Qualitative Research Methods in Psychology: Deborah Biggerstaff
Qualitative Research Methods in Psychology: Deborah Biggerstaff
Qualitative Research Methods in Psychology: Deborah Biggerstaff
net/publication/236158419
CITATIONS READS
32 20,452
1 author:
Deborah Biggerstaff
The University of Warwick
26 PUBLICATIONS 454 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
Experiences of women in early parenting, and seeking support for PTSD following Birth Trauma View project
All content following this page was uploaded by Deborah Biggerstaff on 01 June 2014.
1. Introduction
In the scientific community, and particularly in psychology and health, there has been an
active and ongoing debate on the relative merits of adopting either quantitative or
qualitative methods, especially when researching into human behaviour (Bowling, 2009;
Oakley, 2000; Smith, 1995a, 1995b; Smith, 1998). In part, this debate formed a component of
the development in the 1970s of our thinking about science. Andrew Pickering has described
this movement as the “sociology of scientific knowledge” (SSK), where our scientific
understanding, developing scientific ‘products’ and ‘know-how’, became identified as
forming components in a wider engagement with society’s environmental and social context
(Pickering, 1992, pp. 1). Since that time, the debate has continued so that today there is an
increasing acceptance of the use of qualitative methods in the social sciences (Denzin &
Lincoln, 2000; Morse, 1994; Punch, 2011; Robson, 2011) and health sciences (Bowling, 2009;
Greenhalgh & Hurwitz, 1998; Murphy & Dingwall, 1998). The utility of qualitative methods
has also been recognised in psychology. As Nollaig Frost (2011) observes, authors such as
Carla Willig and Wendy Stainton Rogers consider qualitative psychology is much more
accepted today and that it has moved from “the margins to the mainstream in psychology in
the UK.” (Willig & Stainton Rogers, 2008, pp. 8). Nevertheless, in psychology, qualitative
methodologies are still considered to be relatively ‘new’ (Banister, Bunn, Burman, et al.,
2011; Hayes, 1998; Richardson, 1996) despite clear evidence to the contrary (see, for example,
the discussion on this point by Rapport et al., 2005). Nicki Hayes observes, scanning the
content of some early journals from the 1920s – 1930s that many of these more historical
papers “discuss personal experiences as freely as statistical data” (Hayes, 1998, 1). This can
be viewed as an early development of the case-study approach, now an accepted
methodological approach in psychological, health care and medical research, where our
knowledge about people is enhanced by our understanding of the individual ‘case’ (May &
Perry, 2011; Radley & Chamberlain, 2001; Ragin, 2011; Smith, 1998).
The discipline of psychology, originating as it did during the late 19th century, in parallel
with developments in modern medicine, tended, from the outset, to emphasise the ‘scientific
method’ as the way forward for psychological inquiry. This point of view arose out of the
previous century’s Enlightenment period which underlay the founding of what is generally
agreed to be the first empirical experimental psychology laboratory, established by Wilhelm
Wundt, University of Leipzig, in 1879. During this same period, other early psychology
www.intechopen.com
176 Psychology - Selected Papers
researchers, such as the group of scientific thinkers interested in perception (the Gestaltists:
see, for example, Lamiell, 1995) were developing their work. Later, in the 20th century, the
introduction of Behaviourism became the predominant school of psychology in America
and Britain. Behaviourism emphasised a reductionist approach, and this movement, until its
displacement in the 1970-80s by the ‘cognitive revolution’, dominated the discipline of
psychology (Hayes, 1998, pp. 2-3). These approaches have served the scientific community
well, and have been considerably enhanced by increasingly sophisticated statistical
computer programmes for data analysis.
A recent feature of the debate in the future direction for psychology has been a concern for
the philosophical underpinnings of the discipline and an appreciation of their importance.
In part, this is an intrinsic part of theoretical developments in psychology and the related
social sciences, in particular sociological research, such as Grounded Theory, developed by
the sociologists Glazer and Strauss during the 1960s and 1970s (e.g. Charmaz, 1983; Glaser &
Strauss, 1967; Searle, 2012); modes of social inquiry such as interviewing and content
analysis (Gillham, 2000; King & Horrocks, 2010); action research (Hart & Bond, 1999;
Sixsmith & Daniels, 2011); discourse and discourse analysis (Tonkiss, 2012; Potter &
Wetherell, 1995); narrative (Polkinghorne, 1988; Reissman, 2008); biographical research
methods (Roberts, 2002); phenomenological methods (Giorgi,1995; Langdridge, 2007;
Lawthom &Tindall, 2011; Smith et al., 2009 ); focus groups (Carey, 1994; Vazquez-Lago et
al., 2011); visual research methods (Mitchell, 2011); ethnographic methods (Boyles, 1994;
Punch, 2011); photo-biographic-elicitation methods (Rapport et al., 2008); and, finally, the
combining or integrating of methods, the approach often known as ‘mixed methods’ (Frost,
2011; Pope et al., 2007; Thomas et al., 2004; Todd et al., 2004).
Qualitative methods have much to offer when we need to explore people’s feelings or ask
participants to reflect on their experiences. As was noted above, some of the earliest
psychological thinkers of the late 19th century and early 20th century may be regarded as
proto-qualitative researchers. Examples include the ‘founding father’ of psycho-analysis,
Sigmund Freud, who worked in Vienna (late 19th century – to mid 20th century), recorded
and published numerous case-studies and then engaged in analysis, postulation and
theorising on the basis of his observations, and the pioneering Swiss developmental
psychologist, Jean Piaget (1896 – 1980) who meticulously observed and recorded his
children’s developing awareness and engagement with their social world. They were
succeeded by many other authors from the 1940s onwards who adopted qualitative
methods and may be regarded as contributors to the development of qualitative
methodologies through their emphasis of the importance of the idiographic and use of case
studies (Allport,1946; Nicholson, 1997)1 . This locates the roots of qualitative thinking in the
long-standing debate between empiricist and rationalistic schools of thought, and also in
social constructionism (Gergen, 1985; King & Horrock, pp. 6 – 24)2.
1 Allport states “[…] among the methods having idiographic intent, and emphasised by me, are the case
study, the personal document, interviewing methods,matching, personal structure analysis, and other
procedures that contrive to keep together what nature itself has fashioned as an integrated unit – the
single personality.” (Allport, 1946, pp. 133).
2 A notable milestone in the development of qualitative methodologies in the UK for example, was the
publication, in 1992, of a paper proposing a role for qualitative methods for psychology, by Karen
Henwood and Nick Pigeon in the British Journal of Psychology.
www.intechopen.com
Qualitative Research Methods in Psychology 177
More recently, in the UK, the British Psychological Society now has a members’ section for
Qualitative Methods in Psychology (QMiP) which held a successful inaugural conference, in
2008, at the University of Leeds. The Section now boasts a membership of more than 1000
members, making it one of the largest BPS Sections. The undergraduate psychology
curriculum, which confers BPS graduate basis for registration (GBR), now includes
qualitative research methods teaching in the core programme for UK universities degrees.
Elsewhere, qualitative psychology has taken a little longer to be accepted e.g. by the
American Psychological Association (APA). This is somewhat surprising given the large
volume of qualitative research papers which originate from the American research
community. However, US researchers, alongside their international colleagues, have finally
managed to petition successfully for the inclusion of qualitative methodologies to be
admitted to Section 5, the methodology section, of the APA, during 2011.
These developments can be tracked by a search for qualitative research across the main
electronic databases and exploring the ‘hits’ recovered. A quick scan using the umbrella
terms ‘qualitative’ and / or ‘qualitative research’ for example, provides the researcher with a
result for a relatively low number of papers from the earlier years of last century. However
there is a noticeably sharp increase in the number of papers published from 1990 onwards.
A search of the main databases, using the term “qualitative” as a key word (January, 1990 -
December, 2011) produced a retrieval rate for qualitative papers of over 51744 hits
(CINAHL); 122012 hits (PsycInfo); 12108 for Medline (OVID); and 18431 for Applied Social
Sciences Index and Abstracts (ASSIA). Prior to 1990 the number of papers recorded in these
databases is noticeably lower: searching in ASSIA for papers published between 1985 – 1990,
for example, results in 13 papers, while a Medline search for the years 1985 – 1990 returns 6
papers. Searching in CINAHL for the same period (1985 – 1990) results in no papers (zero
result).
(See Henwood, K. & Pidgeon, N. (1992) Qualitative research and psychological theorising. British
Journal of Psychology, 83: 97 – 111).
3 For readers interested in more on the history of the philosophy of science and its relationship to
developments in psychology, I recommend the following authors: Andrew Pickering (1992); John
Richardson (1996); Mark Smith (1998); Clive Seale (2012); and especially Jonathan Smith and colleagues
with the publication of Rethinking Methods in Psychology (Smith et al., 1995b).
www.intechopen.com
178 Psychology - Selected Papers
process.4 The qualitative researcher accepts that s/he is not ‘neutral’. Instead s/he puts
herself in the position of the participant or 'subject' and attempts to understand how the
world is from that person's perspective. As this process is re-iterated, hypotheses begin to
emerge, which are 'tested' against the data of further experiences e.g. people's narratives.
One of the key differences between quantitative and qualitative approaches is apparent
here: the quantitative approach states the hypothesis from the outset, (i.e. a ‘top down’
approach), whereas in qualitative research the hypothesis or research question, is refined
and developed during the process. This may be thought of as a ‘bottom-up’ or emergent
approach, as, for example, in Grounded Theory (Charmaz, 1995).This contrast is part of the
epistemological positions that shape our assumptions about the world. King and Horrocks
summarise some of these main differences in position as being either realist, contextual or
constructionist. They compare these to assumptions about the world, the knowledge
produced and the role of the researcher (King & Horrocks, 2010). These authors, along with
others, such as Colin Robson, advocate adopting a pragmatic approach to qualitative
research. As Robson observes, “Pragmatism is almost an ‘anti-philosophical’ philosophy
which advocates getting on with the research rather than philosophizing – hence providing
a welcome antidote to a stultifuying over-concern with matters such as ontology and
epistemology.” (Robson, 2011, pp.30)5.
It may be helpful to think of qualitative research as situated at one end of a continuum with
its data from in-depth interviews, and with quantitative ‘measurable’ data at the other end
(see Figure 1). At the centre-point of this continuum may rest such data as content analysis
and questionnaire responses transformed from the written or spoken word into numerical
‘codes’ for statistical analysis. Examples include standardised questionnaires, e.g. for
depression and anxiety such as Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS), or Beck’s
Depression Inventory. With limited space given on questionnaires, respondents can only
give the briefest answers to pre-formulated questions from the researchers. Respondents’
replies are coded and ‘scored’, but does that mean that we can measure feelings or emotion?
How do we 'calculate' levels of depression or anxiety? How does the experience of
depression affect people’s lives? Have we, as researchers, asked appropriate questions in the
first place? Qualitative research methodology looks to answer these types of questions – the
exploratory approach. An example of this exploratory approach is Jonathan Smith’s work
examining young mothers’ lived-world experiences of the psychological transition to
motherhood (see, for example, Smith, 1999; 1998; 1994).
4 This is in contrast to the positivist, hypothetico-deductive methodology, associated with the philosopher
Karl Popper, and enthusiastically adopted by the psychology discipline, of 'refuting the null
hypothesis', commonly taken to be the 'gold standard' of quantitative scientific research methodology
i.e. where hypotheses are defined at the start of the research (see, for example, Popper,1935/2002). One
of the challenges however of attempting to fit the ‘scientific’ approach into researching human
behaviour, is that sometimes this scientific experimental methodology, the design of which originates in
the laboratory, may not quite provide what is needed when attempting to investigate psychological and
human behaviours. The Medical Research Council (MRC) in the UK also acknowledges this. In 2008
they provided new guidance to their 2000 MRC Framework for the development and evaluation of RCTs for
complex interventions to improve health to include non-experimental methods, and complex interventions
outside healthcare. See
http://www.mrc.ac.uk/Utilities/Documentrecord/index.htm?d=MRC004871
5 See also Robson, 2011, pp. 30 – 35 for further discussion on this topic.
www.intechopen.com
Qualitative Research Methods in Psychology 179
6 I thank the book’s editor, Gina Rossi for this helpful comment.
www.intechopen.com
180 Psychology - Selected Papers
Jonathan Smith and his colleagues, for example, announce at the beginning of their
Rethinking psychology, that “Psychology is in a state of flux” with an “unprecedented degree
of questioning about the nature of the subject, the boundaries of the discipline and what
new ways of conducting psychological research are available.” (Smith et al., 1995a, pp. 1).
Rom Harré, heralded these new ways of thinking as marking the ‘discursive turn’ (Harré,
1995a, pp. 146), while Ken Gergen, writes about there being a ‘revolution in qualitative
research’ (Gergen, 2001, pp. 3).
Additionally, as Karen Henwood suggests, integrating qualitative with quantitative
methods in psychology also provides researchers with a tool for the potential
“democratisation of the research process”. She observes how among clinical psychologists
working in the United Kingdom’s National Health Service (NHS) for instance, the research
process can be “opened to include the views of service users”with an increasing emphasis
on exploring “people’s personal and cultural understandings and stocks of knowledge”
(Henwood, 2004, pp. 43). Henwood suggests that integrating methods may thus also help
establish and embed research validity by communicating responsibly and honestly when
exploring multiple perspectives.
In a parallel movement, qualitative methods have also come to be increasingly
acknowledged across the social sciences more generally (Banister, et al. 2011; Oakley, 2000;
www.intechopen.com
Qualitative Research Methods in Psychology 181
Potter, 1996; Radley & Chamberlain, 200; Richardson, 1996; Strauss & Corbin, 1990; Willig,
2008). Meanwhile, as already noted above, the use of narrative and meaning in psychology
and the human sciences also re-emerged (Bruner, 1990, 1991; Crossley, 2000; Polkinghorne,
1988; Reissman, 2008). Interestingly, Polkinghorne observes that, in contrast to other related
disciplines in the social sciences, psychology very largely ignored the use of narrative until
the end of the 1980s – early 1990s with a shift towards a "renewed interest in narrative as a
cognitive structure" (Polkinghorne, 1988, pp. 101) as an element in the field of cognitive
psychology. Polkinghorne suggests that the re-emergence of narrative thinking in
psychology took place during this period due to the increased attention being given by
psychologists to the utility of exploring life histories, self-narrative (for example in
establishing one’s personal identity) and a renewal of interest in the case study and
biographical research (Roberts, 2002). Polkinghorne, along with other authors, such as
Jerome Bruner (1990, 1991, 2002) and Ricoeur (1981/1995) proposes that our use of narrative
is linked to the perception of time and our place in the lived-world where
“[...] people use self-stories to interpret and account for their lives. The basic dimension
of human existence is temporality, and narrative transforms the mere passing away of
time into a meaningful unity, the self. The study of a person’s own experience of her or
his life-span requires attending to the operations of the narrative form and how this life
story is related to the stories of others.”
(Polkinghorne, 1988, pp. 119).
During the same period, (i.e. over the past ten – fifteen years), psychology and social science
journals, such as the British Journal of Psychology, Journal of Health Psychology, Social Science
and Medicine etc., as, indeed, did the British Medical Journal, began to include qualitative
research papers, indicating that a qualitative approach, in parallel with the quantitative
scientific paradigm, can illuminate important areas in the behavioural sciences and
psychology. In the early days there was some debate about academic ‘rigour’ and validity
suggesting some unease about using qualitative methods, both in psychology and related
areas. This is now much improved as researchers address these issues (Bloor, 1997; Henwood,
2004; Yardley, 2008). However, this is less of a challenge today, with increasing acceptance of
these methods and the introduction of appraisal checklists. Nevertheless, as with any research,
poor understanding of the methodology and what it can offer, or the inappropriate selection of
a method, is likely to lead to poor quality results and the resultant lack of any real insight into
the area being explored. Today, the introduction of evidence-based tools such as the Critical
Appraisal Skills Programme (CASP) based at the Centre for Evidence Based Medicine,
University of Oxford, include a qualitative paper checklist on their website providing evidence
of a much greater acceptance of these methods (CEBMhttp://www.sph.nhs.uk/what-we-
do/public-health-workforce/resources/critical-appraisals-skills-programme).
Additionally, the British Psychological Society has now developed guidelines for the
appraisal of qualitative papers indicating the wider academic acceptance of qualitative
psychology today. As Peter Banister and his colleagues note, writing in the preface to their
Qualitative methods in psychology, in benchmarking UK psychology degrees, The Quality
Assurance Agency of 2007, include a section on the need for students to cover both
quantitative and qualitative research methods. This includes being able to analyse both
types of data. This ability is now (2010) also highlighted as being a requirement for
conferring of BPS Chartership status (Banister et al., 2011: vii - viii)
www.intechopen.com
182 Psychology - Selected Papers
Thus, in order to best gain insight into the field of qualitative psychology some of this
background knowledge of the specific theoretical and philosophical underpinnings outlined
earlier is needed by researchers today who decide to explore their chosen research topic
using qualitative methods. These theoretical and philosophical concerns inform the
discipline and it is important that the researcher understands this.
3. Triangulation
The term ‘triangulation’, according to Huberman and Miles, is thought to originate from
Campbell and Fiske’s 1959 work on “multiple operationalism” developed from geometry
and trigonometry (Huberman & Miles, 1998). Huberman and Miles caution that the term
‘triangulation’ may have more than one interpretation. However, it is usually used to
describe data verification of data, and considered as a method for
“…checking for the most common or the most insidious biases that can steal into the
process of drawing conclusions.”
(Huberman & Miles, 1998, pp. 198)
www.intechopen.com
Qualitative Research Methods in Psychology 183
When researchers employ triangulation, multiple measures are used to ensure that any data
variance is not due to the way in which the data were collected or measured. By linking
different methods, the researcher intends that each method enhances the other, since all the
information that is collected potentially offers to be contextually richer than if it were seen
from only one vantage point. Each area provides a commentary on the other areas of the
research (Frost, 2009). Triangulation can be a useful tool to examine data overload, where
researchers analysing data may miss some important information due to an over-reliance on
one portion of the data which could then skew the analysis. Another use is to provide
checks and balances on the salience of first impressions. Triangulation is also a useful tool to
help avoid data selectivity, such as being over-confident about a particular section of the
data analysis such as when trying to confirm a key finding, or without taking into account
the potential for sources of data unreliability (Huberman & Miles, 1998, pp.198-9).
It should be noted however, that, although triangulation is generally considered helpful
when using qualitative methods, it can just as equally be applied to quantitative or mixed-
methods research. It is a pragmatic and strategic approach, whether applied to qualitative or
qualitative research (Denzin & Lincoln, 1998). It may be viewed as providing a way of
expanding the research perspective and becomes another means of strengthening research
findings (Krahn et al.,1995).
Banister et al. (2011) point out that any method of enquiry, whether quantitative or
qualitative, can be open to bias and/or value laden, a fact that should be acknowledged,
“[…] a researcher and research cannot be value-free, and that a general ‘objectivist’
notion that science can be value-free is impossible, given that we are all rooted in a
social world that is socially constructed. Psychology (at least in the West) has general
values (even if these are often left implicit) of communicating broadening knowledge
and understanding about people, with a commitment to both freedom of enquiry and
freedom of expression.”
(Banister et al., 2011, pp. 204)
Triangulation can help balance out, if not overcome, some of the challenges inherent in
research, of whatever methodological persuasion (Todd et al., 2004). Triangulation can be
separated into four broad categories: data triangulation, investigator triangulation,
triangulation of method and triangulation of theory.
www.intechopen.com
184 Psychology - Selected Papers
The approach supports research being a reflexive, organic process, enriched by researchers’
increasing depth of knowledge as they investigate the area (Finlay, 2003). This is linked to
the role of reflexivity in qualitative research, considered by many to be an essential
component in qualitative inquiry (Banister et al., 2011, pp. 200-201; Frost, 2011, pp. 11-12).
The researcher is expected to be able to stand back from the completed research and
consider, in retrospect, the selected methodology, whether the approach adopted suited the
analysis undertaken what the experience may have been like for both the researcher and the
participants etc. Other factors which may be considered include whether flaws were found
in the research design, how the research study might be improved or refined, what further
research might be needed etc. Some researchers advocate keeping a journal or diary
recording these reflexions during the actual research process (Robson, 2011, pp. 270).
www.intechopen.com
Qualitative Research Methods in Psychology 185
“it cannot be assumed that that those around the table will have an equally shared
degree of responsibility and contribution. If not, then once again fairness is challenged
and ultimately more problems are created than solved.”
(Sixsmith & Daniels, 2011, pp. 32-32)
www.intechopen.com
186 Psychology - Selected Papers
www.intechopen.com
Qualitative Research Methods in Psychology 187
Methodology
www.intechopen.com
188 Psychology - Selected Papers
www.intechopen.com
Qualitative Research Methods in Psychology 189
sometimes has a limited applicability, proposing that, “It could be argued that research
questions about the nature of experience are more suitably addressed using
phenomenological research methods.” (Willig, 2008, pp. 47). In all fairness, GT was
originally developed for researching from a sociological perspective and, while there is
some commonality between sociology and social psychology, the use of GT to analyse data
might not always provide a sufficiently robust and flexible way of capturing psychological
nuances and complexities contained in participants’ narratives about lived experiences. GT,
as a methodology, was therefore adopted and adapted by some qualitative psychologists
(Pidgeon & Henwood, 1997). Willig concludes that GT can be “reserved for the study of
social psychological processes” as a descriptive method (Willig, 2008, pp. 47).A further
challenge, when considering using GT, is the challenge provided by the different types of
GT that have developed within the field such as the debate on the two main ‘schools’ of GT:
Straussian and Glaserian (see Stern, 1994, pp. 213 on for discussion).
www.intechopen.com
190 Psychology - Selected Papers
This approach to studying human behaviour uses words, contained in language, as symbols
with meaning, where the ‘subject’ i.e. the person, is seen as discursive in order that they may
make sense of their environment by signifying “the order of things” (Foucault, quoted by
Harré & Gillett, 1994, pp. 26).
The discursive view sees people as active agents within their own lives and, as such, cannot,
“be defined in isolation from a context and whose mental processes can be unravelled by
objective measurement and description.” (Harré & Gillett, 1994, pp. 26).
However, when considering the selection of a qualitative method, and thinking about using
discourse analysis, we need to be clear about what our research aims and objectives are.
Participants’ narratives frequently include elements relating to feelings and emotions rather
than how reality is manufactured and portrayed in conversation. Willig suggests that
discourse analysis can be used to explore “the internal organisation of the discourse itself
and ask ‘what is this discourse doing?’” (Willig, 2008, pp. 99). Here is a prime distinction
between DA and other psychological qualitative methods such as IPA, in that DA explores
the role of language in participants’ descriptions of events and conversations while the
phenomenological approach examines how people ascribe meaning to their experiences in
their interactions with their environment (Biggerstaff & Thompson, 2008; Pringle et al., 2011;
Shinebourne, 2011; Smith et al., 2009; Smith et al., 1999).
www.intechopen.com
Qualitative Research Methods in Psychology 191
Using psychology of narrative, for example, the researcher may examine people’s life stories
or their accounts of such experiences (Esin, 2011). It explores the biographical lives of
participants’ lives or social and cultural stories (Goodley, 2011). Psychology of narrative can
be helpful to explore and interpret findings from such research, since this type of enquiry
helps the researcher to enter more fully into understanding people’s lives and their
experiences (Crossley, 2000; Greenhalgh & Hurwitz, 1999 & 1998; Murray, 2008 & 1995). Esin
defines essential features of narrative as connections between events that help make these
events meaningful for the audience, stating that “Sequence is necessary for narrative. A
narrative always responds to the question ‘And then what happened?’” (Esin, 2011, pp. 93).
Narrative in psychology can provide an important method for exploring psychological
development, self-understanding and people’s inter-relationship with their world (Gergen,
2001b). Examining human experiences and ‘making sense’ of our environment offers a core
method of enquiry across many disciplines and cultures (Brockmeier, 2009; Brown et al.,
1996; Bruner, 1999; Bruner, 2002; Charon, 2005; Harré, 2003; Murray, 2008; Riessman, 2008).
The use of narrative methods in both psychology and medicine, assumes a narrator and a
listener. Narrative is an interactive transaction with the potential for narrator and listener to
assign their own meanings to their experiences as the topic under discussion unfolds
(Bruner, 1991; 1990). Bruner for example, proposes that the interpretation of people’s actions
and their narratives about what happens to them provides us with explanations of those
experiences. Such interpretation “is concerned with ‘reasons’ for things happening, rather
than strictly with their ‘causes’ ” (Bruner, 1991).
7 The IPA website and research forum may be found at: www.ipa.bbk.ac.uk
www.intechopen.com
192 Psychology - Selected Papers
experience, the idiographic case-study approach proposed by methods such as IPA can be
especially helpful (Smith et al., 2009). IPA was specifically developed by Jonathan Smith
(Smith et al., 1995) to rigorously explore idiographic, subjective experiences and, specifically,
social cognitions. It is now widely used within British psychology (e.g. Clare, 2003; Duncan et
al., 2001; French et al., 2005; Smith, 2011; Thompson et al. 2002).
Phenomenology in psychology places the experience of the self at the centre of the current
psychological dialogue about people’s lived experiences and their meanings (Cohen &
Omery, 1994; Giorgi, 1995; Giorgi & Giorgi, 2008; Langdridge, 2007; Smith et al., 1995;
Spinelli, 2005). The qualitative psychologist is aiming to see and understand what surrounds
us (Cohen & Omery, 1994). When exploring the 'taken for granted' - the everyday lives of
participants, especially those aspects relating to the psychology of how people feel about an
issue, event, or experience for example - the use of a phenomenological approach highlights
such issues and brings them to the fore. This might be useful for example, when considering
the background of health service delivery (Oakley, 1993, pp. 235).
IPA’s theoretical basis stems from the phenomenology originating with Husserl’s attempts
to construct a philosophical science of consciousness, with hermeneutics (the theory of
interpretation), and symbolic interactionism. This last proposes that meanings an individual
may ascribe to an event are of central concern, but that access to such meaning can only be
obtained through an interpretative process. IPA acknowledges that the researcher’s
engagement with the participant’s ‘text’ has an interpretative element, in contrast to some
other methods (e.g., discourse analysis, DA; see Potter, 1996). IPA assumes an
epistemological stance whereby, through its careful and explicit interpretative methodology,
it becomes possible to access the meanings an individual gives to their feelings and their
cognitive inner world. IPA also draws on Gadamer’s philosophy of hermeneutics and the
study of the understanding of the text (Smith, 2007).
Attention is drawn however to one of the main differences between IPA and Discourse
Analysis (DA): DA aims to examine the role of language in describing a person’s experience,
whereas IPA intends to explore how people may ascribe meaning to their experiences when
interacting with their environment (Smith et al., 1999). It is thus especially suited to
behavioural and psychological studies that relate findings to the bio-psycho-social theories
informing discourse among healthcare professions (Smith, 1996; Smith, 2004; Willig, 2008).
IPA is a qualitative methodology with a clearly set out methodology that is both rigorous
and yet sufficiently flexible fit for a wide range of types of study (Biggerstaff & Thompson,
2008; Brocki & Wearden, 2006; Smith & Osborn, 2008; Willig, 2008). It is important to note
that IPA is only one version of phenomenological research methodologies (Willig, 2008) and
other phenomenological approaches are also useful to the qualitative researcher (Giorgi,
1995). As van Manen observes
“the simple phenomenological precept (is) to always try to understand someone from
his or her situation. [….]
The phenomenological approach asks of us that we constantly measure our
understandings and insights against the lived reality of our concrete experiences,
which, of course, are always more complex than any particular interpretation can
portray”
(van Manen, 1998, pp. 8,pp. 10)
www.intechopen.com
Qualitative Research Methods in Psychology 193
8 From the concept of the Life-world or Lebenswelt, from Husserl’s unpublished works after his death
(see for example Ashworth, 2008: 10 - 12; Philipse, 1995: 277; Cohen and Omery, 1994: 139, for further
discussion).
www.intechopen.com
194 Psychology - Selected Papers
www.intechopen.com
Qualitative Research Methods in Psychology 195
about the topic for instance? Alternatively, perhaps the researchers have explored the
chosen topic from a different angle, or incorporated different viewpoints from their
participant sample. As identified earlier in this chapter, people are seen as an important
resource for collaboration, thus highlighting the need for qualitative research to
acknowledge reflexivity and subjectivity (Sixsmith & Daniels, 2011, pp. 26 – 7). An example
of this type of approach might be where the research examines the views of a minority
whose opinions have not previously been sought. In turn, such a paper would then pave the
way for further research.
Next we need to think about the method(s) the researchers have used for their study. Does it
seem appropriate? Does the study design lend itself to using a qualitative approach? In
examining the reasons for conducting any such study, we need to bear in mind questions
such as: does the research team situate their reasons for carrying out their work within an
appropriate body of research literature?
Alternatively, the approach used may incorporate theoretical interest. Perhaps the research
topic is approached in a different way, or from a different and newer theoretical context?
Again, this needs to be clear to the reader with appropriate support from the theoretical
literature. Does the research reported contribute to the development of knowledge in the
direction of theory?
www.intechopen.com
196 Psychology - Selected Papers
The paper should clearly state how data were collected and managed. This includes
information about areas such as data collection and storage: how was the transcription of
recorded interviews handled, how are data stored, and for how long does the research team
plan to store their data? Exact details are usually governed by local or national research data
governance but these details must be considered. Research governance details are
particularly important in qualitative research since a person’s audio interview is more likely
to be unique to that individual compared to a briefer set of responses contained from a
questionnaire for example. While researchers generally are able to offer anonymity, such as
using pseudonyms, it can be harder for the qualitative researcher to offer total
confidentiality since a person’s narrative about an experience may be unique to them, thus
offering more likelihood for possible identification. If this is the case, then the researcher
must inform the reader what steps they have taken to protect their participants – perhaps by
changing identifying features or details of that particular participant’s situation (Frith, 2007,
pp. 126).
How data are recorded is also important. The use of field notes is recommended, and can
prove invaluable as an aide-memoire, they also provide ‘back-up’ insurance should there be
recording failure. However, data verification is also very important. I strongly recommend
all interview and focus group data be recorded, whether using audio or video, in order to
obtain the highest quality ‘raw data’ prior to analysis.
www.intechopen.com
Qualitative Research Methods in Psychology 197
excerpts tell us about the research results (Frith, 2007, pp. 124-5). Are any quotations used to
highlight findings? It is helpful if the paper states whether the excerpts are illustrative, or
provide contrasting viewpoints by participants. It is also helpful to ask oneself whether the
researchers comment if they consider their findings are transferable from one context to
another and, should that be the case, what particular elements do they highlight as being
most important or relevant?
When we consider these results and themes as a whole, do we find the researchers’
conclusions useful and applicable? Do the conclusions drawn seem reasonable and
appropriate, given what we know about the background and stated aims for the study?
Does the paper state if excerpts provided are illustrative or, have the researchers provided
the reader with contrasting viewpoints?
Another area to examine is that of ‘credibility checks’. Sometimes researchers may feedback
their proposed analysis to individual participants and ask for comments. Not all methods
adopt this approach. However, if this has been carried out by the research team, is the
process clear to the reader?
Sometimes there may be an alternative explanation or thematic summary to a particular
perspective. This needs to be considered and discussed by the research team. Do the
researchers adopt a reflexive approach? If so, do they state how they handled these
reflections and how these relate to their reported findings? As discussed earlier (section 3.1).
reflexivity is important in qualitative research since researchers need “to be aware of their
own positions and interests and to explicitly situate themselves within the research.”(Finlay,
2003, pp. 5). From the perspective of the critical reader, it is helpful to stand back from the
results and ask oneself if the researchers’ results and conclusions drawn are credible and
appropriate in relation to the original research question. Other areas which need to be
considered include whether these results relate back to the research literature and
theoretical background reported in the authors’ original literature review and research aims.
Finally, the reviewer needs to consider whether the researchers discuss whether their
findings are transferable from one context to another. Are the results and conclusions useful
and applicable?
www.intechopen.com
198 Psychology - Selected Papers
“then finds a whole web of cultural structures, knowledge and meanings which are
knotted and superimposed on one another and which constitute a densely layered
cultural script”
(Walsh, 2012, pp. 247, original author’s emphasis)
Data sources in qualitative research include interviews, focus groups, observation
techniques, analysis of text, such as historic diaries and journals, film, video or art work.
Validity can be confirmed with research participants. Finally, qualitative research does not
pretend to be other than it is: it is situation specific
Of course, some of these features also have their counterpart. For example the issue of data
being context specific means that it is not always generalisable to a different context. This
should be borne in mind when considering qualitative approaches. The researcher is not
separate from the research process but instead can be seen as part of the process. This may
have an impact on the research and the data collected and the researcher usually
acknowledges her role in the research process (Willig, 2008).
Although the smaller size of qualitative data sets and the eventual findings may limit
generalisable applicability, nevertheless such findings may reflect and inform what is
happening within a larger population. This can then be examined further. The idiographic
approach that forms a major trend in qualitative research does however take account of
individuals, their values, and their experiences in a way that places people at the centre of
the field of study.
To summarise, Table 3 provides a brief check-list for the appraisal of qualitative psychology
research papers. When assessing a paper for quality the reader may wish to consider the
following points adapted from the British Psychological Society guidelines for authors and
reviewers. These guidelines are available online:http://www.bps.org.uk
www.intechopen.com
Qualitative Research Methods in Psychology 199
www.intechopen.com
200 Psychology - Selected Papers
12. References
Allport, G. W. (1946. Personalistic psychology as science: a reply. Psychological Review, 53:
132-135,
Ali, S. & Kelly, M. (2012). Ethics and social research. In C. Searle (Ed.) Researching society and
culture 3rd edition. London: Sage.
Ashworth, P. (2008). Conceptual foundations of qualitative psychology. In J. A. Smith (Ed.).
Qualitative psychology: A practical guide to research methods. London. Sage
Publications.
Banister, P., Dunn, G., Burman, E., Daniels, J., Duckett, P., Goodley, D., Lawthom, R., Parker,
I., Runswick-Cole, K., Sixsmith, J., Smailes, S., Tindall, C. & Whelan, P. (2011).
Qualitative methods in psychology: A research guide. 2nd edition. Maidenhead: Open
University Press/ McGraw Hill.
Biggerstaff, D. L. & Thompson, A. R. (2008). Interpretative phenomenological analysis (IPA):
A qualitative methodology of choice in healthcare research. Qualitative Research in
Psychology, 5: 214 – 224.
Bloor, M. (1997). Techniques of Validation in Qualitative Research: a Critical Commentary.
In G. Miller & R. Dingwall (Eds.) Context and Method in Qualitative Research.
London: Sage Publications.
Bowling, A. (2009) Research methods in health: investigating health and health services (3rded.)
Maidenhead: Open University Press.
www.intechopen.com
Qualitative Research Methods in Psychology 201
www.intechopen.com
202 Psychology - Selected Papers
Denzin, N.K. & Lincoln, Y.S. (Eds.) (1994 / 2000).Handbook of qualitative research. London:
Sage Publications.
Denzin, N., K. & Lincoln, Y. S. (Eds.) (2008). The landscape of qualitative research. London: Sage
Dixon-Wood, M., & Fitzpatrick, R. (2001) Qualitative research in systematic reviews: has
established a place for itself. British Medical Journal, 323: 765- 766.
Duncan, B., Hart, G., Scoular, A. & Brigg, A. (2001). Qualitative analysis of psychosocial
impact of diagnosis of Chlamydia trachomatis: implications for screening. British
Medical Journal, 315: 1659 – 1663.
Elwyn, G. J. (1997). So many precious stories: a reflective narrative of patient based medicine
in general practice, Christmas, 1996. British Medical Journal, 315, 1659 - 1663.
Esin, C. (2011). Narrative analysis approaches. In N. Frost (Ed.). Qualitative research methods
in psychology: combining core approaches. Maidenhead: McGrawHill/Open University
Press.
Finlay, L. (2003). The reflexive journey; mapping multiple routes. In L. Finlay & B. Gough
(Eds.) Reflexivity: A Practical Guide for Researchers in Health and Social Sciences.
Oxford: Blackwell.
Fitzpatrick, R. (1997). The assessment of patient satisfaction. In C. Jenkinson (Ed.) Assessment
and evaluation of health and medical care Buckingham: Open University Press.
Flick, U. (2007). Managing quality in qualitative research. London: Sage
Flick, U. (1992). Triangulation revisited: strategy of or an alternative to validation of
qualitative data. Journal of Theory of Social Behavior, 22: 175 – 197.
French, D. P., Maissi, E. & Marteau, T. M. (2005). The purpose of attributing cause: beliefs
about the causes of myocardial infarction. Social Science and Medicine, 60: 1411 –
1421.
Frost, N. (2011). Qualitative research methods in psychology: combining core approaches.
Maidenhead: McGrawHill/Open University Press.
Frost, N. (2009). ‘Do you know what I mean?’: the use of a pluralistic narrative analysis
approach in the interpretation of an interview. Qualitative Research, 9 (1): 9 – 29.
Gantley, M., Harding, G., Kumar, S., Tissier, J. (1999). An Introduction to Qualitative Methods
for Health Professionals.London: Royal College of General Practitioners.
Gergen, K. J. (2001) Social Construction in Context. London: Sage Publications.
Gergen, K. J. (1985).The social constructionist movement in modern psychology.American
Psychologist, 40: 266 – 275.
Gillham, B. (2000). The research interview. London: Continuum
Giorgi, A. (1995). Phenomenological Psychology.In J. A. Smith, R. Harré & K. Van
Langenhove (Eds.) Rethinking Psychology. London: Sage Publications
Glaser, B. G. & Strauss, A. L. (1967).The discovery of grounded theory. Chicago: Aldine.
Goodley, D. (2011). Narrative inquiry. In P. Banister, G. Dunn, E. Burman, et al.(Eds.)
Qualitative methods in psychology: A research guide. 2nd edition. Maidenhead: Open
University Press/ McGraw Hill.
Greenhalgh, T. & Hurwitz, B. (Eds.) (1998).Narrative Based Medicine: Dialogue and discourse in
clinical practice. London: BMJ Books.
Greig, A., Taylor, J., & MacKay, T. (2008). Doing research with children.2nd edition. London:
Sage.
Harré, H. (1995a). Discursive psychology. In J. A. Smith, R. Harré, & L. Van Langenhove,
(Eds.) Rethinking psychology. London: Sage.
www.intechopen.com
Qualitative Research Methods in Psychology 203
Hart, E., & Bond, M. (1999). Action research for health and social care. Buckingham: Open
University Press.
Hayes, N. (Ed.) (1998). Doing qualitative analysis in psychology.Hove: Psychology Press.
Helson, H. (1933). The fundamental propositions of Gestalt psychology.Psychological Review,
40 (1): 13 -32.
Henwood, K.L. (1996). Qualitative inquiry: perspectives, methods and psychology. In J
.T.E.Richardson (Ed.) Handbook of Qualitative Research Methods for Psychology and the
Social Sciences. Leicester: The British Psychological Society.
Henwood, K. (2004). Reinventing validity: reflections on principles and practices from
beyond the quality-quantity divide. In Z. Todd, B. Nerlich, S. McKeown, D. D.
Clarke. (Eds.) Mixing methods in psychology: the integration of qualitative and
quantitative methods in theory and practice.Hove: Psychology Press.
Henwood, K. & Pidgeon, N. (1992) Qualitative research and psychological theorising.British
Journal of Psychology, 83: 97 - 111.
Holt, A. (2011). Discourse analysis approaches. In N. Frost (Ed.) (2011). Qualitative research
methods in psychology: combining core approaches. Maidenhead: McGrawHill/Open
University Press.
Huberman, A.M. & Miles, M.B. (1998).Data Management and Analysis Methods.In
N.K.Denzin & Y.S. Lincoln.Collecting and Interpreting Qualitative Materials:Handbook
of qualitative research.Volume 3. London: Sage Publications.
King, N., &Horrocks, C. (2010).Interviews in qualitative research. London: Sage.
Kok, G., Schaalma, H., Ruiter, R. A. C., Van Empelen, R. (2004). Intervention mapping: a
protocol for applying health psychology theory to prevention programmes. Journal
of Health Psychology, 9 (1): 85 – 98.
Langdridge, D. (2007). Phenomenological psychology: Theory, research and method. Harlow:
Pearson Prentice Hall.
Lamiell, J. T. (1995). Rethinking the role of quantitative methods in psychology. In Smith,J.
A., Harré, R. & Van Langenhove, L. (Eds.) (1995b). Rethinking methods in psychology.
London: Sage
Lawthom, R. & Tindall, C. (2011). Phenomenology. In P. Banister, G. Dunn, E. Burman, et al.
(Eds.) Qualitative methods in psychology: A research guide. 2nd edition. Maidenhead:
Open University Press/ McGraw Hill.
May, T. (Ed.) Social research: issues, methods and process. 4th ed. Maidenhead: McGrawHill/
Open University Press.
May, T., & Perry, B. (2011).Case study research. In T. May (Ed.) Social research: issues, methods
and process. 4th ed. Maidenhead: McGrawHill/ Open University Press.
Mays, N., & Pope, C. (2000).Assessing quality in qualitative research.British Medical
Journal,320 : 50 – 52.
Mays, N., & Pope, C. (2000). Qualitative research: rigour and qualitative research. British
Medical Journal, 320: 50 - 52.
Mitchell, C. (2011). Doing visual research. London: Sage.
Morse, J.M. (Ed.) (1994) Critical Issues in Qualitative Research Methods. Thousand Oaks: Sage
Publications.
Murphy, E. & Dingwall, R. (1998).Qualitative methods in health services research. In N.
Black, J. Brazier, R. Fitzpatrick, B. Reeves (Eds.). Health Services Research Methods: A
guide to best practice. London: BMJ Books.
www.intechopen.com
204 Psychology - Selected Papers
www.intechopen.com
Qualitative Research Methods in Psychology 205
Rapport, F., Wainwright, P., Elwyn, G. (2005). “Of the edgelands”: broadening the score of
qualitative methodology. Medical Humanities, 31: 37 -42.
Reissman, C. K. (2008). Narrative methods for the human sciences. Thousand Oaks: Sage.
Richardson, J. T. E. (Ed.). (1996). Handbook of Qualitative Research Methods for Psychology and
the Social Sciences. Leicester: B.P.S. Books
Ricoeur, P. (1981 / 1995). Hermeneutics and the Human Sciences.Trans. & Ed. J. B. Thompson.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Roberts, B. (2002). Biographical Research. Buckingham: Open University Press.
Robson, C. (2011). Real world research: A resource for social-scientists and practitioner- researchers.
3rd edition. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.
Shinebourne, P. (2011). Interpretative phenomenological analysis. In N. Frost (Ed.).
Qualitative research methods in psychology: combining core approaches. Maidenhead:
McGrawHill/Open University Press.
Silverman, D. (1993). Theory and Method in Qualitative Research. Ch.1, in Interpreting
Qualitative Data: Methods for Analysing Talk, Text and Interaction. London: Sage
Publications.
Sixsmith, J., & Daniels, J. (2011). Action Research. In P. Banister, G. Dunn, E. Burman, et
al.(Eds.) Qualitative methods in psychology: A research guide. 2nd edition. Maidenhead:
Open University Press/ McGraw Hill.
Smith, J. A. (1994). Reconstructing selves: a case study of changing identities during the
transition to motherhood.Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 10: 225 – 243.
Smith, J. A. (1998). Developing theory from case studies: self-reconstruction and the
transition to motherhood.In N. Hayes (Ed.).Doing qualitative analysis in
psychology.Hove: Psychology Press.
Smith, J. A. (1999). Towards a relational self: social engagement during pregnancy and
psychological preparation for motherhood. British Journal of Social Psychology,38:
409 – 426.
Smith, J. A. (2007). Hermeneutics, human sciences and health: linking theory and practice.
International Journal of Qualitative Studies on health and Well-being.2: 3 – 11.
Smith, J. A. (Ed.) (2008). Qualitative psychology: a practical guide to research methods. London:
Sage.
Smith, J. A. (2011). Evaluating the contribution of interpretative phenomenological analysis.
Health Psychology Review, 5 (1)): 9 – 27.
Smith, J. A. & Osborn, M. (2008).Interpretative Phenomenological Analysis. In J. A. Smith
(Ed.), Qualitative Psychology: A practical guide to research methods (2nded.) (pp. 53 –
80). London: Sage.
Smith, J. A., Flowers, P., & Larkin, M. (2009).Interpretative phenomenological analysis: Theory,
method and research. London: Sage.
Smith,J. A., Harré, R. & Van Langenhove, L. (Eds.) (1995a). Rethinking psychology. London:
Sage.
Smith,J. A., Harré, R. & Van Langenhove, L. (Eds.) (1995b). Rethinking methods in psychology.
London: Sage
Smith, J. A., Jarman, M., Osborn, M. (1999).Doing Interpretative Phenomenological Analysis.
In M. Murray & K. Chamberlain (Eds.), Qualitative Health Psychology: Theories and
Methods (pp. 218 – 240). London: Sage.
Smith, M.J. (1998). Social Science in question. London: Sage Publications
www.intechopen.com
206 Psychology - Selected Papers
www.intechopen.com
Psychology - Selected Papers
Edited by Dr. Gina Rossi
ISBN 978-953-51-0587-9
Hard cover, 330 pages
Publisher InTech
Published online 02, May, 2012
Published in print edition May, 2012
This book represents a selection of chapters that address several topics from the broad domains of
psychology: alcoholism, clinical interventions, treatment of depression, personality psychology, qualitative
research methods in psychology, and social psychology. As such we have interesting blend of studies from
experts from a diverse array of psychology fields. The selected chapters will take the reader on an exciting
journey in the domains of psychology. We are sure the content will appeal to a great audience.
How to reference
In order to correctly reference this scholarly work, feel free to copy and paste the following:
Deborah Biggerstaff (2012). Qualitative Research Methods in Psychology, Psychology - Selected Papers, Dr.
Gina Rossi (Ed.), ISBN: 978-953-51-0587-9, InTech, Available from:
http://www.intechopen.com/books/psychology-selected-papers/qualitative-research-methods-in-psychology