Health Psychology Review: Click For Updates
Health Psychology Review: Click For Updates
Health Psychology Review: Click For Updates
I write about illness to work out some terms in which it can be acceptedSeizing the
opportunity means experiencing it fully, then letting go and moving on.
- Sociologist Arthur W. Frank (1991, p. 3) on writing about his cancer
Cancer can be an overwhelming and traumatic event in a persons life, impacting not only
physical health but also emotional, social and spiritual functioning. Over the past several
decades, much attention has been paid to how cancer affects the whole person, and
particularly how to promote psychosocial adjustment to the disease. Interventions
including psychotherapy (Hart et al., 2012), peer groups (Hoey, Ieropoli, White, &
Jefford, 2008) and Internet-based support (Hybye et al., 2010) have been used in cancer
patients with some benefits. Another complementary intervention that has gained
substantial interest among psycho-oncology clinicians is expressive writing (EW), an
340
emotional disclosure technique wherein a person writes about a major life event or
trauma.
The first standardised EW prompt, developed in 1986 by Pennebaker and Beall,
instructed respondents to write for approximately 20 minutes over four consecutive days
about their deepest emotions and thoughts regarding traumatic/upsetting experiences.
Early studies reported that, compared to control participants who wrote factual essays
about superficial (i.e., non-traumatic) topics, those who wrote expressively gained
physical and psychological health benefits over time (e.g., Pennebaker, 1993), suggesting
that a simple writing exercise can improve health. These findings have been supported by
Frattarolis (2006) meta-analysis, which reported a small health-enhancing effect of
written or spoken emotional disclosure across 10,994 people (unweighted mean effect
size = .075; d = .15).
How does expressive writing work?
Several theoretical frameworks have been proposed to explain the mechanisms
underlying the benefits of EW. There is likely no single explanatory route, but rather a
complex combination of interconnected and mutually influential processes. It is possible
that EW serves a cathartic function, allowing the writer to express and release their
emotions, which in turn improves their well-being (Pennebaker & Chung, 2007).
Cognitive restructuring of a trauma and habituation to the emotions associated with that
trauma may also facilitate the effectiveness of EW (Pennebaker & Chung, 2011). That is,
repeatedly writing about ones experience with cancer may serve an exposure function.
Writing may lead to initial distress, but it also enables the writer to process, reflect and
reframe their experience, extinguishing the connection between the event and their
emotional reaction to it, and assimilating it into their self-schema (Low, Stanton, &
Danoff-Burg, 2006; Pennebaker & Chung, 2011). Another explanation is that EW offers
an opportunity for personal mastery and self-regulation (Creswell et al., 2007; Lepore,
Greenberg, Bruno, & Smyth, 2002). EW may enable people to step back and observe
how they handled their cancer experience, increasing their confidence to manage stress
and regulate their emotions, thoughts and behaviours. It is also possible that EW serves to
restore a sense of perceived control, which can be lost or diminished in the context of a
serious illness (Andersson & Conley, 2008).
Written content
The linguistic characteristics of EW passages provide information regarding whether the
intervention was followed (i.e., manipulation check) and the emotional processing of the
writer, which can provide insight into the mechanisms underlying the impact of EW.
However, reliably and validly evaluating written content is challenging. Computer text
analysis programs have been developed, including the Linguistic Inquiry Word Count
program (LIWC; Pennebaker, Booth, & Francis, 2007), which calculates the frequency
and percentage of words in 74 content categories. Using LIWC, researchers have
demonstrated that emotional (e.g., happy and depressed) and cognitive mechanism (e.g.,
because and realise) words predict changes in physical and psychological health after
EW (Pennebaker, Mayne, & Francis, 1997). Although such programs offer quick,
inexpensive and reliable assessment, they are not sensitive to contextual aspects of
language (e.g., tone, cultural jargon and abstraction; Tausczik & Pennebaker, 2010).
341
Trained independent judges and self-ratings have also been widely adopted to
appraise aspects of written passages (e.g., degree of emotionality, personal meaning and
narrative structure) that are not detected by computers. Both methods have advantages
and shortcomings. Judges are more objective than self-rating, but they are time
consuming and costly, and inter-rater reliability can be problematic (Tausczik &
Pennebaker, 2010). Although self-rating is easier to implement, it assumes a sophisticated
level of evaluation on behalf of the writer, and ratings may be influenced by recognition
of condition assignment (i.e., expectation/performance bias from the informed consent
process and knowledge of the prompt). However, both methods have been shown to
sufficiently analyse text characteristics (e.g., Danoff-Burg, Mosher, Seawell, & Agee,
2010; Smyth, True, & Souto, 2001).
342
343
Over 8 weeks, EW did not directly improve pain or well-being (insufficient information
to calculate ds; ps > .05).
Emotional disclosure interacted with EW to predict pain and well-being over time.
Respondents with at least one essay rated as very much disclosure (vs. respondents
with no essays rated as very much disclosure) reported improved pain (2.5
difference; 95% CI: 4.2 to 0.9) and well-being (+1.37 difference; 95% CI: 1 to 1.7).
At 1 and 6 months, EMO did not significantly improve quality of life compared to NWc
(ds = .40, .06) or USUAL (ds = .62, .56). At 1 and 6 months, EMO did not
significantly improve quality of life compared to NWc (ds = .62, .48) or USUAL (ds =
.34, .25).
For EMO, quality of life increased from baseline to 1 and 6 months (ps < .05). For NWc,
quality of life increased from baseline to 1 month (p < .05).
Scores were pooled across the five follow-ups. Over time, EW did not significantly
improve distress (d = .35), perceived stress (d = .17) or overall mood disturbance (d =
.18) excepting the vigour subscale (d = .64). EW significantly improved overall sleep
disturbance (d = .64), sleep quality (d = 1.3), sleep duration (d = .87), daytime
dysfunction (d = .87); however, sleep latency (d = .65), sleep efficiency (d = .11),
sleep disturbances (d = .44), sleep medications (d = .09) not significantly changed.
At 3 days pre-surgery and 2 weeks post-surgery, EW did not directly improve distress (ds
= .07, .01), perceived stress (ds = .24, .16), sleep disturbance (ds = .13, .33), worst
pain (ds = .10, .67), least pain (ds = .28, .45), average pain (ds = .05, .40) or pain
interference (ds = .23, .54). EW reported increased sleep medication use 3 days before
surgery (d = .57); group differences at baseline were not reported.
Social constraints interacted with EW to predict pre-surgery (p = .05) and post-surgery (p
= .05) average pain. High social constraints in EW associated with less pain; low
social constraints in EW associated with more pain.
344
Table 1. Studies (N = 13) reporting experimental trials of expressive writing interventions in cancer patients.
Table 1 (Continued)
Task and follow-up
At 1, 3 and 6 months, EW did not directly improve quality of life (ds = .16, .00, .07),
mood disturbance (ds = .15, .07, .10), healthcare utilisation or satisfaction with
perceived practical support (insufficient information to calculate latter ds; ps > .05).
EW directly improved perceived emotional support (ds = .36, .48, .50; overall d over
time = .47) at all three follow-ups.
Jensen-Johansen
et al. (2012)
N = 507
Breast cancer
stages 1-2
Race: NR
Mage = 53.6 9.1
All female
At 3 and 9 months, EW did not directly improve distress (ds = .15, .04), intrusive
thoughts (ds =.07, .03), avoidance (ds = .18, .08), depression (ds = .12, .07), negative
mood (ds = .04, .10) or positive mood (ds =.06, .01).
Externally oriented thinking interacted with EW to predict distress at 3 months (p =
.021). In EW (vs. NW), lower external orientation predicted less distress. Difficulties
describing feelings interacted with EW to predict positive mood at 3 months (p =
.049). In NW (vs. EW), greater difficulties describing feelings predicted better mood.
Writing topic (cancer vs. another trauma) moderated the effects of EW to predict
avoidance (p < .05), depression (p = .013), negative mood (p = .052), positive mood (p
= .014). Writing about another trauma (vs. cancer) predicted less avoidance. Writing
about cancer (vs. NW) improved depression and mood. Number of essays written,
social constraints, other alexithymia variables did not interact with EW (ps > .05).
345
Gellaitry et al.
(2010)
N = 80
Breast cancer
stages 12
Race: NR
Mage = 57.9 9.9
All female
Rosenberg et al.
(2002)
N = 30
Prostate cancer
stage NR
Race: 97% White
Mage = 70.4 5.4
All male
346
Table 1 (Continued)
Table 1 (Continued)
Task and follow-up
stages 12
Race: 93% White
Mage = 49.5 12.2
All female
At the four follow-ups, EW (1 and 3 doses) did not directly improve (p > .0125) negative
affect (1 dose ds = .22, .53, .64, .49; 3 dose ds = .11, .32, .48, .49), positive affect (1
dose ds = .79, .31, .49, .66; 3 dose ds = .40, .43, .05, .25), avoidance of cancer
reminders (1 dose ds = .04, .48, .35, .15; 3 dose ds = .00, .20, .01, .02) or intrusiveness
of thoughts (1 dose ds = .15, .13, .39, .60; 3 dose ds = .01, .18, .04, .34).
Zakowski et al.
(2004)
N = 104
Prostate,
gynaecological
stages 14
Race: 95.2% White
Mage = 59.8 11.1
51.9% female
347
ATT = non-cancer attention; BEN = benefit-finding (cancer) prompt; EMO = emotional (cancer) prompt; EMO = emotional (any trauma) prompt; MULTIPLE= emotional, benefitfinding, cognitive-appraisal, coping prompts; NR = not reported; NW = neutral writing (trivial); NWc = neutral writing (cancer); NWb = neutral writing (health behaviour); STORY =
cancer story; SURVEY = took survey; USUAL = usual care
348
emotionally about cancer, the other wrote emotionally about any trauma (the authors did
not report whether any patients in the latter group chose to write about cancer). Stanton
et al. (2002) used two experimental groups: one wrote emotionally about cancer, the other
wrote about their positive thoughts and feelings related to cancer (i.e., benefit-finding).
Henry, Schlegel, Talley, Molix, and Bettencourt (2010) used the benefit-finding prompt
only. Alternatively, the study by Gellaitry, Peters, Bloomfield, and Home (2010) used a
different topic for each of the four writing sessions (emotional, benefit-finding, cognitive
appraisals of cancer and coping strategies); and the study by Cepeda et al. (2008)
prompted patients to write a story about how cancer affected their lives (p. 625).
The majority of control conditions instructed participants to engage in writing. The
studies by Low, Stanton, Bower, and Gyllenhammer (2010) and Stanton et al. (2002)
used a neutral prompt regarding the facts of their cancer (e.g., number of chemotherapy
sessions, oncologists name and medication). Alternatively, deMoor et al.s (2002, 2008)
neutral prompt asked participants to write factually about health behaviours (e.g., diet and
sleep). Three studies used a neutral prompt to write about the facts of a trivial/everyday
topic (e.g., daily activities; Jensen-Johansen et al., 2012; Mosher et al., 2012; Zakowski,
Ramati, Morton, Johnson, & Flanigan, 2004). Craft et al. (2013) had two control
conditions: neutral writing about the facts of their cancer2 and usual care (with no
writing). Cepeda et al. (2008) had two conditions: usual care and completing a pain
questionnaire. Usual care was the only control condition in three studies (Gellaitry et al.,
2010; Henry et al., 2010; Rosenberg et al., 2002). One study used an attentional control
wherein patients met with a researcher to discuss non-cancer life events (Walker, Nail, &
Croyle, 1999).
Effects of expressive writing on physical and mental health
The effects of EW on physical and mental health outcomes are presented in Table 1. All
13 studies reported some null effects of experimental condition on the outcomes of
general or existential well-being, quality of life, social support satisfaction, coping,
distress, stress, affect, mood, depression, intrusive thoughts, avoidance, somatisation,
vigour, pain, sleep characteristics, healthcare utilisation and cancer-related immune
functioning. Six studies reported some significant benefits for EW. Standardised effect
sizes for statistically significant main effects indicating that EW improved physical and/or
mental health were small (d = .20) to large (d = 1.21) in magnitude, with the majority
being in the moderate range (ds = .54 to .76). Improvements were noted in energy and
sleep characteristics (deMoor et al., 2002), depressive (Henry et al., 2010) and physical
symptoms (Henry et al., 2010; Stanton et al., 2002), emotional support (Gellaitry et al.,
2010), pain (Rosenberg et al., 2002), uptake of mental health services (Mosher et al.,
2012) and healthcare utilisation (Stanton et al., 2002).
Six studies reported significant moderators of EW. The extent of perceived social
constraints (i.e., inhibition from expressing thoughts/feelings due to social restrictions or
norms) affected the impact of the intervention in two studies (deMoor et al., 2008;
Zakowski et al., 2004). deMoor et al. (2008) found that patients with high levels of social
constraints who wrote expressively had lower average daily pain before and after surgery
compared to high-constraint patients who wrote about health behaviours. The pain
reported by the EW group actually paralleled all patients who reported low social
constraints, suggesting that EW buffers the deleterious effects of inhibition. Similarly,
Zakowski et al. (2004) reported that patients with high social constraints who wrote
349
350
writing (Henry et al., 2010). Stanton et al. (2002) also reported that respondents found
EW to be a valuable/meaningful experience that provided them with positive, long-lasting
effects, and did not elicit negative effects.
351
Table 2. Risk of bias in studies reporting experimental trials of expressive writing interventions in
cancer patients.
Random
sequence
Allocation
generation concealment
Cepeda
et al. (2008)
Craft
et al. (2013)
deMoor
et al. (2002)
deMoor
et al. (2008)
Gellaitry
et al. (2010)
Henry
et al. (2010)
Jensen-Johansen
et al. (2012)
Low
et al. (2010)
Mosher
et al. (2012)
Rosenberg
et al. (2002)
Stanton
et al. (2002)
Walker
et al. (1999)
Zakowski
et al. (2004)
Blinding of
participants
and
personnel
Blinding
of
outcome
assessment
Incomplete
outcome
data
addressed
Selective
reporting
Note: low risk of bias: +, high risk of bias: , unclear risk of bias: ?
Johansen et al., 2012; Mosher et al., 2012). Two studies did not indicate the method of
random assignment (Rosenberg et al., 2002; Zakowski et al., 2004). The three studies
with the most significant risk used sequential assignment (Craft et al., 2013; Walker et al.,
1999) or matched controls (Henry et al., 2010).
Three studies were considered low risk of selection bias due to allocation
concealment (Cepeda et al., 2008; Low et al., 2010; Stanton et al., 2002) and three
studies were considered high risk (Craft et al., 2013; Henry et al., 2010; Walker et al.,
1999). The remaining studies did not provide sufficient details to draw a conclusion,
although Jensen-Johansen et al. (2012) did indicate that allocation was concealed prior to
group assignment without specifying the method.
As in any EW study, the risk of performance bias in the reviewed papers was high
overall. It is not possible to fully blind participants to condition assignment, as awareness
of the prompt is required to complete the task. Only one study described efforts to
352
353
(Mosher et al., 2012), health behaviours (deMoor et al., 2002) or cancer facts (Stanton
et al., 2002)], making it difficult to ascertain which aspects of EW are therapeutic, if any.
The implications of the demonstrated benefits of EW should also be considered
within the context of potential threats to the validity of these findings. Risk of biases
varied across the studies in the current review, although several trends were observed. In
general, there was little risk of biased findings due to selection, attrition or reporting bias.
Among the studies with significant main effects, Henry et al. 2010) conferred the greatest
amount of risk mainly because the matched groups design increased selection bias.
All EW studies carry a high risk of performance and detection biases. As part of the
informed consent process, experimenters warn participants about the risks and benefits of
the study and that they may be asked to write about their emotional experience with
cancer, which could be upsetting. Upon receiving instructions, participants become aware
of which condition they were assigned to, potentially creating different emotional and
behavioural reactions between the groups. One consequence of performance and
detection biases is that individuals in the experimental condition may experience (i.e.,
performance bias) or report (i.e., detection bias) improvements that are due to their
expectations about EW, rather than the actual EW task. The highest risk for this is in
studies where non-writing controls are used (e.g., usual care or attentional control; six
studies in the current review), because allocation is more obvious than in studies that
employ some type of neutral writing. Three of these non-writing control studies yielded
main effects (Gellaitry et al., 2010; Henry et al., 2010; Rosenberg et al., 2002), which is
noteworthy given that these represent half of the studies in the review with significant
results of experimental condition.
Although performance and detection biases cannot be totally eliminated given the
nature of the EW task, they can be attenuated by blinding study personnel to condition or
to purpose/hypotheses during all parts of the study that require participant contact. Some
studies reported efforts within the early stages of a participants protocol [e.g.,
interviewers in Rosenberg et al.s (2002) study were blind to condition before the
baseline assessment], during the intervention (Stanton et al., 2002) and during outcome
assessment (Cepeda et al., 2008; Mosher et al., 2012; Walker et al., 1999). However, it
should be noted that even in studies that did not report these efforts, contact between
researchers and participants was typically minimal after the instructions were
administered.
A number of methodological issues should also be considered when appraising the
quality of the evidence in the reviewed studies. Variability in the experimental and control
conditions may have impacted the findings. Most studies used a uniform EW prompt
across administrations, although Gellaitry et al., (2010) used different prompts across
sessions, and Jensen-Johansen et al. (2012) varied the instructions, threatening the
consistency (and thus the internal validity) of the study. Emotional, benefit-finding and
multiple-prompt writing instructions all elicited significant and null main effects across a
myriad of outcomes. Control conditions also ranged from usual care, to receiving
attention, to neutral writing about non-cancer topics, to cancer facts, which more
stringently controls for mere exposure to cancer-related content. Thus, the mixed results
produced by the included studies may not be due entirely to EW being ineffective in this
population, but rather an artefact of comparing apples with oranges. The specific features
of EW that are effective in cancer populations are clearly not well understood.
It is interesting that most EW protocols instructed participants to write expressively
about their cancer. Although it is reasonable to assume that a cancer diagnosis is the most
354
central traumatic issue in ones life, this is not necessarily the case. For example, the
study by Jensen-Johansen et al. (2012), which allowed participants to self-modify the
expressive writing topic, found no main effects for EW. However, chosen topic (cancer
vs. other) moderated the efficacy of EW, with each group reporting different benefits
(Jensen-Johansen et al., 2012). This raises an interesting point: in healthy populations, for
which EW effects have been greater, participants can write about any trauma; in cancer,
researchers have generally tailored the prompt to be cancer specific. In doing so, the
possibility that some patients may be even more distressed by other issues in their lives
may have been overlooked. In allowing for a choice, Jensen-Johansen et al. (2012)
provided the opportunity for participants to tailor the intervention, leading to improved
outcomes for both topics. Such flexibility in clinical settings has also been generally
recommended by Pennebaker (2010) who wrote, Encourage people to write about what
is bothering them, rather than what you think is bothering them (p. 24). Interestingly,
Craft et al. (2013) also included a condition wherein patients were instructed to write
about any trauma (the nature of the chosen topics was not reported; therefore, it is
unknown whether any of these patients wrote about cancer) in addition to the cancer
trauma EW group. All of the writing groups in Crafts study (i.e., emotional writing about
cancer, emotional writing about any trauma, neutral writing about cancer) had an increase
in quality of life from baseline to the first follow-up, whereas the usual care group did
not. However, when all four groups were compared against one another, the only
significant group difference that emerged was between those who wrote neutrally about
cancer and those who received usual care and therefore did not write anything.
Threats to internal validity due to lower control in clinical studies are also relevant.
Given the challenges of recruiting and retaining medical patients, location of settings
varied (e.g., home and waiting rooms) and participants often completed writing sessions
over periods that were longer than the original recommendation of four consecutive days.
The extent to which these differences influenced outcomes is unclear; however, it is
important to recognise that the many differences between the clinic and highly controlled
laboratory settings may be important.
Differences in the timing of follow-up assessments should also be considered. In the
study by deMoor et al. (2002), the first follow-up assessment was conducted immediately
following the intervention, which may have affected the pooled estimates of the
outcomes. People typically feel somewhat worse after EW (Pennebaker & Beall, 1986;
Smyth, 1998); thus, the observed effects for EW on distress, stress and mood may have
been diminished in this study. In addition, Cepeda et al. (2008) conducted the first three
assessments (out of eight) during the same weeks that patients engaged in EW. Although
no main effects for EW on pain or quality of life emerged even by the eighth week of the
study, it is possible that the immediacy of the first several follow-ups contaminated the
outcome measurements.
Another possibility is that the EW protocols (whether one session of writing or
multiple sessions) were considered single interventions. Health-promoting therapies are
not generally administered once and then expected to provide benefits into the future
without repetition. Perhaps EW should be considered a repeatable exercise, or part of a
larger therapeutic programme that includes other components such as psychoeducation
and group therapy, as was recently demonstrated in patients with colorectal cancer
(Carmack et al., 2011).
A wide array of dependent variables was evaluated in the included studies, increasing
the possibility of chance findings. Also, p-value corrections were not typically applied,
355
including in studies that evaluated a variety of outcomes from the same domain [e.g.,
deMoor et al. (2002) evaluated 19 outcomes of stress, mood and sleep; and Craft et al.
(2013) analysed the same quality of life variable in several ways with alphas set at .05].
The reviewed studies included outcomes ranging from affect and cognitions, to sleep and
pain, to disease markers and immune functioning. It seems that a kitchen sink of
outcomes has been evaluated, rather than identifying key areas of change expected from
EW in the context of cancer.
Alternatively, the effectiveness of EW could be dependent upon individual differences
(e.g., social constraints, alexithymia, avoidance, emotional support and time since
diagnosis) and thus the effects of the intervention may not be detectable until these
features are partialed out. Several studies evaluated potential moderators to this end;
however, more research on moderators is warranted as these findings were derived from
single studies, except those regarding social constraints.
Another potential explanation is that the effect size for EW is simply smaller for
clinical samples as compared to healthy samples (Frisina et al., 2004). Perhaps clear
reductions in pathology are more than should be expected from EW in the context of a
life-altering and potentially life-ending disease. Even when EW did not elicit changes in
the presence or absence of symptoms, participant feedback was overwhelmingly positive
and patients generally reported appreciating the exercise. Researchers and clinicians may
want to consider the possibility that EW provides benefits that are more existential in
nature. Notably, one of the reviewed studies (Mosher et al., 2012) investigated the
possibility of existential gains using standardised self-report tools, without significant
findings. Nevertheless, there is merit in continuing to explore this possibility.
Use of written content
This review found that text analysis is used primarily as a manipulation check and is
greatly underused as a process variable within experimental trials of cancer patients. This
is unfortunate given that linguistic information could enhance theoretical understanding
of why EW works and for whom, within this population. Such information could also
serve as an aid in decision-making with regard to intervention design (e.g., how many
sessions to prescribe). For example, Walker et al. (1999) evaluated written content in their
substantive analyses, finding that emotional disclosure and present/future orientation
words increased from the first essay to the third. The authors concluded that the practical
implication of this finding is that greater doses of EW in clinical settings encourage
more disclosure, although notably this did not elicit differences on the studied outcomes
(Walker et al., 1999). The only study from the current review to investigate linguistic
characteristics as a process variable found that greater disclosure (i.e., exposure to ones
emotions) predicted reduced pain, suggesting that disclosure may be the key mechanism
through which EW is effective (Cepeda et al., 2008). However, caution regarding this
finding is warranted given the evidence for selective reporting bias in this study.
Other studies have analysed text in cancer studies with promising results. In a reanalysis of Stanton et al.s (2002) data, a greater number of negative emotion words was
associated with fewer somatic symptoms, suggesting that the positive effects of EW are
contingent on habituation to an emotional reaction to cancer through the writing process
(Low et al., 2006). That is, it may be necessary to expose the writer to their emotions
related to cancer, rather than the facts of the event itself, for EW to be effective (Low
et al., 2006). In a correlational study of women with metastatic breast cancer, Laccetti
356
(2007) found that more positive words were associated with better emotional well-being.
Smith et al. (2005) also found that greater negative emotion words predicted higher
anxiety and depression after adjusting for baseline mood in a prepost study of newly
diagnosed breast cancer patients. In contrast, Morgan, Graves, Pogi, and Cheson (2008)
found that while greater use of affect, positive emotion and anxious words correlated with
better physical health-related quality of life in leukaemia and lymphoma patients, and
using more sad and anxious words correlated with poorer mental health-related quality of
life, the relationship was explained by pre-intervention quality of life.
357
who are inhibited from expressing their emotions to their spouses suffer more problems in
mood and intrusive thoughts than female cancer patients with similar levels of inhibition.
This recommendation has both theoretical and practical advantages because further
analysis of moderators may enhance understanding of the fundamental bases of how EW
works within the context of cancer, and also may inform the identification of individuals
in cancer clinics who may benefit from or be injured by EW.
More practical questions should focus on feasibility and effectiveness in clinical
settings. Great efforts were taken in the current studies to encourage task compliance, but
clinical researchers may want to design studies wherein these contingencies are not in
place to determine how viable EW is in real-world settings. It is also notable that there
was significant attrition among the pre-surgical patients in the study by deMoor et al.
(2008), whereas there was less attrition in studies that enrolled patients at other points in
their cancer treatment. At least one other published study has also reported completion
issues, with only two of the 24 participants in palliative care for advanced cancer
completing all four sessions (Bruera, Willey, Cohen, & Palmer, 2008). It is possible that
writing during a trauma or immediately afterwards could cause more harm than good
(Pennebaker, 2010), potentially explaining this high level of dropout. Thus, it may be
worthwhile to determine whether there is an optimal time or condition for EW
interventions.
In addition, while symptomatology relevant to cancer patients (e.g., physical health,
affect, mood and fatigue) should continue to be assessed in EW studies, qualitative and
descriptive outcome assessments should also be considered. Pennebaker and Beall (1986)
suggested that EW improves physical health (e.g., number of illnesses and number of
days one is restricted due to illness), which may be more relevant in healthy populations.
For cancer patients, EW may have a less obvious effect on health; rather, it may provide
comfort in other ways, such as with existential concerns, personal growth or meaningmaking.
Finally, it is also recommended that researchers expand their use of text analysis. In
particular, LIWC and trained judges (whenever possible, given time and cost constraints)
are recommended. The LIWC program appears to have many potential benefits, including
demonstrated sensitivity, convergent validity and discriminant validity for identifying
emotional expression in cancer patients (Bantum & Owen, 2009). It has also been used to
demonstrate how cognitive and affective word choice in EW passages predicts quality of
life outcomes in cancer studies that did not meet the current reviews inclusion criteria.
Conclusions
Taken together, EW appears to be generally feasible for cancer patients, and there were
some positive outcomes. Given the current findings, a reliable recommendation cannot be
made, and healthcare providers should recognise that the evidence is not clear about the
therapeutic value of EW in cancer. There were generally no adverse consequences, with
the exception of increased sleep disturbance among patients who had been living with
breast cancer for more than 4.7 years (Low et al., 2010) and increased sleep medication
usage in breast cancer patients three days prior to surgery (although it is unknown what
the mechanism for the uptake of these hypnotics was; deMoor et al., 2008). This is
consistent with other EW studies, suggesting that EW is likely safe to use with this
population, even if the measured gains are not remarkable (Baikie & Wilhelm, 2005).
Many participants reported enjoying the intervention, and thus, for cancer patients who
358
Supplemental material
Notes
1. Although including grey literature may attenuate publication bias, given that published studies
generally have larger treatment effects than unpublished studies (Hopewell, McDonald, Clarke,
& Egger, 2007), grey literature can also introduce bias due to poorer methodological quality
(Egger, Juni, Barlett, Holenstein, & Sterne, 2003) and comprehensiveness of locatable grey
literature (Sterne, Egger, & Moher, 2011). Given that reviews that include grey literature produce
similar results to those that do not (Egger et al., 2003), it was decided to restrict the search to
published findings.
2. The authors described the neutral writing condition as an experimental group; however, for
consistency across studies it was categorized as a control condition for the purpose of this
review.
3. Jensen-Johansen and colleagues (2012) performed their manipulation check by evaluating
participants mood immediately following the EW intervention, rather than assessing the
linguistic characteristics of the essays.
References
Andersson, M. A., & Conley, C. S. (2008). Expecting to heal through self-expression: A perceived
control theory of writing and health. Health Psychology Review, 2, 138162. doi:10.1080/
17437190802660890
Baikie, K. A., & Wilhelm, K. (2005). Emotional and physical health benefits of expressive writing.
Advances in Psychiatric Treatment, 11, 338346. doi:10.1192/apt.11.5.338
Bantum, E. O., & Owen, J. E. (2009). Evaluating the validity of computerized content analysis
programs for identification of emotional expression in cancer narratives. Psychological
Assessment, 21, 7988. doi:10.1037/a0014643
Broderick, J. E., Stone, A. A., Smyth, J. M., & Kaell, A. T. (2004). The feasibility and effectiveness
of an expressive writing intervention for rheumatoid arthritis via home-based videotaped
instructions. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 27(1), 5059. doi:10.1207/s15324796abm2701_7
Bruera, E., Willey, J., Cohen, M., & Palmer, J. L. (2008). Expressive writing in patients receiving
palliative care: A feasibility study. Journal of Palliative Medicine, 11(1), 1519. doi:10.1089/
jpm.2007.0112
Carmack, C. L., Basen-Engquist, K., Yuan, Y., Greisinger, A., Rodriguez-Bigas, M., Wolff, R. A.,
Pennebaker, J. W. (2011). Feasibility of an expressive-disclosure group intervention for posttreatment colorectal cancer patients: Results of the healthy expressions study. Cancer, 117, 4993
5002. doi:10.1002/cncr.26110
Cepeda, M. S., Chapman, C. R., Miranda, N., Sanchez, R., Rodriguez, C. H., Restrepo, A. E.,
Carr, D. B. (2008). Emotional disclosure through patient narrative may improve pain and wellbeing: Results of a randomized controlled trial in patients with cancer pain. Journal of Pain and
Symptom Management, 35, 623631. doi:10.1016/j.jpainsymman.2007.08.011
Craft, M. A., Davis, G. C., & Paulson, R. M. (2013). Expressive writing in early breast cancer
survivors. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 69, 305315. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2648.2012.06008.x
359
Creswell, J. D., Lam, S., Stanton, A. L., Taylor, S. E., Bower, J. E., & Sherman, D. K. (2007). Does
self-affirmation, cognitive processing, or discovery of meaning explain cancer-related health
benefits of expressive writing? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 238250.
doi:10.1177/0146167206294412
Danoff-Burg, S., Mosher, C. E., Seawell, A. H., & Agee, J. D. (2010). Does narrative writing
instruction enhance the benefits of expressive writing? Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 23, 341352.
doi:10.1080/10615800903191137
deMoor, C., Moy, L., Low, D., Rivera, E., Singletary, S. E., Fouladi, R. T., & Cohen, L. (2008).
Expressive writing as a presurgical stress management intervention for breast cancer patients.
Journal of the Society for Integrative Oncology, 6, 5966.
deMoor C., Sterner J., Hall M.Warneke, C., Gilani, Z., Amato, R., & Cohen, L. (2002). A pilot
study of the effects of expressive writing on psychological and behavioral adjustment in patients
enrolled in a Phase II trial of vaccine therapy for metastatic renal cell carcinoma. Health
Psychology, 21, 615619. doi:10.1037//0278-6133.21.6.615
Egger, M., Juni, P., Bartlett, C., Holenstein, F., & Sterne, J. (2003). How important are
comprehensive literature searches and the assessment of trial quality in systematic reviews?
Empirical study. Health Technology Assessment, 7, 176.
Frank, A. W. (1991). At the will of the body: Reflections on illness. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.
Frattaroli, J. (2006). Experimental disclosure and its moderators: A meta-analysis. Psychological
Bulletin, 132, 823865. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.6.823
Frisina, P. G., Borod, I. C., & Lepore, S. J. (2004). A meta-analysis of the effects of written
emotional disclosure on the health outcomes of clinical populations. Journal of Nervous and
Mental Disease, 192, 629634. doi:10.1097/01.nmd.0000138317.30764.63
Gellaitry, G., Peters, K., Bloomfield, D., & Home, R. (2010). Narrowing the gap: The effects of an
expressive writing intervention on perceptions of actual and ideal emotional support in women
who have completed treatment for early stage breast cancer. Psycho-Oncology, 19, 7784.
doi:10.1002/pon.1532
Harris, A. H. S., Thoresen, C. E., Humphreys, K., & Faul, J. (2005). Does writing affect asthma? A
randomized trial. Psychosomatic Medicine, 67, 130136. doi:10.1097/01.psy.0000146345.73
510.d5
Hart, S. L., Hoyt, M. A., Diefenbach, M., Anderson, D. R., Kilbourn, K. M., Craft, L. L.,
Stanton, A. L. (2012). Meta-analysis of efficacy of interventions for elevated depressive
symptoms in adults diagnosed with cancer. Journal of the National Cancer Institute, 104, 990
1004. doi:10.1093/jnci/djs256
Henry, E. A., Schlegel, R. J., Talley, A. E., Molix, L. A., & Bettencourt, B. A. (2010). The
feasibility and effectiveness of expressive writing for rural and urban breast cancer survivors.
Oncology Nursing Forum, 37, 749757. doi:10.1188/10.ONF.749-757
Hoey, L. M., Ieropoli, S. C., White, V. M., & Jefford, M. (2008). Systematic review of peer-support
programs for people with cancer. Patient Education and Counseling, 70, 315337. doi:10.1016/j.
pec.2007.11.016
Hopewell, S., McDonald, S., Clarke, M. J., & Egger, M. (2007). Grey literature in meta-analyses of
randomized trials of health care interventions (Review). Cochrane Database of Systematic
Reviews, 2, MR000010. doi:10.1002/14651858.MR000010.pub3
Horner, M. J., Ries, L. A. G., Krapcho, M., Neyman, N., Aminou, R., Howlander, N., Edwards,
B. K. (Eds.). (2009). SEER cancer statistics review, 19752006. Bethesda, MD: National Cancer
Institute. Retrieved from http://seer.cancer.gov/csr/1975_2006
Hybye, M. T., Dalton, S. O., Deltour, I., Bidstrup, P. E., Frederiksen, K., & Johansen, C. (2010).
Effect of Internet peer-support groups on psychosocial adjustment to cancer: A randomised study.
British Journal of Cancer, 102, 13481354. doi:10.1038/sj.bjc.6605646
Jensen-Johansen, M. B., Christensen, S., Valdimarsdottir, H., Zakowski, S., Jensen, A. B., &
Bovbjerg, D. H. (2012). Effects of an expressive writing intervention on cancer-related distress in
Danish breast cancer survivors results from a nationwide randomized clinical trial. PsychoOncology, 22, 14921500. doi:10.1002/pon.3193
Kallay, E., & Baban, A. (2008). Emotional benefits of expressive writing in a sample of Romanian
female cancer patients. Cognition, Brain, Behavior, 12, 115129.
Laccetti, M. (2007). Expressive writing in women with advanced breast cancer. Oncology Nursing
Forum, 34, 10191024. doi:10.1188/07.ONF.1019-1024
360
Lepore, S. J., Greenberg, M. A., Bruno, M., & Smyth, J. M. (2002). Expressive writing and health:
Self-regulation of emotion-related experience, physiology, and behavior. In S. J. Lepore & J. M.
Smyth (Eds.), The writing cure: How expressive writing promotes health and emotional well
being (pp. 99117). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Low, C. A., Stanton, A. L., Bower, J. E., & Gyllenhammer, L. (2010). A randomized controlled trial
of emotionally expressive writing for women with metastatic breast cancer. Health Psychology,
29, 460466. doi:10.1037/a0020153
Low, C. A., Stanton, A. L., & Danoff-Burg, S. (2006). Expressive disclosure and benefit finding
among breast cancer patients: Mechanisms for positive health effects. Health Psychology, 25,
181189. doi:10.1037/0278-6133.25.2.181
Lu, Q., Zheng, D., Young, L., Kagawa-Singer, M., & Loh, A. (2012). A pilot study of expressive
writing interventions among Chinese-speaking breast cancer survivors. Health Psychology, 31,
548551. doi:10.1037/a0026834
Manzoni, G. M., Castelnuovo, G., & Molinari, E. (2011). The written-heart study (expressive
writing for heart healing): Rationale and design of a randomized controlled clinical trial of
expressive writing in coronary patients referred to residential cardiac rehabilitation. Health and
Quality of Life Outcomes, 9, 51. doi:10.1186/1477-7525-9-51
Morgan, N. P., Graves, K. D., Poggi, E. A., & Cheson, B. D. (2008). Implementing an expressive
writing study in a cancer clinic. The Oncologist, 13, 196204. doi:10.1634/theoncologist.
2007-0147
Mosher, C. E., DuHamel, K. N., Lam, J., Dickler, M., Li, Y., Massie, M. J., & Norton, L. (2012).
Randomised trial of expressive writing for distressed metastatic breast cancer patients,
Psychology & Health, 27, 88100. doi:10.1080/08870446.2010.551212
Owen, J. E., Giese-Davis, J., Cordova, M., Kronenwetter, C., Golant, M., & Spiegel, D. (2006).
Self-report and linguistic indicators of emotional expression in narratives as predictors of
adjustment to cancer. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 29, 335345. doi:10.1007/s10865-0069061-8
Park, E. Y., & Yi, M. (2012). Development and effectiveness of expressive writing program for
women with breast cancer in Korea [Abstract]. Journal of the Korean Academy of Nursing, 42,
269279. doi:10.4040/jkan.2012.42.2.269
Pennebaker, J. W. (1993). Putting stress into words: Health, linguistic and therapeutic implications.
Behavior Research and Therapy, 31, 539548. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(93)90105-4
Pennebaker, J. W. (2010). Expressive writing in a clinical setting. Independent Practitioner, 30,
2325.
Pennebaker, J. W., & Beall, S. K. (1986). Confronting a traumatic event: Toward an understanding
of inhibition and disease. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 274281. doi:10.1037/0021843X.95.3.274
Pennebaker, J. W., Booth, R. J., & Francis, M. E. (2007). Linguistic inquiry and word count: LIWC
[Computer software]. Austin, TX: LIWC.net.
Pennebaker, J. W., & Chung, C. K. (2007). Expressive writing, emotional upheavals, and health. In
H. S. Friedman & R. C. Silver (Eds.), Foundations of health psychology (pp. 263284). New
York, NY: Oxford University.
Pennebaker, J. W., & Chung, C. K. (2011). Expressive writing and its links to mental and physical
health. In H. S. Friedman (Ed.), Oxford handbook of health psychology (pp. 417437). New
York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Pennebaker, J. W., Mayne, T. J., & Francis, M. E. (1997). Linguistic predictors of adaptive
bereavement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 166183. doi:10.1037/00223514.72.4.863
Range, L. M., & Jenkins, S. R. (2010). Who benefits from Pennebakers expressive writing
paradigm? Research recommendations from three gender theories. Sex Roles, 63, 149164.
doi:10.1007/s11199-010-9749-7
Rivkin, I. D., Gustafson, J., Weingarten, I., & Chin, D. (2006). The effects of expressive writing on
adjustment to HIV. AIDS and Behavior, 10(1), 1326. doi:10.1007/s10461-005-9051-9
Rosenberg, H. J., Rosenberg, S. D., Ernstoff, M. S., Wolford, G. L., Amdur, R. J., Elshamy, M. R.,
Pennebaker, J. W. (2002). Expressive disclosure and health outcomes in a prostate cancer
population. International Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine, 32, 3753. doi:10.2190/AGPFVB1G-U82E-AE8C
361
Servaes, P., Vingerhoets, A. J. J. M., Vreugdenhil, G., Keuning, J. J., & Broekhuijsen, A. M.
(1999). Inhibition of emotional expression in breast cancer patients. Behavioral Medicine, 25,
2327. doi:10.1080/08964289909596735
Smith, S., Anderson-Hanley, C., Langrock, A., & Compas, B. (2005). The effects of journaling for
women with newly diagnosed breast cancer. Psycho-Oncology, 14, 10751082. doi:10.1002/
pon.912
Smyth, J. M. (1998). Written emotional expression: Effect sizes, outcome types, and moderating
variables. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 174184. doi:10.1037/0022006X.66.1.174
Smyth, J., True, N., & Souto, J. (2001). Effects of writing about traumatic experiences: The
necessity for narrative structuring. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 20, 161172.
doi:10.1521/jscp.20.2.161.22266
Stanton, A. L., Danoff-Burg, S., Sworowski, L. A., Collins, C. A., Branstetter, A. D., RodriguezHanley, A., Austenfeld, J. L. (2002). Randomized, controlled trial of written emotional
expression and benefit finding in breast cancer. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 20, 41604168.
doi:10.1200/JCO.2002.08.521
Sterne, J. A. C., Egger, M., & Moher, D. (Eds.). (2011). Chapter 10.3.2: Including unpublished
studies in systematic reviews. In J. P. T. Higgins and S. Green (Eds.), Cochrane handbook for
systematic reviews of interventions, Version 5.1.0. Retrieved from http://handbook.cochrane.org/
Tamagawa, R., Giese-Davis, J., Speca, M., Doll, R., Stephen, J., & Carlson, L. E. (2013). Trait
mindfulness, repression, suppression, and self-reported mood and stress symptoms among
women with breast cancer. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 69, 264277. doi:10.1002/jclp.21939
Tausczik, Y. R., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2010). The psychological meaning of words: LIWC and
computerized text analysis methods. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 29(1), 2454.
doi:10.1177/0261927X09351676
Walker, B. L., Nail, L. M., & Croyle, R. T. (1999). Does emotional expression make a difference in
reaction to breast cancer? Oncology Nursing Forum, 26, 10251032.
Zakowski, S. G., Harris, C., Krueger, N., Laubmeier, K. K., Garrett, S., Flanigan, R., & Johnson, P.
(2003). Social barriers to emotional expression and their relations to distress in male and female
cancer patients. British Journal of Health Psychology, 8, 271286. doi:10.1348/135910703322
370851
Zakowski, S. G., Herzer, M., Barrett, S. D., Milligan, J. G., & Beckman, N. (2011). Who benefits
from emotional expression? An examination of personality differences among gynaecological
cancer patients participating in a randomized controlled emotional disclosure intervention trial.
British Journal of Psychology, 12, 355372. doi:10.1348/000712610X524949
Zakowski, S. G., Ramati, A., Morton, C., Johnson, P., & Flanigan, R. (2004). Written emotional
disclosure buffers the effects of social constraints on distress among cancer patients. Health
Psychology, 23, 555563. doi:10.1037/0278-6133.23.6.555