This document discusses how reading has cognitive consequences that extend beyond comprehending text. It argues that reading ability develops through a reciprocal relationship, where early reading skill influences later exposure to text, which further impacts the development of other cognitive skills. Less skilled readers experience less practice and develop reading habits that hinder comprehension and vocabulary growth over time compared to more skilled readers. The document aims to explore how the volume of reading shapes cognitive development through this reciprocal relationship between reading ability and exposure to print.
This document discusses how reading has cognitive consequences that extend beyond comprehending text. It argues that reading ability develops through a reciprocal relationship, where early reading skill influences later exposure to text, which further impacts the development of other cognitive skills. Less skilled readers experience less practice and develop reading habits that hinder comprehension and vocabulary growth over time compared to more skilled readers. The document aims to explore how the volume of reading shapes cognitive development through this reciprocal relationship between reading ability and exposure to print.
This document discusses how reading has cognitive consequences that extend beyond comprehending text. It argues that reading ability develops through a reciprocal relationship, where early reading skill influences later exposure to text, which further impacts the development of other cognitive skills. Less skilled readers experience less practice and develop reading habits that hinder comprehension and vocabulary growth over time compared to more skilled readers. The document aims to explore how the volume of reading shapes cognitive development through this reciprocal relationship between reading ability and exposure to print.
This document discusses how reading has cognitive consequences that extend beyond comprehending text. It argues that reading ability develops through a reciprocal relationship, where early reading skill influences later exposure to text, which further impacts the development of other cognitive skills. Less skilled readers experience less practice and develop reading habits that hinder comprehension and vocabulary growth over time compared to more skilled readers. The document aims to explore how the volume of reading shapes cognitive development through this reciprocal relationship between reading ability and exposure to print.
beyond its immediate task of lifting meaning from a particular passage. Furthermore, these consequences warding early reading experiences that lead to less in- volvement in reading-related activities. Lack of expo- sure and practice on the part of the less-skilled reader are reciprocal and exponential in nature. Accumulated delays the development of automaticity and speed at over timespiraling either upward or downward the word recognition level. Slow, capacity-draining they carry profound implications for the development word recognition processes require cognitive re- of a wide range of cognitive capabilities. sources that should be allocated to comprehension. Concern about the reciprocal influences of reading Thus, reading for meaning is hindered; unrewarding achievement has been elucidated through discussions reading experiences multiply; and practice is avoided of so-called Matthew effects in academic achieve- or merely tolerated without real cognitive involve- ment (Stanovich, 1986;Walberg & Tsai, 1983).The term ment. Matthew effects is taken from the Biblical passage The disparity in the reading experiences of children that describes a rich-get-richer and poor-get-poorer of varying skill may have many other consequences for phenomenon.Applying this concept to reading, we see their future reading and cognitive development.As skill that very early in the reading process poor readers, develops and word recognition becomes less resource who experience greater difficulty in breaking the demanding and more automatic, more general lan- spelling-to-sound code, begin to be exposed to much guage skills, such as vocabulary, background knowl- less text than their more skilled peers (Allington, 1984; edge, familiarity with complex syntactic structures, Biemiller, 1977-1978). Further exacerbating the prob- etc., become the limiting factor on reading ability lem is the fact that less-skilled readers often find them- (Chall, 1983; Sticht, 1979). But the sheer volume of selves in materials that are too difficult for them reading done by the better reader has the potential to (Allington, 1977, 1983, 1984; Gambrell,Wilson, & Gantt, provide an advantage even here ifas our research 1981). The combination of deficient decoding skills, suggestsreading a lot serves to develop these very lack of practice, and difficult materials results in unre- skills and knowledge bases (Cunningham & Stanovich, 1997; Echols, West, Stanovich, & Zehr, 1996; Stanovich Anne E. Cunningham is visiting associate professor & Cunningham, 1992, 1993). From the standpoint of a in cognition and development in the graduate school reciprocal model of reading development, this means of education at the University of California, Berke- that many cognitive differences observed between ley. Her research examines the cognitive and motiva- readers of differing skill may in fact be consequences tional processes that underlie reading ability and the of differential practice that itself resulted from early cognitive consequences of reading skill and engage- differences in the speed of initial reading acquisition. ment. Keith E. Stanovich is professor of applied psy- The increased reading experiences of children who chology at the University of Toronto/Ontario Insti- master the spelling-to-sound code early thus might tute for Studies in Education. His recent awards in- have important positive feedback effects that are de- clude the Sylvia Scribner Award from the American nied the slowly progressing reader. In our research, we Educational Research Association and the Oscar S. have begun to explore these reciprocal effects by ex- Causey Award from the National Research Confer- amining the role that reading volume plays in shaping ence for his distinguished and substantial contribu- the mind and will share many of our findings in this ar- tions to literacy research. ticle. We should say at the outset that the complexity of This research was supported by a Spencer Founda- some of the work we will describe in this article was tion Small Grant to Anne E. Cunningham and grant necessitated in large part by the fact that it is difficult No. 410-95-0315 from the Social Sciences and Hu- to tease apart the unique contribution that reading vol- manities Research Council of Canada to Keith E. ume affords. One of the difficulties is that levels of Stanovich. reading volume are correlated with many other cogni-
1 AMERICAN EDUCATOR/AMERICAN FEDERATION OF TEACHERS SPRING/SUMMER 1998
tive and behavioral characteristics. Avid readers tend adult speech in two contexts vary- to be different from nonreaders on a wide variety of ing in formality. The words used in cognitive skills, behavioral habits, and background vari- the different contexts were analyzed ables (Guthrie, Schafer, & Hutchinson, 1991; Kaestle, according to a standard frequency 1991; Zill & Winglee, 1990). Attributing any particular count of English (Carroll, Davies, & outcome to reading volume is thus extremely difficult. Richman, 1971). This frequency count ranks the 86,741 different word forms in English according to Theoretical Reasons To Expect their frequency of occurrence in a Positive Cognitive large corpus of written English. So, for example, the word the is ranked Consequences from number 1, the 10th most frequent word is it, the word know is ranked Reading Volume 100, the word pass is ranked 1,000, In certain very important cognitive domains, there the word vibrate is 5,000th in fre- are strong theoretical reasons to expect a positive and quency, the word shrimp is 9,000th in unique effect of avid reading. Vocabulary development frequency, and the word amplifier is provides a case in point. Most theorists are agreed that 16,000th in frequency.The first column, the bulk of vocabulary growth during a childs lifetime labeled Rank of Median Word, is simply occurs indirectly through language exposure rather the frequency rank of the average word than through direct teaching (Miller & Gildea, 1987; (after a small correction) in each of the Nagy & Anderson, 1984; Nagy, Herman, & Anderson, categories. So, for example, the average 1985; Sternberg, 1985, 1987). Furthermore, many re- word in childrens books was ranked searchers are convinced that reading volume, rather 627th most frequent in the Carroll et than oral language, is the prime contributor to indi- al. word count; the average word in vidual differences in childrens vocabularies (Hayes, popular magazines was ranked 1,399th 1988; Hayes & Ahrens, 1988; Nagy & Anderson, most frequent; and the average word 1984; Nagy & Herman, 1987; Stanovich, 1986). in the abstracts of scientific articles The theoretical reasons for believing that reading had, not surprisingly, a very low rank volume is a particularly effective way of ex- (4,389). panding a childs vocabulary derive from What is immediately apparent is the differences in the statistical distribu- how lexically impoverished is most tions of words that have been found be- speech, as compared to written lan- tween print and oral language. Some of guage. With the exception of the spe- these differences are illustrated in Table 1, cial situation of courtroom testimony, which displays the results of some of the re- the average frequency of the words in search of Hayes and Ahrens (1988), who all of the samples of oral speech is quite have analyzed the distributions of words low, hovering in the 400-600 range of ranks. used in various contexts. The relative rarity of the words in childrens books is, The table illustrates the three different in fact, greater than that in all of the adult conversa- categories of language that were analyzed: tion, except for the courtroom testimony. Indeed, the written language sampled from genres as words used in childrens books are considerably difficult as scientific articles and as sim- rarer than those in the speech on prime-time adult ple as preschool books; words spoken on television. The categories of adult reading matter television shows of various types; and contain words that are two or three times rarer than
SPRING/SUMMER 1998 AMERICAN EDUCATOR/AMERICAN FEDERATION OF TEACHERS 2
more rare words in them than does adult prime-time Table 1 television and the conversation of college graduates. Selected Statistics for Major Sources of Spoken and Popular magazines have roughly three times as many Written Language (Sample Means) opportunities for new word learning as does prime- time television and adult conversation.Assurances by Rank of Rare Words some educators that What they read and write may Median Word per 1000 make people smarter, but so will any activity that en- I. Printed texts gages the mind, including interesting conversation Abstracts of scientific articles 4389 128.0 (Smith, 1989) are overstated, at least when applied to Newspapers 1690 68.3 the domain of vocabulary learning. The data in Table Popular magazines 1399 65.7 1 indicate that conversation is not a substitute for Adult books 1058 52.7 reading. Comic books 867 53.5 Childrens books 627 30.9 It is sometimes argued or implied that the type of Preschool books 578 16.3 words present in print but not represented in speech are unnecessary wordsjargon, academic II. Television texts doublespeak, elitist terms of social advantage, or Popular prime-time adult shows 490 22.7 words used to maintain the status of the users but Popular prime-time childrens shows 543 20.2 Cartoon shows 598 30.8 that serve no real functional purpose. A considera- Mr. Rogers and Sesame Street 413 2.0 tion of the frequency distributions of written and spoken words reveals this argument to be patently III. Adult speech false. Table 2 presents a list of words that do not Expert witness testimony 1008 28.4 occur at all in two large corpora of oral language College graduates to friends, spouses 496 17.3 (Berger, 1977; Brown, 1984), but that have apprecia- Adapted from Hayes and Ahrens (1988). ble frequencies in a written frequency count (Fran- cis & Kucera, 1982). The words participation, lux- those heard on television. ury, maneuver, provoke, reluctantly, relinquish, These relative differences in word rarity have di- portray, equate, hormone, exposure, display, in- rect implications for vocabulary development. If variably, dominance, literal, legitimate, and infi- most vocabulary is acquired outside of formal teach- nite are not unnecessary appendages, concocted to ing, then the only opportunities to acquire new exclude those who are unfamiliar with them. They words occur when an individual is exposed to a are words that are necessary to make critical dis- word in written or oral language that is outside his tinctions in the physical and social world in which current vocabulary.That this will happen vastly more we live. Without such lexical tools, one will be se- often while reading than while talking or watching verely disadvantaged in attaining ones goals in an television is illustrated in the second column of Table advanced society such as ours. As Olson (1986) 1. The column lists how many rare words per 1000 notes: are contained in each of the categories. A rare word It is easy to show that sensitivity to the subtleties of is defined as one with a rank lower than 10,000; language are crucial to some undertakings.A person who roughly a word that is outside the vocabulary of a does not clearly see the difference between an expression fourth to sixth grader. For vocabulary growth to of intention and a promise or between a mistake and an occur after the middle grades, children must be ex- accident, or between a falsehood and a lie, should avoid a posed to words that are rare by this definition.Again, legal career or, for that matter, a theological one. it is print that provides many more such word-learn- The large differences in lexical richness between ing opportunities. Childrens books have 50 percent speech and print are a major source of individual dif- ferences in vocabulary development. These differ- ences are created by the large variability among chil- Table 2 dren in exposure to literacy. Table 3 presents the data Examples of words that do not appear in two large from a study of the out-of-school time use by fifth corpora of oral language (Berger, 1977; Brown, 1984) but graders conducted by Anderson, Wilson, and Fielding that have appreciable frequencies in written texts (1988). From diaries that the children filled out daily (Carroll, Davies & Richman, 1971; over several months time, the investigators estimated Francis & Kucera, 1982): how many minutes per day that individuals were en- gaged in reading and other activities while not in display literal school. The table indicates that the child at the 50th dominance legitimate percentile in amount of independent reading was dominant luxury reading approximately 4.6 minutes per day, or about a exposure maneuver half an hour per week, over six times as much as the equate participation child at the 20th percentile in amount of reading time equation portray (less than a minute daily). Or, to take another example, gravity provoke the child at the 80th percentile in amount of indepen- hormone relinquish dent reading time (14.2 minutes) was reading over infinite reluctantly twenty times as much as the child at the 20th per- invariably centile. Anderson et al. (1988) estimated the childrens read-
3 AMERICAN EDUCATOR/AMERICAN FEDERATION OF TEACHERS SPRING/SUMMER 1998
ing rates and used these, in conjunction with the In all, we developed two measures of adults reading amount of reading in minutes per day, to extrapolate a volume and one for childrens reading volume. Briefly, figure for the number of words that the children at var- the childrens measure, named the Title Recognition ious percentiles were reading.These figures, presented Test (TRT), requires children to pick out the titles of in the far right of the table, illustrate the enormous dif- popular childrens books from a list of titles that in- ferences in word exposure that are generated by chil- cludes equal numbers of made-up titles.This task is easy drens differential proclivities toward reading. For ex- to administer to large numbers of children, it does not ample, the average child at the 90th percentile reads al- make large cognitive demands, and its results are reli- most two million words per year outside of school, ableit is not possible for children to distort their re- more than 200 times more words than the child at the sponses toward what they perceive as socially desirable 10th percentile, who reads just 8,000 words outside of answers. Because the number of wrong answers can be school during a year. To put it another way, the entire counted against correct ones, it is possible to remove years out-of-school reading for the child at the 10th the effects of guessing from the results (see Cunning- percentile amounts to just two days reading for the ham & Stanovich, 1990; 1991; and Stanovich and West, child at the 90th percentile! These dramatic differ- 1989 for a full description of these instruments and a ences, combined with the lexical richness of print, act discussion of the logic behind them). The adults mea- to create large vocabulary differences among children. sures, named the Author Recognition and Magazine Recognition Test, have the same task requirements and are described fully in Stanovich and West (1989). Examining the Consequences A score on the Title Recognition Test, of course, is of Differential Degrees not an absolute measure of childrens reading volume and previous literacy experiences, but it does provide of Reading Volume us with an index of the relative differences in reading volume.This index enables us to ask what effects read- It is one thing to speculate on how these differences in reading volume may result in specific cognitive con- ing volume (rather than general reading comprehen- sequences in domains like vocabulary; it is another to sion and word decoding ability) has on intelligence, vo- demonstrate that these effects are occurring. In our re- cabulary, spelling, and childrens general knowledge. In search, we have sought empirical evidence for the spe- short, it enables us to ask the question, does reading cific effects of reading volume, effects that do not sim- in and of itselfshape the quality of our mind? ply result from the higher cognitive abilities and skills The titles appearing on the TRT were selected from a of the more avid reader. Although there are consider- sample of book titles generated in pilot investigations by able differences in amount of reading volume in groups of children ranging in age from second grade school, it is likely that differences in out-of-school read- through high school. In selecting the items that appear ing volume are an even more potent source of the on any one version of the TRT, an attempt was made to rich-get-richer and poor-get-poorer achievement pat- choose titles that were not prominent parts of classroom terns. Therefore, we have sought to examine the reading activities in these particular schools. Because we unique contribution that independent or out-of-school wanted the TRT to probe out-of-school rather than reading makes toward reading ability, aspects of verbal school-directed reading, an attempt was made to choose intelligence, and general knowledge about the world. titles that were not used in the school curriculum. As part of this research program, our research group In our technical reports on this work, we have used a has pioneered the use of a measure of reading volume powerful statistical technique known as hierarchical that has some unique advantages in investigations of multiple regression to solve the interpretive problem this kind (Cunningham and Stanovich, 1990; Stanovich that avid readers excel in most domains of verbal learn- and West, 1989). ing and that, therefore, our measures of reading volume might be spuriously correlated to a host of abilities (Cunningham & Stanovich, 1990, 1991; Stanovich & Table 3 Cunningham, 1992, 1993; Stanovich & West, 1989). We Variation in Amount of Independent Reading have found that even when performance is statistically equated for reading comprehension and general ability, Independent Reading reading volume is still a very powerful predictor of vo- % Minutes Per Day Words Read Per Year cabulary and knowledge differences. Thus, we believe 98 65.0 4,358,000 that reading volume is not simply an indirect indicator 90 21.1 1,823,000 of ability; it is actually a potentially separable, indepen- 80 14.2 1,146,000 dent source of cognitive differences. 70 9.6 622,000 60 6.5 432,000 50 4.6 282,000 Reading Volume as 40 3.2 200,000 a Contributor to Growth 30 1.3 106,000 20 0.7 21,000 in Verbal Skills 10 0.1 8,000 In several studies, we have attempted to link chil- 2 0.0 0 drens reading volume to specific cognitive outcomes Adapted from Anderson,Wilson, and Fielding (1988). after controlling for relevant general abilities such as IQ. In a study of fourth-, fifth-, and sixth-grade children, SPRING/SUMMER 1998 AMERICAN EDUCATOR/AMERICAN FEDERATION OF TEACHERS 4 we examined whether reading volume accounts for nature of these analyses is illustrated in a longitudinal differences in vocabulary development once controls study that we have conducted (Cipielewski & for both general intelligence and specific verbal abili- Stanovich, 1992). We addressed the question of ties were invoked (Cunningham & Stanovich, 1991). whether reading volume can predict individual differ- We employed multiple measures of vocabulary and ences in growth in reading comprehension from third controlled for the effects of age and intelligence. We grade to fifth grade.We found that reading volume pre- also controlled for the effect of another ability that dicted variance in fifth-grade reading comprehension may be more closely linked to vocabulary acquisition ability after third-grade reading comprehension scores mechanisms: decoding ability. Decoding skill might me- had been removed.Thus, in removing the contribution diate a relationship between reading volume and a of reading comprehension in our adult studies, we are variable like vocabulary size in numerous ways. High undoubtedly removing some of the variance in vari- levels of decoding skill, certainly a contributor to ables such as vocabulary and general knowledge that is greater reading volume, might provide relatively com- rightfully attributed to reading volume. plete contexts for figuring out the meaning of words during reading. Thus, reading volume and vocabulary might be linked via their connection to decoding abil- Reading Volume and ity: Good decoders read a lot and have the best con- Declarative Knowledge text available for inferring new words. This potential linkage was accounted for by statistically controlling In other studies, we have focused even more directly for decoding ability prior to investigating reading vol- on content knowledge by addressing the issue of ume. But we found that even after accounting for gen- Where Does Knowledge Come From?. Stanovich and eral intelligence and decoding ability, reading volume Cunningham (1993) examined general ability, reading contributed significantly and independently to vocabu- volume, and exposure to other media sources as deter- lary knowledge in fourth-, fifth-, and sixth-grade chil- minants of individual differences in content knowl- dren. edge. This study contained a particularly stringent test These findings demonstrate that reading volume, al- of the role of reading volume and individual differ- though clearly a consequence of developed reading ences in knowledge acquisition among 268 college stu- ability, is itself a significant contributor to the develop- dents. We administered five different measures of gen- ment of other aspects of verbal intelligence. Such rich- eral knowledge to the students. Then we stacked the get-richer (and of course their converse, poor-get- deck against reading volume once again by statistically poorer) effects are becoming of increasing concern in entering four measures of general ability before look- the educational community (Adams, 1990; Chall, 1989) ing at the contribution of reading volume: high school and are playing an increasingly prominent role in theo- grade-point average, performance on an intelligence ries of individual differences in reading ability and test, an SAT-type mathematics test, and an adult reading growth (Anderson, et al., 1988; Chall, Jacobs, & Bald- comprehension test. This set of tasks surely exhausts win, 1990; Hayes, 1988; Hayes & Ahrens, 1988; Juel, the variance attributable to any general ability con- 1988, 1994; Stanovich 1986, 1989, 1993). struct; and, as one would expect, we found that general In a study we conducted involving college students, ability accounted for a substantial proportion of vari- we employed an even more stringent test of whether ance in the composite measure of general knowledge. reading volume is a unique predictor of verbal skill Next we entered a composite measure of exposure to (Stanovich & Cunningham, 1992). In this study we ex- television, but it did not account for any additional amined many of the same variables as in our study of variance. However, a composite index of reading vol- fourth- to sixth-grade students. However, we decided ume accounted for a substantial 37.1 percent of the to stack the deck against reading volume by first re- variance when entered after the four ability mea- moving any contribution of reading ability and sures and television exposure. general intelligence. By structuring the analyses This pattern was replicated in each of the five in this way, we did not mean to imply that read- measures of general knowledge we employed, ing volume is not a determinant of reading com- including a homemade instrument we called prehension ability. Indeed, we argue that there the Practical Knowledge Test. This task was are grounds for believing that reading volume designed to address the criticism that our facilitates growth in comprehension ability. other measures of general knowledge were However, we wanted to construct the most too academicthat they tapped knowl- conservative analysis possible by deliberately edge that was too esoteric or elitist and allowing the comprehension measure to steal that was not useful in daily life. We didnt some variance that is rightfully attributed to think this was true; many items on these the measure of reading volume.The results of measures were mundane and concrete our study again attest to the potency of read- questions such as, In what part of the ing volume. We found that reading volume body does the infection called pneumo- made a significant contribution to multiple nia occur? Nevertheless, in the Practical measures of vocabulary, general knowledge, Knowledge Test, we made an effort to spelling, and verbal fluency even after reading devise questions that were directly rel- comprehension ability and nonverbal ability evant to daily living in a technological had been partialed out. society in the late twentieth century; One way of demonstrating the conservative for example, What does the carbure-
5 AMERICAN EDUCATOR/AMERICAN FEDERATION OF TEACHERS SPRING/SUMMER 1998
tor in an automobile do? If a substance is carcinogenic, associated with higher scores on the question, but tele- it means that it is ______? After the Federal Reserve vision exposure was associated with lower scores. Board raises the prime lending rate, the interest that you Scores among the group high in reading volume and will be asked to pay on a car loan will generally in- low in television exposure were highest, and the low- crease/decrease/stay the same? What vitamin is highly est scores were achieved by those high in television concentrated in citrus fruits? When a stock exchange is exposure and low in reading volume. Our analyses in a bear market, what is happening? and so forth. confirmed that these relationships were not due to dif- The results indicated that the more avid readers in ferences in general ability. our studyregardless of their general abilitiesknew Similarly, we have analyzed a variety of other mis- more about how a carburetor worked, were more conceptions in a number of other different do- likely to know who their United States senators were, mainsincluding knowledge of World War II, the more likely to know how many teaspoons are equiva- worlds languages, and the components of the federal lent to one tablespoon, were more likely to know what budgetand all of them replicate the pattern shown a stroke was, and what a closed shop in a factory was, for this question. The cognitive anatomy of misinfor- etc. One would be hard pressed to deny that at least mation appears to be one of too little exposure to some of this knowledge is relevant to living in the print (or reading) and over-reliance on television for United States in the late 20th century. information about the world. Although television In other questions asked of these same students, we viewing can have positive associations with knowl- attempted to probe areas that we thought might be edge when the viewing is confined to public televi- characterized by misinformation. We then attempted sion, news, and/or documentar y material (Hall, to trace the cognitive anatomy of this misinforma- Chiarello, & Edmondson, 1996; West & Stanovich, tion. One such question concerned the sizes of the 1991; West et al., 1993), familiarity with the prime- worlds major religions and was designed to assess time television material that defines mass viewing in awareness of the multicultural nature of the modern North America is most often negatively associated world.The question was phrased as follows:The 1986 with knowledge acquisition. Encyclopedia Britannica estimates that there are ap- In another study, Stanovich, West, & Harrison proximately nine hundred million people in the world (1995) examined a much older population in order (not just the United States) who identify themselves as to investigate the extent to which age-related growth Christians. How many people in the world (not just in knowledge can be accounted for by differences in the United States) do you think identify themselves as reading volume. Although much research effort has ______? Space was then provided on the form for the been expended on describing cumulative growth in subjects to make estimates of the number of Moslems, crystallized intelligence (e.g., acquired knowledge Jews, Buddhists, Hindus, etc. such as vocabulary and general information), we We will focus here on the estimates of Moslem and know little about the experiences that relate to Jewish people because of our a priori hypothesis that knowledge growth in older individuals. For example, availability effects caused by televised coverage of Is- educational experience (years in school) is a predic- rael in the U.S. had skewed the perception of this ratio. tor of intellectual functioning in older individuals While our samples median estimate of the number of (e.g., Schwar tzman, Gold, Andres, Arbuckle, & Jewish people (20 million) was quite close to the actual Chaikelson, 1987). It is assumed that education figure of 18 million according to the 1990 Universal Al- (which is received early in life) in part determines manac, the number of estimated Moslemsa mean of the extent and quality of many intellectual activities 10 millionwas startlingly low (817 million is the esti- later in life. And it is presumably this intellectual ac- mate in the Universal Almanac). For each participant tivity as one ages that is so crucial to the preserva- in our study, we calculated the ratio of the Moslem to tion of cognitive capacities. Thus, while considerable Jewish estimates to see how many students were aware development of cognitive skills and abilities can re- of the fact that the number of Moslems is an order of sult from formal educational experiences, it is the magnitude larger (the actual estimated ratio is approxi- lifetime use of these skills that is assumed to have mately 33:1 according to the World Almanac; 45:1 ac- the beneficial effect. cording to the Universal Almanac). The median ratio In this study, Stanovich, et al. (1995) examined the in our sample was 0.5.That is, 69.3 percent of our sam- performance of college students and senior citizens ple thought that there were more Jewish people in the on general knowledge, vocabulary, working memory, world than Moslems. syllogistic reasoning, and several measures of reading This level of inaccuracy is startling given that ap- volume. The older individuals outperformed the col- proximately 40 percent of our sample of 268 students lege students on the measures of general knowledge were attending one of the most selective public insti- and vocabulary, but did significantly less well than tutions of higher education in the United States (the the college subjects on the working memory and syl- University of California, Berkeley). We have explored logistic reasoning tasks. This dissociation between the correlates of this particular misconception in a va- fluid intelligence (all-purpose general problem-solv- riety of ways. We looked at the performance on this ing capacity) and crystallized intelligence (general question as a function of students level of reading vol- knowledge and vocabulary) is a standard finding in ume and television watching. We observed a clear ef- the literature (Baltes, 1987; Horn & Hofer, 1992; Salt- fect of reading volume on the scores on the question house, 1988). However, a series of analyses indicated and a significant effect of television viewing, but the ef- that when measures of reading volume were used as fects were in opposite directions! Reading volume was control variables, the positive relationships between
SPRING/SUMMER 1998 AMERICAN EDUCATOR/AMERICAN FEDERATION OF TEACHERS 6
age and vocabulary and age and declarative knowl- eleventh-grade reading comprehension ability had edge were eliminated (in contrast, the negative rela- been partialed out! In contrast, we observed that first- tionships between age and fluid abilities were largely grade intelligence measures do not uniquely predict unchanged). Thus, the results of this study are consis- eleventh-grade reading volume in the same way. Thus, tent with the conjecture thatin the domain of ver- this study showed us that an early start in reading is bal abilitiesreading a lot can even help to compen- important in predicting a lifetime of literacy experi- sate for the normally deleterious effects of aging! enceand this is true regardless of the level of read- (See also, Smith, 1996.) ing comprehension ability that the individual eventu- ally attains. This is a stunning finding because it means that stu- How Do We Become dents who get off to a fast start in reading are more Avid Readers? likely to read more over the years, and, furthermore, this very act of reading can help children compensate Moving back again to the other end of the age spec- for modest levels of cognitive ability by building their trum, we switch focus to the question: Given that life- vocabularly and general knowledge. In other words, long reading habits are such strong predictors of ver- ability is not the only variable that counts in the devel- bal cognitive growth, what is it that predicts these opment of intellectual functioning. Those who read a habits? Weve been looking at reading volume as a pre- lot will enhance their verbal intelligence; that is, read- dictor of reading comprehension and cognitive ability, ing will make them smarter. but what predicts reading volume or avid reading? It is generally agreed that comprehension ability and reading volume are in a reciprocal relationship. In an The Reciprocal Effects of attempt to tease apart this reciprocal relationship, we explored the linkages between childrens first-grade Reading Volume reading and cognitive abilities and eleventh-grade out- We can begin to sketch a view of the reciprocal in- comes in a unique ten-year longitudinal study (Cun- fluences of early reading acquisition and reading vol- ningham and Stanovich, 1997). Most of our earlier stud- ume as determinants of later reading comprehension ies involved assessing contemporaneous relations, but and other cognitive abilities. Early success at reading in this study, we examined the performance of a sam- acquisition is one of the keys that unlocks a lifetime of ple of students who had been tested as first graders reading habits. The subsequent exercise of this habit (see Stanovich, Cunningham, and Feeman, 1984). serves to further develop reading comprehension abil- About one half of these students were available ten ity in an interlocking positive feedback logic (Juel, Grif- years later for testing as eleventh graders. At this time, fith, & Gough, 1986; Juel, 1988; Snow, Barnes, Chandler, we administered a set of reading comprehension, cog- Goodman, & Hemphill, 1991; Stanovich, 1986, 1993).Al- nitive ability, vocabulary, and general knowledge tasks, though it is difficult to tease apart, we have attempted as well as several measures of reading volume. Addi- to trace the increasing divergence in childrens reading tionally, some standardized test scores from the inter- ability, as well as other cognitive outcomes, by examin- vening period were available. We were therefore able ing both sides of the important role of reciprocal cau- to examine what variables in the first grade predicted sation. Our longitudinal study has permitted us to ob- these cognitive outcomes in the eleventh grade.We in- serve these effects, whereby children who get out of terpreted the reading volume measures administered the gate quicklywho crack the spelling-to-sound in the eleventh grade as cumulative indicators of vari- code early onappear to enter into a positive feed- ance in reading volume that had taken place many back loop. One of the benefits of these reciprocating years earlier.Thus, we viewed the measures as in some effects may be a level of participation in literacy activi- sense retrospective indicators tapping the cumulative ties that leads to a lifetime habit of reading and thus experiences and habits of the students some distance sets the stage for future opportunitiesopportunities in time before actual assessment. As a result, we were not enjoyed by children who enter into this feedback able to examine how far this retrospective feature loop more slowly. could be stretched. A positive dimension of our research is that all of We addressed the question of whether the speed of our studies have demonstrated that reading yields sig- initial reading acquisition in the first grade could pre- nificant dividends for everyonenot just for the dict later tendencies to engage in reading activities smart kids or the more able readers. Even the child even after differences in general cognitive abilities with limited reading and comprehension skills will were controlled, as some models of Matthew effects in build vocabulary and cognitive structures through educational achievement would predict (Chall, Jacobs, reading. & Baldwin, 1990; Juel, 1994; Stanovich, 1986).We statis- We can thus elicit two crucial messages from our re- tically removed the contribution of eleventh-grade search findings. First, it is difficult to overstate the im- reading comprehension ability, in order to remove the portance of getting children off to an early successful direct association between reading volume and cur- start in reading. We must ensure that students decod- rent reading ability. Then we examined the contribu- ing and word recognition abilities are progressing tion of three standardized measures of first grade read- solidly. Those who read well are likely to read more, ing ability (decoding, word recognition, and compre- thus setting an upward spiral into motion. hension) and observed that all three measures pre- Second, we should provide all children, regardless of dicted eleventh-grade reading volume even after their achievement levels, with as many reading experi-
7 AMERICAN EDUCATOR/AMERICAN FEDERATION OF TEACHERS SPRING/SUMMER 1998
ences as possible. Indeed, this becomes doubly impera- Horn, J. L., & Hofer, S. (1992). Major abilities and development in the tive for precisely those children whose verbal abilities adult period. In R. J. Sternberg & C.A. Berg (Eds.), Intellectual devel- opment (pp. 44-99). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. are most in need of bolstering, for it is the very act of Juel, C. (1988). Learning to read and write:A longitudinal study of fifty- reading that can build those capacities.An encouraging four children from first through fourth grade. Journal of Educa- message for teachers of low-achieving students is im- tional Psychology, 80, 437-447. plicit here. We often despair of changing our students Juel, C. (1994). Learning to read and write in one elementary school. New York: Springer-Verlag. abilities, but there is at least one partially malleable Juel, C., Griffith, P. L., & Gough, P. B. (1986). Acquisition of literacy: A habit that will itself develop abilitiesreading! l longitudinal study of children in first and second grade. Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 243-255. REFERENCES Kaestle, C. F. (1991). Literacy in the United States. New Haven, CT.: Yale Press. Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Miller, G. A., & Gildea, P. M. (1987). How children learn words. Scien- Allington, R. L. (1977). If they dont read much, how they ever gonna tific American, 257(3), 94-99. get good? Journal of Reading, 21, 57-61. Nagy, W. E., & Anderson, R. C. (1984). How many words are there in Allington, R. L. (1983).The reading instruction provided readers of dif- printed school English? Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 304-330. fering reading abilities. The Elementary School Journal, 83, 548- Nagy, W. E., Herman, P. A. (1987). Breadth and depth of vocabulary 559. knowledge: Implications for acquisition and instruction. In M. G. Allington, R. L. (1984). Content coverage and contextual reading in McKeown & M. E. Curtis (Eds.), The nature of vocabulary acquisi- reading groups. Journal of Reading Behavior, 16, 85-96. tion (pp. 19-35). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Anderson, R. C.,Wilson, P.T., & Fielding, L. G. (1988). Growth in reading Nagy,W. E., Herman, P.A. & Anderson, R. C. (1985). Learning words from and how children spend their time outside of school. Reading Re- context. Reading Research Quarterly, 20, 233-253. search Quarterly, 23, 285-303. Olson, D. R. (1986). Intelligence and literacy: The relationships be- Baltes, P. B. (1987).Theoretical propositions of life-span developmental tween intelligence and the technologies of representation and com- psychology: On the dynamics between growth and decline. Devel- munication. In R. J. Sternberg & R. K.Wagner (Eds.), Practical intel- opmental Psychology, 23, 611-626. ligence (pp. 338-360). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Berger, K. W. (1977). The most common 100,000 words used in con- Salthouse,T.A. (1988). Resource-reduction interpretations of cognitive versations. Kent, Ohio: Herald Publishing House. aging. Developmental Review, 8, 238-272. Biemiller,A. (1977-1978). Relationships between oral reading rates for let- Schwartzman, A., Gold, D., Andres, D., Arbuckle, T., & Chaikelson, J. ters, words, and simple text in the development of reading achieve- (1987). Stability of intelligence:A 40-year follow-up. Canadian Jour- ment. Reading Research Quarterly, 13, 223-253. nal of Psychology, 41, 244-256. Brown, G. D. (1984). A frequency count of 190,000 words in the Lon- Smith, F. (1989). Overselling literacy. Phi Delta Kappan, 70, 5, 354. don-Lund Corpus of English Conversation. Behavior Research Smith, M.C. (1996). Differences in adults reading practices and liter- Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 16 502-532. acy proficiencies. Reading Research Quarterly, 31, 196-219. Carroll, J. B., Davies, P., & Richman, B. (1971). Word frequency book. Snow, C. E., Barnes, W. S., Chandler, J., Goodman, I., & Hemphill, L. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. (1991). Unfulfilled expectations: Home and school influences on Chall, J. S. (1983). Stages of reading development. New York: McGraw- literacy. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Hill. Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some conse- Chall. J.S. (1989) Learning to read:The great debate twenty years later. quences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. A response to Debunking the great phonics myth. Phi Delta Kap- Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 360-407. pan. 71, 521-538. Stanovich, K. E. (1989). Has the learning disabilities field lost its intelli- Chall, J. S., Jacobs, V., & Baldwin, L. (1990). The reading crisis: Why gence? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22, 487-492. poor children fall behind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Stanovich, K. E. (1993). Does reading make you smarter? Literacy and Press. the development of verbal intelligence. In H. Reese (Ed.), Advances Cipielewski, J., & Stanovich, K. E. (1992). Predicting growth in reading in child development and behavior, 24, 133-180. San Diego, CA: ability from childrens exposure to print. Journal of Experimental Academic Press. Child Psychology, 54, 74-89. Stanovich, K. E., & Cunningham, A. E. (1992). Studying the conse- Cunningham,A. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (1990).Assessing print exposure quences of literacy within a literate society:The cognitive correlates and orthographic processing skill in children: A quick measure of of print exposure. Memory & Cognition, 20, 51-68. reading experience. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 733- Stanovich, K. E., & Cunningham, A. E. (1993). Where does knowledge 740. come from? Specific associations between print exposure and in- Cunningham, A. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (1991). Tracking the unique ef- formation acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 211- fects of print exposure in children: Associations with vocabulary, 229. general knowledge, and spelling. Journal of Educational Psychol- Stanovich, K. E., Cunningham, A. E., & Feeman, D. J. (1984). Intelli- ogy, 83, 264-274. Cunningham, A.E., & Stanovich, K. E. (1997). Early reading acquisition gence, cognitive skills, and early reading progress. Reading Re- and its relation to reading experience and ability 10 years later. De- search Quarterly, 19, 278-303. velopmental Psychology, 33, 6, 934-945. Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1989). Exposure to print and ortho- Echols, L. D.,West, R. F., Stanovich, K. E., & Zehr, K. S. (1996). Using chil- graphic processing. Reading Research Quarterly, 24, 402-433. drens literacy activities to predict growth in verbal cognitive skills: Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F., & Harrison, M. (1995). Knowledge A longitudinal investigation. Journal of Educational Psychology, growth and maintenance across the life span: The role of print ex- 88, 296-304. posure. Developmental Psychology, 31, 811-826. Francis, W. N., & Kucera, H. (1982). Frequency analysis of English Sternberg, R. J. (1985). Beyond IQ: A triarchic theory of human intel- usage: Lexicon and grammar. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. ligence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Gambrell, L. B.,Wilson, R. M., & Gantt,W. N. (1981). Classroom observa- Sternberg, R. J. (1987). Most vocabulary is learned from context. In M. G. tions of task-attending behaviors of good and poor readers. Journal McKeown & M. E. Curtis (Eds.), The nature of vocabulary acquisi- of Educational Research, 74, 400-404. tion (pp. 89-105). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates. Guthrie, J.T., Schafer,W. D. & Hutchinson, S. R. (1991). Relation of docu- Sticht,T. (1979). Applications of the audread model to reading evalua- ment literacy and prose literacy to occupational and societal char- tion and instruction. In L. B. Resnick & P.Weaver (Eds.), Theory and acteristics of young black and white adults. Reading Research practice of early reading (pp. 209-226). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Asso- Quarterly, 26, 30-48. ciates. Hayes, D. P. (1988). Speaking and writing: Distinct patterns of word Walberg, H. J., & Tsai, S. (1983). Matthew effects in education. Ameri- choice. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 572-585. can Educational Research Journal, 20, 359-373. Hayes, D. P., & Ahrens, M. (1988). Vocabulary simplification for chil- West, R. F., & Stanovich, K. E. (1991).The incidental acquisition of infor- dren:A special case of motherese. Journal of Child Language, 15, mation from reading. Psychological Science, 2, 325-330. 395-410. West, R. F., Stanovich, K. E., & Mitchell, H. R. (1993). Reading in the real Hall, V. C., Chiarello, K., & Edmondson, B. (1996). Deciding where world and its correlates. Reading Research Quarterly, 28, 34-50. knowledge comes from depends on where you look. Journal of Ed- Zill, N., & Winglee, W. (1990). Who reads literature? Cabin John, MD: ucational Psychology, 88, 305-313. Seven Locks Press. SPRING/SUMMER 1998 AMERICAN EDUCATOR/AMERICAN FEDERATION OF TEACHERS 8
Future Forward: Leadership Lessons from Patrick McGovern, the Visionary Who Circled the Globe and Built a Technology Media Empire: Leadership Lessons from Patrick McGovern, the Visionary Who Circled the Globe and Built a Technology Media Empire
Effective Decision Making – The Best Strategies for Sovereign Leaders: With Bonus – Quick & Easy become & stay a Boss with Decision Management & Sovereign Decision-Making Processes