Applying Self-Determination Theory To Organizational Research
Applying Self-Determination Theory To Organizational Research
Applying Self-Determination Theory To Organizational Research
2
3
4
5
APPLYING SELF-DETERMINATION
6
7
THEORY TO ORGANIZATIONAL
8
9
RESEARCH
10
11
12 Kennon M. Sheldon, Daniel B. Turban, Kenneth
13
14
G. Brown, Murray R. Barrick and Timothy A. Judge
15
16
ABSTRACT
17
18
In this paper we argue that self-determination theory (SDT; Deci & Ryan,
19
2000) provides a useful conceptual tool for organizational researchers, one
20
that complements traditional work motivation theories. First, we review SDT,
21
showing that it has gone far beyond the intrinsic versus extrinsic motiva-
22
tion dichotomy with which it began. Then we show how the theory might be
23
applied to better understand a variety of organizational phenomena, includ-
24
ing the positive effects of transformational leadership, the nature of true
25
goal-commitment, the determinants of employees motivation to learn, and
26
the positive impact of certain human resource practices. We note that SDT
27
may yield signicant new understanding of work motivation, and suggest
28
opportunities to rene the theory for research on work-related phenomena.
29
30
31 INTRODUCTION
32
33 Questions regarding what dispositional and situational factors lead employees
34 to learn, perform, and be satisfied at work are enduring themes in organizational
35
36
37 Research in Personnel and Human Resources Management
Research in Personnel and Human Resources Management, Volume 22, 357393
38 Copyright 2003 by Elsevier Science Ltd.
39 All rights of reproduction in any form reserved
40 ISSN: 0742-7301/doi:10.1016/S0742-7301(03)22008-9
357
358 KENNON M. SHELDON ET AL.
1 research. Many motivation theories have been brought to bear on these questions,
2 including expectancy theory (Van Erde & Thierry, 1996; Vroom, 1964), goal
3 setting theory (Locke & Latham, 1999), and self-efficacy theory (Bandura, 1997;
4 Stajkovic & Luthans, 1998). Reviews of motivational research on organizationally
5 relevant dependent variables such as job performance and training outcomes
6 suggest the continued dominance of these theories (Locke, 2000; Mathieu &
7 Martineau, 1997), despite the much broader array of motivation theories available
8 within the psychological literature (Higgins & Kruglanski, 2000).
9 One such theory, which has had a substantial influence on research in domains
10 such as health, education, and social psychology, is Self-Determination Theory
11 (SDT). Recent commentators state that SDT is an impressive accomplishment
12 (Psyszczynski, Greenberg & Solomon, 2000, p. 301), provides new impetus to
13 research on human motivation (Coleman, 2000, p. 291), and may be the most
14 ambitious contribution to what some have termed the rebirth of motivational re-
15 search (Hennessey, 2000, p. 293). However, organizational scholars have been
16 relatively slow to apply the theory, perhaps because of a lack of understanding of
17 SDTs current formulation.1 Thus, the purpose of this paper is to describe new
18 developments within SDT, and to show how the theory might be applied to some
19 enduring research themes with organizational research.
20 SDT is grounded in the organismic perspective upon human nature and
21 motivation (Angyal, 1941; Goldstein, 1939; Rogers, 1961; Werner, 1957). The
22 organismic perspective is a long-standing one within psychology, philosophy,
23 and theoretical biology, which can walk a line between general systems and
24 cognitive-developmental approaches, on the one hand, and humanistic and
25 existential approaches, on the other. Organismic perspectives assume that humans
26 are inherently motivated to develop their interests and skills, to connect and
27 contribute to other people, and to move towards their fullest potential; in other
28 words, the energy and impulse to grow and develop are innate. However, this
29 perspective also asserts that the growth impulse is easily derailed or distorted,
30 if environments or peoples own inner processes do not support it. Thus, much
31 of the empirical and experimental work in SDT has focused on delineating what
32 characteristics of intrapersonal, social and task environments enhance or detract
33 from the desire to grow and develop, and thus enhance or detract from positive
34 outcomes such as persistence, creativity, flexibility, well-being, and happiness.2
35 Ryan and Deci (2001) noted that organismic motivation theories make
36 somewhat different assumptions about human nature than do traditional hedonic
37 motivation theories (such as expectancy theories and utility theories). Although
38 a full discussion of such differences is beyond the scope of this paper (see Ryan
39 & Deci, 2001), hedonic theories generally assume that individuals are motivated
40
Applying Self-Determination Theory to Organizational Research 359
1 to maximize pleasure and reward and to minimize pain and cost. In contrast,
2 SDT focuses on peoples motivation to grow, both in terms of developing their
3 potential, and in terms of enhancing their connection with others and their
4 community. We believe that both types of theory, while on the surface appearing
5 somewhat contradictory, are useful in understanding the full range of work
6 behavior. However, given the relative inattention to SDT in the organizational
7 literature, we believe it is worthy of attention in this paper.
8 Self-determination theory is a macro-theory consisting of several mini-theories:
9 Cognitive Evaluation Theory, Organismic Integration Theory, Causality Ori-
10 entations Theory, and Basic Needs Theory. Below we will cover each of the
11 four mini-theories within SDT, thereby both providing a historical perspective
12 and bringing readers up-to-date on the theory as it now stands. Subsequently,
13 we will offer an overarching conceptual framework that incorporates all four
14 mini-theories into a single process model of goal-directed behavior. Afterwards,
15 we will consider the applicability of this process model for several important areas
16 of organizational research, namely transformational leadership, goal commitment,
17 motivation to learn, and strategic human resource management. The variety of
18 topics addressed is intended to suggest that SDT has applicability both for
19 refining study on particular constructs (e.g. goal commitment and motivation
20 to learn), and for expanding the focus of research on broader organizational
21 processes (e.g. leadership and human resource practices). The selection of
22 topics is not intended to be exhaustive but instead is representative of some
23 important areas in organizational research where SDT may be fruitfully applied.
24 Finally, we will discuss some potential limitations and boundary conditions for
25 the theory.
26
27
28 SELF-DETERMINATION THEORY
29
30 SDT began in the late 1960s with the pioneering work of Edward Deci, who
31 explored the conditions that can undermine intrinsic motivation (i.e. the desire
32 to engage in an activity because one enjoys, or is interested in, the activity). Using
33 a free choice methodology, in which intrinsic motivation is operationalized as
34 the number of seconds spent doing an appealing target activity after being left
35 alone, Deci found many factors that can undermine intrinsic motivation. These
36 include certain types of performance-contingent rewards, time pressures, threats
37 of punishment, and certain types of competition. These experimental results (and
38 other supporting survey and field data) were summarized in a theory that became
39 known as Cognitive Evaluation Theory.
40
360 KENNON M. SHELDON ET AL.
1 they can successfully perform the behavior. However, SDT proposes that it is also
2 important to address the quality of a persons motivation, an issue that is typically
3 not considered within expectancy and utility theories. As argued at the beginning
4 of this paper, we believe such perspectives may provide a useful complement to
5 traditional work-motivation theories.
6 Originally, the consideration of quality went no further than the distinction
7 between intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. However, SDT now specifies three types
8 of extrinsic motivation (the three middle forms in Fig. 1), that vary in their degree
9 of self-determination: external motivation (acting only to get a reward, or because
10 the external situation seems to compel or require action), introjected motivation
11 (acting to avoid feeling anxious or guilty over not doing what one should do),
12 and identied motivation (acting to express ones values and uphold ones self-
13 investments). These are all classified as extrinsic motivations because they do not
14 involve engaging in the activity for its own sake (i.e. they are not intrinsically
15 motivated). However, whereas external and introjected motivations are classified
16 as non-autonomous or controlled motivations, identified motivation is classified as
17 autonomous because people feel fully self-endorsing of the behavior, even if they
18 do not enjoy it.
19 Thus, contemporary SDT asserts that some forms of extrinsic motivation can
20 indeed be autonomous and organismically integrated, if the person identifies
21 with them. For example, an employee may engage in a work behavior (such
22 as assembling a computer) primarily to earn money or to not be punished by
23 a supervisor (external motivation), primarily to avoid feeling guilty or to avoid
24 being a bad worker (introjected motivation), or primarily because of a genuine
25 identification with her role in the company, and a real concern for the customers
26 need for a quality computer (identified motivation, which has been integrated
27 into the persons sense of self). In none of these examples would assembling a
28 computer be intrinsically enjoyable, but in the third case, it is at least tolerable and
29 even meaningful! Doubtless, the reader can think of many similar examples, both
30 in his/her own work-life, and in the problems faced by managers in motivating
31 their employees. Thus, an important question arises what factors lead employees
32 to feel a sense of identification with their work behaviors, especially when those
33 behaviors are tedious or even aversive to perform?
34 Relevant to this question, an additional development within SDT in the late
35 1980s was the concept of internalization. Although the concept of internalization
36 is not new within psychological theory (see, for example, Erikson, 1963, or
37 Kelman, 1961), SDT has provided perhaps the most elaborated account of the
38 process. Specifically, Deci and Ryan (1985a, 2000) posited that non-autonomous
39 motivations can be transmuted into autonomous ones over time (i.e. internal-
40 ization can occur). Furthermore, Deci and Ryan argued that this process tends
364 KENNON M. SHELDON ET AL.
1 research twenty to twenty-five years ago (Ambrose & Kulik, 1999). There are
2 several differences, the most important one being that SDT does not assume any
3 hierarchical relation among the three needs. Instead, everybody needs all three
4 kinds of experiences, to an approximately equal extent, all the time. Thus, SDT
5 does not assume strong individual, cultural, or developmental differences in the
6 needs, although their means of being satisfied and expressed may certainly differ
7 between individuals, cultures, or ages. Work thus far supports these assumptions.
8 For example, in the domain of cross-cultural psychology, Sheldon, Ryan, Elliot,
9 Kim, Chirkov, Demir and Wu (2002) recently found that having autonomous
10 motivation for ones personal goal-pursuits (i.e. to be self-concordant; Sheldon,
11 2002) predicts positive well-being in Turkey, Russia, Taiwan, China, and South
12 Korea, as well as in the U.S. In the domain of developmental psychology, Sheldon
13 and Kasser (2001) recently showed that goal-autonomy predicts well-being in
14 people of all ages. Interestingly, Sheldon and Kasser (2001) also showed that older
15 people were more autonomous in their goals. That is, consistent with organismic
16 integration theory, people tend to better internalize their own strivings over time.
17 Given the importance of need satisfaction for SDT, an important question is:
18 what characteristics of social, academic and work environments best support
19 psychological need-satisfaction? In accordance with basic needs theory, three
20 factors are theorized to result in need satisfaction: relationship support, compe-
21 tence support, and autonomy support. In other words, a boss, coach, parent, or
22 teacher who is trying to motivate an individual should try to help that person to
23 feel competent in the behavior by expressing confidence in the persons abilities,
24 providing encouragement, and providing appropriate material and task support;
25 should help the person feel related to the motivator, by evidencing genuine concern
26 for his/her thoughts and feelings and by empathizing; and should help the person
27 feel autonomous in the behavior, by helping him or her to endorse and own the
28 task, even if he/she does not enjoy it. Because it is most controversial, most prior
29 SDT research has focused on the characteristics and effects of autonomy-support.
30 Thus we consider autonomy-support in greater detail below, and also later in
31 the paper.
32 As demonstrated by Deci, Eghrari, Patrick and Leone (1994), autonomy support
33 has at least three components: taking the persons perspective upon the situation,
34 giving as much choice as possible, and providing a meaningful rationale when
35 choice-provision is not possible. Specifically, Deci et al. (1994) showed that when
36 all three factors were present, people were most likely to spontaneously continue
37 doing the boring task of pressing a spacebar whenever a light appeared, after
38 the tasks formal completion. To take a work-related example: a supervisor might
39 need an employee to check spreadsheets for accuracy of data entered. Although the
40 employee does not have a choice about whether or not to do it, the supervisor can
368 KENNON M. SHELDON ET AL.
1 give the employee some choice about how to do it, when to do it, and perhaps with
2 whom to do it. In addition, the supervisor can be sympathetic to the subordinates
3 perspective (I know this may not always seem like fun, because I can remember
4 having to do this myself), and explain why it is so important (If the data arent
5 accurate then the analyses will be wrong, which will damage the company). In
6 this case the employee is most likely to own the task, so that he/she might even
7 work on it over the weekend without being asked, if the company needs it.
8 Considerable evidence, much of it from educational contexts, indicates that such
9 autonomy-support helps maintain and enhance intrinsic motivation, and also helps
10 to promote quicker and deeper internalization of formerly extrinsically-motivated
11 behaviors (Ryan & Stiller, 1991). Focusing on work settings, Hackman and Oldham
12 (1976) argued that workers would experience more internal work motivation when
13 the job provided greater autonomy. Indeed, a meta-analysis of studies investigating
14 Hackman and Oldhams job characteristics model found a corrected correlation
15 of 0.42 between jobs providing autonomy and internal work motivation (Fried
16 & Ferris, 1987). In addition, some studies applying self-determination theory in
17 work contexts have also found that autonomy support is important in such contexts
18 (Deci, Connell & Ryan, 1989; Deci, Ryan, Gagne, Leone, Usunov & Kornazheva,
19 2001; Hackman & Oldham, 1976). We will consider such work in more detail in
20 the final section.
21
22
23 Bringing the Four Mini-Theories Together: An Integrated Process Model
24
25 We have now discussed the four mini-theories that comprise the current state of
26 SDT and have touched upon some of the research that has investigated SDT (see
27 Deci & Ryan, 2000; Ryan & Deci, 2000). Figure 2 provides a summary causal
28 model of how the various pieces of SDT currently fit together. As shown in the
29 model, both contextual factors and personality factors are theorized to influence
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40 Fig. 2. SDTs General Casual-Process Model.
Applying Self-Determination Theory to Organizational Research 369
1 the extent to which individuals internalize goals and tasks. More specifically,
2 individuals who receive greater autonomy support from the environmental
3 context are theorized to be more likely to internalize goals and tasks. Similarly,
4 individuals with an autonomous personality style are more likely to have inter-
5 nalized motivation when performing a goal or task. As a result of the internalized
6 motivation, individuals are likely to derive positive momentary feelings of
7 autonomy, competence, and relatedness from doing the task. Such feelings satisfy
8 the organism and give access to full cognitive and motivational resources, thus
9 leading to a variety of positive outcomes, including performance, creativity, and
10 psychological well-being. Much recent work supports this general ordering of
11 factors and processes, in the domains of medicine, sports, parenting, education,
12 politics, religion, and intimate relationships, although there are sometimes direct
13 effects in addition to the mediated effects depicted in Fig. 2.
14 It is also worth mentioning that empirical evidence does not support the
15 proposition that control-oriented participants, who report a stronger preference
16 for structure and direction, benefit from being in treated in controlling ways. In
17 terms of Fig. 2, causality orientations do not interact with autonomy-supportive
18 versus controlling environmental characteristics to predict outcomes. Rather, the
19 evidence indicates that although control-oriented employees may in some ways
20 feel more comfortable being treated as a pawn (DeCharms, 1968), even they
21 benefit if they are instead treated as self-creating, self-responsible agents. Stated
22 more broadly, the evidence indicates that all individuals benefit when they are
23 allowed to fulfill their universal need for autonomy, as proposed by SDT (Deci &
24 Ryan, 2000; Sheldon, Joiner & Williams, in press).
25 This concludes our brief overview of contemporary SDT (for further infor-
26 mation, readers also may consult the SDT website, http://www.psych.rochester.
27 edu/SDT/, where they may also gain access to measures often used in SDT
28 research). A question that readers may have at this point is, how does SDT
29 explicitly differ from the motivation theories that are often used to study behavior
30 in organizations? Although a full answer to this question is beyond the scope
31 of this paper (see Deci & Ryan, 2000, for one view), it is worth noting that we
32 see important differences among expectancy, goal, self-efficacy theories (as the
33 dominant motivational theories in organizational research), and SDT. Perhaps
34 most important are differences in focus and scope that allow SDT to complement
35 the prevailing theories in organizational research.
36 Expectancy, self-efficacy, and goal theories generally focus on understanding
37 and predicting rational, deliberate behaviors (Mitchell & Daniels, 2002). For
38 example, research suggests that expectancy theory is essentially a hedonic
39 decision-making theory that best predicts choice examined from a within-subject
40 perspective (e.g. predicting which choice from a set of options a person will select;
370 KENNON M. SHELDON ET AL.
1 Van Erde & Thierry, 1996). Thus, expectancy theory, and perhaps by extension
2 the other theories, may be most useful for understanding and predicting specific
3 choices that follow deliberate reflection. In contrast, SDT may be best suited for
4 predicting and understanding what Mitchell and Daniels (2002) refer to as not
5 rational behavior; that is, behaviors that derive from who people are (including
6 traits and dispositions) and what they feel and need, rather than on what they think
7 and believe (p. 236). The study of such behavior is essential not only because
8 of its prevalence, but also because the research offers a window to understanding
9 more than just specific choices. SDT provides a framework to examine broad-
10 based psychological outcomes in organizations, such as commitment, satisfaction,
11 and well-being.
12 With this said, we also believe that SDT is difficult to categorize using the
13 Mitchell and Daniels (2002) framework because, in its current state of theoretical
14 development (i.e. Fig. 2), it describes connections among dispositions, beliefs,
15 needs, feelings, and actions. Thus, its scope is broad enough to cross the boundary
16 of rational and not rational theory. The model offered by SDT integrates dis-
17 position and situational influences on need satisfaction, and links need satisfaction
18 to affect (e.g., satisfaction) and behavior (e.g. learning and performance).
19 The remainder of the paper will demonstrate the breadth of SDT by applying
20 it to various issues and domains within organizational research. Specifically, we
21 apply SDT to the following, notably quite different, organizational phenomena:
22 transformational leadership, goal commitment, training motivation, and high-
23 performance human resource practices. Our intent in discussing such a wide
24 range of organizational phenomena is to demonstrate the potentially far-reaching
25 applicability of SDT and thereby, hopefully, to stimulate additional theorizing and
26 research in other domains. As we discuss the organizational phenomena, we will
27 attempt to apply three key SDT concepts: (1) the internalization continuum, as
28 we consider causes and outcomes of peoples ability to own their work-tasks;
29 (2) autonomy supportive contexts, as we consider what managerial styles and
30 behaviors best help people to become more self-directing in their lives; and (3)
31 need satisfaction, as we consider what kinds of work environments and reward
32 structures lead to maximal satisfaction, performance, and thriving.
33
34
35 APPLICATIONS IN ORGANIZATIONAL RESEARCH
36
37 Transformational Leadership
38
39 In the past two decades, more research has been conducted on transformational
40 or charismatic leadership than on all of the other major theories of leadership
Applying Self-Determination Theory to Organizational Research 371
1 Elliot, 1998, 1999; Sheldon & Kasser, 1995, 1998, 2001), similar to evidence
2 from goal setting theory that goal commitment is positively related to performance
3 (Klein et al., 1999).
4 There is an important difference in these literatures, however. Although
5 organizational goal-setting researchers have tended to assume that individuals
6 are automatically committed to self-set goals (Klein et al., 1999), Sheldon and
7 colleagues have found variability in the extent to which individuals internalize
8 self-set personal goals (Sheldon & Elliot, 1998, 1999; Sheldon & Houser-Marko,
9 2001; Sheldon & Kasser, 1998). For example, Sheldon and his colleagues asked
10 subjects to list goals they were pursuing and found that some individuals reported
11 pursuing self-set personal goals for non-autonomous reasons (i.e. because they felt
12 others wanted them to), indicating that not all self-set goals are felt as autonomous.
13 In other words, just because self-set goals are nominally self-determined does
14 not mean that they feel phenomenologically self-determined. As noted earlier
15 in the section on the organismic integration sub-theory of SDT, people do not
16 necessarily feel full ownership of their motivated behavior.
17 This reasoning suggests that commitment to self-set goals is affected not only by
18 attractiveness and value, but also by the extent the person feels internally motivated
19 to pursue the goal. Simply put, individuals who felt that they freely chose a goal
20 are more committed to the goal than individuals who felt that they chose a goal
21 because of external pressures. Interestingly, results from a recent meta-analysis
22 of the organizational literature indicated that one of the strongest antecedents of
23 goal commitment was felt volition (Klein et al., 1999); the corrected correlation
24 was 0.40, which is somewhat stronger than the corrected correlations found for
25 expectancy and attractiveness.
26 The above reasoning suggests that one way to improve the prediction of positive
27 outcomes would be to measure peoples degree of internalization of goals, in
28 addition to their degree of commitment to such goals. Demonstrating the added
29 value of conducting such an assessment, Sheldon and Elliot (1998, 1999) and
30 Sheldon and Kasser (1998) showed that measured internalization predicted pos-
31 itive downstream effects above and beyond the effects of alternative motivational
32 constructs, including expectancy, commitment, and implementation intentions.
33 In other words, people who strive for autonomous reasons gain motivational
34 resources that cannot be accounted for by conventional utility, expectancy, and
35 plan theories.
36 To formalize and summarize the above, we offer the following proposition:
37
38 Proposition 2. Not all self-set goals feel autonomously chosen. The degree of
39 goal-internalization will predict goal-commitment above and beyond the pre-
40 dictive effects of expectancy and attractiveness.
Applying Self-Determination Theory to Organizational Research 375
1 Motivation to Learn
2
3 Motivation in the training and development literature is often captured with
4 the construct motivation to learn, which has been found to predict employees
5 learning from training and development opportunities (for a meta-analytic review,
6 see Colquitt, LePine & Noe, 2000). As defined by Hicks and Klimoski (1987) and
7 (Noe, 1986; Noe & Schmitt, 1986), motivation to learn is an employees desire
8 to learn the content of a particular program. Conceptually, the motivation to learn
9 construct has been rooted in expectancy theory. That is, employees motivation
10 to learn has been theorized to be largely determined by their assessments of the
11 usefulness of mastering training content for obtaining valued outcomes at work
12 (Mathieu, Tannenbaum & Salas, 1992; Noe, 1986; Tharenou, 2001). Although
13 this perspective has been useful for empirically validating the role of motivation in
14 learning, it has also limited the motivational research questions posed by training
15 researchers. More specifically, the emphasis on a single, aggregate assessment
16 of motivation does not fully capture the nature of learners personal goals (i.e.
17 variety of goals they hope to accomplish and, perhaps more importantly with
18 regard to internalization, why), nor does it lend itself to detailed study of instructor
19 and manager behaviors that may influence these goals. Each issue is addressed
20 by SDT, although to date SDT has been neglected in training and development
21 research. In fact, reviews of the training motivation literature suggest a total
22 absence of research on the internalization of learners goals and on the influence
23 that instructors and managers have on learners needs satisfaction (Colquitt et al.,
24 2000; Mathieu & Martineau, 1997; Noe, Wilk, Mullen & Wanek, 1997). Each of
25 these issues is explored below along with propositions derived from SDT.
26
27 Learners Internalized Goals
28 Research on motivation to learn typically focuses on the content of training as
29 the determinant of motivation. Thus, measures of motivation to learn contain
30 statements like I have a strong desire to learn the content of this program (e.g.
31 Noe & Schmitt, 1986). Such an approach does not acknowledge that employees
32 come to a learning experience with goals that may only overlap partially with
33 program content (such as having a goal to learn a small portion of the training
34 content) and may not deal with training content at all (such as having a goal to
35 impress colleagues and/or the instructor). Although there are emerging streams of
36 organizational research on goals and learning that begin to address these questions
37 (e.g. Brett & VandeWalle, 1999; Ford, Smith, Weissbein, Gully & Salas, 1998;
38 Kozlowski, Gully, Brown, Salas, Smith & Nason, 2001a), we found no organiza-
39 tional research that has investigated the degree to which learners have internalized
40 different types goals related to training. Educational research, however, clearly
376 KENNON M. SHELDON ET AL.
1 suggests that higher quality learning occurs when learners experience internalized
2 motivation (Rigby, Deci, Patrick & Ryan, 1992). More recent educational research
3 directly demonstrates the learning benefit of having internalized reasons for being
4 in a course (Black & Deci, 2000). Thus, SDT theory and research would suggest
5 the following:
6
7 Proposition 3. Trainees with internalized goals for learning training content
8 will exert greater effort toward learning and learn more from training than
9 trainees without such internalized goals.
10
11 Instructor Effects on Learners
12 Research examining motivation in training seldom examines what learners
13 actually do and experience in the training environment (Brown, 2001), and thus
14 it may overlook important motivational phenomena that occur after training
15 begins. Motivation measures are typically administered once, either before
16 (e.g. Quinones, 1995) or after (e.g. Hicks & Klimoski, 1987) training. Such an
17 approach does not allow for an examination of how the training experience may
18 alter learners goals and motivational states. For example, a trainee who starts out
19 with high motivation to learn may quickly lose it. Alternatively, a trainee with low
20 initial motivation may become inspired to learn by a talented or creative teacher.
21 We will focus on the latter effect below because the field knows the least about
22 how trainers inspire others to learn (Towler & Dipboye, 2001).
23 First, let us consider several possible explanations for this inspiration effect
24 from other motivation theories. Expectancy theory suggests that motivation-
25 enhancing teachers succeed by illuminating the connections between training con-
26 tent and valued job outcomes. In other words, they show trainees how learning the
27 training material will help them perform more effectively on the job. Instructional
28 design models that focus on gaining and keeping learner attention often suggest
29 this approach (e.g. Gagne, Briggs & Wager, 1992). Self-efficacy theory suggests
30 that motivation-enhancing teachers succeed by raising learner self-efficacy,
31 perhaps by convincing trainees verbally of their own competence, or perhaps
32 more concretely by providing opportunities to succeed early in training (Gist &
33 Mitchell, 1992; Kozlowski, Toney, Mullins, Weissbein, Brown & Bell, 2001b).
34 In contrast, SDT begins by focusing on learners felt autonomy in doing a task,
35 that is, the extent they feel an internal perceived locus of causality for their own
36 behavior. According to Fig. 2, trainers who support learners autonomy enhance
37 trainees intrinsic and/or identified motivation to learn (i.e. their autonomous
38 work-motivation), thereby helping fulfill trainees psychological needs. As a
39 result, trainees better learn and retain the material. Indeed, the importance of
40 teacher autonomy-support has been confirmed by research on teacher behavior
Applying Self-Determination Theory to Organizational Research 377
1 in educational settings (see Ryan & Stiller, 1991; Sheldon & Biddle, 1998, for
2 reviews). Research suggests that autonomy supportive teachers listen carefully
3 to learners, allow them to learn in their own way, and continually work to engage
4 learners interest (Reeve, Bolt & Cai, 1999).
5 The extent to which such behaviors describe trainers in corporate settings
6 is an open one, in large part because the focus of organizational research has
7 been on learner characteristics and work environment characteristics, rather than
8 on trainer characteristics and behaviors. As suggested by Towler and Dipboye
9 (2001), however, training effectiveness could be much improved by giving greater
10 attention to characteristics and behaviors of trainers. SDT provides a promising
11 framework for conducting such research. Notably, although the research reviewed
12 above focuses on autonomy need-satisfaction, similar positive effects should
13 be expected for satisfying competence and affiliation needs. Needs satisfaction
14 may help to explain the positive results found for certain training programs, such
15 as self-management (Frayne & Geringer, 2000, which may affect competence
16 need-satisfaction) and team training (Salas & Cannon-Bowers, 1997, which may
17 affect relatedness need-satisfaction). Thus, we suggest that the SDT model not
18 only offers new research directions, it also can help to organize what is already
19 known.
20
21 Manager Effects on Learners
22 In addition to shedding light on the nature of effective trainers, SDT can also help
23 illuminate the construct of managerial support for training and development.
24 Organizational research often suggests that managerial support for learning
25 predicts employees motivation for and participation in learning experiences
26 (Baldwin & Magjuka, 1997; Facteau, Dobbins, Russell, Ladd & Kudisch, 1995).
27 And indeed, management support for learning, as rated by subordinates, has been
28 found to be an important predictor of participation in developmental experiences
29 (Birdi, Allan & Warr, 1997; Noe & Wilk, 1993; Tharenou, 2001). Unfortunately,
30 the specific process by which managers promote (or fail to promote) learning by
31 their employees have been little studied. Again, SDT suggests that such managers
32 succeed by supporting their employees autonomy needs, thus helping employees
33 to develop and pursue internalized goals related to learning and personal
34 growth.
35 In sum, SDT offers a perspective seldom employed by training and development
36 research, which has been dominated by the motivation to learn construct and
37 expectancy and self-efficacy theories. In particular, the theory suggests ways of
38 understanding what characteristics of learners, trainers, and managers promote
39 motivation to learn. To formalize the latter issue, the following proposition
40 is offered:
378 KENNON M. SHELDON ET AL.
1 Although the above example focuses on the role of HR practices for promoting
2 acceptance of a firms compensation policies, there are many other issues besides
3 compensation that could be more thoroughly examined, such as self-managed
4 teams and decentralization of decision making and reduced status differentials,
5 to name a few. In general, we postulate that firm performance will be maximized
6 when every HR practice contributes to internalized work-motivation and employee
7 need-satisfaction, or when HR practices are internally consistent with each other
8 (Delery & Shaw, 2001). Such consistency in practices should lead employees to see
9 themselves and their colleagues as owners of their tasks and duties, thus taking full
10 responsibility for the results. To formalize and summarize, we offer the following
11 proposition:
12
13 Proposition 5. High performance HR practices result in greater firm perfor-
14 mance because such practices promote greater employee internalization of work
15 tasks, thereby promoting employee need-satisfaction.
16
17
18 DISCUSSION
19
20 The purpose of this paper has been to explicate contemporary self-determination
21 theory (SDT), and show how it might be applied to substantive research domains
22 within organizational behavior and human resource management. Research on
23 SDT has increased rapidly over the last decade, and the theory is now quite
24 sophisticated and well supported. As we have argued here, SDT provides an
25 alternative, yet complementary, approach to the dominant motivation theories
26 in the management literature, because of its somewhat different assumptions
27 about human nature. In this concluding section we will briefly reiterate the key
28 propositions of SDT, describe the few existing studies that have specifically
29 applied SDT in work domains, and then briefly consider the limitations, boundary
30 conditions, and promises of the theory.
31 Again, SDT is an organismic theory of human motivation, which assumes
32 people have an inherent need to grow and develop, although both contextual
33 and interpersonal factors can inhibit that inherent need. A key aspect of SDT is
34 the internalization process, by which people come to identify with and own
35 less-than-enjoyable tasks. As shown in Fig. 2, both contextual and personal
36 factors are theorized to influence the internalization of such tasks, which subse-
37 quently results in greater satisfaction of the three innate psychological needs of
38 autonomy, competence, and relatedness, which positively impacts outcomes such
39 as persistence, well-being, creativity, and performance. Although this general
40 model has been well supported in the domains of health, educational, and social
Applying Self-Determination Theory to Organizational Research 381
1 surprisingly, these objectives seem quite similar to the three posited SDT needs
2 of autonomy, competence, and relatedness, although Wrzesniewski and Dutton
3 (2001) do not discuss SDT in their article. Clearly, further research is needed to
4 test whether job crafting results in greater internalization of work tasks and thence
5 greater satisfaction of autonomy, competence, and relatedness needs.
6
7
8 Limitations, Boundary Conditions, and Opportunities
9
10 A major limitation of much SDT research for organizational scholars has been
11 SDTs focus on well-being as the primary outcome of interest. Although employee
12 well-being is an important variable, in part because of its relationship with job
13 satisfaction (Judge, Thoresen, Bono & Patton, 2001), organizational scholars are
14 also interested in productivity and performance measures, which are not as often
15 measured by SDT researchers (notably, however, another major outcome focused
16 on by SDT is behavioral persistence, which is likely to be of interest to managers).
17 Other methodological limitations of prior SDT research include cross-sectional
18 designs, limited samples (mostly college students), and self-report measures. Of
19 course, one might also view these limitations as opportunities opportunities for
20 organizational researchers to contribute to the further testing and shaping of an
21 important theory of human motivation.
22 It is also important to consider some possible boundary conditions that may
23 limit the conceptual applicability of SDT to organizational contexts. One such
24 boundary condition may be individual differences in employees needs for growth
25 or self-actualization. Does everyone want to grow and develop? A related bound-
26 ary condition may be individual differences in employees needs for autonomy,
27 competence, and relatedness. Does everyone want to feel effective, connected, and
28 free? In other words, would it do any good to support the growth needs of someone
29 who prefers stability and stasis, or to support the autonomy needs of someone who
30 prefers controls and constraints, or to support the relatedness needs of someone
31 who prefers to be a loner?
32 Although Deci and Ryan (2000) noted that such differences may exist, they
33 do not think that examining individual differences in need strength is the most
34 fruitful place to focus empirical attention (Deci & Ryan, 2000, p. 328). In part,
35 this is due to the fact that past SDT research has not found personality/situation
36 interactions (i.e. as discussed earlier, there is no empirical support for a matching
37 hypothesis, according to which control-oriented participants do better when treated
38 controllingly). Other motivation scholars, however, have argued that examining
39 individual differences in psychological needs may help us better understand many
40 motivational processes (Vallerand, 2000). For example, the job characteristics
384 KENNON M. SHELDON ET AL.
1 model proposes that individuals vary in their growth need strength and that
2 this variability moderates the effects of job characteristics on work outcomes
3 (Hackman & Oldham, 1976). Indeed, meta-analyses support this proposition for
4 the outcomes of job satisfaction (Loher, Noe, Moeller & Fitzgerald, 1985) and job
5 performance (Fried & Ferris, 1987). Such results suggest that Deci and Ryans
6 assumption of invariant psychological needs and growth processes may need
7 further scrutiny.
8 Another conceptual boundary condition that may limit the applicability of SDT
9 to organizations is the inevitably tedious or aversive nature of some jobs, such as
10 peeling shrimp, making cold telemarketing calls, or working on assembly lines.
11 Can humanistic management practices really make a difference in such cases?
12 Again, SDT assumes that the answer is YES, because autonomy support helps
13 people to internalize the doing of boring or tedious tasks (Deci et al., 1994). To
14 show how this can happen, consider a woman with the job of assembling simple
15 rheostats, a job which requires her to produce hundreds of the items every day.
16 This person is not growth-oriented on the job, and she usually prefers the security
17 of being told exactly what she is supposed to do. Also, she has firm expectations
18 that work will always be boring and aversive, an activity that she only does for
19 the money. Still, according to SDT, if this persons immediate supervisor began
20 to talk to her and take her perspective, offer her choices about when and how to
21 do her work, and offer explanatory rationales when making non-ordinary requests
22 (i.e. if the supervisor supported the womans autonomy, rather than commanding,
23 coercing, or controlling her), then the woman might eventually feel better respected
24 and appreciated, and might also learn to take more interest and pride in aspects
25 of the job (i.e. striving for a reduced error rate or increased productivity). Indeed,
26 given autonomy supportive management, the woman might eventually realize that
27 she has potentials that go beyond her current job, and thus move on to more
28 challenging work. In other words, the support of a caring manager might help this
29 woman to re-connect with the growth impulse that SDT assumes is present all
30 people, even though they may be temporarily stalled in a limited way of being.
31 Obviously, more research is required to document these rather optimistic ideas.
32 Another potential limitation of self-determination theory may be its assumption
33 of a motivational continuum, and its emphasis on creating an aggregate self-
34 determination measure that locates participants upon this continuum. Specifically,
35 SDT researchers often create a single measure of self-determined motivation by
36 adding identified and intrinsic motivation, and subtracting external and introjected
37 motivation. As noted earlier, however, some evidence suggests that it is important
38 to differentiate among the different types of motivation (Sansone & Smith, 2000).
39 By keeping the different forms separate one can examine the independent effects
40 of extrinsic and intrinsic motivation, potentially demonstrating that the two types
Applying Self-Determination Theory to Organizational Research 385
1 NOTES
2
3 1. As evidence of this misunderstanding, Ambrose and Kulik (1999) used the term
4 Cognitive Evaluation Theory in their literature review on work motivation. This theory
5 represents only one part of the SDT theoretical framework. Despite their narrow focus,
6 Ambrose and Kulik (1999, p. 257) encouraged further organizational research in this area.
2. Notably, happiness and well-being may be somewhat unfamiliar outcomes for organi-
7 zational researchers. In contemporary social psychology, well-being is typically defined in
8 terms of high positive mood, high life-satisfaction, and low negative mood (Diener, 1984,
9 1994; Sheldon & Elliot, 1999; but see Ryff, 1995, for a different view of well-being). We
10 believe that enhancing employee well-being is a worthy goal for managers, given the many
11 positive cognitive, performance, and coping benefits that accrue from positive well-being
(for a review, see Lyubomirsky, King & Diener, 2003).
12 3. Consistent with other researchers (e.g. House & Shamir, 1993), we use the terms
13 transformational and charismatic leadership interchangeably.
14
15
16
17 REFERENCES
18
19 Amabile, T. (1996). Creativity in context. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
20 Amabile, T. M., Hill, K. G., Hennessey, B. A., & Tighe, E. M. (1994). The work preference inventory:
21 Assessing intrinsic and extrinsic motivational orientations. Journal of Personality and Social
22 Psychology, 66, 950967.
23 Ambrose, M. L., & Kulik, C. T. (1999). Old friends, new faces: Motivation research in the 1990s.
Journal of Management, 25, 231292.
24 Anderson, N., & West, M. A. (1996). The Team Climate Inventory: Development of the TCI and its
25 applications in teambuilding for innovativeness. European Journal of Work & Organizational
26 Psychology, 5, 5366.
27 Angyal, A. (1941). Foundations for a science of personality. New York: Commonwealth Fund.
28 Austin, J. T., & Vancouver, J. B. (1996). Goal constructs in psychology: Processes and content.
Psychological Bulletin, 120, 338375.
29 Baldwin, T. T., & Magjuka, R. J. (1997). Training as an organizational episode: Pretraining influences
30 on trainee motivation. In: J. K. Ford & Associates (Eds), Improving training Effectiveness in
31 Work Organizations (pp. 99128). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
32 Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efcacy: The exercise of control. New York: Freeman.
33 Barrick, M. R., Mount, M. K., & Judge, T. A. (2001). Personality and performance at the beginning
of the new millennium: What do we know and where do we go next. International Journal of
34 Selection and Assessment, 9, 930.
35 Bass, B. M. (1985). Leadership and performance beyond expectations. New York: Free Press.
36 Bass, B. M. (1999). Two decades of research and development in transformational leadership. European
37 Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 8, 932.
38 Bateman, T. C., & Crant, J. M. (1993). The proactive component of organizational behavior: A measure
and correlates. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 14, 103118.
39 Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as
40 a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497529.
Applying Self-Determination Theory to Organizational Research 387
1 Birdi, K., Allan, C., & Warr, P. (1997). Correlates and perceived outcomes of four types of employee
2 development activity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 845857.
3 Black, A. E., & Deci, E. L. (2000). The effects of instructors autonomy support and students
autonomous motivation on learning organic chemistry: A Self-Determination Theory perspec-
4 tive. Science & Education, 84, 740756.
5 Bono, J. E., & Judge, T. A. (in press). Self-concordance at work: Toward understanding the motivational Pl. update this
reference.
6 effect of transformational leaders. Academy of Management Journal.
7 Brett, J. F., & VandeWalle, D. (1999). Goal orientation and goal content as predictions of performance
8 in a training program. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 863873.
Brown, K. G. (2001). Using computers to deliver training: Which employees learn and why? Personnel
9 Psychology, 54, 271296.
10 Burns, J. M. (1978). Leadership. New York: Harper & Row.
11 Cameron, J., & Pierce, W. D. (1994). Reinforcement, reward, and intrinsic motivation: A meta-analysis.
12 Review of Educational Research, 64, 363423.
13 Coleman, P. G. (2000). Aging and the satisfaction of psychological needs. Psychological Inquiry, 11,
291293.
14 Colquitt, J. A., LePine, J. A., & Noe, R. A. (2000). Toward an integrative theory of training motivation:
15 A meta-analytic path analysis of 20 years of research. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85,
16 678707.
17 Colquitt, J. A., & Simmering, M. J. (1998). Conscientiousness, goal orientation, and motivation to learn
18 during the learning process: A longitudinal study. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 654665.
Conger, J. A., & Kanungo, R. N. (1998). Charismatic leadership in organizations. Thousand Oaks,
19 CA: Sage Publications.
20 Crant, J. M. (2000). Proactive behavior in organizations. Journal of Management, 26, 435462.
21 DeCharms, R. (1968). Personal Causation: The internal affective determinants of behavior. New York:
22 Academic Press.
23 Deci, E. L. (1975). Intrinsic motivation. New York: Plenum Press.
Deci, E. L., Connell, J. P., & Ryan, R. M. (1989). Self-determination in work organization. Journal of
24 Applied Psychology, 74, 580590.
25 Deci, E. L., Eghrari, H., Patrick, B. C., & Leone, D. R. (1994). Facilitating internalization: The self-
26 determination theory perspective. Journal of Personality, 62, 119142.
27 Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (1999a). A meta-analytic review of experiments examining
28 the effects of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 627668.
Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (1999b). The undermining effect is a reality after all-
29 Extrinsic rewards, task interests, & self-determination: A reply to Eisenberger, R., Rhoades, L.,
30 & Cameron, J. (1999) & Lepper, M. R., Henderlong, J., & Gingras, I. (1999). Psychological
31 Bulletin, 125, 692700.
32 Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985a). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior.
33 New York: Plenum.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985b). The general causality orientation scale: Self-determination in
34 personality. Journal of Research in Personality, 19, 109134.
35 Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1991). A motivational approach to self: Integration in personality. In:
36 R. Dienstbier (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Perspectives on Motivation (Vol. 38,
37 pp. 237288). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.
38 Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The what and why of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-
determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227268.
39 Deci, E. L., Ryan, R. M., Gagne, M., Leone, D. R., Usunov, J., & Kornazheva, B. P. (2001). Need satis-
40 faction, motivation, and well-being in the work organizations of a former Eastern bloc country:
388 KENNON M. SHELDON ET AL.
1 Higgins, E. T., & Kruglanski, A. W. (Eds) (2000). Motivational science: Social and personality
2 perspectives. Philadelphia, PA: Taylor & Francis.
3 Hollenbeck, J. R., & Klein, H. J. (1987). Goal commitment and the goal-setting process: Prob-
lems, prospectsand proposals for future research. Journal of Applied Psychology, 72, 212
4 220.
5 House, R. J., & Shamir, B. (1993). Toward the integration of transformational, charismatic, and
6 visionary theories. In: M. M. Chemers & R. Ayman (Eds), Leadership Theory and Research:
7 Perspectives and Directions (pp. 81107). New York: Academic Press.
8 Huselid, M. A. (1995). The impact of human resource management practices on turnover, productivity,
and corporate financial performance. Academy of Management Journal, 38, 635672.
9 Huselid, M. A., Jackson, S. E., & Schuler, R. S. (1997). Technical and strategic human resource man-
10 agement effectiveness as determinants of firm performance. Academy of Management Journal,
11 40, 171188.
12 Ilardi, B. C., Leone, D., Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1993). Employee and supervisor ratings of
13 motivation: Main effects and discrepancies associated with job satisfaction and adjustment
in a factory setting. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 23, 17891805.
14 Judge, T. A., & Ilies, R. (2002). Relationship of personality to performance motivation: A meta-analysis.
15 Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 797807.
16 Judge, T. A., Thoresen, C. J., Bono, J. E., & Patton, G. K. (2001). The job satisfaction-job performance
17 relationship: A qualitative and quantitative review. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 376407.
18 Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1993). A dark side of the American dream: Correlates of financial success
as a central life aspiration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 410422.
19 Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1996). Further examining the American dream: Well being correlates of
20 intrinsic and extrinsic goals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 281288.
21 Kelman, H. C. (1961). Processes of opinion change. Public Opinion Quarterly, 25, 5778.
22 Kernis, M. H., Paradise, A. W., Whitaker, D. J., Wheatman, S. R., & Goldman, B. N. (2000). Master of
23 ones psychological domain? Not likely if ones self-esteem is unstable. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 26, 12971305.
24 Kirkpatrick, S. A., & Locke, E. A. (1996). Direct and indirect effects of three core charismatic leadership
25 components on performance and attitudes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81, 3651.
26 Kirkpatrick, S. A., Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1996). Implementing the vision: How is it done?
27 Polish Psychological Bulletin, 27, 93106.
28 Klein, H. J., Wesson, M. J., Hollenbeck, J. R., & Alge, B. J. (1999). Goal commitment and the goal-
setting process: Conceptual clarification and empirical synthesis. Journal of Applied Psychol-
29 ogy, 84, 885896.
30 Klein, H. J., Wesson, M. J., Hollenbeck, J. R., Wright, P. M., & DeShon, R. P. (2001). The assessment of
31 goal commitment: A measurement model meta-analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human
32 Decision Processes, 85, 3255.
33 Koestner, R., & Zuckerman, M. (1994). Causality orientations, failure, and achievement. Journal of
Personality, 62, 321346.
34 Kozlowski, S. W. J., Gully, S. M., Brown, K. G., Salas, E., Smith, E. M., & Nason, E. R. (2001).
35 Effects of training goals and goal orientation traits on multi-dimensional training outcomes
36 and performance adaptability. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 85,
37 131.
38 Kozlowski, S. W. J., Toney, R. J., Mullins, M. E., Weissbein, D. A., Brown, K. G., & Bell, B. S. (2001).
Developing adaptability: A theory for the design of integrated-embedded training systems. In:
39 E. Salas (Ed.), Advances in Human Performance and Cognitive Engineering Research (Vol. 1,
40 pp. 59124). New York: Elsevier Science.
390 KENNON M. SHELDON ET AL.
1 LaGuardia, J., Ryan, R. M., Couchman, C. E., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Within-person variation in security
2 of attachment: A self-determination theory perspective on attachment, need fulfillment, and
3 well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 367384.
Lepper, M. R., & Henderlong, J. (2000). Turning play into work and work into play: 25 years of research
4 on intrinsic versus extrinsic motivation. In: C. Sansone & J. Harackiewicz (Eds), Intrinsic and
5 Extrinsic Motivation: The Search for Optimal Motivation and Performance (pp. 257307). San
6 Diego, CA: Academic Press.
7 Lepper, M. R., Henderlong, J., & Gingras, I. (1999). Understanding the effects of extrinsic rewards
8 on intrinsic motivation Uses and abuses of meta-analysis: Comment on Deci, Koestner and
Ryan (1999). Psychological Bulletin, 125, 669676.
9 Locke, E. A. (Ed.) (2000). Handbook of principles of organizational behavior. Malden, MA: Blackwell.
10 Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1999). A theory of goal setting and task performance. Englewood Cliffs,
11 NJ: Prentice-Hall.
12 Loher, B. T., Noe, R. A., Moeller, N. L., & Fitzgerald, M. P. (1985). A meta-analysis of the relation of
13 job characteristics to job satisfaction. Journal of Applied Psychology, 70, 280289.
Lowe, K. B., Kroeck, K. G., & Sivasubramaniam, N. (1996). Effectiveness correlates of transforma-
14 tion and transactional leadership: A meta-analytic review of the MLQ literature. Leadership
15 Quarterly, 7, 385425.
16 Lyubomirsky, S., King, L. A., & Diener, E. (2003). Is happiness a good thing? A theory of the benefits of
17 positive affect. Manuscript in preparation, Department of Psychology, University of California,
18 Riverside.
MacDuffie, J. P. (1995). Human resource bundles and manufacturing performance: Organizational
19 logic and flexible production systems in the world auto industry. Industrial & Labor Relations
20 Review, 48, 197221.
21 Mathieu, J. E., & Martineau, J. W. (1997). Individual and situational influences on training motivation.
22 In: J. K. Ford & Associates (Eds), Improving Training Effectiveness in Work Organizations
23 (pp. 193222). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Mathieu, J. E., Tannenbaum, S. I., & Salas, E. (1992). Influences of individual and situational char-
24 acteristics on measures of training effectiveness. Academy of Management Journal, 35, 828
25 847.
26 McAdams, D. (1996). Personality, modernity, and the storied self: A contemporary framework for
27 studying persons. Psychological Inquiry, 7, 295321.
28 Mitchell, T. R., & Daniels, D. (2002). Motivation. In: W. C. Borman, D. R. Ilgen & R. J. Klimoski (Eds),
Handbook of Psychology: Industrial and Organizational Psychology (Vol. 12, pp. 225254).
29 Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley.
30 Noe, R. A. (1986). Trainees attributes and attitudes: Neglected influences on training effectiveness.
31 Academy of Management Review, 11, 736749.
32 Noe, R. A., & Schmitt, N. (1986). The influence of trainee attitudes on training effectiveness: Test of
33 a model. Personnel Psychology, 39, 497523.
Noe, R. A., & Wilk, S. L. (1993). Investigation of the factors that influence employees participation
34 in development activities. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 291302.
35 Noe, R. A., Wilk, S. L., Mullen, E. J., & Wanek, J. E. (1997). Employee development: Issues in con-
36 struct definition and investigation of antecedents. In: J. K. Ford & Associates (Eds), Improving
37 Training Effectiveness in Work Organizations (pp. 153189). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
38 Osterloh, M., & Frey, B. S. (2000). Motivation, knowledge transfer, and organizational forms. Orga-
nization Science, 11, 538550.
39 Pfeffer, J. (1998). The human equation. Boston, MA: Harvard Business School Press.
40
Applying Self-Determination Theory to Organizational Research 391
1 Pillai, R., Schriesheim, C. A., & Williams, E. S. (1999). Fairness perceptions and trust as mediators for
2 transformational and transactional leadership: A two-sample study. Journal of Management,
3 25, 897933.
Psyszczynski, R., Greenberg, J., & Solomon, S. (2000). Toward a dialectical analysis of growth and
4 defensive motives. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 301305.
5 Quinones, M. A. (1995). Pretraining context effects: Training assignment as feedback. Journal of
6 Applied Psychology, 80, 226238.
7 Reeve, J., Bolt, E., & Cai, Y. (1999). Autonomy-supportive teachers: How they teach and motivate
8 students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 537548.
Reis, H. T., Sheldon, K. M., Gable, S. L., Roscoe, R., & Ryan, R. (2000). Daily well-being: The role
9 of autonomy, competence, and relatedness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26,
10 419435.
11 Rigby, C. S., Deci, E. L., Patrick, B. C., & Ryan, R. M. (1992). Beyond the intrinsic-extrinsic dichotomy:
12 Self-determination in motivation and learning. Motivation & Emotion, 16, 165185.
13 Rogers, C. (1961). On becoming a person. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement.
14 Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 80, 128.
15 Ryan, R. M. (1982). Control and information in the intrapersonal sphere: An extension of cognitive
16 evaluation theory. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 43, 450461.
17 Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation,
18 social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55, 6878.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2001). On happiness and human potentials: A review of research on hedonic
19 and eudaimonic well-being. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 141166.
20 Ryan, R. M., & Stiller, J. (1991). The social contexts of internalization: Parent and teacher influences
21 on autonomy, motivation, and learning. Advances in Motivation and Achievement, 7, 115149.
22 Ryff, C. D. (1995). Psychological well-being in adult life. Current Directions in Psychological Science,
23 4(4), 99104.
Salancik, G. R., & Pfeffer, J. (1978). A social information processing approach to job attitudes and
24 task design. Administrative Science Quarterly, 23, 224253.
25 Salas, E., & Cannon-Bowers, J. A. (1997). Methods, tools, and strategies for team training. In: M. A.
26 Quinones & A. Ehrenstein (Eds), Training for a Rapidly Changing Workplace: Applications of
27 Psychological Research (pp. 249279). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
28 Sansone, C., & Smith, J. L. (2000). Interest and self-regulation: The relation between having to and
wanting to. In: C. Sansone & J. Harackiewicz (Eds), Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation: The
29 Search for Optimal Motivation and Performance (pp. 341372). San Diego, CA: Academic
30 Press.
31 Seligman, M., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology: An introduction. American
32 Psychologist, 55, 514.
33 Shamir, B., House, R. J., & Arthur, M. B. (1993). The motivational effects of charismatic leadership:
A self-concept based theory. Organization Science, 4, 577594.
34 Sheldon, K. M. (2001). The self-concordance model of healthy goal-striving: Implications for
35 well-being and personality development. In: P. Schmuck & K. Sheldon (Eds), Life Goals
36 and Well-Being: Towards a Positive Psychology of Human Striving (pp. 1735). Seattle, WA:
37 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers.
38 Sheldon, K. M. (2002). The self-concordance model of healthy goal-striving: When personal goals
correctly represent the person. In: E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds), Handbook of Self-
39 Determination Theory (pp. 6586). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.
40
392 KENNON M. SHELDON ET AL.
1 Sheldon, K. M., & Biddle, B. J. (1998). Standards, accountability, and school reform: Perils and pitfalls.
2 Teachers College Record, 100, 164180.
3 Sheldon, K. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Book review: Creativity in context. The Journal of Creative
Behavior, 34, 285290.
4 Sheldon, K. M., & Elliot, A. J. (1998). Not all personal goals are personal: Comparing autonomous
5 and controlled reasons for goals as predictors of effort and attainment. Personality and Social
6 Psychology Bulletin, 24, 546557.
7 Sheldon, K. M., & Elliot, A. J. (1999). Goal striving, need satisfaction, and longitudinal well-
8 being: The self-concordance model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 482
497.
9 Sheldon, K. M., Elliot, A. J., Kim, Y., & Kasser, T. (2001). Whats satisfying about satisfying events?
10 Comparing ten candidate psychological needs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
11 80, 325339.
12 Sheldon, K. M., & Houser-Marko, L. (2001). Self-concordance, goal attainment, and the pursuit of
13 happiness: Can there be an upward spiral? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80,
152165.
14 Sheldon, K. M., Joiner, T., & Williams, G. (in press). Motivating health: Applying self-determination Pl. update
15 theory in the clinic. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. reference.
16 Sheldon, K. M., & Kasser, T. (1995). Coherence and congruence: Two aspects of personality integration.
17 Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 531543.
18 Sheldon, K. M., & Kasser, T. (1998). Pursuing personal goals: Skills enable progress, but not all
progress is beneficial. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 13191331.
19 Sheldon, K. M., & Kasser, T. (2001). Getting older, getting better? Personal strivings and personality
20 development across the life-course. Developmental Psychology, 37, 491501.
21 Sheldon, K. M., Ryan, R. M., Elliot, A. J., Kim, Y., Chirkov, V., Demir, M., & Wu, C. (2002).
22 Autonomous goal pursuit and collectivist values: Complementary, not conflictual. Unpublished
23 manuscript.
Sheldon, K. M., Ryan, R. M., Rawsthorne, L., & Ilardi, B. (1997). True self and trait self: Cross-role
24 variation in the Big Five traits and its relations with authenticity and well-being. Journal of
25 Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 13801393.
26 Sheldon, K. M., Ryan, R. M., & Reis, H. R. (1996). What makes for a good day? Competence
27 and autonomy in the day and in the person. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22,
28 12701279.
Sheldon, K. M., & Schmuck, P. (2001). Conclusion: Suggestions for healthy goal striving. In:
29 P. Schmuck & K. Sheldon (Eds), Life Goals and Well-Being: Towards a Positive Psychology of
30 Human Striving (pp. 213226). Seattle, WA: Hogrefe & Huber Publishers.
31 Spector, P. E. (1982). Behavior in organizations as a function of employees locus of control. Psycho-
32 logical Bulletin, 91, 482497.
33 Spreitzer, G. M., Kizilos, M. A., & Nason, S. W. (1997). A dimensional analysis of the relationship
between psychological empowerment and effectiveness, satisfaction, and strain. Journal of
34 Management, 23, 679704.
35 Srivastava, A., Locke, E. A., & Bartol, K. M. (2001). Money and subjective well-being: Its not the
36 money, its the motives. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 80, 959971.
37 Stajkovic, A., & Luthans, F. (1998). Self-efficacy and work-related performance: A meta-analysis.
38 Psychological Bulletin, 124, 240261.
Tang, S. H., & Hall, V. C. (1995). The overjustification effect: A meta-analysis. Applied Cognitive
39 Psychology, 9, 365404.
40
Applying Self-Determination Theory to Organizational Research 393
1 Tharenou, P. (2001). The relationship of training motivation to participation in training and develop-
2 ment. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 74, 599621.
3 Towler, A. J., & Dipboye, R. L. (2001). Effects of trainer expressiveness, organization, and trainee goal
orientation on training outcomes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 664673.
4 Vallerand, R. (2000). Deci and Ryans self-determination theory: A view from the hierarchical model
5 of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 312318.
6 Van Erde, W., & Thierry, H. (1996). Vrooms expectancy models and work-related criteria: A meta-
7 analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81, 575586.
8 Vroom, V. H. (1964). Work and motivation. New York: Wiley.
Werner, H. (1957). The concept of development from a comparative and organismic point of view.
9 In: D. Harris (Ed.), The Concept of Development (pp. 125147). MN: University of Minnesota
10 Press.
11 Wrzesniewski, A., & Dutton, J. E. (2001). Crafting a job: Revisioning employees as active crafters of
12 their work. Academy of Management Review, 26, 179201.
13
14
15 Uncited references
16
17 Colquitt and Simmering (1998) and Sheldon (2001).
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40