Fundamentals of Canine Ethology Telecourse Lecture Notes
Fundamentals of Canine Ethology Telecourse Lecture Notes
Fundamentals of Canine Ethology Telecourse Lecture Notes
LECTURE NOTES
January 2004
www.AnimalBehaviorAssociates.com
[email protected]
303-932-9095 phone
303-932-2298 fax
Suzanne Hetts, Ph.D., CAAB, CPDT
Dan Estep, Ph.D., CAAB
Copyright ABA, Inc., 2003. All Rights Reserved. May not be reproduced,
excerpted from, distributed or sold without express written permission from the
authors.
LECTURE NOTES FOR FUNDAMENTALS OF CANINE ETHOLOGY
TELECOURSE
January 2004
www.AnimalBehaviorAssociates.com
[email protected]
303-932-9095 phone
303-932-2298 fax
Suzanne Hetts, Ph.D., CAAB, CPDT
Dan Estep, Ph.D., CAAB
Week 1
Week 2
Social Dominance
Definitions
Dominance hierarchies
Why is it important
How it is used and misused in dog behavior
Dealing with dominant dogs
Social Conflicts
Agonistic behavior
Threats
Submission
Avoidance
Aggression
Week 4
WEEK I
INTRODUCTION TO COURSE
Animal behavior is more than just common sense. Scientific studies have shown
us that some things we think are true about dogs aren’t. Consider this statement: “Dogs
know they have done wrong because they look guilty when you confront them with their
bad behavior. If you take a dog over to something he has chewed up and punish him, he
will learn not to do it again.” Is this true?
This course is going to introduce you to ethological concepts and principles that
will help you understand canine behavior from a more scientific, objective perspective.
You must be able to do this is you are going to develop effective training protocols, and
know what to do when you run into problems. Misinterpreting the “why” of behavior can
cause you to choose the wrong action to take to – whether you want to elicit, suppress, or
change behavior.
DOMESTICATION
GLOSSARY
Anthropomorphism – People attributing human characteristics to animals. This is a
very normal, human thing to do, but it can lead to false conclusions about why dogs do
what they do and even to abuse of the animal. There is no evidence that dogs experience
guilt, spite, revenge or shame. Attributing these reactions to dogs as motivations for their
behavior will lead to inappropriate and/or ineffective attempts to change the behavior.
Artificial selection – the process by which people choose different traits of animals they
perceive as desirable and purposely breed animals for particular phenotypes.
Commensalism – a relationship (symbiosis – living together) between organisms of
different species (can be either animals or plants) in which one benefits from the
association, but the other is neither harmed nor derives any benefit from the relationship.
Some authors have described the dog-human relationship as commensal; others have
described it as mutualistic (both benefit), while others (Budinsky) have described it as
parasitic (the dog benefits, but people are harmed).
Copyright ABA, Inc., 2003. All Rights Reserved. May not be reproduced,
excerpted from, distributed or sold without express written permission from the
authors.
Conspecifics – members of the same species.
Domesticated - An animal that’s breeding and care has been under the control of humans
for several generations and that now is physically and behaviorally different from wild
animals. Dogs, cats and horses are examples.
Feral - A domesticated animal that is in the process of returning to a wild existence. It is
no longer under human care and control, although its behavior may be influenced by
humans. It may also be genetically different from its domestic counterpart and its wild
ancestors.
Flight distance – the distance between an animal and its predator (or other threat) that
will cause it to flee. The distance that a wolf might allow a human to approach before
fleeing.
Natural selection – the process by which certain individuals in a population leave more
offspring (and thus more copies of their genes) because their genotype conveys a survival
advantage. Said another way, naturally occurring factors favor certain genotypes over
others. (Note that artificial selection is usually based on phenotype, while natural
selection is based on genotype).
Neoteny – the persistence of infantile or juvenile characteristics into adulthood.
Neotenic traits can be both physical and behavioral. Playful behavior is a neotenic trait.
Large eyes in relation to the size of the face, and short, rounded noses or muzzles are
examples of neotenic physical traits.
Niche – the place an animal occupies in ecological space; the living space it occupies.
This may be defined not only by physical features such as climate and geography, but
also by its biological inter-relationships. An animal’s behavior often has a strong
influence over which niches it can inhabit.
Socialized – there are many definitions of this term, both precise and very imprecise. We
will define it as a dog that has acquired normal social capabilities with both conspecifics
and people.
Tamed - An animal that will tolerate physical contact with humans. The flight distance
regarding humans is zero.
Wild animal - An animal whose breeding and care are not under the control of people.
They may live in close association with people, like house mice, or they may live apart
from humans like porcupines.
Zoomorphism – Animals attributing animal characteristics to people. This means that
dogs will tend to interpret human behavior as if humans were dogs. A dog may not
interpret a person staring at it as friendly but as threatening.
OVERVIEW
Animals such as dogs, cats, chickens, horses, hamsters and Siamese fighting fish
are domesticated. Domesticated animals may not be tame and vice-versa. For example,
dogs, cats and horses that are not exposed to people early in their lives will not be tame
even though they are domesticated. Some wild animals, such as birds in the Galapagos
Islands that have never seen people, are not fearful of people and are tame.
Domesticated animals can revert to a wild existence, such as the case of feral cats,
dogs, pigs and horses. These animals will not be tame unless they get early exposure to
people and have good experiences with them. The behavioral and physical traits, such as
Copyright ABA, Inc., 2003. All Rights Reserved. May not be reproduced,
excerpted from, distributed or sold without express written permission from the
authors.
coat color, of feral animals often revert to those of wild animals in a very few
generations.
Most experts now agree that the wild ancestors of dogs were wolves (Canis
lupus). It is possible that some populations of dogs have been crossed with other wild
canids such as jackals. Dogs have been domesticated for at least 14,000 years and are
thought to be our oldest domesticated animal.
Traditional:
As presented by Clutton-Brock, 1995; 1999:
Domestication was an active process initiated by humans
Made possible because hunting ranges of wolves and early people overlapped
Reason for domestication – mutual benefit – cooperative hunting
Sequence of events –
o live pup may have been rescued, and if not eaten, would be carried around,
remain with the family group and become tamed
o the pups that matured with “normal” or typical characteristics, would have
been driven out of camp
o the more submissive ones might have stayed and bred with other ‘tame’
wolves hanging out around the settlement, or humans purposely bred the
‘tame’ animals
o as this small group of “tamed wolves” began to breed with each other –
they began to be genetically different from wild population
Alternative Theory
as presented by Coppinger, 2001
People create a new niche, the village
Some wolves invade the new niche and discover a new food source
those wolves that can use the new niche are genetically predisposed to show less
‘flight distance’ than those who can’t – e.g. they would have had to be more tolerant
of the presence of humans.
Copyright ABA, Inc., 2003. All Rights Reserved. May not be reproduced,
excerpted from, distributed or sold without express written permission from the
authors.
Wolves naturally shy of people – alert ones wouldn’t have been able to exploit the
dump because would have been constantly alerting, fleeing, spending too much
energy. Probably not coming back.
Exploiting dump doesn’t require a pack – individuals could do. These dump dogs
were semi-solitary animals without a complex pack structure which calls into
question the applicability of the whole “dominance, pack theory” to dogs (another
reason why taking a second look at domestication is so important and relevant).
Ability to eat in proximity of people, shorter flight distance – trait that was exploited
by natural selection.
Hypothesis of self-domestication of the dogs rests on variations in flight distance the
tamer wolves gain selective advantage in the new niche over the less tame ones. – e.g.
more food, better survival of offspring.
DISCUSSION QUESTIONS:
1. Think of reasons why the dog/human relationship might be described as mutualistic,
commensal, or parasitic.
2. If early domesticated dogs had much less of a group structure than wolves, what
implications does this have for the popular “pack theory” and the importance of and
procedures used to be dominant over your dog that have been ubiquitous in the dog
training literature?
Copyright ABA, Inc., 2003. All Rights Reserved. May not be reproduced,
excerpted from, distributed or sold without express written permission from the
authors.
DOG DEVELOPMENT, EARLY EXPERIENCES AND BEHAVIOR
GLOSSARY
Heterospecifics – members of different species
Neonatal – the period immediately after birth
Sensitive period - the time when a particular environmental influence can more strongly
influence the organism than at other times.
Site attachments – probably represents the same process as socialization, except applied
to locations or the physical environment. However, the process has not been studied
separately from socialization.
Socialization - the process by which animals learn their species identity and develop
primary attachments to conspecifics and other species.
Two factors act together to determine the growth and behavior of animals - their
genetic complement and the environments in which they are raised and live. Here we
will focus on how environmental influences early in life can affect behavior.
The behavior of dogs changes rapidly and dramatically during the first weeks of
life. An outline of these changes can be seen in the accompanying figure. The early
development of puppies has been divided into four periods – the neonatal from birth to
about 2 ½ weeks, the transitional from 2 ½ to 3 ½ weeks, the socialization from 3 to 12
weeks and the juvenile from 12 weeks to sexual maturity which can be 9 to 18 months of
age.
While learning can occur in puppies younger than three weeks, incompletely
developed sensory and motor systems greatly reduce what can be learned. Learning
becomes easier during the socialization period and into the juvenile period. Puppies as
young as 8 weeks can be taught to sit and come, but short attention spans make complex
learning more difficult, so owners and trainers must be more patient and persistent in
teaching younger animals.
Early experiences are very important for the development of normal social
behavior in animals. There is a sensitive period early in the life of animals during which
they learn their species identity and learn which animals they should affiliate with later in
life. A sensitive period is a time when a particular environmental influence can more
strongly influence the organism than at other times.
Socialization is the process by which animals learn their species identity and
whom they should be friendly with and who should be feared and avoided. The sensitive
period for socialization in dogs is from 3 -12 weeks of age, that is, during the
socialization period of growth.
The sensitive period for socialization in cats is earlier and shorter, occurring from
2 -7 weeks of age. Animals exposed in a friendly, non-threatening way to people, other
dogs, cats or other animals will be more friendly with them as adults and less likely to
treat them as enemies or prey.
The sensitive period for socialization starts when the puppy’s eyes and ears are
open and functional. It ends with the development of fearfulness of new animals, people,
places and things.
Copyright ABA, Inc., 2003. All Rights Reserved. May not be reproduced,
excerpted from, distributed or sold without express written permission from the
authors.
To be precise, there are two fear periods in puppies, one that starts at about 8
weeks of age and another that starts at about 12 weeks of age. These tend to keep the
puppy from developing new, positive relationships with others.
This socialization can be done later in the animal’s life but usually requires much
more time and effort to accomplish. Socialization does not require reinforcement or
rewards to occur, but high arousal in the animal may facilitate it.
During the transition and sensitive periods puppies practice social communication
with their littermates and mom and learn social limits to their behavior. Puppies isolated
or restricted from this social contact often develop severe social problems – what has
been called “kennel dog syndrome”.
There may be a sensitive period for exposure to different situations, places and
things that is at about the same time as the sensitive period for socialization. This early
acclimatization of animals to new things is sometimes referred to as socialization as well,
although to be precise, it has nothing to do with social relationships.
Socialization and acclimatization experiences of puppies must be pleasant and
non-threatening from the puppy’s point of view. Forcing a fearful dog to experience new
people, animals, places or things can be worse than no experiences at all and can create
long-lasting fears that are difficult to change.
What should breeders do to help create normal, well socialized dogs?
What should owners, trainers and veterinarians do to help prevent problems with
these dogs?
Puppy tests have been used to predict the adult behavior of dogs. Dominance
aggression can purportedly be predicted from puppy tests. Two different studies have
shown however that puppy tests are not predictive of adult behavior.
Observations of puppies in their litters show that they “try on” different roles, and
that dominance status is not stable and predictive of later relationships.
Is there a “best” way to pick a puppy? What should prospective owners look for
in a puppy or what should they avoid?
THOUGHT QUESTIONS:
1) If you want a dog to be good with cats, at what age would it be best to expose the dog
to cats?
2) If you have a dog that is afraid of people, can you get her to be better with people by
forcing her to be around them? Why or why not?
3) Given what is known about sensitive periods for socialization in puppies and the time
of onset of fear periods, when is the best time to take a puppy home? When is the worst
time?
4) If puppy tests are not predictive of adult behavior, is there any value to using them? If
so how might they be used?
Copyright ABA, Inc., 2003. All Rights Reserved. May not be reproduced,
excerpted from, distributed or sold without express written permission from the
authors.
Created by D.Q. Estep
Reproduced from Estep, D.Q., 1996. The ontogeny of behavior. pp. 19 – 31 in Readings
in Companion Animal Behavior. V.L. Voith and P.L. Borchelt, Eds., Trenton NJ:
Veterinary Learning Systems.
Copyright ABA, Inc., 2003. All Rights Reserved. May not be reproduced,
excerpted from, distributed or sold without express written permission from the
authors.
LECTURE NOTES FOR FUNDAMENTS OF CANINE ETHOLOGY
TELECOURSE
January 2004
www.AnimalBehaviorAssociates.com
[email protected]
303-932-9095 phone
303-932-2298 fax
Suzanne Hetts, Ph.D., CAAB, CPDT
Dan Estep, Ph.D., CAAB
WEEK II
GENETICS AND BEHAVIOR
INTRODUCTION
Some of the factors that can influence a dog’s behavior are: 1) the genetic
constitution of the animal, 2) the early socialization of the animal, 3) the later learned
experiences of the animal, 4) the age of the animal, 5) sex and reproductive condition
of the animal, and 6) the medical condition of the animal. These factors are not
operating independently of another, but instead co-act to influence behavior. Experiences
and early socialization for example may affect different effects on different genotypes.
The first two factors will be dealt with in this course, while learning will be
covered in the course on canine learning. We will not cover age, sex and medical
conditions here, but realize that sick animals may behave differently than healthy
animals, old dogs behave differently than puppies and neutered males behave differently
from intact males.
Genes don’t directly cause behavior. Genes make proteins and enzymes and
through a complex process involving many genes and environmental influences, behavior
results. Neither genetic factors nor environmental ones are more or less important for
development. Nature (genes) co-acts with nurture (the environment).
Genes influence all behavior, and research has shown them to influence
behavioral patterns such as aggression, fear, friendliness and learning. It is not correct to
speak of some behaviors (such as aggression) as being genetic or inherited and others not.
It is also not appropriate to talk about a behavior being a certain proportion genetic (such
as fear is 60% genetic or inherited).
GLOSSARY
Allele – different versions of a gene, which are often (but not always) in a dominant or
recessive form.
Dominant gene – a gene that requires only one copy of the allele in order to be expressed
(e.g. a trait ‘N’ will be expressed whether the genotype is NN or Nn).
Copyright ABA, Inc., 2003. All Rights Reserved. May not be reproduced,
excerpted from, distributed or sold without express written permission from the
authors.
Environment - all the parts of the animate and inanimate world that can influence
animals, such as things they eat, temperature, humidity, and the different experiences
they have including learned experiences.
Gene – the minimum unit of replication in genetic transmission; the basic unit of
heredity. Genes are contained on chromosomes. Most traits are affected by multiple
genes, and each gene usually affects multiple traits.
Genetics – the study of heredity
Genotype - the genetic complement of an animal
Phenotype – the observable characteristics of an animal. Is the result of the interaction
between the dog’s genotype and his environment. Dogs with the same genotype can have
a different phenotype (look and act differently), while dogs with similar phenotypes can
have different genotypes.
Recessive gene – a gene that requires two copies of the same allele (homozygous) in
order to be expressed (e.g. for a trait ‘n’ to be expressed, nn is required; the trait ‘n’ won’t
be expressed if the genotype is Nn)
Copyright ABA, Inc., 2003. All Rights Reserved. May not be reproduced,
excerpted from, distributed or sold without express written permission from the
authors.
What are the right questions?
What do genes do?
they code for enzymes
they make proteins
some enzymes turn other enzymes on and off.
How do genes affect behavior?
PKU example
Go to --- http://www.abc.net.au/catalyst/stories/s953902.htm
DISCUSSION QUESTIONS:
1. What should a breeder do if she discovers a puppy from one of her litters has
developed an aggression problem? Should she continue breeding this particular sire and
dam?
2. What influence does the information presented have on your interpretation of such
statements as “Pit bulls are inherently dangerous”, or “Golden retrievers make great
family pets”?
Copyright ABA, Inc., 2003. All Rights Reserved. May not be reproduced,
excerpted from, distributed or sold without express written permission from the
authors.
MOTIVATION IN ANIMALS
GLOSSARY
Copyright ABA, Inc., 2003. All Rights Reserved. May not be reproduced,
excerpted from, distributed or sold without express written permission from the
authors.
Innate behaviors were once thought to be inherited, stereotyped in form,
common to all members of the species and unaffected by the environment (unlearned).
Research showed that these things are not always related. Behaviors common to a
species may not be stereotyped. Almost all dogs bark, but when examined closely the
form is not stereotyped. Some stereotyped behaviors can be changed through learning.
For example, the sequence of acts in the mating of male dogs is fairly stereotyped but can
be modified by experiences. As a result, the term innate behavior is no longer used in
animal behavior but has been replaced with Fixed Action Pattern. This refers to a
relatively invariant behavior but implies nothing about its development or the role of
learning in its expression.
If it is not a good idea to use terms like instinct, drive or innate, then how do we
talk about motivation and temperament traits in dogs? Motives can be used to describe
behavior if it is recognized that they are only crude descriptions of behavior and not
causes (eg. “that dog looks really hungry today” or “ that dog is really motivated to fetch
balls”). To really explain the motivations for a dog’s interest in fetching balls or
threatening visitors at the door, we need to understand both the physiological changes and
environmental changes that produce it. Things like neurochemistry, early experiences,
more recent learning and so on. Our understanding of these causes is very poor for most
behaviors.
There have been some studies of temperament in dogs but temperament traits only
accurately describe the specific group of dogs studied in the place where they were
studied. Studies of temperament of Labrador Retrievers being trained as assistance dogs
will probably not apply to all dogs. For the present, we are probably better off talking
about specific behaviors in specific dogs and trying to understand them rather than
talking about temperament traits as explanations. So we may want to ask “Rover likes to
chase balls, can I train him to compete in Fly Ball?” or “Tucker threatens the mailperson
when she comes to the door, can I get Tucker to not threaten her?”
THOUGHT QUESTIONS
1) Can you think of two other fixed action patterns in dogs? Does labeling these
behaviors as fixed action patterns tell you anything about how they develop or the role of
learning in their expression?
2) Can you think of two other drives that people have talked about in dogs? Are they
used to talk about short term changes in behavior or temperament traits? Do these drives
really explain the behavior?
3) Can you think ways to train Rover to be good at Fly Ball? How could you change
Tucker’s threatening behavior? Try to think of physiological as well as environmental
means to do this.
Copyright ABA, Inc., 2003. All Rights Reserved. May not be reproduced,
excerpted from, distributed or sold without express written permission from the
authors.
LECTURE NOTES FOR FUNDAMENTS OF CANINE ETHOLOGY
TELECOURSE
January 2004
www.AnimalBehaviorAssociates.com
[email protected]
303-932-9095 phone
303-932-2298 fax
Suzanne Hetts, Ph.D., CAAB, CPDT
Dan Estep, Ph.D., CAAB
WEEK III
GLOSSARY
Aggression - behaviors intended to do harm. Examples - dog bites, cat scratches, and
horse kicks.
Agonistic Behavior - A group of related behaviors displayed in social conflict contexts.
Includes aggression, threats, appeasement and avoidance behaviors.
Appeasement or submission - behaviors that turn off threats or aggression. Example -
dog rolled over on the back exposing the belly.
Avoidance behavior – behavior by which an animal ends a conflict by moving away
from the other. Example - a dog running away from another dog that is growling at him.
Social Dominance - A pattern of social relationships within a group that allows resources
to be divided among the members, and allows prediction of the outcome of competitive
interactions, and what type of social signals individuals are likely to give and receive.
Territory - A space actively defended by an animal. Defense may include marking the
area with scents (odors) or visual markers, or with threats and attacks. The area can be
very large (a whole neighborhood) or small (a crate that the animal sleeps in). Example -
A dog may growl and bark and lunge at the mail delivery person when she comes near
the house.
Threats - behaviors that warn others of impending attacks. Example - dogs growling,
cats hissing, horses laying their ears back.
INTRODUCTION
The term “dominance” or “dominant dog” or “he’s an alpha dog” are comments that we
hear frequently when pet owners or others are describing their dogs or the reasons for
behavior problems. The concept of “dominance” is very ingrained in the way we think
about our relationship with our dogs, but what does it really mean, and does it do a good
job of explaining dog behavior and helping us have better relationships with our dogs?
Has “dominance” been confused with control?
Copyright ABA, Inc., 2003. All Rights Reserved. May not be reproduced,
excerpted from, distributed or sold without express written permission from the
authors.
Where Does The Concept of Social Dominance Come From?
Dominance in its basic sense is related to outcomes of direct, competitive interactions and
the giving and receiving of social signals between individuals. The basic concept of a
“dominance relationship” merely allows for predicting the directionality of agonistic
behaviors. Dominant individuals are more likely to be the recipients of submissive
behaviors or avoidance and more likely to be the initiators of threats and aggression.
Dominance relationships can be useful to a group because it helps to sort out who gets
access to what and when. Dominant individuals usually have priority access to certain
resources. However, dominance relationships are neither permanent nor absolute.
The term “dominant dog” has been used more often than not to describe a personality
trait rather than to describe a role relationship. It is often erroneous to invoke the term
“dominant dog” to describe an unchangeable personality trait. While there are some dogs
who tend to assume a dominant role in most or all of their relationships, it’s more likely
that what role a dog assumes will change based on the dog’s social partner and the
context.
Copyright ABA, Inc., 2003. All Rights Reserved. May not be reproduced,
excerpted from, distributed or sold without express written permission from the
authors.
What do dogs really learn from the following interactions?
Copyright ABA, Inc., 2003. All Rights Reserved. May not be reproduced,
excerpted from, distributed or sold without express written permission from the
authors.
How Has Dominance Aggression Been Described?
It has been used to describe threats and aggression that occur when the dog is in direct
competition with the owner over a resource (the same space on the couch for example),
or when the owner directs socially dominant (from the dog’s perspective) signals to the
dog. The dog threatens or aggresses when he is, or the owner:
Copyright ABA, Inc., 2003. All Rights Reserved. May not be reproduced,
excerpted from, distributed or sold without express written permission from the
authors.
TYPES OF AGGRESSION MOST COMMONLY SEEN IN DOGS
1. Medically caused - Due to disease like rabies, brain tumors or epilepsy, or due to an
injury.
2. Pain-elicited - Aggression in response to a painful stimulus such as hitting a dog with
a rolled-up newspaper.
3. Fear-elicited - Caused by a fear-provoking stimulus. A dog frequently hurt by a
young child may begin to growl and bite out of fear when the child approaches her.
4. Maternal - Aggression mothers show in defense of their young. A mare may attack a
person who gets too close to her newborn foal.
5. Territorial / Protective - Aggression shown in defense of a territory, or people or
other animals the protective animal is attached to. A cat may attack other cats coming
too close to its home. A dog may attack a jogger that comes too close to his owner when
on a walk.
6. Possessive - Shown in defense of food or other prized objects such as toys, treats or
stolen objects. A dog may bite at people who walk by his feeding dish while he eats.
7. Dominance - Shown by an animal defending her social position or challenging the
social position of another. A dog may bite another to be the first to get attention from a
person.
8. Predatory - Not really aggression because it’s behavior intended to do harm to
another but for purposes of feeding the attacking animal. Dogs and cats can prey on a
variety of other species. A group of owned dogs running at large attacks and kills several
sheep owned by a farmer.
9. Redirected - Aggression caused by one person or animal that is directed to another
when access to the first is blocked. Two dogs growling at another dog outside a fence
may turn and attack each other.
10. Mixed - Aggression due to activation of two or more of the above types at the same
time. A mix of territorial and fear-motivated aggression can frequently be seen in dogs
that attack unfamiliar persons that come to the home.
Copyright ABA, Inc., 2003. All Rights Reserved. May not be reproduced,
excerpted from, distributed or sold without express written permission from the
authors.
LECTURE NOTES FOR FUNDAMENTS OF CANINE ETHOLOGY
TELECOURSE
January 2004
www.AnimalBehaviorAssociates.com
[email protected]
303-932-9095 phone
303-932-2298 fax
Suzanne Hetts, Ph.D., CAAB, CPDT
Dan Estep, Ph.D., CAAB
WEEK IV
OTHER IMPORTANT ASPECTS OF CANINE SOCIAL BEHAVIOR
GLOSSARY
Communication - A process by which one animal sends information that changes the
behavior of another. There must be a sender, a receiver and a signal and a behavioral
change must be seen. Example - A dog (the sender) growls (the signal) at the pizza
delivery person (the receiver), who moves away from the dog (the change in behavior).
Personal Space - An area surrounding an animal that when invaded by another produces
threat, aggression or flight. Example - A dog walks towards another dog. When the first
dog gets within 3 feet of the second, the second gets up and moves away. All animals
and people have personal space, although this area varies between animals and in
different situations.
Flight Distance – The distance between two animals that elicits a flight response by one
of them. In the example above, 3 feet was the flight distance of the second dog.
Territory - A space actively defended by an animal. Defense may include marking the
area with scents (odors) or visual markers, or with threats and attacks. The area can be
very large (a whole neighborhood) or small (a crate that the animal sleeps in). Example -
A dog may growl and bark and lunge at the mail delivery person when she comes near
the house.
Social Facilitation - An increase in the likelihood or frequency of a behavior because of
the presence or activities of others. It is more common in highly social species like dogs,
horses and chimpanzees. Only a few behaviors can be socially facilitated in dogs such as
play, barking, feeding, fighting, aggression, or predation. Example - A dog that had
stopped eating and seemed full, begins to eat again when he sees another hungry dog
begin to eat.
Affiliation - Behavior that brings animals together or keeps them in close proximity to
each other. This is “friendly” behavior. Examples – The face-licking greeting responses
of dogs, cats rubbing their heads against others, mutual grooming in horses, play, sharing
of food, maternal care.
Play – Behaviors made up of elements of other behaviors but out of context, incomplete,
repetitive, exaggerated and having no apparent goal.
Greeting Displays – Specific behaviors that are used when animals come together after
being apart for some period of time. They are usually affiliative or submissive behaviors.
Copyright ABA, Inc., 2003. All Rights Reserved. May not be reproduced,
excerpted from, distributed or sold without express written permission from the
authors.
Not all social behavior in dogs is concerned with dominance and aggression. In
fact most of the time when dogs are being social, the behavior is friendly or cooperative.
To understand what bonds dogs to other dogs or dogs to people, you need to know about
these other concepts as well.
Communication has been defined in many different ways, but the definition
above allows us to be clear when communication is occurring because we see a change in
the receiver’s behavior. The change need not be immediate, it can take several minutes
or hours to occur. People intend to communicate with their dogs by lecturing them or
yelling at them, but unless a change is seen in the dog’s behavior you can’t conclude
communication has occurred.
When different species try to communicate, like people and dogs,
miscommunication can occur. The message that the sender intended may not be what
the receiver perceives. Example – A person bends over and pets a dog on the head,
intending to be friendly. The dog perceives the bending over and petting as a threat
(because looming over another is what dogs do when threatening) and bites the person.
Because the communication systems of dogs and people aren’t the same,
miscommunication happens quite frequently. The more people know about dog signals
and their meaning (that is, learn to read and speak dog) the less likely the
miscommunication will be.
Personal space and flight distance are related concepts. There is no set size of
the personal space of dogs. It will depend upon the past experiences of the dog, how he
was socialized, and the relationship he has with the other individual. It might be 50 feet
for one dog with an unfamiliar person and 2 inches for the same dog with his owner. The
importance of this is that if you get too close to a dog, bad things might happen.
Territory is like personal space but is tied to a particular plot of ground. The
ancestors of dogs (wolves) are territorial, but most dogs are not territorially aggressive
(thank goodness!). Some dogs mark their territories with urine or even use urine to mark
sign posts away from their territories. Urine marking isn’t always related to territories or
aggression. Dogs that are territorially aggressive might be friendly with an unfamiliar
person away from the territory and threatening to the same person on the territory. Dogs
sometimes treat the family vehicles as their moving territories and become threatening
around them.
Dogs are highly social animals and are greatly influenced by the behavior of other
dogs, and sometimes, people. Social facilitation refers to the tendency for behaving
dogs to draw others into their activities. We’ve all seen dogs start barking at one end of a
street and soon all the dogs on the street are barking. Not all behaviors are socially
facilitated but some important ones like barking, eating and aggression can be. Be aware
that if you are near a group of dogs and one becomes threatening, the others may become
threatening as well. Also if you become threatening to another person in the presence of
your dog, or if your dog thinks you are threatening, your dog may become threatening
too.
Affiliative behaviors help to keep animals, or animals and people, close to one
another and to cement bonds between them. In dogs, laying close to another (sometimes
on top of them!) is a very friendly behavior as is leaning on another, putting a paw on
them and following them around. Submissive behaviors like face licking, submissive
Copyright ABA, Inc., 2003. All Rights Reserved. May not be reproduced,
excerpted from, distributed or sold without express written permission from the
authors.
grinning, or rolling over on the back are also friendly behaviors that are meant to keep
animals together. Other affiliative behaviors in dogs are social play, sexual and parental
behavior, care soliciting by young animals and sharing of food. Encouraging affiliative
behaviors among your dogs and doing some of these behaviors yourself my help to
strengthen your bonds.
Play behavior can be social, with another animal or person, or object play, where
the dog plays alone with a stick or ball, or can be locomotor play such as a dog racing
around the yard by herself. Dogs play far more than most other animals and do much
more as adults than other animals. Encouraging social play can strengthen bonds
between the players.
Greeting displays such as barking, jumping up, licking faces and showing other
submissive behaviors help animals to identify the intentions of the other and clearly give
signals of friendly intentions. It can be hard to control obnoxious greetings from dogs
such as jumping up and barking.
Some submissive behaviors and maybe other behaviors such as staying near
another, can be used to help patch up strained relationships after some conflict. It is sort
of a doggie way of saying “I’m sorry”. These behaviors are seen in other highly social
animals like chimpanzees and elephants and are known as reconciliation behaviors.
THOUGHT QUESTIONS
1) Can you think of two other common miscommunications between dogs and people?
2) Can you think of ways you can use social facilitation to encourage behaviors you
want? Give an example.
3) What can people do who want to strengthen the bonds between them and their dogs?
Copyright ABA, Inc., 2003. All Rights Reserved. May not be reproduced,
excerpted from, distributed or sold without express written permission from the
authors.
CANINE BODY POSTURES
Dogs reveal their motivations, emotions and intentions in their body language. Key
features to focus on include general body position, piloerection, and orientation of the
dog to you or others. Observe tail position, ear position, the showing of teeth, facial
muscle contraction around the mouth, opening of the eyes and dilation of pupils, and
orientation of the eyes. Listen for vocalizations. The different motivations, such as
offensiveness and defensiveness, can often be discriminated by differences in body
language.
No one feature or cue is a reliable indicator of the animal’s motivation or intentions.
Observe the whole complex of the dog’s features. Body postures and motivations are
dynamic and can change quickly. You should continuously monitor dogs you are
working with for sudden changes in behavior.
Offensively threatening dogs will usually show one or more of the following:
1. Standing up tall with a stiff body posture, oriented towards the subject of the threat.
2. Piloerection (erection of the hair) on the back.
3. Tail straight up in a vertical line, it may be wagging slowly.
4. Ears up and forward or pricked forward.
5. Direct eye contact or staring.
6. Teeth bared with vertical retraction of the lips.
7. Barking and/or growling.
Defensively threatening dogs will usually show a mixture of offensive and fearful
characteristics:
1. Crouched body posture.
2. Piloerection (“hackles up”) may occur.
3. May or may not be directly oriented towards the subject of the threat.
4. Tail usually down.
5. Ears may be pinned back.
6. Eyes not directly staring. May look away from subject of threat, or alternate between
staring and avoidance of contact.
7. Teeth bared in horizontal retraction of lips.
Copyright ABA, Inc., 2003. All Rights Reserved. May not be reproduced,
excerpted from, distributed or sold without express written permission from the
authors.
8. May be growling, barking or whining and whimpering.
Copyright ABA, Inc., 2003. All Rights Reserved. May not be reproduced,
excerpted from, distributed or sold without express written permission from the
authors.
LECTURE NOTES FOR FUNDAMENTS OF CANINE ETHOLOGY
TELECOURSE
January 2004
www.AnimalBehaviorAssociates.com
[email protected]
303-932-9095 phone
303-932-2298 fax
Suzanne Hetts, Ph.D., CAAB, CPDT
Dan Estep, Ph.D., CAAB
Domestication
Clutton-Brock, J. 1995. Origins of the dog: domestication and early history. In Serpell,
J.Ed. The Domestic Dog: Its Evolution, Behaviour and Interactions With People.
New York: Cambridge Univ. Press, pp.7-20.
Coppinger, R. & Schneider, R. 1995. Evolution of working dogs. In Serpell, J.Ed. The
Domestic Dog: Its Evolution, Behaviour and Interactions With People. New York:
Cambridge Univ. Press, pp.21-47.
Genetics of Behavior
Willis, M.B. 1995. Genetic aspects of dog behaviour with particular reference to
working ability. In Serpell, J.Ed. The Domestic Dog: Its Evolution, Behaviour and
Interactions With People. New York: Cambridge Univ. Press, pp.51-64.
Hart, B.L. 1995. Analysing breed and gender differences in behaviour. In Serpell, J.Ed.
The Domestic Dog: Its Evolution, Behaviour and Interactions With People. New
York: Cambridge Univ. Press, pp.65-78.
Copyright ABA, Inc., 2003. All Rights Reserved. May not be reproduced,
excerpted from, distributed or sold without express written permission from the
authors.
Early Experiences and Behavior
Serpell, J. & Jagoe, J.A. 1995. Early experience and the development of behaviour. In
Serpell, J.Ed. The Domestic Dog: Its Evolution, Behaviour and Interactions With
People. New York: Cambridge Univ. Press, pp.79-102.
McCune, S., McPherson, J.A. & Bradshaw, J.W.S. 1995. Avoiding problems: the
importance of socialization. In Robinson, I. Ed. The Waltham Book of Human-
Animal Interaction: Benefits and Responsibilities of Pet Ownership. New York:
Pergamon Press pp.71-86.
Hetts, S. Pet Behavior Protocols. What to Say, What to Do, When to Refer.
1999.Lakewood, CO: American Animal Hospital Association Press, pp. 50-51.
Bradshaw, J.W.S. & Nott, H.M.R. 1995. Social and communication behaviour of
companion dogs. In Serpell, J.Ed. The Domestic Dog: Its Evolution, Behaviour and
Interactions With People. New York: Cambridge Univ. Press, pp.115-130.
Hetts, S. Pet Behavior Protocols. What to Say, What to Do, When to Refer.
1999.Lakewood, CO: American Animal Hospital Association Press, pp. 53-55.
Copyright ABA, Inc., 2003. All Rights Reserved. May not be reproduced,
excerpted from, distributed or sold without express written permission from the
authors.