Unit-1. 2
Unit-1. 2
Unit-1. 2
Structure
1.0 Introduction
1.1 Objectives
1.2 The Nature of Intelligence
1.2.1 Theoretical Definitions of Intelligence
1.2.2 Operational Definition of Intelligence
1.2.3 Historical Perspective on Individual Differences and Human Abilities
1.3 Spearmen’s Two-Factor Theory of Intelligence
1.3.1 Critical Appraisal of Two-Factor Theory
1.3.1.1 Thorndike’s Theory
1.3.1.2 Thomson’s Theory
1.3.1.3 Thurstone’s Theory
1.4 Das, Nagliery and Kirby’s PASS Theory
1.4.1 Critical Appraisal of the PASS Theory
1.5 Let Us Sum Up
1.6 Unit End Questions
1.7 Suggested Readings and References
1.0 INTRODUCTION
Right from the dawn of civilisation man has often wondered about individual
differences in abilities, yet it was not until the third quarter of the nineteenth
century that efforts could be made about understanding its complex nature.
Intelligence is a broad term that is employed by layman to denote the presence of
such qualities as alertness, quickness of mind, level of one’s academic success,
status in an occupation, or the acquisition of an eminence in a particular field of
endeavour and so on. In this unit we will deal with the nature and meaning of
intelligence. We will also discuss some of the most important theoretical models
of intelligence so as to understand its nature. We will begin with some definitions
to bring home the point that even among the psychologists there is a lack of
unanimity about this term. We will also briefly outline a historical perspective
on individual differences and human abilities. Following this there will be a
discussion on the Spearman’s Two factor theory consisting of G and S factors,
and a critical appraisal of the theory. We would also be dealing with Thorndike’s
theory of intelligence and PASS theory of intelligence put forward by JP Das
and colleagues.
1.1 OBJECTIVES
After reading this unit, you will be able to:
• Define the term intelligence;
• Obtain a brief overview of individual differences and intelligence; 5
Intelligence and Creativity • Discuss Spearman’s Two-Factor theory of intelligence;
• Make a critical appraisal of Two-Factor theory;
• Describe J. P. Das, Nagliery, and Kirby’s PASS theory of intelligence; and
• Analyse the PASS theory.
In 1986, Sternberg and Determan found that twenty-four prominent scholars had
twenty-four different definitions of intelligence. Sternberg (1997) has attempted
a comparison of the two surveys. He has remarked that in the 1921 survey, the
elements that appeared most often in the definitions were “(a) higher level abilities
(such as abstract reasoning, mental representation, problem solving, and decision
making), (b) ability to learn, and (c) adaptation to meet the demands of the
environment. In the 1986 survey, the most common elements were (a) higher
6
level abilities, (b) that which is valued by culture, and (c) executive process” Theories of Intelligence (G
and S Factor and the Model
(Sternberg, 1997, p.1030). of JP Das)
Snyderman and Rothman (1987) have presented responses of over 1,000 experts
that belonged to different disciplines such as psychology, sociology, education,
and genetics. Of the thirteen descriptions rated by the respondents, there was
nearly unanimous agreement that abstract reasoning, the capacity to acquire
knowledge, and problem solving ability were important elements of intelligence.
Most of the earlier definitions as well as recent ones include the elements that
have been given above. We will mention a couple of recent definitions to support
our statement.
Alfred Binet’s early work on human abilities resembled the work of Galton.
Binet’s work on intelligence testing took a practical turn during the opening
years of the twentieth century, when he was commissioned by the French
government to identify mentally deficient children in French schools. Faced with
this problem, Binet, in collaboration with Theodore Simon, completed his first
test in 1905. This test comprised a list of 30 problems concerning the child’s
ability to understand and reason with the objects in the environment. The problems
ranged in level of difficulty and the test was tried on a sample of 50 children.
8
This was a very important step in the testing of intelligence. Theories of Intelligence (G
and S Factor and the Model
In 1908, a revision was made in which items were arranged in terms of age of JP Das)
levels. The highest age level that a child could perform successfully was called
his mental age. Later, William Stern (1914) suggested that this be divided by the
chronological age for each child, which multiplied by 100 became the intelligence
quotient, the IQ, as it has come to be known. The influence of Binet on the
measurement of intelligence can hardly be overstated. All subsequent work on
the measurement of intelligence is modeled after Binet’s test.
To understand his theory, let us assume that any correlation between two tests
used by Spearman implies a factor common to both, plus two specific factors.
Let the two tests be called a and b, the common factor “g”, and the two specific
factors sa and sb, as shown in the diagram drawn by Guilford (1953), which are
reproduced below in Fig. 1.1.
Subtests 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Analogies … .50 .49 .55 .49 .45 .45
Completion .50 … .54 .47 .50 .38 .34
Understanding paragraphs .49 .54 … .49 .39 .44 .35
Opposites .55 .47 .49 … .41 .32 .35
Instructions .49 .28 .39 .41 … .32 .40
Resemblances .45 .38 .44 .32 .32 … .35
Inferences .48 .34 .35 .35 .40 .35 …
Source: Spearman (1927) The abilities of man. New York: Macmillan, (p.149)
Guilford (1953, pp. 473-474) has shown that for any correlation matrix the
criterion of proportionality can be easily calculated, following which the tetrad
difference between various subtests can also be found.
Let us understand what is tetrad difference? In recent years the quantity F, called
the ‘tetrad-difference’, has become very important in psychological investigations
as to the possible nature of the underlying causes of mental activities. If there are
four such activities, and r13, r24, etc., the six correlation coefficients, (F is defined
by the equation F = r13r24 - r14r23. The value of F, in practice, approximates to
zero.)
The tetrad difference thus in all cases comes to zero. The variation in measured
intelligence that was not explainable in terms of this general factor or “g” was
attributed by Spearman to specific factors or “s”. There were many different
specific factors. All intellectual tasks require some amount of “g.” according to
Spearman, the more highly the two functions were correlated, the more highly
saturated they were with “g.” Tests that are thought to have high “g” loadings
involve abstract reasoning, comprehension, and problem solving.
Graphic illustration of “g” and “s” can be made following Guilford (1953, pp.
474-475), which is reproduced below (Fig.2). In this Figure “Spearman’s “g”
factor is shown as the large central circle and the specifics as small circles grouped
11
Intelligence and Creativity about G. Each ellipse stands for a mental test. The ellipses are permitted to overlap
G to different extents in order to indicate the fact that some tests are more heavily
“loaded” with G than others. The amount of correlation between any two tests is
determined by the extent to which the two tests are loaded with G. Thus, tests a
and b will have a relatively high correlation, since they have much in common in
G. Tests a and c will be scarcely correlated at all, since both have small loadings
with G.” (Guilford, 1953, pp. 474-475)
Fig. 1.2: Graphical representation of group factor in Spearman’s two factor theory
(Source: Guilford, 1953, p. 475)
Inter correlation matrices prepared by Spearman and his students showed that
some tests had something in common besides factor G. That meant that there
were some correlations that were over and above that demanded by a single
common factor G. Spearman at first attributed this to overlapping s factors.
However, some tests may have a higher correlation than that attributable to G
alone. Such an additional common factor became known as a group factor, which
was found to play a role not only in two tests but also in a number of tests.
“Among the group factors that Spearman and his associates came to recognise
are verbal ability, numerical ability, and possible factors of mental speed,
mechanical ability, attention, and imagination” (Guilford, 1953, p. 475).
Carroll (1993) also noted the presence of “g” at Stratum III in her hierarchical
factor analysis. We will now attempt a critical appraisal of the two-factor theory
and see how it has helped in the development of newer models of intelligence.
For Thorndike, intelligence was more like a series of skills or talents and several
or many tasks might call for the same kind of ability. According to him, the
correlations between various tests are the result of the fact that the tests have
features in common with each other even though they are called as measures of
different aspects.
Thorndike’s contention that there is no general intelligence but very specific acts
has, however, does not hold water in view of the fact that some tasks have so
many elements in common that it is desirable to classify them into groups such
as arithmetical reasoning, visual perception, word meaning, analogy, etc.
13
Intelligence and Creativity Thorndike has classified intellectual activity into three broad types: (i) social
intelligence, (ii) concrete intelligence, and (iii) abstract intelligence. However,
this is a classification of the type of tasks and not an analysis of mental organisation
itself. One can notice that the discrepancy of point of view between Spearman
and Thorndike is basically a theoretical one and the types that interested Thorndike
are essentially the same as the measures which Spearmen used in his correlation
matrix.
Eysenck (1972) has given two reasons of this type of apparently conflicting
findings. The first related to population sampled. Spearman had worked with
random samples of the population (usually children), Thurstone worked only
with students. The second related to the choice of tests: “Spearman has explicitly
stated that tests should not be too similar to each other; if they were, then the “s”
factors would overlap and cause additional correlations which would emerge as
separate factors and disturb the unit rank of the matrix. Thurstone used groups
of tests which were very similar, often almost identical, and consequently his
study could certainly not be considered as a test of Spearman’s hypothesis”
(Eysenck, 1972, p. 2).
14
Thurstone later on recognised the force of these points and incorporated them in Theories of Intelligence (G
and S Factor and the Model
his subsequent work which enabled him to find “a hierarchical structure of of JP Das)
intellect, with “g” at the top, and the “primary abilities” (whose inter correlations
necessitated the postulation of “g”) at a lower level; the actual tests used, whose
inter correlations gave rise to the “primary abilities”, would if course be at a
lower level still” (Eysenck, 1972, p.2).
Input Output
Serial Concurrent Serial Concurrent
First Third
Functional Unit Functional Unit
AROUSAL/
ATTENTION PLANNING
Conceptual
Conceptual
Perceptual
Perceptual
Knowledge Base
Knowledge Base
Memory
Memory
Brain Stem Frontal
Functiona Unit
The essential aspect of simultaneous processing is the surveyability; that is, each
element is related to every other element. Das (2004) has explained with the
help of following example.
“To produce a diagram correctly when given the instruction, “draw a triangle
above a square that is to the left of a circle under a cross,” the relationships
among the shapes must be correctly comprehended” (Das, 2004, p. 9).
The third functional unit is located in the prefrontal divisions of the frontal lobes
of the brain (Luria, 1980). Luria stated that “the frontal lobes synthesize the
information about the outside worlds . . . and are the means whereby the behaviour
of the organism is regulated in conformity with the effect produced by its actions”
(p.263).
All four processes of the PASS theory have been operationally defined by Das,
Nagliery and Kirby (1994). Planning processes are required when a test demands
that the individual makes some decisions about how to solve a problem, execute
an approach, activate attentional, simultaneous, and successive processes, monitor
the effectiveness of the approach and modify it as needed.
Planning processes are involved when a person is asked to decide how to perform
a test and is inhibited by the imposition of strict rules about how to perform. For
17
Intelligence and Creativity example, writing a composition involves generation of a plan, organisation of
the ideas, control over what is presented when, examination of the product, and
revisions to make the final result consistent with the intended goal.
Planning is clearly associated with the frontal lobes, especially the prefrontal
cortex. It has connections with the rest of the brain as described before, including
the parietal, temporal, and occipital lobes that are responsible for information
coding (simultaneous and successive processing), as well as with sub cortical
areas that determine the level of arousal and affective reactions to different
conditions on the basis of past experiences.
Attention arousal is a complex process of the PASS theory. Arousal keeps the
persons alert. It is associated with the activity of the brain stem and the lower
part of the cerebral cortex. Attention on the other hand is associated with the
frontal lobes and the lower portion of the cortex together.
Simultaneous processing is broadly associated with the occipital and the parietal
lobes, while successive processing is associated with frontal temporal lobes
Knowledge base is an integral component of the PASS model and therefore all
processes are embedded within this dimension. The base of knowledge included
in the PASS model is intended to represent all information obtained from the
cultural and social background of the individual, because this determines the
form of mental activity. Children’s use of language to analyse, generalise, and
encode experience is a critical determinant of the base of knowledge, because
mental processes cannot develop apart from the appropriate forms of social life.
The final component of the PASS model is output or action and behaviour. It is
suggested that both simultaneous and successive processes must be used in the
processing of cognitive tasks. Das (1998, p. 221) has thus explained its salient
features: “The PASS theory of intelligence (1) has given us tests to measure
intelligence as a set of cognitive processes, (2) discusses what the major processes
are, and (3) guides us in the remediation of processing difficulties.”
Cognition is a dynamic process that works within the context of the individual’s
knowledge base, responds to his experiences, and is subject to developmental
variations When considering the measurement of cognitive processes, it must
be noted that the effective processing is accomplished through the integration of
knowledge with planning, attention, simultaneous, and successive processes as
demanded by the particular task. Although these processes are interrelated and
nonstop, they are not equally involved in all tasks. For that reason, cognitive
assessment tasks for planning, attention, simultaneous, and successive processing
were developed to adhere to PASS theory and predominantly require a specific
cognitive process (Das, Nagliery, & Kirby, 1994).
Taking the lead of Das and by using the multivariate techniques of cluster analysis,
Ronning (2004) developed ability/achievement normative taxonomies for reading
and mathematics of children in the age group of 8 to 17 years. The core profiles
that emerged provided important comparisons for evaluating individual profiles,
as well as added to the information explaining common variability in the child
population. The taxonomies werebased upon 711 children in the 8 to 17 year old
portion of the standardisation sample ofthe Cognitive Assessment System (CAS)
who were co-administered the Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Achievement–Revised
18
(WJ-R ACH). Ability/reading and ability/mathematics normative taxonomies Theories of Intelligence (G
and S Factor and the Model
were developed from the Planning, Attention, Simultaneous, and Successive of JP Das)
scales of the Cognitive Assessment System (Das, Nagliery, & Kirby, 1994) in
conjunction with four reading and three math WJ-RACH subscales. Eight reading
and five math clusters were identified and described using demographics and
overall ability and achievement levels, which enabled Ronning (2004) to develop
intervention programme also.
It is a known fact that tests of intelligence, beginning with that of Binet and
Simon (1905), have played an important input in predicting school success (or
its absence) since the turn of the century. The various tests that were developed
in criticism of Spearman, however, incorporated a number of similar
characteristics. Intelligence test batteries differ in other ways, such as the
theoretical underpinnings and appropriate uses of the test, as well as the types of
questions utilised.We have already discussed the views of Thurstone who
extended support to Spearman when methodology was followed as suggested by
Spearman. Jensen (1998) and Carroll (1993) have found the presence of “g” in
their factor analyses.
The PASS theory of Das, Nagliery and Kirby (1994) is an information processing
theory, which has taken its inspiration from the pioneering neuropsychological
and cognitive psychological researches of Alexander Luria (1966; 1973; 1980).
Luria described human cognitive processes within the framework of three
functional units. The function of the first unit is cortical arousal and attention;
the second unit codes information using simultaneous and successive processes;
and the third unit provides for planning, self-monitoring, and structuring of
cognitive activities. Luria’s work on the functional aspects of brain structures
formed the basis of the PASS model and was used as a blueprint for defining the
important components of human intellectual competence.
20
A Cognitive Assessment System (CAS) has also been developed by Das, Nagliery Theories of Intelligence (G
and S Factor and the Model
and Kirby (1994) and a number of researches on various aspects of human of JP Das)
cognition have extended increasing support to the contentions of the proponents
of this theory. The Cognitive Assessment System is an individualised assessment
that may be used for a variety of purposes, including diagnosis, eligibility,
determination of discrepancies, reevaluation, and instructional planning.