BJC Azaa065
BJC Azaa065
BJC Azaa065
Social media are now utilized extensively by Neighbourhood Watch-style initiatives; however, the
impact social media have on the practices and mechanisms of community crime prevention remains
under-theorized. Drawing on our observations of an Australian-based community crime preven-
tion group over two-and-a-half years, this article develops a grounded theory of the mechanisms
underpinning the group’s social media-facilitated practices of responding to local crime. We find
that social media-facilitated Neighbourhood Watch is shaped by two phenomena that have yet to
receive sustained attention in crime prevention research. These are swarm intelligence—a form of
self-organization wherein collectives process information to solve problems that members cannot
solve individually—and stigmergy: work that stimulates further work. In explaining how swarm
intelligence and stigmergy interact with several of the long-acknowledged mechanisms and issues
associated with Neighbourhood Watch, we emphasize the importance of examining how the media
context of community crime prevention groups shapes their practices, behaviour and (in)efficacy.
Key Words: community crime prevention, neighbourhood watch, swarm intelligence,
social media, stigmergy, digital criminology
Introduction
Hailing from a municipality in Victoria, Australia, Springsclare Crime Prevention
is a community crime prevention group that has accumulated over 13,000 members
since being established in 2016. Unlike traditional Neighbourhood Watch schemes,
Springsclare Crime Prevention is run entirely through social media, using a semi-
public Facebook group to quickly spread information about crimes in the area, crime
prevention tips and other local concerns. As groups like Springsclare Crime Prevention
demonstrate, mobile and social media represent potentially disruptive technologies for
community crime prevention. Despite this, the potentially transformative impact of
mobile and social media on community crime prevention has received relatively limited
attention within criminology (Akkermans and Vollaard 2015; Lub 2017; 2018; Mols and
Pridmore 2019; Pridmore et al. 2019). In particular, few studies have conceptualized
the mechanisms underlying social media-facilitated community crime prevention and
how they interface with well-established mechanisms, such as natural surveillance and
collective efficacy (Cozens 2008; Sampson 2012). Better understanding this interface
between old and new mechanisms provides much-needed knowledge on whether, when
*Mark A. Wood, School of Humanities and Social Sciences, Deakin University, 221 Burwood Hwy, Burwood, Victoria 3125,
Australia; [email protected]; Chrissy Thompson, Independent Researcher.
Page 1 of 20
© The Author(s) 2020. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Centre for Crime and Justice Studies (ISTD).
All rights reserved. For permissions, please e-mail: [email protected]
WOOD AND THOMPSON
Literature Review
Neighbourhood Watch-style community crime prevention groups have been the subject
of extensive research, with criminologists examining their aims, preventative efficacy,
mechanisms and pitfalls (see Rosenbaum 1987; Bennett 1990). In this research, scholars
have proposed numerous mechanisms to explain why Neighbourhood Watch programs
might lower crime rates. These include that Neighbourhood Watch programs may rep-
resent a visible deterrent (Laycock and Tilley 1995; Newburn 2007; see Jacobs 1961);
that they may limit opportunities for crime to occur through informing residents about
security (Cirel et al. 1977; Bennett 1990) and that they may indirectly affect crime rates
through providing the police with information about suspicious activities (Clarke and
Hough 1984; Garofalo and McLeod 1989). Furthermore, scholars have argued that,
Neighbourhood Watch groups may lower crime rates through enforcing standards of
desired behaviour and engaging in other direct interventions (Rosenbaum 1987; Lub
2017). Neighbourhood Watch groups may, in other words, encourage greater informal
social control among residents (Rosenbaum 1987; Sampson 2012), often through
increasing the collective efficacy of communities, i.e. ‘social cohesion among neigh-
bors combined with their willingness to intervene on behalf of the common good’
(Sampson et al. 1997: 918; Sampson 2012). Collective efficacy has been employed exten-
sively in the fields of crime prevention and social ecology, with empirical studies finding
strong empirical support for collective efficacy as a mechanism of informal social con-
trol (Pratt and Cullen 2005; Sampson 2012).
These theoretical foundations of Neighbourhood Watch have, however, been prob-
lematized by studies examining the limitations and potential pitfalls of such programs.
Much of this literature notes that Neighbourhood Watch programs can only be suc-
cessful when a very specific set of conditions are met (Palumbo et al. 1997). Thus, whilst
evidence suggests that Neighbourhood Watch programs can have a modest effect
in reducing crime (Garofalo and McLeod 1989; Bennett et al. 2008), such programs
are not viable for all neighbourhoods. Research has, e.g., consistently indicated that
Neighbourhood Watch programs are considerably more common in relatively affluent
and comparatively low-crime areas (Husain 1988; Hope 1995; Laycock and Tilley 1995;
Brunton-Smith and Bullock 2019) and face considerable challenges in disadvantaged
and high-crime neighbourhoods. For these reasons, Rosenbaum (1987: 127) also ques-
tions several of the core foundations of the Neighbourhood Watch ‘Implant Hypothesis’,
i.e. that ‘citizen participation, and eventually informal social control mechanisms,
Page 3 of 20
WOOD AND THOMPSON
Methodology
Background: Springsclare Crime Prevention
Located in a suburb of Melbourne and home to over 100,000 people, the pseud-
onymous Springsclare is one of several local government areas in Victoria. Springsclare
is a relatively affluent municipality with a low crime rate and bears many of the demo-
graphic hallmarks of areas where community crime prevention groups are more likely
Page 4 of 20
CRIME PREVENTION, SWARM INTELLIGENCE AND STIGMERGY
Page 6 of 20
CRIME PREVENTION, SWARM INTELLIGENCE AND STIGMERGY
Page 7 of 20
WOOD AND THOMPSON
This post elicited over 100 reactions from members and 28 comments, including imme-
diate responses from members asking what time the attempted burglary occurred so
that neighbours on the street could check their own CCTV footage. Here, we see a clear
example of members combining and processing information about potential criminal
activity in their area in real-time. In the discussion between members on this post, in-
formation is exchanged:
If you end up identifying the vehicle let me know. Did it look like a station wagon by any chance?
About 6 months ago I was driving up a side street … around 4am. As I was driving through the street
I had to come to a complete stop because there was a SILVER HOLDEN STATION WAGON parked
in the middle of the road
I live on [street name] and the house next door got his work car broken into a few nights ago and stole
all his tools. It may be the same person
In the following sections, we examine, in turn, two of the key implications of this swarm
intelligence: the creation of a ‘many eyes’ effect in detecting and responding to crime;
and the fostering of ‘suspicion creep’—a widening of what group members perceived
to be suspicious behaviour.
Page 8 of 20
CRIME PREVENTION, SWARM INTELLIGENCE AND STIGMERGY
Page 9 of 20
WOOD AND THOMPSON
This post, which was deleted within a day by administrators, highlights several short-
comings of Facebook-facilitated crime prevention groups: their propensity to increase
members’ fear of crime, their tendency to only report rather than actively prevent
crime and, subsequently, their failure to systematically engage in actual crime preven-
tion behaviours.
In addition to these shortcomings, swarm intelligence contributed to what we term
suspicion creep. In suspicion creep, the forms of behaviour perceived to be suspicious by
group members expands as individual members report witnessing new behaviours that
they perceived to be suspicious. These behaviours are then recoded by sections of the
group as suspicious, leading to an increased repertoire of behaviours deemed suspi-
cious and, by corollary, reportable. Though some of the behaviours reported by mem-
bers are undoubtedly crime-related, many are not. Many of the posts we analysed, e.g.,
concerned groups of young people doing nothing more than occupying public space.
Others concerned individuals wearing hoodies at night, Pokémon GO players or individ-
uals taking pictures of flowers in an individual’s garden. Suspicion creep can, therefore,
reinforce an ‘us vs them’ mentality, an assertion of community and national borders as
in need of protection from outsiders—a key issue long identified with Neighbourhood
Watch (Laycock and Tilley 1995; Crawford 1997; 1999) but, in this case, amplified by
the many-to-many and real-time communication afforded by social media.
Another member responded in the comments that they had ‘also patrolled around’,
while another raised alarm over a car driving around the streets:
Not sure if one of us, but a beige Honda CR-V was driving in and out of streets inside Primrose drive
just after 6am this morning. Didn’t get plate as it was pretty dark.
Page 11 of 20
WOOD AND THOMPSON
Page 12 of 20
CRIME PREVENTION, SWARM INTELLIGENCE AND STIGMERGY
This last example is particularly noteworthy in demonstrating how new mobile technolo-
gies—in this case, augmented reality smartphone applications—can disrupt expected
uses of public space. The temporary flood of people in public space eagerly hunting
Pokémon on their phone application, utilizing public space and coming together to
participate in a collective activity is ostensibly one of the key aims of Springsclare Crime
Prevention, i.e. to bring the community together. However, due to the positive feedback
loop produced on this page, the presence of Pokémon GO players sparked disquiet
and suspicion such that locals were encouraged to download the Pokémon GO app to
better distinguish ‘suspicious’ behaviour from the Pokémon hunting behaviour associ-
ated with the augmented reality game.
This propensity for amplifying vigilance indicates that stigmergy may be a pro-
ductive force for crime prevention when the preventative efficacy of an action is an
indirect by-product of the stigmergic behaviour rather than the direct aim of that be-
haviour. Here, we can productively distinguish between first-order and second-order
forms of crime prevention. First-order crime prevention represents practices and ini-
tiatives aimed at reducing crime. Examples of first-order crime prevention include
neighbourhood watch patrols and sharing crime prevention tips. Second-order crime
prevention, by contrast, represents practices and initiatives that prevent crime as a
byproduct of pursuing other aims. Here, we can include initiatives that foster com-
munity cohesion, which consequently increases informal social control and collective
efficacy.
In being narrowly conceived as a crime prevention page, Springsclare Crime
Prevention did little to foster such forms of second-order crime prevention; its remit
was practices aimed directly at preventing crime. As demonstrated by the group, this
narrow remit can be circumvented through branding not as a crime prevention page
specifically but as a broader local community group concerned with a range of issues
affecting an area. Indeed, the utility of this approach has long been recognized by
researchers examining crime prevention. As Bursik and Grasmick (1999: 154) note,
‘crime prevention programs have the greatest likelihood of success if they are inte-
grated into the activities of more general, multi-issue neighbourhood organizations’.
Such an approach is precisely what occurred with Springsclare Crime Prevention within
the group’s second year, when it rebranded as Springsclare Community Hub. Upon re-
branding, the group flourished as a platform where residents of Springsclare assisted
one another in a variety of tasks, from offering advice on service providers in the area
to locating lost animals. Such actions still benefit from the stigmergic, marker-based
mechanism of self-organization provided by Facebook groups; members’ trace-making
actions are stimulated by traces left by other members, which, in turn, stimulate further
trace-making actions. But unlike first-order crime prevention, they do not simultan-
eously amplify hypervigilance and fear of crime among group members. The second-
order crime prevention they allow for stokes a very different affective register among
members, for the positive feedback of members’ contributions is put in check by the
attainment of small-scale goals that do not overtly pertain to crime prevention. The
Page 13 of 20
WOOD AND THOMPSON
Conclusion
As our analysis demonstrates, the swarm intelligence of Facebook crime prevention
groups can generate hyperconsciousness of crime among members. Viewed purely
from an instrumental perspective, this hyperconsciousness may increase the efficacy of
Page 14 of 20
CRIME PREVENTION, SWARM INTELLIGENCE AND STIGMERGY
Page 15 of 20
WOOD AND THOMPSON
References
Akkermans, M. and Vollaard, B. (2015). Effect van het WhatsApp-project in Tilburg op het
Aantal Woninginbraken—Een Evaluatie. Tilburg University.
Beni, G. and Wang, J. (1993), ‘Swarm intelligence in cellular robotic systems’, in P. Dario,
G. Sandini and P. Aebischer, eds, Robots and Biological Systems: Towards a New Bionics?,
703–12. Springer.
Bennett, T. H. (1990), Evaluating Neighborhood Watch. Cambridge University Press.
Bennett, T., Holloway, K. and Farrington, D. (2008), ‘The Effectiveness of Neighborhood
Watch’, Campbell Systematic Reviews, 4: 1–46.
Bhaskar, R. (1986), Scientific Realism and Human Emancipation. The Thetford Press.
Bonabeau, E. and Meyer, C. (2001), ‘Swarm Intelligence: A Whole New Way to Think
About Business’, Harvard Business Review, 79: 106–15.
Braithwaite, J. (2020), ‘Crime as a Cascade Phenomenon’, International Journal of
Comparative and Applied Criminal Justice, 44: 137–69.
Brown, S. (2006), ‘The Criminology of Hybrids: Rethinking Crime and law in Technosocial
Networks’, Theoretical Criminology, 10: 223–44.
Brunton-Smith, I. and Bullock, K. (2019), ‘Patterns and Drivers of Co-Production in
Neighbourhood Watch in England and Wales: From Neo-Liberalism to New Localism’,
The British Journal of Criminology, 59: 85–106.
Bursik, R. J. and Grasmick, H. G. (1999), Neighborhoods & Crime. Lexington Books.
Caskey, T. R., Wasek, J. S. and Franz, A. Y. (2018), ‘Deter and Protect: Crime Modeling
With Multi-Agent Learning’, Complex & Intelligent Systems, 4: 155–69.
Charmaz, K. (2006), Constructing Grounded Theory: A Practical Guide Through Qualitative
Analysis. Sage Publications.
Cirel, P., Evans, P., McGillis, D. and Whitcomb, D. (1977), Community Crime Prevention
Program, Seattle: An Exemplary Project. Government Printing Office.
Clarke, R. and Hough, M. (1984), Crime and Police Effectiveness, Home Office Research Study
No. 79. Home Office.
Collins, R. (2004), Interaction Ritual Chains. Princeton University Press.
Cozens, P. (2008), ‘Crime Prevention Through Environmental Design’, in R. Wortley and
L. Mazerolle, eds, Environmental Criminology and Crime Analysis, 153–77. Willan.
Cozens, P. and Davies, T. (2013), ‘Investigating “Eyes on the Street,” Perceptions of Crime
and the use of Security Shutters—Insights from a Residential Suburb in Perth (WA)’,
Crime Prevention and Community Safety, 15: 175–91.
Page 17 of 20
WOOD AND THOMPSON
Page 18 of 20
CRIME PREVENTION, SWARM INTELLIGENCE AND STIGMERGY
Page 20 of 20