DOCUMENT RESUME
ED 468 852
AUTHOR
TITLE
PUB DATE
NOTE
PUB TYPE
EDRS PRICE
DESCRIPTORS
IDENTIFIERS
SE 066 790
Seng, Alice Seok Hoon; Tan, Lee Choo
Cultural and Gender Differences in Spatial Ability of Young
Children.
2002-04-00
9p.; Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Association
for Childhood Education International (San Diego, CA, April
3-6, 2002).
Reports
Research (143)
Speeches/Meeting Papers (150)
EDRS Price MF01/PC01 Plus Postage.
*Cultural Differences; Cognitive Processes; Elementary
Education; Foreign Countries; Mathematics Education;
Piagetian Theory; Problem Solving; *Sex Differences; *Spatial
Ability
China; Malaysia; Singapore
ABSTRACT
This study reports on cultural and gender differences in the
spatial abilities of children based on the Water Level Task. The Piagetian
theory of age-related developmental differences in performance on the Water
Level Task was explored with Chinese and Malay children living in Singapore.
Results indicate that children in this study did not perform as well as those
described ih the Piagetian developmental stage. There was a significant
difference in the performance of Chinese and Malay children with Chinese
children performing better than Malay children. At all grade levels, boys had
higher proportions of correct scores than girls; however, differences were
not statistically significant. (KHR)
Reproductions supplied by EDRS are the best that can be made
from the original document.
ACEI 2002 Annual Conference
April 3-6 San Diego, California, USA
Cultural and Gender Differences in Spatial Ability of Young Children
Alice Seng Seok Hoon &
PERMISSION TO REPRODUCE AND
DISSEMINATE THIS MATERIAL HAS
BEEN GRANTED BY
TO THE EDUCATIONAL RESOURCES
INFORMATION CENTER (ERIC)
Tan Lee Choo
National Institute of Education
Nanyang Technological University
Singapore
[email protected]
U.S. DEPARTMENT OF EDUCATION
Office of Educational Research
CENTER (EIC)
R
it,..re._
This document has been reproduced as
ceived from the person or organization
originating it.
Minor changes have heen made to
improve reproduction quality
Points of view or opinions stated in this
document do not necessarily represent
official OERI position or policy.
1
Studies on Spatial Ability
Studies on cultural and sex differences in spatial abilities have been conducted
from as early as 1946 when the WAIS (Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale) was used.
Most of these were carried out in USA and Europe but very few in Asia. In Singapore,
research in gender differences related to spatial abilities is scarce and it was not till
the 1980s that spatial ability studies involving mathematics achievement and gender
differences in the elementary schools were documented.
In 1974, Maccoby and Jack lin published an extensive literature review that
clearly established the existence of sex differences in spatial abilities favouring males.
Hyde (1981) used meta-analysis to estimate the magnitude of the sex difference
reported by Maccoby and Jack lin (1974) and found that sex accounted for only 5% of
the variance in the spatial tasks they sampled. However, another meta-analysis done
by Linn & Petersen (1985) based on effect size procedure, found that there are three
distinct categories of spatial ability tests: spatial perception, mental rotation and
spatial visualization. Sex differences were significant only in the first two categories.
These meta-analyses suggest that spatial ability is not a unitary concept but is made
up of a grouping of several different types of ability.
The inconsistent results of sex differences in spatial ability research can be
due to the lack of a clear definition of what spatial abilities include. A large variety of
tests has been used and several researchers have attempted to identify spatial ability
factors but there has been no agreement on the categorization of spatial ability. In
1995, Voyer, Voyer and Bryden performed another meta-analysis covering 50 years
of spatial ability research. They found significant gender differences in spatial
abilities supporting males on some of the 12 tests that were used. The effect sizes in
favor of males vary considerably from test to test, indicating that different measures
of spatial ability assess somewhat different processes. This raises questions as to why
gender differences do or do not occur.
Furthermore, most of the studies that have found gender differences in spatial
ability were investigated in Western cultures. Boys have superior scores in spatial
ability mainly from these western studies. Not enough is known whether gender
differences in spatial skills also exist in non-Western cultures although a few studies
have been documented. Mann, Sasanuma, Sakuma and Masaki (1990) found gender
2
and Improvement
EDUCATIONAL RESOURCES INFORMATION
BEST COPY AVM _AKe
differences in a Mental Rotation Test in high school students in Tokyo, Japan. In
Figures Test,
1993 Lee found males performing better than females on the Hidden
Card Rotation Test and Paper Folding Test in Taiwan. However, in a study by Pontius
(1997) twenty four male and twenty four female schoolchildren (ages 8 to 10) in
Northwest Pakistan performed two spatial tasks without showing essential gender
differences. Apparently, males in some Asian cultures will be superior in spatial
ability depending on the spatial test used.
In a 1996 study from Norway by Vederhus & Krekling, it was reported that
there were sex differences in 9 year old children on a battery of multiple choice tests
representing all three spatial ability categories described by Linn and Petersen (1985)
Data was collected from a total of 200 children from a random sample of elementary
schools . The tasks included one test for each of the categories : Spatial Perception (
Water Level Test) and Spatial Visualization (Surface Development task) and two tests
of Mental Rotation (2D and 3D tasks). Their study showed that boys performed better
than girls on all tests. This was significant for the Water Level task. On the other
hand, females show superior performance on tasks that require verbal language
abilities. In a related study in 1997 by Benjamin, sex differences in visual-spatial
performance among Ghanaian and Norwegian adults were examined. This study
found that both the patterns and the magnitudes of sex differences on tasks
representing three different visualspatial ability categories were remarkably similar
among Ghanaian and Norwegian adults and that the intercorrelation patterns between
spatial tests were different in the two samples. This finding indicates that culturerelated differences in spatial ability structure may still exist.
Given these findings, this study attempts to extend current research
understanding about cultural and gender differences in spatial ability of children
based on the Water Level Task.
Water Level Task
The Water-Level Task (WLT) was originally developed by Piaget and
Inhelder (1956) to measure a child's ability to perceive space within an Euclidean
reference system. In this task the child is required to correctly anticipate the water
surface orientation in tilted bottles that are half filled. In the paper and pencil version
of the WLT (Li et.al 1999) the child draws a line to represent the water level in
several drawings of tilted containers. (Figure 1)
According to Piaget and Inhelder (1948/1956), success on the WLT reflects a
person's spatial competence that is, the ability to use a Euclidean system of reference
to organize spatial experience. On the basis of the findings from their experiments,
Piaget and Inhelder expected children to master the WLT by approximately 9 years of
age, when concrete operational thought has developed; for some, however, Piaget and
Inhelder said that mastery might not come until age 12. According to Li et.al (1999) a
number of studies indicate that many American students fail the WLT. Neither boys
nor girls performed accurately on the WLT before adolescence and that only boys
showed significant age-related improvement from 5th to 12th grade.
3
2
indicated. Thomas and
Within age groups, gender differences have also been
Turner (1991) found that across age groups, male individuals outperformed female
high performers and low
individuals, and within each gender group, there were
than boys on the WLT
performers. At the high school level, girls had lower scores
The age at which gender
and at the college level, women had lower scores than men.
differences begin to appear is also questionable since some studies reported that
gender differences appear at all age levels, whereas as early as 1976 Geiringer and
12th grade but not in the 5th grade.
Hyde found that gender differences appeared in the
It has been noted that Asian students have performed better than American
students on spatial tasks like the WLT (Li & Shallcross, 1992; Li etal., 1996). About
successful on the
91% of the Chinese men and 76% of the Chinese women were
WLT, compared with 56% of the American men and 26% of the American women. In
Chinese
a later study (Li etal 1999) Chinese college students in China outperformed
American
men,
those
who
American college students. In addition, among the Chinese
write Chinese.
could write Chinese performed better than those who could not
The Singapore Study
Piagetian theory of
In the present study, we explored the applicability of the
with Chinese and
age-related developmental differences in performance on the WLT
cultural society,
a
multi
Malay children living in Singapore. Singapore is
predominantly Chinese in population followed by the Malays, Indians and Eurasians.
elementary school in a
The participants were 100 children (aged 8 to 12) from an
typical housing estate.
The standard paper and pencil WLT was used. It has a total of eight drawings
imagine that each
of bottles tilted at different degrees. The children were told to
under
each bottle. They
bottle was being held over a tabletop, represented by the line
They
were to imagine that the bottle was sealed and that it was half filled with water.
thought
the
surface
of
the
water
were asked to draw a line representing what they
would look like in each bottle. The child's score was the proportion of water lines,
which were within 5 degrees of the horizontal. For this task, the split-half reliability
estimates have been established to be .91 for males and .92 for females.
Results
Performance on WLT by Age/Grade (Table 1)
mastered the waterMost of the children at primary 2 (typical age 8 years) had not
level task. For the eight water-level bottles depicted, the average proportion correct
was .42 (ie the mean score per child was .42 out of a possible 1.00) for primary 2 and
.63 for primary 3 (see Table 1). Only 30% of the primary 2 children and 23.8% of the
answered all
primary 3 (9 years) got all eight correct. For primary 4 (10 years) 34.5%
the same
eight WLT items correctly. The mean proportion correct was .69. This was
show
that
there are
for primary 5 (11 years) and primary 6 (12 years). Data seem to
two stages of performance on the WLT: all primary 2 and below 8 years and primary
3 , 4,5 and 6 and all above 8 years.
3
Proportion correct on the WLT by Race, Gender and Grade (Table 2 and Table 3)
More boys above age 8 have higher proportions of scores correct compared with all
the girls .This however is not statistically significant. The Chinese children however
do better than all the Malay children and this is statistically significant, F(2,99) =
4.589, p=.012. The interaction between race and gender was not significant.
WLT scores for 8 Bottles (Table 4)
Three types of scores are given : Zero for wrong response; Half for responses within
the 5 degree horizontal and Full for correct 'horizontal' response. Bottle 2, 3, 5 and
6 have more incorrect responses than the other bottles.
Discussion
The present results indicate that the Piagetian developmental stage of 9 year
olds , as reflected in the WLT, does not correspond to the scores of the 9 year old
Singapore children tested. Less than half of the 9 year old children had developed the
spatial and cognitive abilities required for the WLT. There were more than half who
could not solve the WLT at ages 10,11 and 12. This finding is similar to a 1976 study
by Geiringer and Hyde who reported that neither boys nor girls perform accurately on
the WLT before adolescence. Overall, children in this study did not perform as well as
those described in Piaget's reports.
At all grade levels, boys perform better on the WLT than girls. There was no
statistically significant interaction between grade level and gender. This finding is
quite consistent with the meta-analytic studies by Linn and Petersen (1985) and Voyer
et al (1995) that male individuals consistently perform better than female individuals
on the WLT at all ages. This study provides cross cultural evidence of gender
difference in WLT performance from age 8 to 12.
It has been pointed out by Linn and Petersen (1985) that males did better than
females on spatial tasks due to less effective strategies used by the latter. It was also
noted that females tend to take more time, reflect more caution and tend to double
check their responses. Females tend to view spatial tasks as more difficult than do
males.
The better overall test performance of the Chinese children compared to the
Malay children may indicate differential experience and socialization from these two
racial groups .The poorer performance of the Malay children is probably based on
their response to the demand characteristics of the WLT test situation rather than
inherent racial or ethnic differences in spatial perception ability. The data in the
present study is too insufficient to warrant any definitive statement about the
different response patterns from these two racial groups.
On the whole the WLT is difficult for children because it requires a number of
cognitive and perceptual competencies. It is a multi dimensional problem requiring
4
both knowledge of physical principles and spatial perceptual abilities. Kalichman
(1989) reduced the WLT to four subabilities: visual-perceptual skills, mental imagery
and rotation skills, use of spatial coordinate systems and recall of relevant
information. Graphic and disembedding skills are also required. This probably
explain why the WLT seem to be more difficult for children from one culture than
another and also for children from within a same culture.
In interpreting the present study, caution is advised. There is much diversity
between the Chinese and the Malay children. In addition to environmental and social
economic factors, there maybe other differences such as home values, practice of
ethnic culture, experience with spatial relations and language differences. Also, it is
important to keep in mind that current behaviorial and neuroscience research have
been examining the processes that give rise to sex differences in the brain from
different cultures. Evidence suggest that the effects of sex hormones on brain
organisation occur so early in life that from the start, the environment is acting on
differently wired brains in girls and boys. Such differences make it almost impossible
to evaluate the effects of experience independent of physiological characteristics.
REFERENCES:
Benjamin, A. (1997). Sex differences in visual-spatial performance among Ghanaian
and Norwegian adults. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 28, 81-92.
Geiringer, F.R. & Hyde, J. S. (1976). Sex differences on Piaget's water-level task:
Spatial ability incognito. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 42 (3,Pt 2), 1323-1328.
Hyde, J.S. (1981)
.
How large are cognitive gender difference?
American
Psychologist, 36, 892-901.
Kalichman, S.C.(1989). The effects of stimulus context on paper-and-pencil spatial
task performance. /The Journal of General Psychology, 116, 133-139.
Li, C., Nuttall, R., & Zhao, S. (1999). The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 160, 3,
369-380.
Li., C., & Shallcross, D. J. (1992) The effect of the assumed boundary in the solving
of the nine-dot problem on a sample of Chinese and American students 6-18 years
old. The Journal of Creative Behaviour, 26 (1) 53-64.
Li, C., Nuttall, R., & Zhao, S. (1996). Gender differences among Chinese
undergraduate students in the performance of the water-level task. Paper presented at
Asian American Psychological Association Annual Convention, Toronto, Canada.
Linn, M.C., & Petersen, A.C. (1985). Emergence and characterization of sex
differences in spatial ability: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 56, 1479-1498.
Maccoby, E.E., & Jack lin, C.N. (1974). The psychology of sex differences. Stanford,
CA: Stanford University Press.
5
Piaget, J & Inhelder, B. (1956). The child's conception of space. London: Rout ledge
& Kegan Paul.
Pontius, A. A. (1997). No gender difference in spatial representation by
schoolchildren in northwest Pakistan. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 28, 6,
779-786.
Thomas, H., & Turner, G.F.W. (1991). Individual differences and development in
waterlevel task performance. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 51, 171194.
Vederhus, L., & Krekling, S. (1996). Sex differences in Visual Spatial Ability in 9
year-old children. Intelligence, 23, 33-43.
Voyer, D., Voyer, S., & Bryden, M.P. (1995). Magnitude of sex differences in spatial
abilities: A meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychological
Bulletin, 117, 250-270.
7
6
Table 1. Scores for the Water-Level Task (WLT) by grade level
Grade
Pr 2
Pr 3
Pr 4
Pr 5
Pr 6
Age (Years)
N
WLT score
8
11
9
21
29
.42
.63
.69
.69
.69
10
31
8
11
12
% all correct
30
23.8
34.5
29
25
Table 2. Proportions correct on the WLT, by race, gender and grade
Age
Race
Chinese
Malay
Others
12
11
10
9
8
Girls Boys Girls Boys Girls Boys Girls Boys Girls Boys
.73
.25
.53
.25
0
0
.68
.67
0
.61
.50
0
.73
.60
.75
.85
.57
.75
0
.77
.75
.50
.75
.63
.19
13
0
.75
.75
.88
Table 3. Means of correct scores by sex and race
Female
N
26
Chinese
Malay
Others
Total
15
3
44
Mean SD
N
2.56
1.79
2.65
2.43
31
5.62
4.27
3.00
4.98
23
2
56
Male
Mean SD
6.06 2.03
4.91
5.50
5.57
1.88
2.12
2.02
N
57
38
5
Total
Mean SD
5.86 2.28
4.66 1.85
4.00 2.55
5.31
100
2.21
Table 4. Water-Level Task (WLT) scores for 8 bottles (B1-B8)
Score
Zero
B1
0
Half
One
0
100
B2
49
9
42
B3
B4
51
19
7
0
42
81
B5
54
6
40
B6
48
B7
B8
5
16
4
1
0
94
84
48
i
Imagine that each of the bottles you see on the paper is being held over a table top,
represented by the line under each bottle.
Imagine that the tops of the bottles are sealed and that they are about half-filled with
water.
Draw a line representing what you think the surface of the water would look like in each
bottle.
4
6
Sq_nlo--letO
U.S. Department of Education
Office of Educational Research and Improvement (OERI)
Educational Resources Information Center (ERIC)
ERIC
REPRODUCTION RELEASE
(Specific Document)
I. DOCUMENT IDENTIFICATION:
Title:
ayvd 6-cp
C L.TR R
lov
IrI ,5PA-70- L P1 g1C(T y
ckf-tQPP-e--)4
ii`ce
Author(s):
FFE, REVURS
se
se-0
Lke_ c_t-ko
Corporate Source:
Publication Date:
9-0o
II. REPRODUCTION RELEASE:
In order to disseminate as widely as possible timely and significant materials of interest to the educational community, documents announced
in the monthly abstract journal of the ERIC system, Resources in Education (RIE), are usually made available to users in microfiche, reproduced
paper copy, and electronic/optical media, and sold through the ERIC Document Reproduction Service (EDRS) or other ERIC vendors. Credit is
given to the source of each document, and, if reproduction release is granted, one of the following notices is affixed to the document.
If permission is granted to reproduce and disseminate the identified document, please CHECK ONE of the following two options and sign at
the bottom of the page.
The sample sticker shown below will be
affixed to all Level 1 documents
The sample sticker shown below will be
affixed to all Level 2 documents
PERMISSION TO REPRODUCE AND
DISSEMINATE THIS MATERIAL
HAS BEEN GRANTED BY
PERMISSION TO REPRODUCE AND
DISSEMINATE THIS
MATERIAL IN OTHER THAN PAPER
COPY HAS BEEN GRANTED BY
[Er
Check here
here
Check here
For Level 1 Release:
Permitting reproduction in
microfiche (4" x 6' film) or
other ERIC archival media
(e.g., electronic or optical)
and paper copy.
For Level 2 Release:
Permitting reproduction in
TO THE EDUCATIONAL RESOURCES
INFORMATION CENTER (ERIC)
microfiche (4' x 6' film) or
other ERIC archival media
(e.g., electronic or optical),
but not in paper copy.
TO THE EDUCATIONAL RESOURCES
INFORMATION CENTER (ERIC)
Level 1
Level 2
Documents will be processed as indicated provided reproduction quality permits. If permission
to reproduce is granted, but neither box is checked, documents will be processed at Level 1.
'I hereby grant to the Educational Resources Information Center (ERIC) nonexclusive permission to reproduce and disseminate
this document as indicated above. Reproduction from the ERIC microfiche or electronicloptical media by persons other than
ERIC employees and its system contractors requires permission from the copyright holder. Exception is made for non-profit
reproduction by libraries and other service agencies to satisfy information needs of educators in response to discrete inquiries.'
Sign
here-4
please
Printed Name/PositIon/Tide:
S oc
Organization/Address:
,A/
y
(5-/ 1\1614-P0A-6 63 7 64,
MICRO
C
it
rAX:
Telephone:
VA-TOP/At- 6NSTriUT6 D F L OW-A-Tee 1-.1
N4A-IslyAt\16- 76-cri nio
(4.1Li VeRst
yik
E Pie-0
6790321
6S-6, e4lot-t-(0
Date:
E-Mail Address:
SilsevL, e
/
,ti 2..
23?
o 2,
(over)
III. DOCUMENT AVAILABILITY INFORMATION
(FROM NON-ERIC SOURCE):
If permission to reproduce is not granted to ERIC, or, if you wish
to cite the availability of the document from another source,
please provide the following information regarding the availability ofERIC
the document. (ERIC will not announce
publicly available, and a dependable source can be specified.
a document unless it is
Contributors should also be aware that ERIC selection criteria are
significantly more stringent for documents that cannot be made available
through EDRS.)
Publisher/Distributor:
Address:
Price:
IV. REFERRAL OF ERIC TO COPYRIGHT/REPRODUCTION
RIGHTS HOLDER:
If the right to grant reproduction release is held by someone other than
the addressee, please provide
the appropriate name and address:
Name:
Address:
V. WHERE TO SEND THIS FORM:
Send this form to the following ERIC Clearinghouse:
KAREN E. SMITH
ACQUISITIONS COORDINATOR
ERIC/EECE
CHILDREN'S RESEARCH CENTER
51 GERTY DRIVE
CHAMPAIGN, ILLINOIS 61820-7469
However, if solicited by the ERIC Facility, or if making an unsolicited contribution
to ERIC, return this form (and the document being
contributed) to:
ERIC Processing and Reference Facility
1100 West Street, 2d Floor
Laurel, Maryland 20707-3598
(Rev. 6/96)
Telephone: 301-497-4080
Toll Free: 800-799-3742
FAX: 301-953-0263
e-mail:
[email protected]
WWW: http://ericfac.piccard.csc.com