were correctly manipulated for this study since the healthy
target item was the Lemon Herb Roasted Chicken, and the unhealthy target item was the
Chicken Fajitas. For the healthy menu context, the Italian Chicken Sandwich and the
Grilled Chicken were placed on the menu; the Chicken Caesar Salad and the Chicken
questions. For the six menu items, the mean values of source credibility ranged from
3.33 (Chicken Caesar Salad) to 3.95 (Grilled Chicken) out of a possible 5 points. The
scale was defined as 1=very inaccurate to 5=very accurate. There were significant
differences in the source credibility scores, depending on the menu item at hand (F=10.17,
df=(5, 946), p<0.01). The tukey post hoc test revealed that the mean value of the source
credibility score for the Grilled Chicken significantly differed from the mean values for
the other five items. This implies that consumers were more likely to trust the nutritional
information that was provided for the Grilled Chicken than they were to trust the
information provided for the other items; this may be because the Grilled Chicken had the
healthiest nutritional values among the items. However, source credibility scores did not
significantly differ with the level of nutritional information for any of the six menu items.
Focusing on the target items, multiple regression analyses were conducted separately for
each of three dependent variables: nutritional perception, overall attitude toward the
menu item, and purchase intention for the target item. To begin, the assumptions
necessary for multiple regression analysis were checked. There are four required
assumptions in multiple regression analysis, and tests were conducted to check each
assumption: the normality test, the test of independence of error terms, the check of
consistent error variance, and the test for linearity of the regression function (Neter,
Kutner, Nachtsheim, & Wasserman, 1996). For all estimated regression models, there
73
were no violations of either the linearity assumption or the normality assumption; these
were checked using the residual plot and the normal probability plot, respectively. The
scatter plots of residuals versus the order of respondents showed that the error terms were
independent of the order in which the data were collected for all models. Plots of
residuals versus the fitted values did not show any particular patterns for any of the
variables: age, gender, and Body Mass Index (BMI), and income. However, no
demographic variables were significant in the model, they were therefore excluded.
Multiple regression analysis was used to test the effect that nutritional information
dependent variables: nutritional perception, overall food attitude, and purchase intention.
Two dichotomous variables for three different nutritional information levels were
variable which distinguished a healthy target item from an unhealthy item; thus, the effect
of the nutritional level of target items could be controlled. The following regression
74
TI = Target Item with 1 = healthy target item or 0 = unhealthy target item
e = error term
information on the menu negatively influenced consumers’ food decision making when
the health of the target item was controlled for. The model fit for each of the three
equations was deemed significant at the 0.05 level by F-tests (Nutritional Perception:
F=36.93, d.f.=(3, 315), p<0.001; Overall food attitude: F=3.83, d.f.=(3, 315), p<0.01;
nutritional information when the health of the target item variable was included explained
25% of variance of nutritional perception, but accounted for only 3% and 2% of variance
of overall food attitude and purchase intention, respectively. Therefore, the level of
healthiness of menu items and nutritional information disclosure were more associated
with perceived nutrition of the menu item than they were with overall food attitude or
purchase intention.
Nutritional perception of the menu item decreased by 0.20 units when nutritional
information for the 6 nutrient contents was provided (Nutritional Perception: b3=-0.20,
S.E.=0.12). The presence of total calories information was also negatively associated
with customers’ nutritional perception, but the relationship was not significant. In the
models explaining overall attitude towards a menu item and purchase intention,
nutritional information levels, total calories only and nutritional information for the 6
nutrient contents, had significant negative effects (Overall food attitude: b2=-0.32
75
(S.E.=0.14), b3=-0.44 (S.E.=0.14); Purchase intention b2=-0.38 (S.E.=0.16), b3=-0.49
(S.E.=0.16)). This result implies that consumers negatively evaluated a menu item and
became less likely to purchase a menu item when nutritional information was provided
on the menu. This result supports hypothesis 1, which explained the influence of
between healthy and unhealthy target items in terms of either overall food evaluation or
purchase intention. Thus, overall attitude and purchase intention were not influenced by
the healthiness of the menu item. Table 4.6 outlines the results of the multiple regression
analyses.
Dependent Variables
Nutritional Purchase
Food Attitude
Perception Intention
Independent
B (S.E.) B (S.E.) B (S.E.)
Variables
Constant 2.66 (0.09)*** 3.95 (0.11)*** 3.89 (0.13)***
Target Item 0.99 (0.09)*** 0.12 (0.11) 0.05 (0.13)
Total Calories Info -0.06 (0.12) -0.32 (0.14)** -0.38 (0.16)**
Nutritional Info
-0.20 (0.12)* -0.44 (0.14)*** -0.49 (0.16)***
of 6 contents
Adj. R2 0.25 0.03 0.02
Model fit (F-Value) 36.93*** 3.83*** 3.41**
Note: *Significant at p<0.1; **Significant at p<0.05; ***Significant at p<0.01.
Standard errors are in the parentheses.
models were obtained, which allowed the predicted values of the three dependent
variables for each menu item to be calculated. Table 4.7 and Figure 4.1 display the
77
5
4
Nutritional Percaption
Overall Food Attitude
3 Purchase Intention
2
No Info Calories only NI of 6 contents
To test the main and moderating effects of nutritional menu context, multiple
regression analysis with two stages was implemented. In the first stage of analysis, a
model which included three independent variables was estimated. The three independent
variables included the indicator variables representing the three nutritional information
levels and the variable representing nutritional menu context. In the second stage, two
interaction terms were added to the model; the interaction variables were meant to
capture the interactions between the nutritional context and the nutritional information
levels. The interaction terms were calculated by determining the product of the
nutritional menu context variable and each of the two indicator variables representing the
three levels of nutritional information. The dummy variable which identified the target
item was also retained in the model as a control variable. Thus, the full regression model
78
DVi = β0 + β1·TI + β2·TC +β3·NI + β4·NC + β5(TC×NC) + β6(NI×NC) +e
e = error term
In the first stage of analysis, the models for all three dependent variables were
significant at the 0.01 level (Nutritional Perception: F=48.77, d.f.=(4, 314), p<0.001;
Overall food attitude: F=7.11, d.f.=(4, 314), p<0.001; Purchase Intention: F=7.50, d.f.=(4,
314), p<0.001). The nutritional information level and the nutritional menu context
and 8% of the variation in overall food attitude and purchase intention, respectively.
The effect of nutritional menu context on all three dependent variables was
significant at the 0.01 level. When other variables were held constant, consumers
perceived an item to be 0.69 units less nutritious in the healthy context than they did in
the unhealthy context (b4=-0.69, S.E.= 0.09). At the same time, consumers’ overall
attitude toward an item and likelihood of purchasing an item decreased by 0.46 and 0.57
79
units, respectively, when an item was placed with healthy items on the menu (Overall
food attitude: b4=-0.46, S.E.= 0.11; Purchase intention: b4=-0.57, S.E.= 0.13). Therefore,
the proposed hypothesis 2, that “the nutritional menu context will significantly influence
Additionally, the nutritional level of the target item had a significant effect on
significantly differed by 0.98 units from their perceived nutritional level of an unhealthy
item. The significant effect of presented nutritional information was maintained after
When the interactions were included in the second stage of analysis, the adjusted
for both the overall food attitude and purchase intention models. In addition, for all three
dependent variables, the changes in F-value due to the addition of the interaction terms
When the level of nutritional information was interacted with the menu context,
the variable had a significant effect on all three of the dependent variables (Nutritional
(S.E.= 0.27), b6=-0.75 (S.E.=0.27); Purchase intention: b5=-0.99 (S.E.= 0.31), b6=-0.72
(S.E.=0.31)). This indicates that consumers’ nutritional perception of a menu item when
the item was placed in the healthy context decreased by 0.55 units when they were
80
exposed to total calories information and by 0.67 units when they were exposed to
nutritional information for the 6 nutrient contents, when compared to the no nutritional
information treatment. Similarly, overall attitude toward a menu item when the item was
placed in the healthy context was 0.79 units less when total calories were disclosed and
0.75 units less when the nutritional information of the 6 nutrient contents was disclosed
than when no nutritional information was provided. In addition, consumers were less
likely to purchase an item placed in the healthy context when total calories only or
nutritional information for the 6 nutrient contents was offered than when no information
was provided. Thus, hypothesis 3, that “the nutritional menu context will significantly
With inclusion of the interaction terms, the main effect of nutritional information
disclosure was eliminated for all three dependent variables, but the main effect of the
The results of these multiple regression analyses can be found in Table 4.8; the
interaction effects between the nutritional information levels and the nutritional context
on the three dependent variables are depicted in Figures 4.2, 4.3, and 4.4.
81
Model I Model II
Nutritional Attitude Purchase Nutritional Attitude Purchase
Independent Perception toward food Intention Perception toward food Intention
Variables
B (S.E.) B (S.E.) B (S.E.) B (S.E.) B (S.E.) B (S.E.)
Constant 3.02 (0.10)*** 4.18 (0.13)*** 4.18 (0.15)*** 2.81 (0.11)*** 3.92 (0.15)*** 3.89 (0.17)***
Target Item 0.98 (0.09)*** 0.12 (0.11) 0.04 (0.13) 0.99 (0.08)*** 0.12 (0.11) 0.04 (0.13)
Total Calories -0.06 (0.11) -0.32 (0.14)** -0.39 (0.16)** 0.21 (0.15) 0.07 (0.19) 0.11 (0.22)
Nutritional Info
-0.21 (0.11)** -0.44 (0.14)*** -0.50 (0.16)*** 0.12 (0.15) -0.07 (0.19) -0.14 (0.22)
of 6 contents
Context -0.69 (0.09)*** -0.46 (0.11)*** -0.57 (0.13)*** -0.29 (0.15)** 0.06 (0.19) 0.002 (0.22)
Cal*Context -0.55 (0.21)*** -0.79 (0.27)*** -0.99 (0.31)***
82
82
5
Nutritional Perception
4
Healthy Context
Unhealthy Context
3
2
No Info Calories only NI of 6 contents
Figure 4.2: Interaction Effect between the Presence of Nutritional Information and the
Nutritional Menu Context on Nutritional Perception
5
Overall Food Attitude
Healthy Context
Unhealthy Context
3
2
No Info Calories only NI of 6 contents
Figure 4.3: Interaction Effect between the Presence of Nutritional Information and the
Nutritional Menu Context on Overall Food Attitude
83
5
4
Purchase Intention
Healthy Context
3 Unhealthy Context
2
No Info Calories only NI of 6 contents
Figure 4.4: Interaction Effect between the Presence of Nutritional Information and the
Nutritional Menu Context on Purchase Intention
Based on the estimated models for the three dependent variables, predicted values
were obtained and are displayed in Table 4.9. Without nutritional information, consumers’
perceived nutritional level for a healthy item was expected to be 3.31 out of 5 points in
the healthy context and 4.00 out of 5 points in the unhealthy context. On the other hand,
84
Healthy Item
Healthy Context Unhealthy Context
Total NI of 6 Total NI of 6
Control Control
Calories contents Calories contents
Nutritional
3.31 3.25 3.10 4.00 3.94 3.79
Perception
Attitude
3.84 3.52 3.40 4.30 3.98 3.86
toward food
Purchase
3.65 3.26 3.15 4.22 3.83 3.72
Intention
Unhealthy Item
Healthy Context Unhealthy Context
Total NI of 6 Total NI of 6
Control Control
Calories contents Calories contents
Nutritional
2.33 2.27 2.13 3.02 2.96 2.81
Perception
Attitude
3.72 3.40 3.28 4.18 3.86 3.74
toward food
Purchase
3.61 3.22 3.11 4.18 3.79 3.68
Intention
Table 4.9: Predicted Values of Dependent Variables without Interaction Terms b/w the
Nutritional Menu Context and the Nutritional Information Level
information (MOT), multiple regression analysis with two stages was conducted. In the
first stage of analysis, two indicator variables for the three nutritional information levels
and a continuous variable for the motivation to process nutritional information were
included as independent variables in the model. In the second stage, two interaction
terms between the motivation to process information and each of the nutritional
information levels expressed by the two indicator variables were added to the model.
85
Due to a multi-collinearity problem among independent variables, a centered variable for
motivation to process information was added to the model. The centered variable was
calculated by subtracting the mean value (mean=3.22) from the original value of
motivation to process. Then, the interaction terms were obtained by multiplying the
centered motivation value and the indicator variables of nutritional information levels.
The effects of the healthiness of the target item and the nutritional context were also kept
in the model. The following regression model was used to test the moderating effect of
+ β7(NI×CMOT) + e
e = error term
When interaction terms were not included, the suggested regression model was
significant at the 0.01 level for all three of the dependent variables (Nutritional
86
Perception: F=38.94, d.f.=(5, 313), p<0.001; Overall food attitude: F=5.68, d.f.=(5, 313),
variables with two control variables. The variables also accounted for 7% of total
nutritional information when other variables were held constant. But, the main effects of
both nutritional context and the presence of nutritional information were still significant
levels and the motivation to process to the nutritional perception model, adjusted R2
increased by 2 percentage points. Also, after including the interaction terms, the F-value
change was only significant in the nutritional perception model (F=4.11, df=(2, 311),
p<0.05). The changes in adjusted R2 were not significant in the using overall food
There was, however, a significant effect of the interaction between the nutritional
perception and overall food attitude. In particular, the interaction between the presence
of nutritional information for the 6 nutrient contents and motivation to process was found
that consumers’ perceived nutritional level of a menu item decreased by 0.18 units as
their motivation to process increased by one unit, only if nutritional information for the 6
87
had more favorable food attitudes for menu items only when total calorie information
was provided (b6=0.23, S.E.=0.13). Thus, overall food attitude increased by 0.23 units
when consumers’ motivation to process increased by 1 unit, only when total calorie
information was disclosed. This result partially supports hypothesis 4 that predicted that
decision making. Table 4.10 displays the results of this multiple regression analysis.
Figure 4.5 and Figure 4.6 show the interaction plots that represent the significant
interaction effects on nutritional perception and overall food attitude. For the interaction
plots of nutritional information levels with motivation to process, the three different
levels of motivation to process were calculated by adding and subtracting two times the
standard deviation from zero, which was the mean of the centered motivation to process
variable (S.D.=1.14). The interaction plots show that consumers with high motivation to
process perceived the nutritional level of a menu item differently than do those with low
or medium levels of motivation to process. The negative association between the high
motivation to process and the nutritional information level is clear (See Figure 4.5). In
terms of overall attitude toward a menu item, consumers with moderate levels of
motivation to process had more favorable attitudes toward a menu item when nutritional
information was provided. However, consumers who had low or high motivation levels
showed a more negative attitude toward menu items when nutritional information was
88
Model I Model II
Nutritional Attitude Purchase Nutritional Attitude Purchase
Independent Perception toward food Intention Perception toward food Intention
Variables
B (S.E.) B (S.E.) B (S.E.) B (S.E.) B (S.E.) B (S.E.)
Constant 3.02 (0.10)*** 4.18 (0.13)*** 4.18 (0.15)*** 3.02 (0.10)*** 4.17 (0.13)*** 4.19 (0.15)***
Target Item 0.98 (0.09)*** 0.12 (0.11) 0.04 (0.13) 0.99 (0.09)*** 0.13 (0.11) 0.05 (0.13)
Context -0.69 (0.09)*** -0.46 (0.11)*** -0.57 (0.13)*** -0.68 (0.09)*** -0.46 (0.11)*** -0.57 (0.13)***
Total Calories -0.07 (0.11) -0.32 (0.14)** -0.39 (0.16)** -0.09 (0.11) -0.32 (0.14)** -0.39 (0.16)**
Nutritional Info
-0.22 (0.11)** -0.44 (0.14)*** -0.50 (0.16)*** -0.22 (0.11)** -0.43 (0.14)*** -0.49 (0.16)***
of 6 contents
Motivation (CMOT) 0.01 (0.04) -0.009 (0.05) 0.000 (0.06) 0.06 (0.07) -0.08 (0.09) -0.02 (0.10)
89
Table 4.10: Results of Regression Analysis: Effect of Motivation to Process Nutritional Information
89
5
Nutritional perception
4
Low MOT
Medium MOT
High MOT
3
2
No Info Calories only NI of 6 contents
Figure 4.5: Interaction Effect between the Presence of Nutritional Information and the
Motivation to Process on Nutritional Perception
5
Overall Food Attitude
4
Low MOT
Medium MOT
High MOT
3
2
No Info Calories only NI of 6 contents
Figure 4.6: Interaction Effect between the Presence of Nutritional Information and the
Motivation to Process on Overall Food Attitude
90
4.4.4 The Role of Nutritional Knowledge
regression analysis. Again, two stages of analyses were completed. Like in the previous
model, two indicator variables representing the three nutritional information levels and a
continuous variable for nutritional knowledge were included in the first stage model as
instruments: objective nutritional knowledge was measured using fifteen multiple choice
questions and subjective nutritional knowledge was measured using a five-point Likert-
scale. Thus, two different models – one for objective nutritional knowledge and one for
+ β7(NI×CONK) + e
+ β7(NI×CSNK) + e
91
NI×CONK = Interaction term between NI and CONK
e = error term
In the second stage, the model included interaction terms between objective
nutritional knowledge and each of the two nutritional information level variables, as well
as between subjective nutritional knowledge and each of the two nutritional information
level variables. The interaction terms were obtained by calculating the product of the
nutritional information indicator variable and the knowledge score, for both the
subjective and objective knowledge scores. The interaction terms again created multi-
collinearity problems in the models, so the centered objective and subjective nutritional
knowledge variables were used instead of the original values of the nutritional knowledge
variables. The centered objective and subjective nutritional knowledge values were
calculated as the differences between the original values and the mean values (mean of
knowledge=3.20 out of 5). Using the centered variables, the interaction terms were
recalculated and included in the model. The effects of the target item and the nutritional
For the models which included the objective nutritional knowledge variable, the
model fits for each of the three dependent variables were significant at the 0.01 level
92
(Nutritional Perception: F=38.90, d.f.=(5, 313), p<0.001; Overall food attitude: F=5.71,
d.f.=(5, 313), p<0.001; Purchase Intention: F=6.02, d.f.=(5, 313), p<0.001). Nutritional
information levels and objective nutritional knowledge accounted for 37% of total
points when the interaction terms were included. The independent variables alone
explained only 7% of total variance of both overall food attitude and purchase intention.
However, for these two independent variables, inclusion of the interaction terms between
nutritional information levels and objective nutritional knowledge did not significantly
increase the explained variation. Including interaction terms did not significantly change
F-values for any of the three dependent variables (See Table 4.11).
did not carry a significant main effect when other variables were kept constant.
information and objective nutritional knowledge was not found for any of the three
dependent variables: nutritional perception, overall food attitude, and purchase intention.
However, the other independent variables such as the nutritional context and the presence
of nutritional information did influence the three dependent variables. This implies that
consumers’ objective nutritional knowledge did not significantly moderate the effect of
the presence of nutritional information. Table 4.11 outlines the result of multiple
regression analysis.
93
Model I Model II
Nutritional Attitude Purchase Nutritional Attitude Purchase
Independent Perception toward food Intention Perception toward food Intention
Variables
B (S.E.) B (S.E.) B (S.E.) B (S.E.) B (S.E.) B (S.E.)
Constant 3.02 (0.10)*** 4.19 (0.13)*** 4.19 (0.15)*** 3.01 (0.10)*** 4.19 (0.13)*** 4.21 (0.15)***
Target Item 0.98 (0.09)*** 0.12 (0.11) 0.04 (0.13) 0.99 (0.09)*** 0.11 (0.11) 0.03 (0.13)
Context -0.69 (0.09)*** -0.46 (0.11)*** -0.57 (0.13)*** -0.70 (0.09)*** -0.45 (0.11)*** -0.56 (0.13)***
Total Calories -0.07 (0.11) -0.33 (0.14)** -0.40 (0.16)** -0.06 (0.11) -0.35 (0.14)** -0.41 (0.16)**
Nutritional Info
-0.22 (0.11)** -0.45 (0.14)*** -0.51 (0.16)*** -0.21 (0.11)* -0.45 (0.14)*** -0.52 (0.16)***
of 6 contents
Objective NK
0.003 (0.01) 0.01 (0.02) 0.01 (0.03) -0.02 (0.03) 0.02 (0.04) 0.05 (0.16)
(CONK)
94
94
The Effect of Subjective Nutritional Knowledge
estimated. The fit of all three models was significant at the 0.01 level (Nutritional
Perception: F=38.89, d.f.=(5, 313), p<0.001; Overall food attitude: F=5.78, d.f.=(5, 313),
the total variation in nutritional perception scores was explained by the independent
variables and control variables, while only 7% of the variation of both overall food
attitude and purchase intention was explained by the independent variables and control
variables. In the second stage, interaction terms between nutritional information levels
and subjective nutritional knowledge were added, but they did not significantly increase
the adjusted R2. Also, the changes in F-value due to the addition of the interaction terms
were not large enough to be significant for any of the three dependent variables:
knowledge did not carry a significant main effect for any of the three dependent variables
(Nutritional perception; b5=0.001, S.E.=0.05; Overall food attitude: b5=-0.049, S.E.= 0.06;
Purchase intention: b5=0.07, S.E.= 0.07). Additionally, the interaction terms between
nutritional information levels and subjective nutritional knowledge did not influence
attitude, and purchase intention were unaffected by the interaction terms. However,
similar to the results of the prior analyses, the nutritional context and the presence of
95
nutritional information were significantly associated with all three dependent variables.
Consequently, the level of consumers’ subjective nutritional knowledge did not moderate
the effect that nutritional information disclosure had on consumers’ food decision making
process. Table 4.12 shows the results of the multiple regression analysis related to
between objective and subjective nutritional knowledge would have any effect on the
three dependent variables. After adding the interaction term to both the objective and
between objective and subjective nutritional knowledge on consumer food evaluation and
insignificant.
making, and thus hypothesis 5 that proposed that nutritional knowledge could act as a
moderator in the effect of nutritional information disclosure was not supported in this
study.
96
Model I Model II
Nutritional Attitude Purchase Nutritional Attitude Purchase
Independent Perception toward food Intention Perception toward food Intention
Variables
B (S.E.) B (S.E.) B (S.E.) B (S.E.) B (S.E.) B (S.E.)
Constant 3.02 (0.10)*** 4.19 (0.13)*** 4.19 (0.15)*** 3.02 (0.10)*** 4.19 (0.13)*** 4.20 (0.15)***
Target Item 0.98 (0.09)*** 0.12 (0.11) 0.05 (0.13) 0.99 (0.09)*** 0.12 (0.11) 0.05 (0.13)
Context -0.69 (0.09)*** -0.46 (0.11)*** -0.58 (0.13)*** -0.70 (0.09)*** -0.47 (0.11)*** -0.58 (0.13)***
Total Calories -0.06 (0.11) -0.33 (0.14)** -0.40 (0.16)** -0.07 (0.11) -0.34 (0.14)** -0.41 (0.16)**
Nutritional Info
-0.21 (0.11)** -0.45 (0.14)*** -0.51 (0.16)*** -0.21 (0.11)** -0.45 (0.14)*** -0.51 (0.16)***
of 6 contents
Subjective NK
0.001 (0.05) 0.04 (0.06) 0.07 (0.07) -0.02 (0.08) 0.06 (0.11) 0.09 (0.12)
(CSNK)
97
97
Based on the results of multiple regression analysis, the results of the hypothesis
Hypotheses Result
Although a hypothesis about the actual choice of a menu item was not pre-
specified, additional analysis was conducted to investigate what factors may be relevant
predictors of item choice. Respondents were asked to select one item among the three
98
provided items on the menu; their choice was recorded as an indicator variable, which
equaled 1 if the target item was selected and equaled 0 otherwise. This variable acted as
the dependent variable in a logistic regression analysis, an analysis method used to find
the first logistic regression model; nutritional level of the target item, level of nutritional
nutritional knowledge. The second model also contained other factors which could
influence the choice of menu item, such as the perceived healthiness of an item, the
expectation of taste and diet concerns for health or weight loss. In addition, while this
information disclosure, in past studies, only consumers’ perceived nutrition had been
considered relevant in food choices (Lee & Cranage, 2007; Yoon, Sydny-Busso, & Parsa,
2009). Overall food attitude and purchase intention, however, have been mainly used as
proxy measures of the actual food choice. Thus, overall food attitude and purchase
intention were included in the final model with other variables. Demographic variables
including age, gender, and BMI were also excluded in the model due to non-significance.
Using Hosmer and Lemeshow (H-L)’ goodness of fit test, the models’ fit was
tested. Results indicated that the second model was the only well fitted model since it
According to the results of the logistic regression, the presence of total calorie
information had a significant effect on the choice of target menu items (Exp (BTC)=0.62).
The odds of a target item over non-target item choice decreased by 0.62 when total
99
calorie information was provided. Further, when objective nutritional knowledge
increased by 1 score, the odds of a target item menu choice decreased by 0.91. However,
consumers selected the target items almost 3 times more often when it was placed on the
menu with unhealthy menu items as compared to when it was placed in the healthy menu
In the second model, it was found that a choice of a target item became 1.44 times
more likely with every 1 unit increase in health concern, but that the odds of the target
item choice decreased by 0.79 when interest in weight loss increased by 1 unit. In
addition, consumers selected target items 2.86 times more often when the expectation of
taste increased by 1 unit and 1.35 times more often when the perceived nutritional value
According to results of the third model, overall food attitude and purchase
intention were significantly associated with the choice of target items. A one unit
increase in overall food attitude and purchase intention made consumers choose target
items 2.27 times more often and 6.25 units more often, respectively. However, nutritional
The relationship between nutritional information disclosure and item choice was
Consequently, consumers were more likely to use the total calories information than they
were to use the nutritional information of 6 nutrient contents in the choice of a menu item.
Nutritional menu context was also an influential factor in choosing a menu item. As
expected, overall attitude toward a menu item and purchase intention were good
predictors of consumer choice behavior. The results of logistic regression are presented
100
in Table 4.14.
101
CHAPTER 5: CONCLUSIONS
The purpose of this study was to examine the effect of nutritional information
the nutritional information process were also investigated. In detail, this study
determined the effect of three different levels of nutritional information and three
variables that influence consumer food decision making: menu context, motivation to
students at a large mid-western university and analyzed using several methods of analysis
such as analysis of variance, multiple regression, and logistic regression. The results of
this study are summarized in this chapter. This chapter also includes conclusions,
menu significantly influenced consumer food attitude and purchase behavior. The
concerning the six nutrient contents, lead consumers to have less favorable attitudes
102
toward a menu item and less purchase intention for that item. Despite providing
nutritional values similar to, or even healthier than, the actual nutritional levels of menu
items, consumers evaluated both healthy and unhealthy menu items more negatively.
This may be due to the fact that the provided nutritional level of an item on the menu is
disappointed with the manipulated nutritional level of an item, and thus their attitude
toward the menu item and purchase intention would be less favorable. This finding
supports the results of previous studies which showed that consumers tended to
underestimate calories or fat, and that the differences between the expected and the actual
nutritional values tend to be greater for unhealthy foods (Burton, et al., 2006; Chandon &
menu item was only influenced when the nutritional information provided included the
total calories plus five nutrient contents of the item. This implies that consumers did not
change their nutritional perception of a menu when total calories alone were provided.
On the other hand, the result of logistic regression revealed that total calorie information
significantly influenced menu choice. Consumers chose a target item less frequently
when exposed to total calories. Rather than processing nutritional information of all six
nutrient contents, consumers used only total calorie information in selecting an item,
perhaps because calorie information is easy and simple to use in the choice. Thus,
consumers are likely to use simple information at the point of purchase, but nutritional
information for the six nutrient contents did influence their perception or attitude
103
Consequently, nutritional information for the six nutrient contents directly influenced
food evaluation and purchase intention but only indirectly influenced the actual choice
behavior, while total calorie information had a direct effect on overall food attitude,
purchase intention, as well as the choice itself. Thus, the presence of total calorie
the menu and to choose the healthy item in a restaurant. However, from a long-term
choices.
In this study, there were several interesting findings which relate to the nutritional
menu context. Consistent with prior studies, the nutritional menu context significantly
influenced consumer food decision making processes, as well as food choice (Kozup, et
al., 2003; Burton & Creyer, 2004). Comparing the provided nutritional information of all
alternatives on the menu influenced how consumers perceived the nutritional levels of a
target menu item: consumers tended to evaluate an item relative to others. Comparing
effects across dependent variables, nutritional menu context had the greatest effect on
consumers’ nutritional perception (See Table 4.8). Thus, a menu item accompanied by
less healthy items was perceived as a more healthy food and evaluated more favorably
than had it been accompanied by healthy items. Furthermore, menu items in the
unhealthy context were three times more likely to be chosen than items in the healthy
context. Consequently, the context effect existed in evaluating menu items using the
104
provided nutritional information, and thus nutritional information of alternatives on the
menu might be considered as the reference point from which a target item is evaluated.
This finding is consistent with prior studies (Burton & Creyer, 2004; Kozup, et al., 2003).
Along with the significant main effect of nutritional menu context, the interaction
of nutritional information disclosure with nutritional menu context also had a significant
effect on consumer food decision making. The healthy menu context further worsened
evaluation and purchase intention. This result is consistent with previous studies wherein
evaluation (Burton & Creyer, 2004; Kozup, et al., 2003). In particular, the presence of
total calories in the healthy context had the greatest negative effect on consumer purchase
intention (See Table 4.8) among the three dependent variables, while nutritional
information for total calories plus five nutrients in the healthy context had an especially
main effect on consumer food decision making, it played a role as a significant moderator
perceived nutrition and overall attitude toward a menu item. More specifically, highly
motivated consumers perceived the nutritional level of a menu item more negatively only
if nutritional information for the six nutrient contents was presented, whereas for
105
consumers with low or medium levels of motivation to process, exposure to nutritional
information for the six nutrient contents lead to improved nutritional perception. Thus,
the change in nutritional perception due to the presence of total calories plus five nutrient
contents information was greater when the motivation to process nutritional information
was high.
Further, when total calorie information was present, the level of consumers’
had medium levels of motivation to process positively evaluated a menu item, while
consumers low or high in motivation showed the more negative attitude toward a menu
item. However, there was no significant interaction effect between motivation and
consumer’s food decision making process; further, knowledge scores did not moderate
the main effect of nutritional information disclosure on consumer food evaluation and
purchase intention.
associations with source credibility (bobj=0.05, p<0.05; bsbj=0.09, p<0.10). This implies
that consumers high in objective or subjective nutritional knowledge believed that the
106
provided nutritional information was more accurate (See Table D.1 in Appendix D).
Although the presented nutritional values of menu items were manipulated, they were
close to the actual nutritional values. Thus, consumers high in nutritional knowledge had
more ability to process the provided nutritional information accurately. This result is
consistent with previous studies, which indicated that nutritional knowledge had a
significant effect on nutrition label use and accuracy of nutritional information use
effective when consumers have a high level of motivation to process information. This
means that there should be a significant effect of nutritional knowledge on consumer food
Thus, the effect of nutritional knowledge interacted with motivation to process was
further investigated. According to the results of multiple regression, the interaction effect
overall food attitude and purchase intention (overall food attitude: b=-0.12, p<0.05;
purchase intention: b=-0.13, p<0.05) (See Table D.2 in Appendix D). This result
illustrates a very interesting point about nutritional knowledge. Consumers high in both
subjective nutritional knowledge and motivation to process, which are associated with
consumers’ psychological state, evaluated a menu item more unfavorably and were less
consumer food evaluation, however, was not influenced by the level of motivation to
process; further, the results of the logistic regression showed that objective nutritional
107
knowledge directly influenced the choice of a menu item (See Table 4.14). When other
variables were held constant, the odds ratio indicated that the choice of a target item over
a non-target item became significantly less likely when consumers’ objective nutritional
knowledge was significantly associated with how accurately they used nutritional
information. Also, objective nutritional knowledge itself affected consumer food choice
directly, but subjective nutritional knowledge influenced food evaluation and purchase
intention only when consumers had enough motivation to process nutritional information.
healthy behaviors.
evaluation, purchase intention and choice itself depending on demographic variables such
as age, income, and gender were not found. However, other attributes such as taste
expectation or interest in weight loss or health were significantly related to food choice.
According to the results of logistic regression model II (Table 4.14), increased nutritional
perception or taste expectation lead consumers to select a target item more frequently. In
particular, the expected taste of an item (Exp(B)=2.86, p<0.01) had a greater effect on the
likelihood of the item choice than did the perceived nutrition of the item (Exp(B)=1.35,
108
p<0.10). This result is consistent with other previous studies (Lee & Cranage, 2007;
Palmer & Leontos, 1995). Moreover, diet concerns for health or weight loss were
influential factors; if consumers were more interested in diet for health reasons, the
likelihood of the choice of a target item increased 1.44 times; if they were concerned with
diet for weight-loss reasons, the likelihood of the choice of a target item decreased. Thus,
the structure of consumer food choice behavior in a restaurant is complicated and other
restaurants becomes increasingly prevalent, the discussion about whether labeling laws
will help improve both consumer healthy eating and the negative image of the restaurant
industry has become controversial. Thus, this study examined the effect of nutritional
information disclosure on menus and several factors affecting the use of nutritional
information in order to provide more effective strategies for policy makers and
restaurateurs. Using the experimental design method, several interesting findings were
obtained; significant implications for both practical and research areas arose.
Firstly, the results of this study indicate that the availability of nutritional
total calorie information had a significant effect. The law requiring only total calories on
the menu, which was enacted by New York City, seems to be more efficient for both
109
nutritional information of other nutrients also played a significant role in changing
consumers’ menu choice indirectly. Thus, the influence that providing nutritional
information of other nutrients should not be passed over in enacting menu labeling laws.
In the case of King County, it is required to post calorie, fat, sodium, and carbohydrate
information on menus. But, restaurants using menu boards display only calorie
information with price on the board, and nutritional information for other nutrients is
offered in a plainly visible format (Conlin, July 27, 2007). The King County model
allows consumers to use more nutritional information in selecting food and policy makers
information for the six nutrient contents – had significant negative effects on consumer
food decision making. When nutritional information was presented on the menu,
consumers evaluated the nutritional levels of menu items and overall food more
unfavorably and were less likely to purchase items. This implies that consumers
expected that menu items were much healthier than they were in reality. Thus, it is
necessary to inform consumers of the exact nutritional information of menu items and
with the accuracy of the use of nutritional information, and there were significant effects
and purchase intention. Thus, it is important not only to provide more opportunities to
110
learn about health and nutrition but also to increase consumers’ interest or concern with
In the restaurant industry, one way to reduce the negative effect of nutritional
information disclosure is to design the menu more effectively. It was found that the
nutritional menu context not only influenced consumer food decision making but
moderated the effect of nutritional information disclosure. When a menu item was
accompanied by healthier items, it was evaluated more unfavorably than when it was
accompanied by less healthy items; this effect became stronger when nutritional
information was provided. Thus, how items are grouped together on a menu becomes a
critical issue. For example, it is possible to design a menu page for only healthy items or
a menu emphasizing a few healthy items in an unhealthy context to reduce the context
particular item in terms of healthiness, restaurateurs can utilize the menu context effect.
Finally, this study contributes to the existing literature in several ways. Although
a number of studies have been conducted meant to determine the effect of nutritional
information disclosure, they have been associated with The Nutrition Labeling and
Education Act (NLEA) and therefore, limited to packaged foods. This study, however,
focused on consumers’ nutritional information use in a restaurant context; the study also
was found that there is a more complicated structure underlying consumer food decision
making. The three dependent variables in this study – nutritional perception, overall food
111
attitude, and purchase intention – were significantly correlated. In addition, nutritional
perception did not have a direct relationship with the actual food choice, while other two
dependent variables were significant predictors of consumer food choice (See Table 4.14).
This result implies that overall food attitude and purchase intention mediated the
relationship between nutritional perception and actual food choice, and that consumers’
choice decision. Accordingly, this study provides the initial outline of the consumer food
decision process when nutritional information is provided; this issue should be dealt with
in future research.
industry, it has several limitations that may have influenced the results. First, the sample
facilitated control of other influential demographic factors in the experiment, but the
homogeneity also limits the scope of this study to one typical segment of consumers.
Student perception and behavior in a restaurant may differ from the behavior of others,
such as people who are older or less educated. Thus, the findings cannot be generalized
to all consumers in other market segments. It would therefore be helpful to conduct the
which is a relatively small sample size even though the total sample size was sufficient,
more than 300. Thus, a larger sample size would allow for stronger results.
112
Target items were always accompanied by alternatives on the menu so as to
mimic a real restaurant situation as closely as possible: there is no restaurant menu where
only one item is offered on the menu. However, this requires that menu context must
always be controlled for in testing other effects such as the main effects of nutritional
of the effects of other variables would have been facilitated by the presence of a control
group, a group which was not provided with a menu context. Thus, future study should
employ a modified experimental design including three different types of menu context:
Another limitation of the study is due to the selected menu items. This study
adopted menu items only available in a casual or fine dining restaurant, and only chicken
items were offered to eliminate food preferences. Thus, there is a possibility that
consumers use different evaluation criteria or decision making processes when nutritional
information is provided in fast food restaurants. Furthermore, it was found that meat was
another preferred food source (34%), so consumers’ food preferences were not
portion of the sample (3%), they remain another segment of consumers. Thus, there
influences food choice. In the study conducted by Dorms (2006), it was found that
consumers are more likely to choose tasty foods when they eat out because they prefer to
enjoy themselves regardless of money or health. Lee and Cranage (2007) also found that
113
the relative importance of taste in menu selection was greater than the importance of
nutrition. Therefore, in future research, how food preferences and taste influence
investigated.
Next, the questions used to measure objective knowledge were selected from
existing questionnaires, which were related to the provided six nutrient contents.
Although the questions were tested through two pilot tests, they did not provide a
standardized instrument for measuring the nutritional knowledge level. There were 7
questions with rates of the correct answer between 50% and 80%, and more than 80% of
respondents answered correctly for other 2 questions (See Table E.1 in Appendix E).
Despite of the moderate level of nutritional knowledge questions, the mean score of
objective nutritional knowledge was 8.17, which indicates the average rate of correct
answers is just 54%. Thus, in this study, the objective nutritional knowledge measures in
A final issue that can be considered in a future study is the relationship among
influential factors on consumer food decision. In this study, how taste and diet concerns
influenced food choice was investigated. However, consumer food choice at a restaurant
could be affected by other important factors such as food quality, taste, menu variety,
price, convenience, and ambience (Lee & Cranage, 2007; Neumark-Sztainer, Story, Perry,
& Casey, 1999; Yoon, et al., 2009). Similar to the results obtained though the logistic
regression in this study, the taste of food, which was considered as the most important
factor on food choice, was closely associated with health. According to Raghunathan, et
114
al. (2006), consumers would choose a tasty food rather than a healthy food if they have to
motivation to process information and nutritional knowledge, should influence the use of
nutritional information and consumer food choice in restaurants. As shown in the logistic
regression result, for instance, consumers’ diet concern was a significant predictor of food
choice. Thus, how the disclosure of nutritional information is associated with other
influential factors and what other factors can influence the use of nutritional information
115
References
Almanza, B. A., Nelson, D., & Chai, S. (1997). Obstacles to nutrition labeling in
restaurants. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 97(2), 157-161.
Andrews, J. C., Burton, S., & Netemeyer, R. G. (2000). Are Some Comparative Nutrition
Claims Misleading? The Role of Nutrition Knowledge, Ad Claim Type and
Disclosure Conditions. [Article]. Journal of Advertising, 29(3), 29-42.
Axelson, M. L., & Penfield, M. P. (1983). Factors associated with food expenditures of
elderly persons living alone. Family and Consumer Sciences Research Journal,
12(2), 228-236.
Bagozzi, R. P., Gurhan-Canli, Z., & Priester, J. R. (2002). The social psychology of
consumer behaviour: Open University Press Buckingham, England.
Balasubramanian, S. K., & Cole, C. (2002). Consumers' search and use of nutrition
information: the challenge and promise of the nutrition labeling and education act.
The Journal of Marketing, 66(3), 112-127.
Bettman, J. R., Johnson, E. J., & Payne, J. W. (1991). Consumer decision making. In T. S.
Robertson & H. H. Kassarjian (Eds.), Handbook of Consumer Behavior (pp. 50-
84): Prentice-Hall.
Binkley, J. K., Eales, J., & Jekanowski, M. (2000). The relation between dietary change
and rising US obesity. International Journal of Obesity, 24(8), 1032-1039.
Bowman, S. A., & Vinyard, B. T. (2004). Fast food consumption of US adults: impact on
energy and nutrient intakes and overweight status. Journal of the American
College of Nutrition, 23(2), 163-168.
Burton, S., & Andrews, J. C. (1996). Age, product nutrition, and label format effects on
consumer perceptions and product evaluations. Journal of Consumer Affairs,
30(1), 68-89.
116
Burton, S., & Creyer, E. H. (2004). What consumers don't know can hurt them: consumer
evaluations and disease risk perceptions of restaurant menu items. Journal of
Consumer Affairs, 38(1), 121-146.
Burton, S., Creyer, E. H., Kees, J., & Huggins, K. (2006). Attacking the obesity epidemic:
the potential health benefits of providing nutrition information in restaurants.
American Journal of Public Health, 96(9), 1669-1675.
Burton, S., Garretson, J. A., & Velliquette, A. M. (1999). Implications of accurate usage
of nutrition facts panel information for food product evaluations and purchase
intentions. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 27(4), 470-480.
CDC (2007a). Economic Consequences in Overweight and Obesity, Retrieved May 28,
2007, Center for Disease and Control Prevention,
from http://www.cdc.gov/obesity/causes/economics.html
CDC (2007b). Health Consequences in Overweight and Obesity, Retrieved May 28, 2007,
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention,
from http://www.cdc.gov/nccdphp/dnpa/obesity/consequences.htm
CDC (2009). About BMI for Adults, Retrieved July 27, 2009, Centers for Disease Control
and Prevention,
from http://www.cdc.gov/healthyweight/assessing/bmi/adult_bmi/index.html
Chandon, P., & Wansink, B. (2007a). The Biasing Health Halos of Fast-Food Restaurant
Health Claims: Lower Calorie Estimates and Higher Side-Dish Consumption
Intentions. Journal of Consumer Research, 34(3), 301-314.
Chou, S. Y., Grossman, M., & Saffer, H. (2004). An economic analysis of adult obesity:
results from the Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System. Journal of Health
Economics, 23(3), 565-587.
117
Cranage, D. A., Conklin, M. T., & Lambert, C. U. (2004). Effect of Nutrition Information
in Perceptions of Food Quality, Consumption Behavior and Purchase Intentions.
Journal of Foodservice Business Research, 7, 43-61.
CSPI (2008). Nutrition Labeling at Fast-food and Other Chain Restaurants, Center for
Science in the Public Interest. ,
from http://www.cspinet.org/nutritionpolicy/Nutrition_Labeling_Fast_Food.pdf
Cutler, D. M., Glaeser, E. L., & Shapiro, J. M. (2003). Why have Americans become
more obese? Journal of Economic Perspectives, 17(3), 93-118.
Dean, A., & Voss, D. (1999). Design and Analysis of Experiments: Springer.
Dhar, R., Nowlis, S. M., & Sherman, S. J. (1999). Comparison effects on preference
construction. Journal of Consumer Research, 26(3), 293-306.
Dhar, R., & Simonson, I. (1992). The effect of the focus of comparison on consumer
preferences. Journal of Marketing Research, 29(4), 430-440.
Drichoutis, A. C., Lazaridis, P., & Nayga, R. M. (2005). Nutrition knowledge and
consumer use of nutritional food labels. European Review of Agricultural
Economics, 32(1), 93-118.
Droms, C. (2006). When I Go Out to Eat I Want to Enjoy Myself: An Investigation into
Consumers' Use of Nutrition Information. Advances in Consumer Research, 33,
282.
Edwards, J. S., & Meiselman, H. L. (2005). The influence of positive and negative cues
on restaurant food choice and food acceptance. International Journal of
Contemporary Hospitality Management, 17(4), 332-344.
FDA (1994). Guide to Nutrition Labeling and Education Act (NLEA) Requirements,
Retrieved April 30, 2009 U.S. Food and Drug Administration,
from http://www.fda.gov/ICECI/Inspections/InspectionGuides/ucm074948.htm
118
Finkelstein, E., French, S., Variyam, J. N., & Haines, P. S. (2004). Pros and cons of
proposed interventions to promote healthy eating. American Journal of Preventive
Medicine, 27(3S), 163-171.
Ford, G. T., Hastak, M., Mitra, A., & Ringold, D. J. (1996). Can consumers interpret
nutrition information in the presence of a health claim? A laboratory investigation.
Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 15(1), 16-27.
Frazier, P. A., Tix, A. P., & Barron, K. E. (2004). Testing Moderator and Mediator
Effects in Counseling Psychology. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 51(1), 115-
134.
French, S. A., Story, M., Neumark-Sztainer, D., Fulkerson, J. A., & Hannan, P. (2001).
Fast food restaurant use among adolescents: associations with nutrient intake,
food choices and behavioral and psychosocial variables. International Journal of
Obesity, 25(12), 1823-1833.
Garretson, J. A., & Burton, S. (2000). Effects of nutrition facts panel values, nutrition
claims, and health claims on consumer attitudes, perceptions of disease-related
risks, and trust. Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 19(2), 213-227.
Glanz, K., Sallis, J. F., Saelens, B. E., & Frank, L. D. (2005). Healthy nutrition
environments: concepts and measures. American Journal of Health Promotion,
19(5), 330-333.
Guthrie, J. F., Fox, J. J., Cleveland, L. E., & Welsh, S. (1995). Who uses nutrition
labeling, and what effects does label use have on diet quality? Journal of
Nutrition Education, 27(4), 163-163.
Haugtvedt, C. P., & Petty, R. E. (1992). Personality and persuasion: Need for cognition
moderates the persistence and resistance of attitude changes. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 63(2), 308-319.
Hill, J. O., & Peters, J. C. (1998). Environmental contributions to the obesity epidemic.
Science, 280(5368), 1371-1374.
Hill, J. O., Wyatt, H. R., Reed, G. W., & Peters, J. C. (2003). Obesity and the
environment: where do we go from here? , 299(5608), 853-855.
HSUS (2006). "Farm Animal Statistics: Meat Consumption" The Human Society of the
United States. Retrieved November 30, 2006,
from http://www.hsus.org/farm/resources/pubs/stats_meat_consumption.html
119
Hu, F. B., Li, T. Y., Colditz, G. A., Willett, W. C., & Manson, J. E. (2003). Television
watching and other sedentary behaviors in relation to risk of obesity and type 2
diabetes mellitus in women. The Journal of American Medical Association,
289(14), 1785-1791.
Huber, J., Payne, J. W., & Puto, C. (1982). Adding asymmetrically dominated
alternatives: Violations of regularity and the similarity hypothesis. Journal of
Consumer Research, 9(1), 90-98.
Huber, J., & Puto, C. (1983). Market boundaries and product choice: Illustrating
attraction and substitution effects. Journal of Consumer Research, 10(1), 31-44.
Hwang, J., & Lorenzen, C. L. (2008). Effective nutrition labeling of restaurant menu and
pricing of healthy menu. Journal of Foodservice, 19(5), 270-276.
Keller, S. B., Landry, M., Olson, J., Velliquette, A. M., Burton, S., & Andrews, J. C.
(1997). The effects of nutrition package claims, nutrition facts panels, and
motivation to process nutrition information on consumer product evaluations.
Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 16(2), 256-269.
Kemp, E., Burton, S., Creyer, E. H., & Suter, T. (2007). When Do Nutrient Content and
Nutrient Content Claims Matter? Assessing Consumer Tradeoffs Between
Carbohydrates and Fat. Journal of Consumer Affairs, 41(1), 47-73.
Kozup, J. C., Burton, S., & Creyer, E. H. (2006). The provision of trans fat information
and its interaction with consumer knowledge. Journal of Consumer Affairs, 40(1),
163-176.
Kozup, J. C., Creyer, E. H., & Burton, S. (2003). Making healthful food choices: The
influence of health claims and nutrition information on consumers' evaluations of
packaged food products and restaurant menu items. Journal of Marketing, 67(2),
19-34.
Lee, S. J., & Cranage, D. A. (2007). The Relative Importance of Menu Attributes at Point
of Menu Selection Through Conjoint Analysis: Focused on Adolescents. Journal
of Foodservice Business Research, 10(2), 3-18.
Levy, A. S., & Fein, S. B. (1998). Consumers' ability to perform tasks using nutrition
labels. Journal of Nutrition Education, 30(4), 210-217.
Lynch, J. G., Chakravarti, D., & Mitra, A. (1991). Contrast effects in consumer
judgments: changes in mental representations or in the anchoring of rating scales?
Journal of Consumer Research, 18(3), 284-297.
120
Marietta, A. B., Welshimer, K. J., & Anderson, S. L. (1999). Knowledge, Attitudes, and
Behaviors of College Students regarding the 1990 Nutrition Labeling Education
Act Food labels. Journal of American Dietetic Association, 99(4), 445-449.
Moorman, C., Diehl, K., Brinberg, D., & Kidwell, B. (2004). Subjective knowledge,
search locations, and consumer choice. Journal of Consumer Research, 31(3),
673-680.
Moorman, C., & Matulich, E. (1993). A model of consumers' preventive health behaviors:
The role of health motivation and health ability. Journal of Consumer Research,
20(2), 208-228.
Nayga, R. M., Lipinski, D., & Savur, N. (1998). Consumers' use of nutritional labels
while food shopping and at home. Journal of Consumer Affairs, 32(1), 106-120.
Neter, J., Kutner, M. H., Nachtsheim, C. J., & Wasserman, W. (1996). Applied Linear
Regression Models (2 ed.): McGraw-Hill/Irwin.
Neumark-Sztainer, D., Story, M., Perry, C., & Casey, M. A. (1999). Factors Influencing
Food Choices of Adolescents Findings from Focus-Group Discussions with
Adolescents. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 99(8), 929-937.
NRA (2008). Philadelphia gives menu labeling a twist, Retrieved November 6, 2008,
Nation's Restaurant News,
from http://www.nrn.com/breakingNews.aspx?id=359940&menu_id=1368
Palmer, J., & Leontos, C. (1995). Nutrition training for chefs: Taste as an essential
determinant of choice. Journal of American Dietetic Association, 95(12), 1418-
1421.
Petty, R. E., & Brock, T. C. (1981). Thought disruption and persuasion: Assessing the
validity of attitude change experiments. Cognitive responses in persuasion.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 55-79.
121
Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1981). Attitude and Persuation: classic and contemprary
approaches: Dubuque, IA: William C. Brown.
Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion.
Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 19, 123-205.
Petty, R. E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Schumann, D. (1983). Central and peripheral routes to
advertising effectiveness: The moderating role of involvement. Journal of
Consumer Research, 10(2), 135-146.
Petty, R. E., & Wegener, D. T. (1999). The elaboration likelihood model: Current status
and controversies. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual-process theories in
social psychology (pp. 41-72): Guilford Press.
Philipson, T. J., & Posner, R. A. (2003). The long-run growth in obesity as a function of
technological change. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine, 46(3), 87-107.
Raghunathan, R., Naylor, R., & Hoyer, W. (2006). The unhealthy= tasty intuition and its
effects on taste inferences, enjoyment, and choice of food products. Journal of
Marketing, 70(4), 170-184.
Saegert, J., & Young, E. A. (1983). Nutrition knowledge and health food consumption.
Nutrition and Behavior, 1, 103-113.
Simonson, I. (1989). Choice based on reasons: The case of attraction and compromise
effects. Journal of Consumer Research, 16(2), 158-174.
Simonson, I., & Tversky, A. (1992). Choice in context: Tradeoff contrast and
extremeness aversion. Journal of Marketing Research, 29(3), 281-295.
Stubenitsky, K., Aaron, J. I., Catt, S. L., & Mela, D. J. (2007). The influence of recipe
modification and nutritional information on restaurant food acceptance and
macronutrient intakes. Public Health Nutrition, 3(02), 201-209.
122
USDA (2008). "Food CPI, Prices and Expenditures: Food Expenditures Table", U.S.
Department of Agriculture. Retrieved September 3, 2008,
from http://www.ers.usda.gov/Briefing/CPIFoodAndExpenditures/data/
Wansink, B., & Chandon, P. (2006). Can HLow-Fat?Nutrition Labels Lead to Obesity?
Journal of Marketing Research, 43(4), 605-617.
Wardle, J., Parmenter, K., & Waller, J. (2000). Nutrition knowledge and food intake.
Appetite, 34(3), 269-275.
Wolf, A. M., & Colditz, G. A. (1998). Current estimates of the economic cost of obesity
in the United States. Obesity Research, 6(2), 97-106.
Wootan, M. G. (2003). Anyone's Guess: Center for Science in the Public Interest.
Yoon, H., Sydny-Busso, S., & Parsa, H. G. (2009). Bayesian approach to assess
consumers’ brand selection process and identification of brand attributes in a
service context. International Journcal of Hospitality Management, 28(1), 33-41.
Yung, L., Contento, I., & Gussow, J. D. (1984). Use of health foods by the elderly.
Journal of nutrition education (USA), 16(3), 127-131.
Zoumas-Morse, C., Rock, C. L., Sobo, E. J., & Neuhouser, M. L. (2001). Children'
patterns of macronutrient intake and associations with restaurant and home eating.
Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 101(8), 923-925.
123
Appendix A: Questionnaire of Pre-test I
124
Pilot Test I
Please carefully read all information presented on the menu. Later we will ask
you to provide your reaction to the information.
125
Imagine you are about to order an entrée from the following menu at a casual dining
restaurant.
126
1. Think about your preferences. Please select three items you like the most from the
menu above.
2. Of the items you listed in Question 1, which would you most prefer?
The following questions are general questions about nutritional knowledge. Please
answer based on your knowledge. If you don’t know the answer, please do not guess.
Circle the “Don’t know” option.
2. Which kind of fat is more likely to raise the blood cholesterol level?
a. Saturated fats b. Polyunsaturated fats
c. Both of them d. None of the above
e. Don’t know
3. Risk of high blood pressure is most likely to be reduced by eating a diet with U .
a. Less sugar b. More fiber
c. More iron d. Less salt
e. Don’t know
127
6. If you eat 2000 calories a day, your daily sodium intake should be less than how many
milligrams?
a. 500mg b. 2400mg
c. 4300mg d. 6000mg
e. Don’t know
9. Which of the following is added to a food label because people sometimes don’t eat
enough of this?
a. Fat b. Calcium
c. Sodium d. Cholesterol
e. Don’t know
10. The bread, cereal, rice and pasta group is a good source of .
a. Carbohydrate b. Vitamin C
c. Calcium d. Vitamin D
e. Don’t know
13. Foods such as the meat, poultry, fish, dry beans, eggs and nuts are an important
source of .
a. Iron b. Fiber
c. Beta carotene d. Calcium
e. Don’t know
128
14. In general, which food contains the most fat?
a. Graham crackers b. Brownies
c. Pudding d. Angel food cake
e. Don’t know
15. The milk, cheese and yogurt group are important for .
a. Strong bones b. Teeth
c. Muscles d. All of the above
e. Don’t know
How important is each of the following when you choose a menu item in a restaurant?
129
Consider the following menu item and answer the questions;
130
Consider the following menu item and answer the questions;
• Chicken Fajitas
Juicy chicken served on a skillet that sizzles like the Southwest sun. Served with
southwest rice, guacamole, sour cream, pico de gallo, caramelized onions, green
peppers and Jack-cheddar cheese.
851 Calories 29g Total Fat 6g Saturated Fat
90g Carbohydrates 56g Protein 6g Dietary Fiber 2002mg Sodium
1. Based on the information, how important would this menu item be as part of a
healthy diet?
Not Important at all 1---------2---------3---------4---------5 Very Important
2. How would you rate the level of nutrition suggested by the information of this menu
item?
Not Nutritious at all 1---------2---------3---------4---------5 Very Nutritious
3. How would you evaluate the healthiness of this menu item?
Very Unhealthy 1---------2---------3---------4---------5 Very Healthy
4. To what extent do you consider the nutritional information of this menu item to be
accurate?
Very Inaccurate 1---------2---------3---------4---------5 Very Accurate
5. To what extent do you consider the nutritional information of this menu item to be
credible?
Very Unreliable 1---------2---------3---------4---------5 Very Reliable
131
Consider the following menu item and answer the questions;
• Grilled Salmon
An herb seasoned filet of Coho Salmon that is fire-grilled served over rice pilaf
comes with Broccoli, tartar sauce, and a lemon wedge, for a light and delicious
seafood feast.
393 Calories 19g Total Fat 4.5g Saturated Fat
14g Carbohydrates 44g Protein 7g Dietary Fiber 520mg Sodium
1. Based on the information, how important would this menu item be as part of a
healthy diet?
Not Important at all 1---------2---------3---------4---------5 Very Important
2. How would you rate the level of nutrition suggested by the information of this menu
item?
Not Nutritious at all 1---------2---------3---------4---------5 Very Nutritious
3. How would you evaluate the healthiness of this menu item?
Very Unhealthy 1---------2---------3---------4---------5 Very Healthy
4. To what extent do you consider the nutritional information of this menu item to be
accurate?
Very Inaccurate 1---------2---------3---------4---------5 Very Accurate
5. To what extent do you consider the nutritional information of this menu item to be
credible?
Very Unreliable 1---------2---------3---------4---------5 Very Reliable
132
Consider the following menu item and answer the questions;
• Classic Marsala
Sautéed chicken breasts in a savory sauce of mushrooms, garlic and marsala wine
973 Calories 57g Total Fat 0g Saturated Fat
65g Carbohydrates 37g Protein 9g Dietary Fiber 1399mg Sodium
1. Based on the information, how important would this menu item be as part of a
healthy diet?
Not Important at all 1---------2---------3---------4---------5 Very Important
2. How would you rate the level of nutrition suggested by the information of this menu
item?
Not Nutritious at all 1---------2---------3---------4---------5 Very Nutritious
3. How would you evaluate the healthiness of this menu item?
Very Unhealthy 1---------2---------3---------4---------5 Very Healthy
4. To what extent do you consider the nutritional information of this menu item to be
accurate?
Very Inaccurate 1---------2---------3---------4---------5 Very Accurate
5. To what extent do you consider the nutritional information of this menu item to be
credible?
Very Unreliable 1---------2---------3---------4---------5 Very Reliable
133
You have received and evaluated the nutritional information of all 8 menu items.
Do you want to change your choice of menu item that you selected at the beginning of
the survey?
a. No b. Yes
Please rank the following from 1 to 11 by the order of importance when you choose a
menu item in a restaurant. (1 = Most Important; 11 = Least Important)
Calories from fat ( )
Carbohydrates ( )
Cholesterol ( )
Dietary Fiber ( )
Protein ( )
Saturated fat ( )
Sodium ( )
Sugar ( )
Total Calories ( )
Total fat ( )
Trans fat ( )
134
Appendix B: Questionnaire of Pre-test II
135
Pilot Test II.
Type3: Nutritional Information of 6 nutrition contents
We are seeking your reactions to a situation that you might encounter concerning
meal selection in a restaurant. Please respond to the questions in a natural manner.
There are no right or wrong answers; We want your honest opinions.
Please carefully read all information presented on the menu. Later we will ask
you to provide your reaction to the information.
Imagine you are about to order an entrée from the following menu at a
casual dining restaurant.
136
Section I: Menu Evaluation
The following questions are general questions about Chicken Caesar Salad on the menu.
Please answer what is being asked based on your opinion.
• Chicken Caesar Salad
Grilled chicken served on top of romaine lettuce in a creamy and
garlicky Caesar dressing topped with parmesan cheese and
croutons.
900 Calories 43g Total fat (66% DV) 13g Saturated Fat (65% DV)
201mg Cholesterol (67% DV) 1560mg Sodium (65% DV) 13g Protein (26% DV)
Percent Daily Values (DV) are based on a 2,000 calorie diet.
1. Based on the information shown, to what extent do you expect the taste of Chicken
Caesar Salad?
Very Poor 1----------2----------3----------4----------5 Very Good
2. Based on the information provided, how important would Chicken Caesar Salad be
as part of a healthy diet?
Not Important at all 1----------2----------3----------4----------5 Very Important
3. How would you rate the level of nutrition suggested by the information of Chicken
Caesar Salad?
Not Nutritious at all 1----------2----------3----------4----------5 Very Nutritious
4. How would you evaluate the healthiness of Chicken Caesar Salad?
Very Unhealthy 1----------2----------3----------4----------5 Very Healthy
5. Overall, how much are you favorable toward Chicken Caesar Salad?
Very Unfavorable 1----------2----------3----------4----------5 Very Favorable
6. Overall, how much do you prefer Chicken Caesar Salad?
Very Dislike 1----------2----------3----------4----------5 Very Like
7. What is your overall feeling toward Chicken Caesar Salad?
Very Negative 1----------2----------3----------4----------5 Very Positive
8. How probable is it that you would consider the purchase of Chicken Caesar Salad?
Not Probable at all 1----------2----------3----------4----------5 Very Probable
9. How willing would you be to purchase Chicken Caesar Salad again?
Very Unlikely 1----------2----------3----------4----------5 Very Likely
10. How likely would you be to purchase Chicken Caesar Salad based on the given
information?
Very Unlikely 1----------2----------3----------4----------5 Very Likely
137
The following questions are general questions about Lemon Herb Roasted Chicken on the
menu. Please answer what is being asked based on your opinion.
460 Calories 8g Total fat (12% DV) 2.5g Saturated Fat (12.5% DV)
40mg Cholesterol (13% DV) 264mg Sodium (11% DV) 34g Protein (68% DV)
Percent Daily Values (DV) are based on a 2,000 calorie diet.
1. Based on the information shown, to what extent do you expect the taste of Lemon
Herb Roasted Chicken?
Very Poor 1----------2----------3----------4----------5 Very Good
2. Based on the information provided, how important would Lemon Herb Roasted
Chicken be as part of a healthy diet?
Not Important at all 1----------2----------3----------4----------5 Very Important
3. How would you rate the level of nutrition suggested by the information of Lemon
Herb Roasted Chicken?
Not Nutritious at all 1----------2----------3----------4----------5 Very Nutritious
4. How would you evaluate the healthiness of Lemon Herb Roasted Chicken?
138
The following questions are general questions about Chicken Fajitas on the menu.
Please answer what is being asked based on your opinion.
• Chicken Fajitas
Juicy chicken served with southwest rice, guacamole, sour
cream, pico de gallo, caramelized onions, green peppers and
Jack-cheddar cheese.
830 Calories 38g Total fat (58% DV) 12g Saturated Fat (60% DV)
186mg Cholesterol (62% DV) 1464mg Sodium (61% DV) 15g Protein (30% DV)
Percent Daily Values (DV) are based on a 2,000 calorie diet.
1. Based on the information shown, to what extent do you expect the taste of Chicken
Fajitas?
Very Poor 1----------2----------3----------4----------5 Very Good
2. Based on the information provided, how important would Chicken Fajitas be as part
of a healthy diet?
Not Important at all 1----------2----------3----------4----------5 Very Important
3. How would you rate the level of nutrition suggested by the information of Chicken
Fajitas?
Not Nutritious at all 1----------2----------3----------4----------5 Very Nutritious
4. How would you evaluate the healthiness of Chicken Fajitas?
139
The following questions are general questions about Grilled Chicken on the menu.
Please answer what is being asked based on your opinion.
• Grilled Chicken
Grilled chicken breasts in an apricot citrus sauce. Served with
broccoli, asparagus and diced tomatoes.
520 Calories 12g Total fat (18% DV) 4g Saturated Fat (20% DV)
54mg Cholesterol (18% DV) 520mg Sodium (21% DV) 31g Protein (62% DV)
Percent Daily Values (DV) are based on a 2,000 calorie diet
1. Based on the information shown, to what extent do you expect the taste of Grilled
Chicken?
Very Poor 1----------2----------3----------4----------5 Very Good
2. Based on the information provided, how important would Grilled Chicken be as part
of a healthy diet?
Not Important at all 1----------2----------3----------4----------5 Very Important
3. How would you rate the level of nutrition suggested by the information of Grilled
Chicken?
Not Nutritious at all 1----------2----------3----------4----------5 Very Nutritious
4. How would you evaluate the healthiness of Grilled Chicken?
140
The following questions are general questions about Chicken Marsala on the menu.
Please answer what is being asked based on your opinion.
• Chicken Marsala
Sauteed chicken breasts in a savory sauce of mushrooms, garlic
and marsala wine.
920 Calories 46g Total fat (71% DV) 15g Saturated Fat (75% DV)
216mg Cholesterol (72% DV) 1680mg Sodium (70% DV) 10g Protein (20% DV)
Percent Daily Values (DV) are based on a 2,000 calorie diet
1. Based on the information shown, to what extent do you expect the taste of Chicken
Marsala?
Very Poor 1----------2----------3----------4----------5 Very Good
2. Based on the information provided, how important would Chicken Marsala be as
part of a healthy diet?
Not Important at all 1----------2----------3----------4----------5 Very Important
3. How would you rate the level of nutrition suggested by the information of Chicken
Marsala?
Not Nutritious at all 1----------2----------3----------4----------5 Very Nutritious
4. How would you evaluate the healthiness of Chicken Marsala?
141
The following questions are general questions about Italian Chicken Sandwich on the
menu. Please answer what is being asked based on your opinion.
440 Calories 6.5g Total fat (10% DV) 2g Saturated Fat (10% DV)
33mg Cholesterol (11% DV) 216mg Sodium (9% DV) 36g Protein (72% DV)
Percent Daily Values (DV) are based on a 2,000 calorie diet
1. Based on the information shown, to what extent do you expect the taste of Italian
Chicken Sandwich?
Very Poor 1----------2----------3----------4----------5 Very Good
2. Based on the information provided, how important would Italian Chicken Sandwich
be as part of a healthy diet?
Not Important at all 1----------2----------3----------4----------5 Very Important
3. How would you rate the level of nutrition suggested by the information of Italian
Chicken Sandwich?
Not Nutritious at all 1----------2----------3----------4----------5 Very Nutritious
4. How would you evaluate the healthiness of Italian Chicken Sandwich?
142
Section II: Menu Choice
1. Based on the previous menu items, which one are you most likely to choose?
2. Which of side items on the below menu would you like to select accompanying with
your entrée that you chose? (Please select one)
a. House Salad
(210 calories, 12g Total Fat, 6g Saturated Fat, 10g protein, 310mg Sodium without
dressing)
b. Mashed Potato
(270 calories, 14g Total Fat, 2g Saturated Fat, 5g protein, 950mg Sodium)
c. Baked Potato
(250 calories, 18g Total Fat, 11g Saturated Fat, 9g protein, 910mg Sodium)
d. Basket of Fries
(430 calories, 26g Total Fat, 4.5g Saturated Fat, 4g protein, 240mg Sodium)
g. None
143
Section III: Nutritional Knowledge
The following questions are general questions about nutritional knowledge. Please
answer based on your knowledge. If you don’t know the answer, please do not guess.
Circle the “Don’t know” option.
2. Risk of high blood pressure is most likely to be reduced by eating a diet with .
a. Less sugar b. More fiber
c. More iron d. Less salt
e. Don’t know
5. If you eat 2000 calories a day, your daily sodium intake should be less than how many
milligrams?
a. 500mg b. 2400mg
c. 4300mg d. 6000mg
e. Don’t know
144
8. Which of the following is added to a food label because people sometimes don’t eat
enough of this?
a. Fat b. Calcium
c. Sodium d. Cholesterol
e. Don’t know
12. Foods as the meat, poultry, fish, dry beans, eggs and nuts are an important source
of .
a. Iron b. Fiber
c. Beta carotene d. Calcium
e. Don’t know
14. The milk, cheese and yogurt group are important for .
a. Strong bones b. Teeth
c. Muscles d. All of the above
e. Don’t know
145
Section IV: Individual Characteristics
The following questions are asking about your general personal characteristics.
Please answer based on your personality.
146
Section V: Demographic Questions
1. Please select the major information source that you use to obtain the nutritional
information of menu items offered by restaurant from the following.
(Please select only one).
a. TV b. Restaurant Website c. Other Website (ex. Yahoo, Google)
d. Newspaper e. Menu f. Ask to a server
g. Others
147
Appendix C: Examples of Menu
148
• Menu including a Healthy Target Item in Healthy Context without
Nutritional Information
149
• Menu including a Unhealthy Target Item in Healthy Context without
Nutritional Information
150
• Menu including a Healthy Target Item in Healthy Context with Calories
151
• Menu including a Unhealthy Target Item in Healthy Context with Calories
152
• Menu including a Healthy Target Item in the Healthy Context with
Nutritional Information of 6 nutrition contents
153
• Menu including a Unhealthy Target Item in the Healthy Context with
Nutritional Information of 6 nutrition contents
154
Appendix D: Results of Additional Multiple Regression Analysis
(Dependent Variable: Source Credibility)
155
Source Credibility
Independent Model I Model II Model III Model IV
Variables B (S.E.) B (S.E.) B (S.E.) B (S.E.)
Constant 3.63 (0.10)*** 3.63 (0.13)*** 3.60 (0.10)*** 3.60 (0.10)***
Target Item -0.03 (0.09) -0.03 (0.09) -0.01 (0.09) -0.01 (0.09)
Context -0.09 (0.09) -0.09 (0.09) -0.08 (0.09) -0.08 (0.09)
Total Calories -0.03 (0.11) -0.03 (0.11) -0.004 (0.11) 0.002 (0.11)
Nutritional Info
-0.01 (0.11) -0.01 (0.11) 0.02 (0.11) 0.02 (0.11)
of 6 contents
Objective NK
0.05 (0.02)** 0.06 (0.03)*
(CONK)
Subjective NK
0.09 (0.05)* 0.02 (0.09)
(CSNK)
Cal*CONK -0.01 (0.05)
NI*CONK -0.02 (0.05)
156
Overall Food Attitude Purchase Intention
Independent Variables
B (S.E.) B (S.E.)
Constant 4.26 (0.13)*** 4.27 (0.15)***
Target Item 0.11 (0.11) 0.04 (0.13)
Context -0.46 (0.11)*** -0.58 (0.13)***
Total Calories -0.34 (0.14)** -0.41 (0.16)**
Nutritional Info
-0.43 (0.14)*** -0.49 (0.16)***
of 6 contents
Motivation (CMOT) -0.04 (0.06) -0.05 (0.07)
Objective NK (CONK) 0.003 (0.02) <0.001 (0.03)
Subjective NK
0.04 (0.07) 0.08 (0.09)
(CSNK)
CMOT*CONK 0.004 (0.02) -0.002 (0.03)
CMOT*CSNK -0.12 (0.05)** -0.13 (0.06)**
Adj. R2 0.08 0.08
Model fit (F-Value) 3.94** 4.13***
Note: *Significant at p<0.1; **Significant at p<0.05; ***Significant at p<0.01.
Standard errors are in the parentheses.
Table D.2: Results of Regression Analysis: Interaction Effect b/w Nutritional Knowledge
and Motivation to Process
157
Appendix E: Objective Nutritional Knowledge Questions
158
“Don’t
Objective Nutritional Knowledge Question Correct
Know”
1. Which kind of fat is higher in calories?
a. Saturated fats b. Polyunsaturated fats 18 78
c. They are both the same d. None of the above (5.6%) (24.5%)
e. Don’t know
2. Risk of high blood pressure is most likely to be reduced
by eating a diet with .
188 62
a. Less sugar b. More fiber
(58.9%) (19.4%)
c. More iron d. Less salt
e. Don’t know
3.Which food group provides protein, B vitamins, iron, and
zinc?
234 41
a. Meat, poultry and fish b. Milk and daily products
(73.4%) (12.9%)
c. Fruits d. Grain products such as
e. Don’t Know bread, cereal, and rice
4. Cholesterol is found in _______.
a. Vegetables and b. All foods containing fat or
vegetables oils oil 103 52
c. Animal products like d. None of above (32.3%) (16.3%)
meat and dairy
e. Don’t know
5. If you eat 2000 calories a day, your daily sodium intake
should be less than how many milligrams?
48 225
a. 500mg b. 2400mg
(15.0%) (70.5%)
c. 4300mg d. 6000mg
e. Don’t know
6. Which food group is our body's best source of energy?
a. Meat Group b. Fats, oils and sweets 183 23
c. Breads and cereals d. Milk and cheese (57.4%) (7.2%)
e. Don’t know
7. Which of the following is not considered a nutrient?
a. Vitamins b. Minerals 82 46
c. Fiber d. Fats (25.7%) (14.4%)
e. Don’t know
Note: The correct answer is in bold.
Continued
159
Table E.1 continued
160