2010 APIR Procrastination-Libre1
2010 APIR Procrastination-Libre1
2010 APIR Procrastination-Libre1
2
0
0
9
T
h
e
A
u
t
h
o
r
s
.
J
o
u
r
n
a
l
c
o
m
p
i
l
a
t
i
o
n
2
0
0
9
I
n
t
e
r
n
a
t
i
o
n
a
l
A
s
s
o
c
i
a
t
i
o
n
o
f
A
p
p
l
i
e
d
P
s
y
c
h
o
l
o
g
y
.
neutral procrastinators in each context. In Canada, negative procrastinators
had signicantly lower GPAs, F(1, 369) = 40.93, p < .001, h
2
= .10; higher
daily procrastination, F(1, 383) = 28.28, p < .001, h
2
= .07; higher procrasti-
nation scale score, F(1, 387) = 41.36, p < .001, h
2
= .10; and lower self-efcacy
for self-regulated learning, F(1, 388) = 29.87, p < .001, h
2
= .07 (p-level
Bonferroni-adjusted to .01), than neutral procrastinators.
In Singapore, negative procrastinators had signicantly higher daily pro-
crastination, F(1, 335) = 9.79, p < .001, h
2
= .03; higher procrastination scale
score, F(1, 336) = 20.11, p < .001, h
2
= .06; and lower self-efcacy for self-
regulated learning, F(1, 336) = 20.10, p < .001, h
2
= .06, than neutral procras-
tinators. The difference for GPA neared signicance, F(1, 198) = 5.87, p = .02,
h
2
= .03.
Brief Discussion
Even though most participants in Canada and Singapore spent less than two
hours per day procrastinating, a sizable minority in each country spent three
hours a day or more engaged in procrastination. However, the proportion of
students spending less than two hours and more than three hours per day did
not differ according to context. Hence, our prediction that students in Sin-
gapore might procrastinate less than Canadians was not borne out. Writing
tasks were most likely to be delayed or avoided in both contexts, but the
replacement activities varied according to context, with Canadians favoring
getting something to eat or drink as a delaying activity, whereas the Sin-
gaporeanundergraduates favoredtaking a napwhenavoiding academic tasks.
Our prediction that Singaporeans would be more likely to engage in group
activities as diversions (e.g. talk with friends or go out with friends) was not
borne out, with the top avoidance activities similar in each context. Aminority
of undergraduates viewed procrastination as negatively inuencing academic
functioning in each context, although the proportion of negative procrastina-
tors was higher in Singapore than in Canada. Hence, our nal prediction
that Canadian students would be less likely than Singaporean students to
interpret procrastination as having a negative impactwas supported.
GENERAL DISCUSSION
The results of the two studies indicate that the correlates of procrastination
were similar in two culturally contrasting contexts, but there were differences
in the perceived negative impact of procrastination and in replacement activi-
ties that students engaged in when procrastinating. Our Study 1 prediction of
similar patterns of procrastination across groups was conrmed, supporting
the limited ndings of cross-national similarities in procrastination tenden-
cies (e.g. Ferrari et al., 2007). The results from Study 1 support previous
374 KLASSEN ET AL.
2009 The Authors. Journal compilation 2009 International Association of Applied
Psychology.
ndings (e.g. Klassen et al., in press; Klassen et al., 2008) that self-efcacy for
self-regulated learning is a generalisable feature of procrastination in diverse
cultural contexts.
Students from both contexts were most likely to procrastinate on writing
tasks. This is consistent with reports from previous studies in Western con-
texts (e.g. Fritzsche et al., 2003), but no previous studies have compared
avoidance tasks across cultures. If procrastination is most likely when tasks
are boring and difcult (Steel, 2007, p. 75), it appears that writing is
perceived as having less interest and to be more challenging than the other
potential procrastination tasks (e.g. reading and researching). Fritzsche et al.
(2003) suggest that providing regular feedback to writers may help reduce
levels of procrastination in undergraduates.
Given the higher levels of collectivism in the Singaporean context, we
predicted that students in Singapore may be more likely to engage in social
activities as replacement activities than students in Canada, but this predic-
tion was not borne out. Both groups of students rated getting something to
eat or drink as a favorite replacement activity (which may or may not be a
social activity), but Canadians rated doing household tasks and talking with
friends among their top ve replacement activities, whereas Singaporean
students rated having a nap, going online for chat, and surng the internet
among their top ve replacement activities. Ironically, Ferrari and Scher
(2000) found household chores as the most frequently procrastinated non-
academic tasks in their sample of American college students, but it may be
that Canadian students are likely to choose an aversive non-academic task
when avoiding academic tasks, along the lines of graduate students reports
of Ive never cleaned my apartment so much as when I was writing my
thesis. This pattern was not seen in Singaporean students, but the difference
may reect the different living conditions for undergraduates in the two
universities (i.e. living in on-campus rooms versus off-campus apartments)
rather than cultural differences. Future studies might investigate in a more
systematic manner the tasks students turn to when procrastinating.
Our prediction of lower daily procrastination in the Singaporean sample
was not conrmed, with similar proportions of students reporting procrasti-
nating more than three hours per day in each sample. Once again, the
universality of procrastination behaviors is supported, with students from
collectivist contexts reporting high levels of procrastination at the same rate
as students from individualist contexts. However, the negative perception of
procrastination varied across contexts, as was consistent with our prediction.
Singaporean students were more likely to rate procrastination as negatively
inuencing their academic functioning than Canadian students. Dietz et al.
(2007) suggest that procrastination is a link between value orientation and
the decision to engage in learning or leisure activities. Although the Dietz
et al. study did not examine value orientations from a cross-cultural perspec-
ACADEMIC PROCRASTINATION 375
2009 The Authors. Journal compilation 2009 International Association of Applied
Psychology.
tive, we believe that the principle that value orientation inuences perception
of procrastinating behaviors is relevant for cross-cultural comparisons.
Value orientations may be more prescriptive in collectivist cultures, with less
freedom of action and narrower socialisation options. The personal goals
formed from value orientations are organisers of behavior (Boekaerts, 2003),
and may inuence perceptions of procrastination. Singaporean students may
interpret procrastination more negatively because the behavior ts less well
with their value orientation and personal goals than it does for Canadian
students. In other words, all students procrastinate, but the interpretation of
procrastination may be inuenced by cultural background.
Our results showed that less than a third of Canadians and nearly half
of Singaporeans classify themselves as negative procrastinators. The conse-
quences of perceived negative procrastination are damaging, and negative
procrastinators in Canada and Singapore reported more time spent procras-
tinating, lower levels of self-efcacy for self-regulated learning, and lower
GPA (for Canadians). The differential relationship between GPA and nega-
tive procrastination in Canada and Singapore may be due to differential
value orientations, with the Canadians who report procrastination as nega-
tive actually earning lower grades, whereas Singaporeans who rate them-
selves as negative procrastinators may do so largely due to feelings of conict
between personal goals and their procrastinating behavior. We did not
measure the link between negative procrastination and self-esteem; future
research might examine the direction and intensity of the association between
self-esteem and negative procrastination.
Findings from this research suggest that the impact of procrastination is
not a simple function of time spent procrastinating, but is also related to
interpretations of behavior that may be inuenced by cultural beliefs. Sin-
gaporeans did not report higher levels of daily procrastination, but they did
report greater negative impact of procrastination. One explanation for this
discrepancy may be differences in the difculty of the respective university
programs, but it is plausible that differing interpretation of procrastination
may also be a function of personal goals and value orientations. Although
this study provides a rst exploration of this phenomenon, future studies
should further examine how students cultural beliefs inuence their inter-
pretations of procrastination.
The study is limited by its reliance on self-report surveys from limited
samples from two universities in Canada and Singapore. Although the
samples shared many similarities, there are differences in education systems
and cultural beliefs (which were not measured, but assumed from previous
studies, e.g. Hofstede, 2001) that make equivalence across the two contexts
difcult to assess. Consequently, the results may not generalise well to
other populations in culturally Western or East Asian contexts. Although
this study provides useful initial data on academic procrastination across
376 KLASSEN ET AL.
2009 The Authors. Journal compilation 2009 International Association of Applied
Psychology.
cultures, future cross-cultural procrastination research should focus on
incorporating other methodological approaches, such as the use of behav-
ioral measures or perceptions of national character (PNC) data that may
provide evidence of between-culture differences in behavior (e.g. Heine,
Buchtel, & Norenzayan, 2008). In this study, negative procrastination was
dened by adverse academic impact, but procrastination also has been
associated with adverse affective impact (e.g. Steel, 2007), and future cross-
cultural studies might examine negative emotional outcomes associated
with procrastination. Finally, a future study would protably investigate
how culturally inuenced beliefs, such as self-enhancing and self-improving
motivations, might be linked to students explanations for academic
procrastination.
REFERENCES
Ackerman, D.S., & Gross, B.L. (2005). My instructor made me do it: Task charac-
teristics of procrastination. Journal of Marketing Education, 27, 513.
Bandura, A. (1993). Perceived self-efcacy in cognitive development and functioning.
Educational Psychologist, 28, 117148.
Boekaerts, M. (2003). How do students from different cultures motivate themselves
for academic learning? In F. Salili & R. Hoosain (Eds.), Teaching, learning, and
motivation in multicultural context (pp. 1331). Greenwich, CT: Information
Age.
Chia, Y.M., Koh, H.C., & Pragasam, J. (2008). An international study of career
drivers of accounting students in Singapore, Australia, and Hong Kong. Journal of
Education and Work, 21, 4160.
Chong, W.H. (2007). The role of personal agency beliefs in academic self-regulation.
School Psychology International, 28, 6376.
Dietz, F., Hofer, M., & Fries, S. (2007). Individual values, learning routines and
academic procrastination. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 893906.
Dunn, L., & Wallace, M. (2004). Australian academic teaching in Singapore: Striving
for cultural empathy. Innovations in Education and Teaching International, 41,
291304.
Ferrari, J.R. (2000). Procrastination and attention: Factor analysis of attention
decit, boredomness, intelligence, self-esteem, and task delay frequencies. Journal
of Social Behavior and Personality, 15, 185196.
Ferrari, J.R., Diaz-Morales, J.F., OCallaghan, J., Diaz, K., & Argumedo, D. (2007).
Frequent behavioral delay tendency by adults: International prevalence rates of
chronic procrastination. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38, 458464.
Ferrari, J.R., OCallaghan, J., & Newbegin, I. (2005). Prevalence of procrastination in
the United States, United Kingdom, and Australia: Arousal and avoidance delays
among adults. North American Journal of Psychology, 7, 16.
Ferrari, J.R., & Scher, S.J. (2000). Toward an understanding of academic and non-
academic tasks procrastinated by students: The use of daily logs. Psychology in the
Schools, 37, 359366.
ACADEMIC PROCRASTINATION 377
2009 The Authors. Journal compilation 2009 International Association of Applied
Psychology.
Fritzsche, B.A., Young, B.R., & Hickson, K.C. (2003). Individual differences in
academic procrastination tendency and writing success. Personality and Individual
Differences, 35, 15491557.
Ghilagaber, G. (2004). Another look at Chows test for the equality of two heterosce-
dastic regression models. Quality and Quantity, 38, 8193.
Hammer, C.A., & Ferrari, J.R. (2002). Differential incidence of procrastination
between blue- and white-collar workers. Current Psychology: Developmental,
Learning, Personality, Social, 21, 333338.
Heine, S.J. (2003). An exploration of cultural variation in self-enhancing and self-
improving motivations. In V. Murphy-Berman & J.J. Berman (Eds.), Cross-
cultural differences in perspectives on the self (pp. 101128). Lincoln, NE:
University of Nebraska Press.
Heine, S.J., Buchtel, E.E., & Norenzayan, A. (2008). What do cross-national com-
parisons of personality traits tell us? The case of conscientiousness. Psychological
Science, 19, 309313.
Hofstede, G. (2001). Cultures consequences (2nd edn.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
Publications.
Howell, A.J., & Watson, D.C. (2007). Procrastination: Associations with achievement
goal orientation and learning strategies. Personality and Individual Differences, 43,
167178.
Klassen, R.M., Ang, R., Chong, W.H., Krawchuk, L.L., Huan, V., Wong, I., & Yeo,
L.S. (in press). A cross-cultural study of adolescent procrastination. Journal of
Research on Adolescence.
Klassen, R.M., Krawchuk, L.L., & Rajani, S. (2008). Academic procrastination of
undergraduates: Low self-efcacy to self-regulate predicts higher levels of procras-
tination. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 33, 915931.
Marsh, H.W., & Hau, K.-T. (2004). Explaining paradoxical relations between
academic self-concepts and achievements: Cross-cultural generalizability of the
internal/external frame of reference predictions across 26 countries. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 96, 5667.
Pintrich, P.R., Smith D.A.F., Garcia, T., & McKeachie, W.J. (1993). Reliability and
predictive validity of the motivated strategies for learning questionnaire (MSLQ).
Educational and Psychological Measurement, 53, 801813.
Rosenberg, M.J. (1979). Conceiving the self. New York: Basic Books.
Schraw, G., Wadkins, T., & Olafson, L. (2007). Doing the things we do: A grounded
theory of procrastination. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 1225.
Sirois, F.M. (2004). Procrastination and intentions to perform health behaviors: The
role of self-efcacy and the consideration of future consequences. Personality and
Individual Differences, 37, 115128.
Steel, P. (2007). The nature of procrastination: A meta-analytic and theoretical review
of quintessential self-regulatory failure. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 6594.
Times Higher Education World University Rankings (2007). The top 200 world
universities. Retrieved 15 February 2008 from http://www.timeshighereducation.
co.uk/hybrid.asp?typeCode=144
Tuckman, B.W. (1991). The development and concurrent validity of the procrastina-
tion scale. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 51, 473480.
378 KLASSEN ET AL.
2009 The Authors. Journal compilation 2009 International Association of Applied
Psychology.
Usher, E.L., & Pajares, F. (2008). Self-efcacy for self-regulated learning: A valida-
tion study. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 68, 443463.
Van Eerde, W. (2000). Procrastination: Self-regulation in initiating aversive goals.
Applied Psychology: An International Review, 49, 372389.
World Bank (2009). Key development data and statistics. Retrieved 25 March 2009
from: http://web.worldbank.org/WBSITE/EXTERNAL/DATASTATISTICS/0,,
contentMDK:20535285~menuPK:1192694~pagePK:64133150~piPK:64133175~
theSitePK:239419,00.html
Zhang, H.-M., & Zhang, Z.-J. (2007). Usability of Tuckman procrastination scale in
Chinese college students. Chinese Journal of Clinical Psychology, 15, 1012.
Zimmerman, B.J., Bandura, A., & Martinez-Pons, M. (1992). Self-motivation for
academic attainment: The role of self-efcacy beliefs and personal goal-setting.
American Educational Research Journal, 29, 663676.
ACADEMIC PROCRASTINATION 379
2009 The Authors. Journal compilation 2009 International Association of Applied
Psychology.