Phonological and Orthographic Processing: One or Both Hemispheres?
Phonological and Orthographic Processing: One or Both Hemispheres?
Phonological and Orthographic Processing: One or Both Hemispheres?
Jeffrey R. Mathesius
University of British Columbia Okanagan
Abstract
Laterality refers to the contention that the left and right hemisphere of the brain process
and orthographic processing. Two experiments used a lexical decision task and a priming
procedure along with a distractor to the left or right visual field to test laterality. Experiment I
similarity between strings. Both experiments tested three hypotheses. First, primed letter
strings would be faster and more accurate than non-primed letter strings. Second, both
hemispheres would show priming. Third, the left hemisphere in Experiment I would show
greater priming than the right hemisphere, whereas the right hemisphere in Experiment II
Contents
Abstract / 2
Table of Contents / 3
General Introduction / 4
Experiment I:
Introduction / 9
Method: Participants / 10
Method: Materials / 10
Method: Procedure / 10
Method: Design / 13
Results / 13
Discussion / 15
Experiment II:
Introduction / 17
Method: Participants / 17
Method: Materials / 17
Method: Procedure / 18
Method: Design / 18
Results / 19
Discussion / 22
General Discussion / 24
References / 26
Appendices
4
Language for most humans is the primary source of communication and as a result is regarded as
an important part of human life. Language can be broken down into two parts: processing of language
and the production of language. The processing of language involves the appropriate sequencing of
mental steps in order to understand language. The production of language involves the verbal, written,
or signed forms of the specific language. Due to its importance, language has been widely studied by
linguists, psychologists, as well as neurologists. One of the most highly researched areas in language is
Laterality of reading refers to the contention that the left and right side of the brain process
information differently. These differences are also known as asymmetries. Some of the first reports of
language lateralization began with Broca and Wernicke who introduced the idea of a unilateral left
hemisphere control over language functions (Broca, 1861; Wernicke, 1911, as cited by Szaflarski,
Holand, Schmithorst, & Byars, 2006). This idea is also known as an absolute model and suggests that
only the left hemisphere can process language. Other findings that have been interpreted to support this
absolute model are the costs to time and accuracy of language processing when the language is
presented to the right hemisphere (Beeman & Chiarello, 1998, as cited by Chiarello, Hasbrook, &
Maxfield, 1999). These findings have been suggested to be the result of callosal relay from the illiterate
right hemisphere to the more language dominant left hemisphere (Efron, 1963). However, there are
alternate interpretations of these findings. For example, relative models suggest that while the left
hemisphere is dominant for language processing (e.g., Iacoboni & Zaidel, 1996; Leiber, 1976;
Rutherford & Lutz, 2004), the right hemisphere does have some limited linguistic competence
(Chiarello, Hasbrooke, & Maxfield, 1999; Rutherford, 2006; Underwood, Rusted, & Thwaites, 1983).
According to this view, the costs to time and accuracy of language processing by the right hemisphere is
5
due to the use of inefficient and less specialized strategies compared to the left hemisphere (Rutherford
Research investigating the laterality of reading has, in the past, been typically conducted on
neurologically impaired humans. In fact, one of the major findings in this line of research was
discovered this way. For example, research with commissurotomy patients (e.g., Sidtis, Volpe, Wilson,
Rayport, & Gazzaniga, 1981; Zaidel & Peters, 1981) has revealed that there are different processing
strategies used by each hemisphere. One of these strategies is known as the lexical strategy. The lexical
strategy involves the matching of a whole letter string to a memory representation in the brain (Iacoboni
& Zaidel, 1996). This type of processing occurs with familiar words (Iacoboni & Zaidel, 1996) and is
believed to be available to both the left and right hemisphere (Rutherford, 2006). The second strategy is
known as the non-lexical strategy. This strategy involves the sounding out of words and usually occurs
with unfamiliar words (Iacoboni & Zaidel, 1996). It is believed that the non-lexical strategy is only
available to the left hemisphere. It is important to note that not only are individuals with different
neurological abilities used to study the laterality of reading but multiple experimental methods are used
as well.
Many different types of experimental methods have been used to analyze the laterality of
reading, some of which include the Wada test (e.g., Watson, Pusakulich, Ward, & Hermann, 1998),
dichotic listening tasks (e.g., Asbjornsen & Helland, 2006), and fMRI techniques (e.g., Szaflarski,
Holland, Schmithorst, & Byars, 2006). The most widely employed method, however, is the lexical
decision task (e.g., Bloch & Zaidel, 1996; Paap & Newsome, 1981; Rutherford, 2006; Rutherford &
Lutz, 2004). The lexical decision task involves the presentation of a group of letters, known as a letter
string, to a participant. The participant is instructed to respond if the letter string is, or is not, a word. In
a typical lexical decision task there are two types of letter strings: words and pseudowords. Word letter
6
strings represent groups of letters that spell a word. Pseudowords are letter strings that follow the rules
In the traditional lexical decision experiment a fixation point is presented in the middle of the
screen and the letter string is presented to either the left or the right side of this fixation point. If the
letter string is presented to the left side of the fixation point it is a left visual field presentation (LVF). If
the letter string is presented to the right of the fixation point it is a right visual field presentation (RVF).
By presenting the letter string to only one of the visual fields, researchers are able to localize which
hemisphere is processing the letter string. The reason for this has to do with the visual and neuronal
connections of the brain. That being said, it should be noted that lateralized displays bias processing to
the contralateral hemisphere but cannot constrain processing to that hemisphere because the corpus
callosum relays information from one to the other hemisphere. Notwithstanding the relay, researchers
determine which hemisphere is better, worse, or the same at processing certain types of letter strings by
Despite the wide spread use of this type of lexical decision task the results may not accurately
generalize to the normal reading situation. The reason for this is that the words are presented to the left
or right of centre. Due to the fact that people read with a central fixation on the word, this type of
experiment has questionable ecological validity. In order to account for this issue, Rutherford and Lutz
(2004) developed a more ecologically valid version of the lexical decision task.
In this version of the lexical decision task (Rutherford & Lutz, 2004) the letter string is
presented to the center of the participants visual field. At the onset of the letter string a blinking
distractor is presented to the left, right, or neither side of the letter string. The purpose of this distractor
is to temporarily disengage the processing of the contralateral hemisphere leaving the unaffected
ispsilateral hemisphere to process the letter string (Cowan 1995, as cited by Rutherford & Lutz, 2004).
7
In other words, if the distractor is presented to the left side of the letter string, this will temporarily
disengage the right hemisphere allowing the left hemisphere to process the letter string. The participant
is to respond if the letter string does or does not spell a word as occurs in the typical lexical decision
task. In order to respond, participants are instructed to press certain keys on a computer keyboard that
will correspond to if the letter string does or does not spell a word.
There are three important features to this novel version of the lexical decision task. First, it is
more ecologically valid as it allows the letter string to be presented centrally, rather than in the
participants periphery, therefore more closely mimicking how people read. Second, the no distractor
condition permits researchers to understand how the hemispheres interact when they are both allowed to
process the letter string at the same time. Third, as was the case with lateralized displays, the procedure
provides evidence of relative hemispheric competence (e.g., Rutherford, 2006; Rutherford & Lutz,
2004).
Within the field of laterality of reading, what is still not fully understood and needs further
investigation is whether the hemispheres differ in the ability for phonological (i.e., non-lexical) and
orthographic (i.e., lexical) processing. For example, past research conducted on neurologically impaired
individuals such as commissurotomy patients (e.g., Sidtis, Volpe, Wilson, Rayport, & Gazzaniga, 1981;
Zaidel & Peters, 1981) and neurologically intact individuals (e.g., Sasanuma, Itoh, Kobyashi, & Mori,
1980) have demonstrated that only the left hemisphere is capable of processing phonological
information, whereas only the right hemisphere is capable of orthographic processing. More recent
research conducted by Lavidor and Ellis (2003) further supports this view.
Lavidor and Ellis (2003) investigated the laterality of phonological and orthographic processing
using a priming procedure. Priming involves the presentation of two consecutive stimuli in which the
first stimulus, the prime, facilitates the processing of the succeeding target stimulus because the prime
8
activates a lexical representational system in the brain (Forster, 1987, as cited by Lavidor and Ellis,
2003) that contains the same characteristics as the target. As has been empirically demonstrated, the
participant should be faster and more accurate to the primed target in comparison to the non-primed
target (Chiarello, 1985; Hutchins & Palmer, 2008; Lavidor & Ellis, 2001; Perfetti & Bell, 1991). In
their study, Lavidor and Ellis (2003) presented the prime letter string to the center of the participants
visual field, while the target was presented to either the left or the right visual field. The two letter
strings were manipulated by phonological or orthographic similarity with one another and the
participants were instructed to respond only to the central letter string. Phonologically similar letter
strings sound alike whereas orthographically similar letter strings look alike. The results revealed
increased speed and accuracy of lexical decision when a phonologically related lateralized target was
presented to the RVF, but not the LVF. Also revealed was an increase in speed and accuracy of lexical
decision when an orthographically related lateralized target was presented to the LVF, but not the RVF.
These results suggest that only the left hemisphere has phonological processing capabilities while only
Not all research, however, is in agreement with the findings that only the left hemisphere can
process phonology and only the right hemisphere can process orthography (e.g., Chiarello, Hasbrooke,
& Maxfield, 1999; Crossman & Polich, 1988). Chiarello et al. (1999) conducted a study that
investigated the effects of an unattended distractor on the pronunciation of a target word in both the left
and right visual fields. The unattended distractor and target words were phonologically and
orthographically manipulated to be similar or dissimilar to one another. The target and distractor words
were presented in a crossword fashion in which the participant was instructed before hand on which
axis was the target (i.e., vertical or horizontal) and that they were to verbally pronounce the target word.
The results showed that both the left and the right hemispheres are capable of phonological and
orthographic processing.
9
A possible reason for these conflicting results for orthographic lateralization may involve the
size refers to how many new words can be created by manipulating one letter of a target word, where
high neighborhood size denotes more words. As shown in past research (Lavidor & Ellis, 2001),
neighborhood size can affect asymmetries in orthographic processing, with large neighborhood sizes
favoring the right hemisphere. In returning to the research presented above, Chiarello, et al., (1999) did
not control for neighborhood size. This lack of third variable control could have potentially biased
hemispheric asymmetry.
Due to these conflicting results, it is still not understood whether the hemispheres differ in
competence for processing phonological and orthographic information. Thus, the purpose of the present
study is to investigate the question and to further the literature within the field of laterality of reading. In
order to accomplish these two separate studies will be conducted using the more ecologically valid
lexical decision task in combination with a priming procedure. The first study will analyze the laterality
of phonological processing whereas the second study will analyze the laterality of orthographic
Experiment I
processing. Three hypotheses are tested: First, primed letter strings will be faster and more accurate
than non-primed letter strings. Second, both hemispheres will show phonological priming. Third, the
left hemisphere will show greater priming than the right hemisphere.
Method
10
Participants
A convenience sample of 65 students (20 Men and 45 Women) from the University of British
Columbia Okanagan campus participated. Six participants data were excluded from the analysis due
to violation of study requirements they were on medication. Thus, a total of 59 students (18 Men and
41 Women) participated. The age of participants ranged from 17 years and three months to 24 years
and four months (M = 18.83, SD = 1.49). Participation was restricted to students at the university who
had never participated in the study before, who were right handed (as deemed by a positive score on the
handedness questionnaire), who spoke English as their first language, who were not on medication with
the possible exception of hormones for birth control, and who had no reading disability. Participants
were able to sign up for the study through one of three ways; (1) through an online website known as
SONA, (2) drop in to the psychology laboratory and book a time slot or (3) email the researcher directly
Participation in this study was completely voluntary, but incentives were provided in two ways.
First, participants enrolled in a psychology course were eligible to receive .5% course credit towards
their psychology class through participation in this study. Second, participants not enrolled in a
psychology course or those in a psychology course who declined credit were eligible to receive a $10
Materials
Adult participants were given a battery of tests that included a demographic form, a
Demographic form.
11
The demographic form (see Appendix A) consisted of a 5-item checklist that provided general
information about the participant. The researcher read each question to the participant and asked the
participant to verbally respond. Each response was recorded by the researcher on the demographic form
and retained for records. The purpose of this form was to determine if the participant had a known
reading disability, was on any medication with the possible exception of hormones for birth control, and
Handedness Questionnaire
operationally determine if a participant was left handed, right handed, or ambidextrous. Each item
identified an activity and the participant reported (R) if he/she would use their right hand, (L) if he/she
would use their left hand or (E) if he/she would use either hand for each activity. Right hand responses
were scored as +1, left hand with -1, either hand with 0. The scores were then totaled. To be classified
as right handed the participant needed to have a score of +1 or greater. To be classified as left handed
the participant needed to have a score of -1 or less. To be classified as ambidextrous the participant
needed a score of 0.
Word Attack
The word attack form (see Appendix C) was a standardized phonological pronunciation task
consisting of a list of 30 nonsense words. These nonsense words, also known as pseudowords, were
letter strings that followed the rules of spelling but did not spell an actual word (e.g., kash). A correct
response was defined as pronouncing the proper grapheme phoneme conversion for the letter string.
Correct answers were denoted by a +1 whereas incorrect answers were denoted by a 0. The correct
responses were totaled out of a possible 30 correct. The score was compared to age or grade norms in
12
order to determine a standardized measure of phonological ability. A cut-off percentile score of 50%
Computer Task
Inquisit 3 was used to program the stimuli to appear on an IBM compatible Pentium I
computer. Strings of 4-6 white letters in System Times New Roman size 14 font were presented to
center of monitor against a black background. There were a total of 196 stimuli, half were words and
A distractor was presented at the onset of each letter string consisting of a 1 dg visual angle
white square. The distractor blinked on and off at 50 ms intervals for 300 ms. The distractor was
presented to either the left or right visual field 7 cm from the center screen as measured from the medial
position of the distractor. A left or right distractor condition occurred with an equal probability across
all trials.
Procedure
The demographic form, handedness questionnaire, word attack, and computer task were
completed in this order once the informed consent (see Appendix D) was signed. After the computer
task was completed, the participant was fully debriefed and given a copy of both the informed consent
During the computer task, participants were seated in a quiet and darkened testing room with
their chin positioned in a chin rest at exactly 57 cm from the center of the computer monitor. To begin
each trial the participant was instructed to press the spacebar. At the start of each trial a fixation cross
appeared in the center of the computer monitor for a duration of 500 ms after which a letter string
replaced the fixation cross. The letter string, which spelled either a word or a pseudoword, was
13
accompanied by a blinking distractor to either the left or right side of the computer monitor. The
participant was instructed to answer if the letter string did or did not spell a word. Affirmative responses
were indicated by simultaneously pressing the f and j keys with their index finger of the left and right
hand, respectively. Negative responses were indicated by simultaneously pressing the d and k keys with
their middle finger of their left and right hand, respectively. A trial was finished after the participant
answered or after 2 s of no response, whichever came first. During this task accuracy and response
The first block was a practice block consisting of 32 trials. This block was followed by three
test blocks consisting of 64 randomized trials each. Out of the total 64 stimuli per test block, 32 trials
were words and 32 were pseudowords. One half of each string type was accompanied by a left
distractor and half by a right distractor. Within each string and distractor type, four trials were
phonologically primed and 12 were not primed. Phonological priming involves the presentation of two
consecutive similar sounding stimuli in which the first stimulus, the prime, facilitates the processing of
the succeeding target stimulus because the prime activates a lexical representational system in the brain
(Forster, 1987, as cited by Lavidor and Ellis, 2003) that contains the same characteristics (i.e., sound) as
target. Where primed strings sounded similar to one another, not primed strings sounded dissimilar.
Design
The present experiment used a 2 X 2 X 2 repeated measures ANOVA design. Thus, there were
three independent variables each consisting of two levels. The first independent variable, letter string,
contained word strings and pseudoword strings. The second independent variable, distractor location,
contained a left visual field distractor and a right visual field distractor. The third independent variable,
phonological priming, contained phonologically primed letter strings and phonologically not primed
letter strings. Both accuracy and reaction time were measured as dependent variables.
14
Results
Separate repeated-measures analyses of string type, distractor location, and prime type were
Reaction Time
The overall analyses revealed a significant main effect for string type [F (1, 58) = 41.13, p <
.001, p2 = 42%]. The means of string type found words (M = 718.272, SD = 14.703) to be significantly
faster than pseudowords (M = 793.520, SD = 18.557). A main effect of prime type [F (1, 58) = 63.67, p
< .001, p2 = 52%] was also found. The means of prime type revealed primed strings (M = 735.404, SD
= 15.667) were significantly faster than non-primed (M = 776.487, SD = 16.112) strings. These main
effects, however, were modified by a significant visual field X string type X prime type interaction [F
(1, 58) = 5.61, p < .05, p2 = 8%]. To elucidate the source of the interaction, separate analyses of each
Pseudowords
The results unveiled a visual field X prime type interaction [F (1, 58) = 5.97, p < .05, p2 = 9%].
Separate analyses of each visual field exposed a significant effect of prime type within left distractor
condition [F (1, 58) = 31.24, p < .001, p2 = 35%], but not the right distractor condition [F (1, 58) = .95,
p > .05, p2 = 2%]. The means of the left distractor condition were faster to primed (M = 763.98, SD =
161.17) than non-primed (M = 818.93, SD = 160.59) pseudowords, suggesting that the left hemisphere
Words
A significant main effect of prime type [F (1, 58) = 52.84, p < .001, p2 = 48%] was found for
word strings. Responses were significantly faster to primed (M = 693.87, SD = 15.11) than non-primed
15
words (M = 742.68, SD = 15.05) suggesting that both hemispheres show phonological priming of word
letter strings.
Accuracy
The overall analyses revealed a significant main effect for prime type [F (1, 58) = 56.06, p <
.001, p2 = 49%], with higher accuracy to primed (M = .947, SD = .006) than non-primed letter strings
(M = .908, SD = .006). These results suggest that both hemispheres showed priming and there was no
Discussion
The results from Experiment I supported all three hypotheses. Consistent with previous research
(e.g., Chiarello, 1985; Hutchins & Palmer, 2008; Lavidor & Ellis, 2001; Perfetti & Bell, 1991) and our
first hypothesis, responses were processed faster and more accurately to a letter string that followed a
similar sounding string (i.e., primed), than one that was not similar in sound (i.e., non-primed). The
enhancement was likely due to the prime activating a lexical representational system in the brain that
contains the same characteristics as the target (Forster, 1987, as cited by Lavidor & Ellis, 2003), which
speeds responses because there is no need to reactivate the lexical representational system and makes
them more accurate because there is less likelihood of the target activating a different, incorrect, system.
Also consistent with previous research (e.g., Chiarello, et al, 1999; Underwood, et al., 1983) and
our second hypothesis, both the left and right hemisphere showed phonological priming for words. This
finding is consistent with evidence that both the left and the right hemispheres contain lexical
processing strategies and suggests both hemispheres have simple phonological processing abilities.
Understanding basic human language processing, enables one to understand the suggestion that both
hemispheres have simple phonological processing abilities. Under normal reading conditions, letter
strings are typically familiar words. Due to this familiarity, readers process each letter string using
16
orthographic (i.e., lexical) processing strategies that are available to both hemispheres. Since the letter
string is familiar to the reader, information such as the look (i.e., orthography), the sound (i.e.,
phonology), and the meaning (i.e., semantics) are stored in interconnected neural networks within the
brain. Even though the reader is using the orthographic processing strategy, there is a spread of
activation to the phonological and semantic neural networks. It is this spread in activation toward the
phonological neural network that makes the phonological processing of words simple. The words
phonology are already known, thus, the simultaneous activation of orthographic and phonological codes
allows a more simplistic top-down processing rather than the more complex, print-to-sound, bottom-up
Consistent with our third hypothesis, the left hemisphere showed greater priming than the right,
but only for pseudowords. This finding is consistent with evidence suggesting that the left hemisphere
can invoke a non-lexical process that converts print-to-sound (e.g., Chiarello, et al, 1999; Iacoboni &
Zaidel, 1996; Rutherford, 2006) in order to match a letter string to entries in the mental lexicon. This
finding also suggests that only the left hemisphere is capable of complex phonological processing. As
stated above, readers typically invoke orthographic processing when reading familiar words. However,
when words are unfamiliar, this type of processing will be ineffective because the letter string
order to determine the sound of the letter string and find a match, if there is one, to other entries in the
mental lexicon. It is this print-to-sound, bottom-up processing, that requires more complex phonological
processing abilities.
There are two possible explanations for the left hemisphere superiority of phonological
processing. First, the superiority may be the result of asymmetries in lexical and non-lexical processing
strategies, as suggested above. More specifically, past research has demonstrated that both hemispheres
have access to lexical processing which is invoked for the processing of familiar letter strings. Only the
17
left hemisphere, however, is capable of the non-lexical, print-to-sound, processing required for
unfamiliar letter strings. Second, the left hemisphere superiority may be caused by the asymmetries in
sequential processing abilities. Prior research has demonstrated that the left hemisphere has faster and
more accurate sequential processing abilities than the right hemisphere (Efron, 1963; Rutherford, 2003).
Since phonological processing requires the quick and accurate ordering of sounds, superior sequential
processing abilities should lead to both a fast and accurate processing of stimuli requiring the
Experiment II
The purpose of the second experiment is to test hemispheric asymmetry in orthographic
processing. Three hypotheses are tested: First, primed letter strings will be faster and more accurate
than non-primed letter strings. Second, both hemispheres will show orthographic priming. Third, the
left hemisphere will show greater priming than the right hemisphere.
Method
Participants
A convenience sample of 68 students (21 Men and 47 Women) from the University of British
Columbia Okanagan campus participated. Nine participants data were excluded from the analysis due
to violation of study requirements they were on medication, were left handed, or could not properly
see the stimuli. Thus, the data from a total of 59 subjects were analyzed (19 Men and 40 Women). The
age of participants ranged from 18 years and three months to 36 years and five months (M = 19.056,
SD = 2.523). The restrictions in the present experiment were the same as the first, as were the sign up
Materials
18
All materials were the same as in Experiment I, except for the stimuli. The word letter string
stimuli were four-to-five letters in length and were all obtained from a study conducted by Lavidor &
Ellis (2003). The stimuli in the present experiment consisted of orthographically primed and
orthographically not primed letter strings. Orthographic priming involves the presentation of two
consecutive similar looking stimuli in which the first stimulus, the prime, facilitates the processing of
the succeeding target stimulus. All orthographically primed words in the present study contain high
orthographic neighborhood size. Orthographic neighborhood refers to the number of words that can be
created by changing one letter of a target word. In the present study high orthographic neighborhood
size is defined as words in which 10 or more other words can be created by changing one letter of the
target word. For example, the letter string cover contains a high orthographic neighborhood size
because it has 13 orthographic neighbors, some of which include coven, covet, cower, and lover
The first block was a practice block consisting of 32 trials. This block was followed by three
test blocks consisting of 64 randomized trials each. Out of the total 64 stimuli per test block, 32 trials
were words and 32 were pseudowords. Half of each string type was accompanied by a left distractor
and half by a right distractor. Within each string and distractor type four trials were orthographically
Procedure
Design
The present experiment used a 2 X 2 X 2 repeated measures ANOVA design. Thus, there were
three independent variables each consisting of two levels. The first independent variable, letter string,
contained word letter strings and pseudoword letter strings. The second independent variable, distractor
19
location, contained a left visual field distractor and a right visual field distractor. The third independent
variable, orthographic priming, contained orthographically primed letter strings and orthographically
not primed letter strings. Both accuracy and reaction time were measured as dependent variables.
Results
prime type were conducted for reaction time and accuracy at an alpha of .05.
Reaction Time
The overall analyses revealed three significant main effects. First, a main effect for string type
[F (1, 58) = 54.21, p < .001, p2 = 48%] was found. Assessing the means of string type exposed words
Second, a significant main effect for prime type [F (1, 58) = 2.48, p < .001, p2 = 81%] was also
uncovered. The means of prime type revealed primed strings (M = 683.721, SD = 16.022) were
significantly faster than non-primed strings (M = 766.255, SD = 16.845). However, these main effects
were modified by a significant visual field X prime type interaction [F (1, 58) = 28.46, p < .001, p2 =
33%] and a significant string type X prime type interaction [F (1, 58) = 4.19, p < .05, p2 = 7%]. To
expose the source of these interactions, separate analyses were conducted for each string type and prime
type.
Pseudowords
A significant visual field X prime type interaction [F (1, 58) = 10.99, p < .01, p2 = 16%] was
found for pseudowords. Separate analyses of each visual field revealed a significant effect of prime type
within the left distractor condition [F (1, 58) = 1.100, p < .001, p2 = 66%]. The means of the left
20
distractor condition were faster to primed (M = 702.198, SD = 17.42) than non-primed (M = 811.275,
SD = 19.672) pseudowords. A significant effect of prime type within the right distractor condition
[F (1, 58) = 49.71, p < .001, p2 = 46%] was also revealed. The means of the right distractor condition
pseudowords. These results suggest that both hemispheres show priming for pseudowords .
Separate analyses of each prime type revealed significantly faster processing of non-primed
pseudowords in the right (M = 791.012, SD = 139.065) compared to the left hemisphere (M = 811.275,
SD = 1851.10), with [t(58) = 2.60, p < .05, 2 = 10.30%]. This suggests that the right hemisphere relies
on a speedier strategy than used by the left when the priming context is removed.
Words
A significant visual field X prime interaction [F (1, 58) = 7.613, p > .01, p2 = 12%] was found
for words. Separate analyses of each visual field revealed a significant effect of prime type within the
left distractor condition [F (1, 58) = 1.362, p < .001, p2 = 70%]. The means of the left distractor
17.904) words. A significant effect of prime type within the right distractor condition [F (1, 58) = 49.71,
p < .001, p2 = 46%] was also uncovered. The means of the right distractor condition were faster to
primed (M = 659.914, SD = 18.559) than non-primed (M = 716.772, SD = 15.604) words. These results
Separate analyses of each prime type revealed significantly faster processing of non-primed
words in the right hemisphere (M = 716.772, SD = 119.858) than the left hemisphere (M = 745.962, SD
= 137.526), with [t(58) = 3.86, p < .001, 2 = 2.04%]. This suggests that the right hemisphere relies on a
speedier strategy than used by the left when the priming context is removed.
21
Accuracy
The overall analysis revealed two significant main effects. First, there was a significant main
effect of visual field [F (1, 58) = 61.405, p < .001, p2 = 51%] with the means revealing the right visual
field (M = .954, SD = .004) was more accurate than the left visual field (M = .923, SD = .005). Second,
a significant main effect for prime type [F (1, 58) = 1.727, p < .001, p2 = 75%] was also found.
According to the means, primed strings (M = .946, SD = .004) were significantly more accurate than
non-primed strings (M = .902, SD = .005). However, these main effects were modified by a significant
visual field X prime interaction [F (1, 58) = 16.917, p < .001, p2 = 23%], a significant string type X
prime type interaction [F (1, 58) = 5.538, p < .05, p2 = .9%] and a significant visual field X string type
X prime type interaction [F (1, 58) = 14.794, p < .001, p2 = 20%]. To uncover the source of these
interactions, separate analyses were conducted for each string type and each prime type.
Pseudowords
Within the pseudoword letter strings there was a significant main effect of visual field [F (1,
58) = 30.279, p < .001, p2 = 34%]. The means revealed pseudowords were more accurate in the right
distractor condition (M = .954, SD = .005) than the left distractor condition (M = .922, SD = .007). Also
within the pseudoword condition there was a significant main effect of prime type [F (1, 58) = 93.593,
p < .001, p2 = 59%]. The means revealed both hemispheres were more accurate to primed (M = .970,
SD = .005) than non-primed (M = .907, SD = .008) pseudowords. These results suggest that
pseudowords are more accurately processed in the right than the left hemisphere and that both
Words
The results revealed a significant visual field X prime interaction [F (1, 58) = 28.838, p < .001,
p2 = 33%]. Separate analyses of each visual field revealed a significant effect of prime type within the
22
left distractor condition [F (1, 58) = 1.278, p < .001, p2 = 69%]. The means of the left distractor
condition were more accurate to primed (M = .983, SD = .006) than non-primed (M = .865, SD = .009)
words. A significant effect of prime type within the right distractor condition [F (1, 58) = 32.781,
p < .001, p2 = 36%] was also revealed. The means of the right distractor condition were more accurate
to primed (M = .982, SD = .007) than non-primed (M = .928, SD = .006) words. These results suggest
Separate analysis of prime types revealed that the right hemisphere (M = .928, SD = .049) was
more accurate than the left (M = .865, SD = .065) in processing non-primed words [t(58) = -7.475, p <
.001, 2 = 49.06%], but not primed words. This finding suggests that the strategy used by the right
hemisphere is different than the left, in a context where there is no priming. These results imply that not
only is non-lexical processing a slower strategy, but a less accurate one as well.
Discussion
The results from Experiment II supported all three hypotheses. Consistent with Experiment I,
past research (Chiarello, 1985; Hutchins & Palmer, 2008; Lavidor & Ellis, 2001; Perfetti & Bell, 1991)
and our first hypothesis, letter strings which were similar in appearance (i.e., primed) were processed
faster and more accurately than letter strings not similar in appearance (i.e., non-primed). As with
Experiment I, this enhancement in processing is likely due to the prime activating a lexical
representational system in the brain which contains similar characteristics to the target string (Forster,
1987, as cited by Lavidor & Ellis, 2003). Thus, the target will be processed faster because it no longer
needs to reactivate the representational system and will be more accurate because there is less
likelihood that the target string will activate a different, incorrect, system.
The current results are also in line with past research (Chiarello, et al., 1999) and our second
hypothesis, that both hemispheres would show orthographic priming. This was supported by the
findings that both hemispheres showed priming for words and pseudowords. These results suggest that
23
both hemispheres have access to orthographic processing abilities and are consistent with evidence
(e.g., Iacoboni & Zaidel, 1996; Rutherford, 2006), as well as Experiment I, that both hemispheres
contain lexical processing strategies that are required for orthographic processing. Lexical processing
involves a whole word matching strategy to a mental lexicon. More specifically, it involves looking at
the overall shape and size of a letter string and matching this to a string with the same shape and size
within the mental lexicon. This is precisely the strategy that is needed for orthographic processing.
Thus, the finding that both hemispheres are capable of orthographic processing further supports past
research on the laterality of lexical processing (e.g., Iacoboni & Zaidel, 1996; Rutherford, 2006).
In accordance with previous research (e.g., Chiarello, 1985; Lavidor & Ellis, 2001, 2003) as
well as our third hypothesis, the right hemisphere revealed greater orthographic priming than the left
hemisphere. This was demonstrated by three findings. First, non-primed pseudowords were processed
quicker in the right compared to the left hemisphere. Second, non-primed words were processed faster
in the right compared to the left hemisphere. Third, non-primed words were processed more accurately
in the right compared to the left hemisphere. These results appear counterintuitive at first. However, it is
believed that participants noticed the visual similarity between primed strings and consequently began
focusing on the visual appearance of all letter strings regardless of priming. This bias would compel the
participant to rely more heavily on orthographic processing strategies rather than other language
strategies such as phonological or semantic processing. Thus, with the results demonstrating quicker
and more accurate processing of non-primed strings in the right hemisphere the implication is that the
The three findings presented above also demonstrate fundamental differences between lexical
and non-lexical processing strategies. The first two findings that non-primed pseudowords and words
were processed faster in the right hemisphere compared to the left hemisphere suggests that lexical
processing is faster than non-lexical processing. As discussed above, it is believed that participants were
24
biased to focus on the visual aspects of the letter strings. This bias would imply that all stimuli
presented to the right hemisphere, primed or non-primed, words or pseudowords, would be processed
orthographically whereas the left hemisphere would maintain normal processing abilities. Thus, the
finding that the right hemisphere, using lexical strategies, was faster than the left hemisphere, using
non-lexical strategies, to process both non-primed pseudowords and words, suggest that non-lexical
processing is slower than lexical processing. The third finding, that non-primed words are more
accurate in the right compared to the left hemisphere, implies that not only is non-lexical processing a
slower strategy, but a less accurate one as well. Again, taking bias into account, the right hemisphere,
which uses lexical processing, performed more accurately than the left hemisphere, which can use both
lexical and non-lexical processing. It appears that non-lexical processing hinders rather than facilitates
accurate processing. This finding makes both theoretical and empirical sense. Non-lexical processing,
as discussed above, requires a much more complex process of sounding out each letter of a word and
then combining these sounds in order to sound out the word. Such a process requires multiple steps,
The finding that words and pseudowords showed significant priming in both hemispheres casts
doubt on the past argument that hemispheric asymmetries in orthographic processing are due to
neighborhood size. There are two reasons for this. First, orthographic neighborhood size refers to the
number of new words that can be created by manipulating one letter of the original word. According to
this definition, it is not entirely certain if pseudowords can have a neighborhood size. While a single
letter of a pseudoword can be manipulated to form another pseudoword they are not actual words and
would not have a lexical representation within the mental lexicon. Consequently, orthographic priming
should not have been seen in pseudoword strings. Second, high orthographic neighborhood is believed
to be processed only by the right hemisphere. Thus, the finding that high neighborhood stimuli revealed
priming in both hemispheres suggests neighborhood size may not produce hemispheric asymmetries.
25
These reasons alone are not enough to disprove the orthographic neighborhood argument; however, past
research has also found conflicting evidence for this argument. More specifically, Forster and Davis
(1991, as cited by Lavidor & Ellis, 2001) found that words with few or no orthographic neighbors,
produced facilitation effects if the two letter strings were only one letter different from one another.
This finding, as well as our own, suggests a better explanation for asymmetries in orthographic
processing is orthographic density (Lavidor & Ellis, 2001). Orthographic density refers to the number of
letters a string shares with another letter string (e.g., targ vs. harg). The primed stimuli in the present
study contained high orthographic density to one another, which would explain why priming was found
for pseudowords.
A possible explanation for right hemisphere superiority may involve asymmetries in spatial
processing. Orthographic processing requires the ability to process the spatial aspects of a word and to
match these same spatial aspects to the word stored in the mental lexicon. As past research has
demonstrated, the right hemisphere specializes at this type of processing (Witelson, 1976). Thus, since
the orthographic process relies on spatial processing and the right hemisphere has superior abilities in
this process, it is logical that the right hemisphere is better at this task than the left hemisphere.
General Discussion
Two experiments were undertaken to clarify conflicting results on the laterality of phonological
and orthographic processing. The results of Experiment I suggested phonologically primed letter strings
are processed more quickly and accurately than phonologically non-primed strings. More importantly,
the results revealed that while both hemispheres have access to simple phonological processing abilities,
only the left hemisphere is capable of complex phonological processing. Besides clarifying conflicting
results, these findings also lend support to what can be named the right hemisphere argument of
phonological dyslexia. Phonological dyslexia is a reading disorder in which individuals have trouble
converting letters-to-sound. One argument that has been proposed to explain phonological dyslexia is
26
that they rely on their inferior right hemisphere to process phonological information. The results from
the present experiment support this argument in a number of ways. First, while individuals with
phonological dyslexia have trouble converting letters-to-sound, they are not completely lacking this
ability. This may be explained by the fact that both hemispheres have access to simple phonological
processing. Second, the results from Experiment I demonstrated that only the left, not the right
hemisphere, contains complex phonological processing abilities. This finding, lends further support to
the idea that individuals with phonological dyslexia are relying on their inferior right hemisphere to
process phonological information. Potentially, this information could be used in a treatment program for
phonological dyslexia. Programs could be tailored to reverse, if possible, the hemispheric asymmetry to
The results of Experiment II showed that orthographically primed strings were processed faster
and more accurately than orthographically non-primed strings. As well, they revealed that both the left
and right hemisphere have orthographic processing abilities, through the right hemisphere has superior
orthographic processing. An important outcome in Experiment II was the finding that asymmetries in
orthographic processing may not be the result of orthographic neighborhood size. While orthographic
neighborhood size may play a role in orthographic processing, the current results suggest orthographic
density has a greater impact. Future studies on the laterality of reading should test more thoroughly the
orthographic neighborhood and orthographic density hypothesis to determine more accurately their
hemispheric influences.
When these experiments are taken together they advocate for a relative model of language
processing rather than an absolute model. As well, both of these studies inform us about hemispheric
specializations beyond that of language processing. They provide support for the laterality of sequential
and spatial processing. More specifically, these results support that the left hemisphere is specialized for
sequential processing, demonstrated by superior phonological processing in the left hemisphere, while
27
the right hemisphere is specialized for spatial processing, demonstrated by superior orthographic
References
Annett, M. (1970). A classification of hand preference by association analysis. British Journal of
Psychology, 61, 303-321.
Asbjornsen, E. A., & Helland, T. (2006). Laterality: asymmetries of body, brain and
cognition. Laterality, 11(3), 251-262.
Beeman, J. M., & Chiarello, C. (1998). Complementary Right- and Left- Hemisphere
Language Comprehension. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7(1), 2-7.
Bloch, I. M., & Zaidel, E. (1996). Differences in Hemisphereic Asymmetry Between
Dyslexic and Normal Children on a Lateralized Lexical Decision Task. Laterality,
1(3), 225-239.
Broca, 1861; Wernicke, 1911, as cited by Szaflarski, P. J., Holland, K. S., Schmithorst, J.
V., & Byars, W. A. (2006). fMRI Study of Language lateralization in children and
adults. Human Brain Mapping, 27, 202-212.
Chiarello, C. (1985). Hemisphere Dynamics in Lexical Access: Automatic and
Controlled Priming. Brain and Language, 26, 146-172.
Chiarello, C., Hasbrook, R., & Maxfield, L. (1999). Orthographic and Phonological
Facilitation from Unattended Words: Evidence for Bilateral Processing.
Laterality, 4(2), 97-125.
Cowan 1995, as cited by Rutherford, J. B., & Lutz, T. K. (2004). Conflicting strategies
and hemispheric suppression in a lexical decision task. Brain and Cognition, 55,
387-391.
Crossman, L. D. & Polich, J. (1988). Hemispheric Differences for Orthographic and
Phonological Processing. Brain and Language, 35, 301-312.
Efron, R. (1963). Temporal perception, aphasia and dj vu. Brain, 86, 403-424.
Forster, 1987, as cited by Lavidor, M. & Ellis, W. A. (2003). Orthographic and
phonological priming in the two cerebral hemispheres. Laterality, 8(3), 201-223.
Hutchins, S. & Palmer, C. (2008). Repetition Priming in Music. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Human perception and performance, 34(3), 693-707.
29
Appendix A
Demographic Form
Experiment Name:
Participant #
Male/Female
Medication?
Birth Date:
Percentile Rank:
Handedness score
33
Appendix B
Which hand do you habitually use for the following tasks? Print left (L), right (R), or either (E) beside each
question:
6. To guide a thread through the eye of a needle (or guide needle on to thread)
Appendix C
Appendix D
Informed Consent
38
Informed Consent
Brain Interhemispheric Communication And Reading
Principal Investigator: Dr. Barbara Rutherford
Psychology Department
3333 University Way, V1V 1V7
(250) 807-8734
[email protected]
Sponsor: Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council
Introduction:
The University of British Columbia-Okanagan subscribes to the ethical conduct of research and to
the protection at all times of the interests, comfort, and safety of the study of subjects. The information
provided in this form is being given to you for your own protection and full understanding of the procedures,
risks and benefits associated with this research.
The consent form is only part of the process of informed consent. It should give you a basic idea of
what the research is about and what your participation will involve. If you would like more details, feel free
to ask the researcher presenting this form at any time. Please take the time to read this carefully and to
understand any accompanying information.
Purpose Of The Study:
This study seeks to better understand how the hemispheres of the brain interact when people read.
Study Procedures:
You will be asked to engage in 3 tasks.
(1) Handedness questionnaire: This asks which hand you habitually use to perform different tasks (e.g.
writing, throwing a ball).
(2) Reading Aloud: You will be asked to read aloud a short list of fake words (e.g. blurp).
(3) Computer task. You will be seated in front of a computer screen with your chin on a chinrest. A letter
string will appear on the monitor. Your job is to decide whether or not the letters spell a word. You will
press one or other of a pair of computer keys to indicate the decision. On some of the trials, a distractor
located to one or other side of the letter string will blink on and off for a short period of time.
Potential Risks And Benefits:
This project has been reviewed and granted a certificate of approval by the UBCO Research Ethics
Board.
This research is important because it will increase our understanding of how the hemispheres of the
brain interact when people read. In turn, the findings will be useful to the development of programs to better
help those who struggle with reading
39
You understand that any risk from participation in this research is minimal because the tasks are those
normally associated with everyday living (e.g. filling out a form, speaking aloud, and sitting in front of a
computer screen and pressing computer keys).
Confidentiality:
You understand that all data collected from you will be coded by a number and not your name;
therefore, your identity will be kept confidential. The only people who will have access to the data are the
principal investigator and designated research assistant(s). The data will be stored in a locked file cabinet and
in a password-protected file on a computer in the psychology laboratory complex. All data will be destroyed
7 years after the findings have been published.
Remuneration/Compensation:
Introductory Psychology students at UBCO will receive a 1% course bonus credit/hour of research
participation. UBCO students who are either ineligible for bonus credits or do not wish to receive bonus
credits may receive $10/hour research participation.
Consent:
Your participation in the study is entirely voluntary and you may refuse to participate at any time
during the testing session without consequence. Prior to leaving the testing session, you may instruct the
researcher to destroy your data and watch while the computer file is deleted and the paper documents are
shredded.
Your signature on this form indicates that you understand the information provided regarding this
research project including all procedures and the personal risks involved. Your participation in this project is
in no way related to your status as a student. You understand that your identity and any identifying
information will be kept confidential.
Your signature below indicates that you consent to participate in this study. You will receive a copy
of this consent form for your own records.
40
Appendix E
Debriefing Form
42