Memory and Its Types
Memory and Its Types
Memory and Its Types
simplypsychology.org/memory.html
“Memory is the means by which we draw on our past experiences in order to use this
information in the present’ (Sternberg, 1999).
Memory is essential to all our lives. Without a memory of the past, we cannot operate
in the present or think about the future. We would not be able to remember what we
did yesterday, what we have done today or what we plan to do tomorrow. Without
memory, we could not learn anything.
For psychologists the term memory covers three important aspects of information
processing:
1. Memory Encoding
When information comes into our memory system (from sensory input), it needs to
be changed into a form that the system can cope with, so that it can be stored. Think
of this as similar to changing your money into a different currency when you travel
from one country to another. For example, a word which is seen (in a book) may be
stored if it is changed (encoded) into a sound or a meaning (i.e. semantic processing).
There are three main ways in which information can be encoded (changed):
1. Visual (picture)
2. Acoustic (sound)
3. Semantic (meaning)
For example, how do you remember a telephone number you have looked up in the
phone book? If you can see it then you are using visual coding, but if you are
repeating it to yourself you are using acoustic coding (by sound).
Evidence suggests that this is the principle coding system in short-term memory (STM)
is acoustic coding. When a person is presented with a list of numbers and letters,
they will try to hold them in STM by rehearsing them (verbally). Rehearsal is a verbal
process regardless of whether the list of items is presented acoustically (someone
reads them out), or visually (on a sheet of paper).
1/3
The principle encoding system in long-term memory (LTM) appears to be semantic
coding (by meaning). However, information in LTM can also be coded both visually
and acoustically.
2. Memory Storage
This concerns the nature of memory stores, i.e., where the information is stored, how
long the memory lasts for (duration), how much can be stored at any time (capacity)
and what kind of information is held. The way we store information affects the way
we retrieve it. There has been a significant amount of research regarding the
differences between Short Term Memory (STM ) and Long Term Memory (LTM).
Most adults can store between 5 and 9 items in their short-term memory. Miller
(1956) put this idea forward and he called it the magic number 7. He though that
short-term memory capacity was 7 (plus or minus 2) items because it only had a
certain number of “slots” in which items could be stored.
However, Miller didn’t specify the amount of information that can be held in each
slot. Indeed, if we can “chunk” information together we can store a lot more
information in our short-term memory. In contrast, the capacity of LTM is thought to
be unlimited.
Information can only be stored for a brief duration in STM (0-30 seconds), but LTM
can last a lifetime.
3. Memory Retrieval
This refers to getting information out storage. If we can’t remember something, it
may be because we are unable to retrieve it. When we are asked to retrieve
something from memory, the differences between STM and LTM become very clear.
STM is stored and retrieved sequentially. For example, if a group of participants are
given a list of words to remember, and then asked to recall the fourth word on the
list, participants go through the list in the order they heard it in order to retrieve the
information.
LTM is stored and retrieved by association. This is why you can remember what you
went upstairs for if you go back to the room where you first thought about it.
Organizing information can help aid retrieval. You can organize information in
sequences (such as alphabetically, by size or by time). Imagine a patient being
discharged from hospital whose treatment involved taking various pills at various
times, changing their dressing and doing exercises. If the doctor gives these
instructions in the order which they must be carried out throughout the day (i.e., in
the sequence of time), this will help the patient remember them.
Psychologists use the term ecological validity to refer to the extent to which the
findings of research studies can be generalized to other settings. An experiment has
high ecological validity if its findings can be generalized, that is applied or extended, to
settings outside the laboratory.
Many experiments designed to investigate memory have been criticized for having
low ecological validity. First, the laboratory is an artificial situation. People are
removed from their normal social settings and asked to take part in a psychological
experiment. They are directed by an 'experimenter' and may be placed in the
company of complete strangers. For many people, this is a brand new experience, far
removed from their everyday lives. Will this setting affect their actions, will they
behave normally?
Often, the tasks participants are asked to perform can appear artificial and
meaningless. Few, if any, people would attempt to memorize and recall a list of
unconnected words in their daily lives. And it is not clear how tasks such as this relate
to the use of memory in everyday life. The artificiality of many experiments has led
some researchers to question whether their findings can be generalized to real life.
As a result, many memory experiments have been criticized for having low ecological
validity.
References
Matlin, M. W. (2005). Cognition. Crawfordsville: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our
capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63 (2): 81–97.
Sternberg, R. J. (1999). Cognitive psychology (2 nd ed.). Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace
College Publishers.
3/3