Neurología y Canto
Neurología y Canto
Neurología y Canto
Author(s): ISABELLE PERETZ, LISE GAGNON, SYLVIE HÉBERT and JOËL MACOIR
Source: Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, Vol. 21, No. 3 (Spring 2004), pp. 373-
390
Published by: University of California Press
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/10.1525/mp.2004.21.3.373 .
Accessed: 24/06/2015 23:51
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].
University of California Press is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Music
Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal.
http://www.jstor.org
ISABELLE PERETZ
University of Montreal & Institut Universitaire de Gériatrie de Montréal
LISE GAGNON
Sherbrooke Geriatric University Institute
S Y LV I E H É B E R T
University of Montreal & Institut Universitaire de Gériatrie de Montréal
JOËL MACOIR
Laval University and Laval University Geriatric Research Unit
Singing abilities are rarely examined despite the fact that their study repre-
sents one of the richest sources of information regarding how music is
processed in the brain. In particular, the analysis of singing performance in
brain-damaged patients provides key information regarding the autonomy
of music processing relative to language processing. Here, we review the
relevant literature, mostly on the perception and memory of text and tunes
in songs, and we illustrate how lyrics can be distinguished from melody in
singing, in the case of brain damage. We report a new case, G.D., who has
a severe speech disorder, marked by phonemic errors and stuttering, with-
out a concomitant musical production disorder. G.D. was found to pro-
duce as few intelligible words in speaking as in singing familiar songs. Sing-
ing “la, la, la” was intact and hence could not account for the speech deficit
observed in singing. The results indicate that verbal production, be it sung
or spoken, is mediated by the same (impaired) language output system and
that this speech route is distinct from the (spared) melodic route. In sum,
we provide here further evidence that the autonomy of music and language
processing extends to production tasks.
373
This stroke left him without speech and deaf to the spoken world. While
Shebalin could no longer communicate verbally, he continued to teach and
to compose until his death, 4 years later. Shebalin was particularly prolific
musically despite his vast left hemispheric lesion; he wrote 14 chorales, 2
sonatas, 2 quatuors, 11 songs, and 1 symphony. According to Shostakovitch,
one of his peers, Shebalin’s music was undistinguishable from what he had
composed before his illness (Luria, Tsvetkova, & Futer, 1965). Although
Shebalin’s case is spectacular, it is not exceptional, given that similar cases
have been reported in the literature (Assal, 1973; Basso & Capitani, 1985;
Signoret, van Eeckhout, Poncet, & Castaigne, 1987).
The fact that Shebalin was an outstanding musician does not preclude
generalization to the brain of ordinary people. Drastic cases of aphasia
without amusia have been observed in nonmusicians. The latter may lose
their ability to recognize spoken words and yet remain able to recognize
music (Godefroy et al., 1995; Laignel-Lavastine & Alajouanine, 1921;
Mendez, 2001). Similarly, brain-damaged patients who are afflicted with
verbal agnosia (or word deafness), and hence have lost their ability to rec-
ognize spoken words, can maintain normal abilities to recognize nonverbal
sounds, including music (Metz-Lutz & Dahl, 1984; Takahashi et al., 1992;
Yaqub, Gascon, Al-Nosha, & Whitaker, 1988). The existence of such cases
of selective sparing of musical abilities, in both musicians and nonmusicians,
suggests that the processing of music is not mediated by the same system as
the one involved in language.
It could still be argued that music processing is spared because it is
computationally less complex, or more primitive, than language. If this
were the case, then brain damage that is sufficiently severe to interfere with
musical abilities (the simple abilities) should also be detrimental to lan-
guage abilities (the complex abilities). This account predicts that one will
not find individuals in whom brain damage has impaired the ability to
process music while sparing the ability to recognize language. Such cases of
amusia without aphasia, however, do exist. For example, Isabelle R., whom
we have studied in some detail (Peretz, Belleville, & Fontaine, 1997), is an
ordinary woman, devoid of special talents, be it musical or linguistic. She
was a restaurant manager when, at the age of 28, she underwent successive
brain surgeries for the repair of ruptured aneurysms in the left and right
middle cerebral arteries. She survived, but with two vast brain lesions in-
vading the auditory cortex bilaterally and extending to the frontal areas on
the right side. In this context, it is surprising to note that Isabelle R. is fully
functional in language, memory, and intelligence. She even writes poems.
Her persisting and major problem concerns music. Isabelle R. can no longer
recognize the music that was familiar to her before her brain accident; she
cannot relearn the musical corpus because melodies no longer leave a trace
in her memory; finally, she can no longer carry a tune. Isabelle R. regularly
practiced these skills before her brain injury, although she never studied
music. This condition of “amusia without aphasia” has been known for
more than a century (Marin & Perry, 1999). However, solid evidence has
been gathered only recently (Ayotte, Peretz, Rousseau, Bard, & Bojanowski,
2000; Griffiths et al., 1997; Peretz et al., 1994; Piccirilli, Sciarma, & Luzzi,
2000; Steinke, Cuddy, & Jakobson, 2001; Wilson & Pressing, 1999).
The major conclusion to be drawn from these neurological cases of se-
lective aphasia and of amusia is that they point to the existence of at least
two distinct series of processing modules: one for music and one for speech.
Cases such as Shebalin show how the neural circuitries specialized for mu-
sic can be selectively spared. Conversely, cases like Isabelle R. demonstrate
that these music-specific circuitries can be selectively damaged. However,
domain-specificity may arise at different levels in the processing of either
language or music. There is no need for all processing components to be
specialized for their respective domain to account for cases of selective apha-
sia and amusia. Damage to only one or two pivotal processing components
that are specialized for language (or music) may result in a dysfunction of
the entire processing system. This is why it is essential to compare music
and language processing in identical task conditions in order to be able to
specify the processing component that is responsible for the observed dis-
sociation.
Such conditions are ideally met in the processing of text and melody in
songs. The tune and the text of a song are typically heard and learned
together. Accordingly, they are expected to leave memory traces that are
equally familiar and recoverable. Hence, selective disruption of one song
component after brain damage would provide evidence that the processing
of tune and text recruit distinct mechanisms. Such demonstrations do ex-
ist. As alluded to previously, there are several reports of patients who can
no longer recognize the melodies (presented without words) of familiar
songs. Yet, they are normal at recognizing the corresponding spoken lyrics
(Hébert & Peretz, 2001; Peretz, 1996). Similarly, there are individuals who
suffer from lifelong difficulties with music and who can recognize the lyrics
of familiar songs even though unable to recognize the tune that usually
accompanies them (Ayotte, Peretz, & Hyde, 2002). These observations sug-
gest that the recognition of text and tune in songs is mediated by distinct
pathways. This division of labor may occur at the perceptual stage or, at a
later stage, when making contact with the song representation in memory.
The evidence is compatible with both possibilities.
Thus, the available evidence questions, but does not rule out, the possi-
bility that melody and lyrics are integrated in memory for songs (Serafine,
Crowder, & Repp, 1984; Serafine, Davidson, Crowder, & Repp, 1986).
The few researchers who have addressed this question with neurologically
impaired patients have reached mixed results. Samson and Zatorre (1991)
Is Singing Special ?
Case Study
CASE DESCRIPTION
Neuropsychological Assessment
A summary of G.D.’s cognitive functioning is available in Table 1. At the
time of testing (April 1997), G.D. presented signs of general cognitive de-
cline, especially in reasoning abilities (executive functions). However,
memory and intellectual functions (as assessed by the Raven progressive
matrices) were still intact.
TABLE 1
Intelligence and Memory Assessments of G.D.
Cognitive Functions Score
Memory
Working memory : digit span (forward) 5
Long term memory : visuoverbal recognition
(Signoret, 1991)
Immediate recall 23/24
Delayed recall 24/24
Praxis
Gestures Normal
Copy of Rey figure 23/36 (impaired)
Visual functions
Discrimination 10/10
Benton’s line orientation test 13/15
Spatial attention test 31/35
Executive functions
Trail making test 25th percentile
(impaired)
Raven progressive matrices 50th percentile
Language Assessment
Language expression was nonfluent. Articulation was preserved but
marked by severe stuttering. Stuttering was characterized by repetitions of
initial syllables of words, often distorting them (e.g., “animaux” becomes
“zaminjo”). Expressive language also contained neologisms (e.g., “ban-
dit” becomes “espiette”) and incomplete responses (e.g., “il n’a pas de …
et lui aussi”/”he has no . . . and he too”). Automatic speech was correct for
digits and months but poor for familiar lyrics of songs due to stuttering
and phonemic transformations. G.D.’s speech was both hard to understand
and irritating. Comprehension was largely preserved. A summary of G.D.’s
language functioning is given in Table 2. His comprehension and repetition
difficulties were generally related to sentence length. The more syllables
there were in words or words in sentences, the more impaired G.D. was.
This length effect was apparent in oral repetition, in reading and in com-
prehension, hence suggesting a common phonemic output problem. Over-
all, performance showed a discrepancy between impaired expressive and
spared receptive language abilities.
Musical Assessment
G.D.’s singing abilities are intact. When prompted with 35 song titles,
G.D. eagerly sang the first line of 26 of them. The melody is remarkably in
TABLE 2
G.D.’s Performance on Language Tests
Tests G.D. Normals’ Mean (S.D.)
Expression
Boston Naming Test (Kaplan, Goodglass, & 7/60 47
Weintraub, 1983)
MT-86 ß Aphasia Battery (Béland, Lecours,
Giroux, & Bois, 1993)
Automatic speech 1/3 3
Verbal fluency 3 words 23 (5.4)
Repetition 27/30 29 (1)
Oral reading 26/30 29 (0.9)
Oral picture description 3/18 13 (3)
Discrimination and comprehension
MT-86 ß Aphasia Battery
Word and sentence picture-matching 35/47 45 (2)
Body-part identification under oral instruction 8/8 8
Token test 22.5/36 29-36
Tasks adapted from PALPA (Kay, Lesser, &
Coltheart, 1992)
Same-different discrimination 40/40 39 (1.6)
Auditory lexical (word vs. nonword) decision 50/60 57 (3.5)
TABLE 3
G.D.’s Performance on Musical Tests
Matched Control Subjects
Test G.D. Mean (Range)
Singing from memory
Melody 26/26
Text 1/26
Discrimination and memory (MBEA;
Peretz et al., 2003)
Scale 24/30 26.0 (21-29)
Contour 21/30 25.1 (23-27)
Interval 18/30 24.7 (22-27)
Rhythm 26/30 28.9 (27-30)
Meter 24/30 26.7 (21-30)
Incidental memory 18/30 25.4 (22-27)
Familiarity decision
Instrumental melody 19/20
Spoken lyrics 18/20
The first song line of the 30 most familiar songs in Quebec (Peretz, Babaï,
Lussier, Hébert, & Gagnon, 1995) were either sung with words, sung as
“la, la, la,” or spoken by the experimenter. G.D. was instructed to repeat
the line immediately after hearing it, in the same expressive mode. Upon
request, the trial was repeated. G.D. was allowed to make as many at-
tempts as he wished. Therefore, G.D. was tested across several sessions,
each time in a single expressive mode. That is, spoken and singing modes
of expression were not mixed in a given session so as to avoid mode-switch-
ing errors. Indeed, G.D.’s tendency was always to sing, not to speak. Each
session was recorded with a Sony tape recorder.
Song lines comprised 10 notes/syllables on average (range, 6-15). As is
normally the case, song lyrics were presented at a faster rate when spoken
(mean duration: 2257 ms; 3.8 syllables/s) than when sung (mean duration:
4351 ms; 2.4 syllables/s).
All productions were saved in a digital format. G.D.’s performance was
highly variable from one repetition attempt to the next, the first attempt
frequently being the best one. Therefore, only G.D.’s best performance for
each song line in each version was submitted to analyses. Two speech thera-
pists independently scored the text. Two musically trained judges indepen-
dently scored the melody. For text, the percentage of correctly articulated
syllables was calculated instead of words because G.D.’s errors are syllabic.
Criteria for considering a syllable as correct were lenient: if one syllable
matched the syllable of the original word in any order, it was considered
correct. One point was withdrawn for each additional syllable produced.
That is, if the total number of syllables exceeded the number of possible
syllables by, say, 5 syllables (as in the spoken rendition represented in Fig-
ure 1), then this number was subtracted from the number of correctly re-
produced syllables (thus, in the example, G.D. produced 5 correct syllables
– 5 additional syllables, hence obtaining a score of 0). For melodies, the
percentage of correctly repeated notes was computed. Out-of-tune notes
were considered as mistakes. One point was withdrawn for each additional
note and for rhythmic mistakes.
Results
Interrater agreements were calculated separately for the syllables and
notes for each song. For the syllables in the spoken and sung versions, the
interrater correlation was r (58) = .95, p < .01. The 20 syllables that were
Fig. 1. Notation and scoring of G.D.’s repetition of the first line of the song “Ce n’est qu’un
au revoir.” The model represents the sung production of the experimenter; Below is the best
sung reproduction with words of G.D. and the best spoken reproduction of G.D. The scor-
ing in terms of syllables and notes are presented in percentages. The auditory files can be
downloaded from our web site (www.fas.umontreal.ca/psy/iperetz.html).
not rated similarly were discarded from the analysis. For the notes in the
sung versions with and without words, the interrater correlation was r (58)
= .96, p < .01. The 28 notes that were not rated similarly were also dis-
carded from the analysis. The precise numbers of syllables and notes con-
sidered in the analysis are presented in Table 4.
The percentage of correctly repeated syllables and notes, for each ex-
cerpt in each condition, served as dependent variables. The data (see Table
4) were submitted to an ANOVA taking songs as the random variable, and
Component (text vs. melody) and Condition (isolated vs. combined) as
within-songs variables. The only effect to reach significance was the song
Component, with F (1, 29) = 54.6, p < .001, indicating that performance
TABLE 4
G.D.’s Percentage of Correct Note and Syllable Repetition of
30 Beginnings of Familiar Songs Presented in Combination (By
Singing) or in Isolation (Spoken or Sung La, La, La)*
Song Presentation Text Melody
Combined (sung) 65 93
(301) (293)
Isolated 58 96
(305) (284)
on melody was much higher than performance on text. The slightly supe-
rior performance observed in text singing over speaking was far from sig-
nificant, t(29) = 1.01. Thus, singing did not help G.D. to articulate syl-
lables in any systematic fashion, although G.D. tended to produce less
supernumerary syllables while singing (with 5%) than speaking (16%; χ2 =
21.4, with 1 d.f., p < .001; see Figure 1). Singing tends to constrain G.D.’s
speech. However, the nature of the errors was similar in both expression
modes (see Table 5)
In general, G.D.’s speech errors are typical of acquired neurological stut-
tering disorder. G.D. involuntarily repeated uttered syllables in both sing-
ing and speaking, a behavior similar to what was observed in spontaneous
speech. G.D. produced similar phonemic errors (e.g., “Au clair de la lune”
becomes “Au kair de la lune”) and neologisms (e.g., “D’où viens-tu bergère”
becomes “D’où viens-tu vieiarchère”), as illustrated in Figure 1 and quan-
tified in Table 5.
CASE DISCUSSION
G.D. has dementia with primary progressive aphasia and preserved mu-
sical expression. In the present study, we show that his loss of speech af-
fected speaking and singing in a similar fashion. This observation argues
against the notion that singing enhances speech fluency. It also questions
the claim that stuttering can be alleviated by singing (Bloodstein, 1995).
G.D.’s spoken and sung production was marked by stuttering, a condi-
tion that can supposedly be improved by singing. Singing is usually de-
scribed as one of the conditions that enhance fluency and word intelligibil-
ity in patients with developmental stuttering (Healey, Mallard, & Adams,
1976). However, cases of acquired stuttering, which occurs as a conse-
quence of brain damage, as in G.D., are less common and the evidence is
less clear. Some have reported fluent singing (Horner & Massey, 1983)
while some have not (Helm, Butler, & Benson, 1978). The evidence is weak-
ened by the fact that these observations are anecdotal and obtained in poorly
controlled conditions. This is not the case with G.D., who suffers from
acquired stuttering with intact singing. Therefore, G.D. can be considered
as the first documented patient with acquired stuttering whose speech is
TABLE 5
Speech Error Types in Singing and Speaking Expressed as
Percentages
Repetition Phonemic Neologisms
Sung 37 43 20
Spoken 25 71 4
examined while singing. As stated previously, this case study does not sup-
port the notion that singing improves speech fluency. Rather, it argues for
the complete autonomy of speech and music in production.
The idea that musical abilities are autonomous from language abilities,
and generally isolable from other cognitive functions, is also given as a
prime characteristic of dementia. Demented people are typically described
as musically apt while being severely compromised by the disease in their
general cognitive functioning (Brontons, 2000). Some demented patients
can even learn new songs while being unable to learn how to find their
own room in the geriatric care unit (Beatty et al., 1988). G.D.’s general
behavior and neurological condition certainly fit this broad characteriza-
tion. One unexpected outcome of the present investigation was that G.D.
stopped his constant mumbling and started humming instead, to the relief
of his wife.
General Discussion
the original combination of text and melody is best understood as the re-
sult of the memorization of contingent links between separate memory
traces.
This conclusion is also consistent with recent research showing the inde-
pendence of melody and lyrics in divided attention tasks. For instance, lis-
teners are able to monitor operatic songs for the presence of semantic and
tonal incongruities, independently. Supportive evidence has been obtained
in the on-line electrophysiological recording of brain responses (Besson,
Faïta, Peretz, Bonnel, & Requin, 1998) and in the analysis of behavioral
responses with measures derived from the signal detection theory (Bonnel,
Faïta, Peretz, & Besson, 2001).
However, as suggested in the introduction of this article, isolation of a
cognitive process of interest is not trivial, and much of the current func-
tional neuroimaging research is plagued by this problem. The method of
cognitive neuropsychology can be more powerful, as illustrated here. The
reason why neurological disorders are so informative regarding the func-
tioning of the brain is that the ensuing deficits often expose the inner work-
ings of a complex system more clearly than with any other method
(McCloskey, 2001). Most human activities rely on the operation of highly
complex systems. The apparent ease and speed with which these abilities
are normally carried out often make it difficult to uncover the underlying
mechanisms. By breaking down this smooth functioning, brain damage
renders the complex system more amenable to investigation.
Studying abnormalities or malfunctions to shed light on normal struc-
ture and processes is a well-established research strategy in health sciences.
For example, research on AIDS has contributed to knowledge of the func-
tioning of the normal immune system. Similarly, research on singing per-
formance in aphasics contributes to the understanding of normal singing
and speaking abilities. Unlike most diseases, however, homogeneity of the
cognitive deficits after brain damage cannot be assumed a priori. This is
why cognitive neuropsychology researchers have developed a methodologi-
cal expertise in the study of single cases. This does not mean that single
case data are less valuable or more subject to artifacts than group data. The
data of a single case, such as G.D.’s, are as valid as any other method used
in cognitive psychology to falsify a theory, because all brain-damaged pa-
tients with the same amount of musical experience are assumed to share
the same architecture of the system before brain damage. Uniformity of
functioning across people is assumed to be as true in cognitive neuropsy-
chology as it is in music cognition and in the cognitive sciences in general.
Thus, to challenge the present findings supporting the existence of two
distinct pathways for singing the tune and the text of songs, one would
need to either argue that the uniformity assumption has been violated in
G.D. or to show that there was a methodological problem in our study.
References
Assal, G. (1973). Wernicke’s aphasia without amusia in a pianist. Revue Neurologique
(Paris), 129, 251–255.
Assal, G., Buttet, J., & Javet, R. C. (1977). Aptitudes musicales chez les aphasiques. Revue
Medicale de la Suisse Romande, 97, 5–12.
Ayotte, J., Peretz, I., & Hyde, K. (2002). Congenital amusia : A group study of adults
afflicted with a music-specific disorder. Brain, 125, 238–251.
Ayotte, J., Peretz, I., Rousseau, I., Bard, C., & Bojanowski, M. (2000). Patterns of music
agnosia associated with middle cerebral artery infarcts. Brain, 123, 1926–1938.
Basso, A., & Capitani, E. (1985). Spared musical abilities in a conductor with global apha-
sia and ideomotor apraxia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 48, 407–
412.
Beatty, W. W., Zavadil, K. D., Bailly, R. C., Rixen, G. J., Zavadil, L. E., Farnham, N., et al.
(1988). Preserved musical skill in a severely demented patient. International Journal of
Clinical Neuropsychology, 10, 158–164.
Béland, R., Lecours, A. R., Giroux, F., & Bois, M. (1993). The MT-86 ß Aphasia Battery: a
subset of normative data in relation to age and level of school education. Aphasiology, 7,
359–382.
Bergeson, T. R., & Trehub, S. E. (2002). Absolute pitch and tempo in mother’s songs to
infants. Psychological Science, 13, 72–75.
Besson, M., Faïta, F., Peretz, I., Bonnel, A.-M., & Requin, J. (1998). Singing in the brain:
Independence of lyrics and tunes. Psychological Science, 9, 494–498.
Bloodstein, O. (1995). A handbook on stuttering. New York: Chapman & Hall.
Bonnel, A.-M., Faïta, F., Peretz, I., & Besson, M. (2001). Divided attention between lyrics
and tunes in operatic songs: Evidence for independent processing. Perception & Psycho-
physics, 67, 1201–1213.
Broca, P. (1861). Remarques sur le siège de la faculté du langage articulé, suivies d’une
observation d’aphémie. Bulletin et Mémoires de la Société Anatomique de Paris, 2, 330–
357.
Brontons, M. (2000). An overview of the music therapy literature relating to elderly people.
In D. Aldridge (Ed.), Music therapy in dementia care: More new voices. London: J.
Kingsley.
Cadalbert, A., Landis, T., Regard, M., & Graves, R. E. (1994). Singing with and without
words: hemispheric asymmetries in motor control. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Neuropsychology, 16, 664–670.
1. We thank Aniruddh Patel for insightful comments, and G.D and his wife for their
collaboration. At the time of testing, G.D. was autonomous. Today, he lives in a nursing
home. The work was supported by a grant from the International Human Frontier Science
Program (I.P.) and from the Natural Science and Engineering Research Council of Canada
(S.H.). L.G., J.M. and S.H. are all supported by fellowships from the Fonds de la Recherche
en Santé du Québec.
Cohen, N. S., & Ford, J. (1995). The effects of musical cues on the nonpurposive speech of
persons with aphasia. Journal of Music Therapy, 32, 46–57.
Coltheart, M. (2003). Cognitive neuropsychology. In J. Wixted (Ed.), Stevens’ handbook of
experimental psychology (Vol. 4). New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Crowder, R. G., Serafine, M. L., & Repp, B. (1990). Physical interaction and association by
contiguity in memory for the words and melodies of songs. Memory & Cognition, 18,
469–476.
Dowling, W. J. (1984). Development of musical schemata in children’s spontaneous sing-
ing. In W. R. Crozier & A. J. Chapman (Eds.), Cognitive processes in the perception of
art (pp. 145–163). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Dowling, W. J., & Harwood, D. (1986). Music cognition. New York: Academic Press.
Godefroy, O., Leys, D., Furby, A., De Reuck, J., Daems, C., Rondepierre, P., et al. (1995).
Psychoacoustical deficits related to bilateral subcortical hemorrhages: A case with ap-
perceptive auditory agnosia. Cortex, 31, 149–159.
Griffiths, T. D., Rees, A., Witton, C., Cross, P. M., Shakir, R. A., & Green, G. G. (1997).
Spatial and temporal auditory processing deficits following right hemisphere infarction:
A psychophysical study. Brain, 120, 785–794.
Halpern, A. R. (1988). Perceived and imagined tempos of familiar songs. Music Perception,
6, 193–202.
Halpern, A. R. (1989). Memory for the absolute pitch of familiar songs. Memory & Cogni-
tion, 17, 572–581.
Healey, E. C., Mallard, A. R. III, & Adams, M. R. (1976). Factors contributing to the reduc-
tion of stuttering during singing. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research, 19, 475–480.
Hébert, S., & Peretz, I. (2001). Are text and tune of familiar songs separable by brain
damage? Brain and Cognition, 46(1-2), 169–175.
Hébert, S., Racette, A., Gagnon, L., & Peretz, I. (2003). Revisiting the dissociation between
singing and speaking in expressive aphasia. Brain, 126, 1838–1850.
Helm, N. A., Butler, R. B., & Benson, D. F. (1978). Acquired stuttering. Neurology, 28,
1159–1165.
Horner, J., & Massey, E. W. (1983). Progressive dysfluency associated with right hemi-
sphere disease. Brain & Language, 18, 71–85.
Jeffries, K. J., Fritz, J. B., & Braun, A. R. (2003). Words in melody: an H2O15 PET study of
brain activation during singing and speaking. NeuroReport, 14, 749–754.
Kaplan, E., Goodglass, H., & Weintraub, S. (1983). Boston Naming Test. Malvern, PA: Lea
& Febiger.
Kay, J., Lesser, J., & Coltheart, M. (1992). Psycholinguistic assessments of language pro-
cessing in aphasia. Hove, England: Psychology Press.
Laignel-Lavastine, M., & Alajouanine, T. (1921). Un cas d’agnosie auditive. Société de
Neurologie, 37, 194–198.
Levitin, D. J. (1994). Absolute memory for musical pitch: evidence from the production of
learned melodies. Perception & Psychophysics, 56, 414–423.
Levitin, D. J., & Cook, P. R. (1996). Memory for musical tempo: additional evidence that
auditory memory is absolute. Perception & Psychophysics, 58, 927–935.
Luria, A. R., Tsvetkova, L. S., & Futer, D. S. (1965). Aphasia in a composer (V. G. Shebalin).
Journal of Neurology Sciences, 2, 288–292.
Marin, O. S. M., & Perry, D. W. (1999). Neurological aspects of music perception and
performance. In D. Deutsch (Ed.), The psychology of music (2nd ed., pp. 653–724).
New York: Academic Press.
McCloskey, M. (1993). Theory and evidence in cognitive neuropsychology : A “radical”
response to Robertson, Knight, Rafal, and Shimamura (1993). Journal of Experimental
Psychology : Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 718–734.
McCloskey, M. (2001). The future of cognitive neuropsychology. In B. Repp (Ed.), The
handbook of cognitive neuropsychology: What deficits reveal about the human mind
(pp. 593–610). Philadelphia: Psychology Press.
Mendez, M. (2001). Generalized auditory agnosia with spared music recognition in a left-
hander: Analysis of a case with a right temporal stroke. Cortex, 37, 139–150.
Metz-Lutz, M. N., & Dahl, E. (1984). Analysis of word comprehension in a case of pure
word deafness. Brain and Language, 23, 13–25.
Neary, D., Snowdon, J. S., Gustafson, L., Passant, U., Stuss, D., Black, S., et al. (1998).
Frontotemporal lobar degeneration: A consensus on clinical diagnostic criteria. Neurol-
ogy, 51, 1546–1554.
Ostwald, P. F. (1973). Musical behavior in early childhood. Developmental Medicine &
Child Neurology, 15, 367–375.
Patel, A. (2003). Language, music, syntax and the brain. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 674–681.
Peretz, I. (1996). Can we lose memories for music? The case of music agnosia in a
nonmusician. Journal of Cognitive Neurosciences, 8, 481–496.
Peretz, I., Babaï, M., Lussier, I., Hébert, S., & Gagnon, L. (1995). Corpus d’extraits musicaux:
indices relatifs à la familiarité, à l’âge d’acquisition et aux évocations verbales. Canadian
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49, 211–239.
Peretz, I., Belleville, S., & Fontaine, S. (1997). Dissociations entre musique et langage après
atteinte cérébrale: un nouveau cas d’amusie sans aphasie. Canadian Journal of Experi-
mental Psychology, 51, 354–368.
Peretz, I., Champod, A.-S., & Hyde, K. L. (2003). Varieties of musical disorders: The Montreal
Battery of Evaluation of Amusia. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, in press.
Peretz, I., & Coltheart, M. (2003). Modularity of music processing. Nature Neuroscience,
6, 688–691.
Peretz, I., & Hyde, K. (2003). What is specific to music processing? Insights from congeni-
tal amusia. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 362–367.
Peretz, I., Kolinsky, R., Tramo, M., Labrecque, R., Hublet, C., Demeurisse, G., et al. (1994).
Functional dissociations following bilateral lesions of auditory cortex. Brain, 117, 1283–
1301.
Piccirilli, M., Sciarma, T., & Luzzi, S. (2000). Modularity of music : Evidence from a case of
pure amusia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 69, 541–545.
Samson, S., & Zatorre, R. J. (1991). Recognition memory for text and melody of songs
after unilateral temporal lobe lesion: Evidence for dual encoding. Journal of Experimen-
tal Psychology : Learning, Memory and Cognition, 17, 793–804.
Serafine, M. L., Crowder, R. G., & Repp, B. H. (1984). Integration of melody and text in
memory for songs. Cognition, 16, 285–303.
Serafine, M. L., Davidson, R. J., Crowder, R. G., & Repp, B. H. (1986). On the nature of
melody: Text integration in memory for songs. Journal of Memory and Language, 25,
123–135.
Signoret, J. L. (1991). Batterie d’efficience mnésique: BEM144. Paris: Elsevier.
Signoret, J. L., van Eeckhout, P., Poncet, M., & Castaigne, P. (1987). [Aphasia without
amusia in a blind organist: Verbal alexia-agraphia without musical alexia-agraphia in
braille]. Revue Neurologique (Paris), 143, 172–181.
Smith, A. (1966). Speech and other functions after left (dominant) hemispherectomy. Jour-
nal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 29, 467–471.
Steinke, W. R., Cuddy, L. L., & Jakobson, L. S. (2001). Dissociations among functional
subsystems governing melody recognition after right-hemisphere damage. Cognitive
Neuropsychology, 18, 411–437.
Takahashi, N., Kawamura, M., Shinotou, H., Hirayama, K., Kaga, K., & Shindo, M. (1992).
Pure word deafness due to left hemisphere damage. Cortex, 28, 295–303.
Wilson, S. J., & Pressing, J. (1999). Neuropsychological assessment and the modeling of
musical deficits. In R. R. Pratt & D. Erdonmez Grocke (Eds.), Music medicine and music
therapy: Expanding horizons (pp. 47–74). Melbourne: The University of Melbourne.
Yamadori, A., Osumi, Y., Masuhara, S., & Okubo, M. (1977). Preservation of singing in
Broca’s aphasia. Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 40, 221–224.
Yaqub, B. A., Gascon, G. G., Al-Nosha, M., & Whitaker, H. (1988). Pure word deafness
(acquired verbal auditory agnosia) in an Arabic speaking patient. Brain, 111, 457–466.