Issciencereallyfacingareproducibilitycrisis, Anddo We Need It To?
Issciencereallyfacingareproducibilitycrisis, Anddo We Need It To?
Issciencereallyfacingareproducibilitycrisis, Anddo We Need It To?
COLLOQUIUM OPINION
Issciencereallyfacingareproducibilitycrisis,anddo
we need it to?
Daniele Fanellia,1
Edited by David B. Allison, Indiana University Bloomington, Bloomington, IN, and accepted by Editorial Board Member Susan T. Fiske
November 3, 2017 (received for review June 30, 2017)
Efforts to improve the reproducibility and integrity of science are typically justified by a narrative of crisis,
according to which most published results are unreliable due to growing problems with research and
publication practices. This article provides an overview of recent evidence suggesting that this narrative is
mistaken, and argues that a narrative of epochal changes and empowerment of scientists would be more
accurate, inspiring, and compelling.
| | | |
reproducible research crisis integrity bias misconduct
Is there a reproducibility crisis in science? Many seem suggests that generalizations are unjustified; and (iii) not
to believe so. In a recent survey by the journal Nature, growing, as the crisis narrative would presuppose. Alter-
for example, around 90% of respondents agreed that native narratives, therefore, might represent a better fit for
there is a “slight” or “significant” crisis, and between empirical data as well as for the reproducibility agenda.
40% and 70% agreed that selective reporting, fraud,
and pressures to publish “always” or “often” contrib- How Common Are Fabricated, False, Biased,
ute to irreproducible research (1). Results of this non- and Irreproducible Findings?
randomized survey may not accurately represent the Scientific misconduct and questionable research
population of practicing scientists, but they echo practices (QRP) occur at frequencies that, while
beliefs expressed by a rapidly growing scientific nonnegligible, are relatively small and therefore
literature, which uncritically endorses a new “crisis unlikely to have a major impact on the literature. In
narrative” about science (an illustrative sample of this anonymous surveys, on average 1–2% of scientists
literature is shown in Fig. 1 and listed in Dataset S1). admit to having fabricated or falsified data at least
Put simply, this new “science in crisis” narrative once (2). Much higher percentages admit to other
postulates that a large and growing proportion of QRP, such as dropping data points based on a gut
studies published across disciplines are unreliable feeling or failing to publish a contradictory result.
due to the declining quality and integrity of research However, the percentage of scientific literature that
and publication practices, largely because of growing is actually affected by these practices is unknown,
pressures to publish and other ills affecting the con- and evidence suggests that it is likely to be smaller,
temporary scientific profession. at least five times smaller according to a survey
I argue that this crisis narrative is at least partially among psychologists (3). Data that directly estimate
misguided. Recent evidence from metaresearch stud- the prevalence of misconduct are scarce but appear
ies suggests that issues with research integrity and to corroborate this conclusion. Random laboratory
reproducibility, while certainly important phenomena audits in cancer clinical trials, for example, found
that need to be addressed, are: (i) not distorting the that only 0.28% contained “scientific improprieties”
majority of the literature, in science as a whole as well (4), and those conducted among Food and Drug
as within any given discipline; (ii) heterogeneously dis- Administration clinical trials between 1977 and 1988
tributed across subfields in any given area, which found problems sufficient to initiate “for cause”
a
Department of Methodology, London School of Economics and Political Science, London WC2A 2AE, United Kingdom
This paper results from the Arthur M. Sackler Colloquium of the National Academy of Sciences, “Reproducibility of Research: Issues and Proposed
Remedies,” held March 8–10, 2017, at the National Academy of Sciences in Washington, DC. The complete program and video recordings of most
presentations are available on the NAS website at www.nasonline.org/Reproducibility.
Author contributions: D.F. wrote the paper.
The author declares no conflict of interest.
This article is a PNAS Direct Submission. D.B.A. is a guest editor invited by the Editorial Board.
Published under the PNAS license.
1
Email: [email protected].
This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1073/pnas.1708272114/-/DCSupplemental.
Downloaded by guest on June 4, 2021
were highly heterogeneously distributed (19). This that reproducibility in psychological science might