CHAPTER FIFTEEN
TANI LANGUAGES
Mark W. Post and Jackson T.-S. Sun
1 INTRODUCTION
Tani refers to a compact cluster of Tibeto-Burman languages situated at the eastern end
of the Himalayas, in a primarily mountainous area skirted on four sides by Bhutan, Tibet,
Burma and the Brahmaputra River in Assam. The main concentration of Tani languages
covers the central part of the Indian state of Arunachal Pradesh, comprising the bulk of
the following districts: East Kameng, Upper Subansiri, Lower Subansiri, Upper Siang,
West Siang, East Siang, Lower Siang and Dibang Valley. Tani languages are spoken by
populations who either are currently or at one time were ‘tribally’ identified as Apatani,
Nyishi, Na, Bangni, Tagin, Galo, Hill Miri or Adi (various subtribes), in addition to
numerous smaller, usually clan-based or village-based self-identifications (such as Komkar).
The single largest population of Tani language speakers, however, is the Mising tribe of
upper Assam. The Tani languages are spoken in a basically continuous area, bordered in
the west by Miji, Puroik, Koro and Hruso speakers, in the east by Idu speakers, in the
south by speakers of Boro and Assamese, and in the north by speakers of Tibetic languages. Lingua francas spoken in the Tani area include Assamese in the south and
Arunachali Hindi in the central area. Tibetic languages were used as lingua francas in
earlier decades, although their use has recently waned.
The 2011 Census of India reports a total of 1,380,878 individuals in Arunachal Pradesh
and Assam who self-identified as belonging to a tribe whose mother tongue is a Tani
language (Census of India 2011): these included 631,042 Tani tribespeople in Arunachal
Pradesh, and 680,424 Mising in Assam (plus an additional 7,412 Mising in Arunachal
Pradesh). These figures must be treated with caution, as it is not the case that all people
who align tribally with a primarily Tani language-speaking population speak this language themselves; given the rapid spread of Hindi throughout Arunachal Pradesh (Modi
2005), and of Assamese among the Mising in Assam (Pegu 2011: 157), the number of
fluent Tani language speakers as a percentage of this total must be considerably lower.
Scattered Tani communities spill over the Sino-Indian border into adjacent areas in
Motuo (Miguba and Misinba tribes), Milin (Bokar and Tagin tribes), and Longzi (Bengni,
Na, Bayi, Dazu and Mara tribes) counties of Tibet (Ou-Yang 1985: 76), where they are
lumped with certain linguistically non-Tani peoples (e.g. the Idu, Sulung and Bangru) to
form the Luoba nationality. Very little current information regarding these groups is
available (although see Huber 2012).
Tani languages vary greatly in terms of number of speakers, adequacy of description, and
degree of endangerment. Mising has perhaps half a million speakers, but lacks a modern,
comprehensive dictionary and grammar, and is currently undergoing considerable retraction under the influence of Assamese. On the other hand, Tangam has only 150 speakers;
however, it has a forthcoming modern (if relatively slight) comprehensive description,
and all Tangam children seem – for the time being, at least – to be learning Tangam as a
first language and speaking it fluently. Relatively urgent descriptive priorities include
TANI LANGUAGES
323
Nyishi, a large and important language about which very little current information of
certain reliability is available, Apatani, a relatively divergent language with a robust and
intricate tone system which lacks a comprehensive and reliable description,1 as well as all
Tani varieties still spoken in Tibet.
2 GENEALOGY AND SUBGROUPING
Tani languages appear to constitute a distinct subgroup within Tibeto-Burman, as argued
in Sun (1993). All Tani languages shown on the family tree in Figure 15.1 share a large
amount of common core vocabulary, exhibit a very large number of regular sound correspondences, and in general support firm reconstruction of the phonology and vocabulary
of their common proto-language ‘Proto-Tani’. Koro and Milang, spoken on the western
and eastern fringes of the Tani area, show many similarities to Tani languages, and
Milang in particular has previously been included within the Tani subgroup. Post and
Modi (2011) argued against the inclusion of Milang in Tani proper, showing that the
strongest resemblances to Tani languages were undoubtedly due to contact with Padam
Adi. Around the same time, Post and Blench (2011) placed Koro and Milang in a distinct
‘Siangic’ subgroup. In addition to Siangic languages, Tani languages show many lexical
and grammatical resemblances to the easterly Idu-Tawrã languages, and to Tawrã in
particular. Unfortunately, however, the current state of description does not enable us to
determine whether these resemblances are due to common inheritance or to earlier language contact.
Sun (1993) divided Tani languages into two major branches, on phonological and lexical grounds: Western Tani (WT), clustered around the Subansiri and Kameng Rivers, and
Eastern Tani (ET), clustered around the Siang/Brahmaputra. The WT/ET division almost
Proto-Tani
Idu-Tawrã
Koro
Western Tani
Eastern Tani
Tangam,
Damu
Apatani
Subansiri
Pailibo
Bori
Karko,
Shimong
Nyishi-Hill
Miri-Lower
Tagin
Siang
Bokar, Ramo
Minyong
Na-BangniUpper Tagin
Milang
Galo
Pasi-Padam
Mising
(= “Lower Adi”)
Central contact zone
FIGURE 15.1 PROVISIONAL FAMILY TREE OF THE TANI LANGUAGES (REVISION OF SUN
1993: 297)
324 TIBETO-BURMAN: LANGUAGES OF NE INDIA
certainly has at least some sort of genealogical basis as Sun proposed; however, language
contact has played an at least equally powerful role in the evolution of Tani languages,
and has led to convergence and shared innovation both within and across genealogical
branches. Post (2015) reviewed 15 Tani phonological innovations exhibited by 15 Tani
languages, and found that no two high-level innovations identified the same set of genealogical branches. In general then, it is best to view the Tani languages as a dialect continuum,
with a reconstructible shared ancestor, but with a subsequent evolution marked at least as
much by areal sharing of innovations as by sharp genealogical branching. Figure 15.1
provides a heuristic Tani family tree on the basis of what might be called ‘aggregate sharing of innovations’. Depending on which of these innovations are attributed to genealogical inheritance, and which are attributed to contact, a large number of alternative trees
could be drawn (particularly with respect to geographically central Tani languages).
3 PHONOLOGY
Syllable and word are both important organizational units in Tani phonology. The ProtoTani (PT) syllable canon is (Ci)(G)V(X), in which Ci is an optional initial consonant, V is
an obligatory nucleus, G is an optional approximant r or j, and X is an optional coda, either
a nucleus-identical V or a final consonant Cf (generally drawn from a relatively restricted
set). In at least one language – Apatani – the X constituent may also be represented as
nasalization over the nuclear vowel. Tani syllables with an X constituent are bimoraic, or
heavy; syllables without an X constituent are monomoraic, or light. Many phonological
processes in Tani languages depend on this weight distinction, including syncope and
prominence assignment in Galo (Post 2007: §4.2.3), and the realization of tones in Apatani
(Post and Tage 2013). The PT syllable canon is well-preserved by the modern languages,
albeit in different ways; generally speaking, onsets (including clusters) are better preserved
in WT languages, while ET languages are more conservative with rhymes.
Tani segment inventories are relatively simple. Consonants are found at labial, alveolar, (alveolo-)palatal, velar and glottal places of articulation (Table 15.1). Dental and retroflex consonants are not found in Tani languages – nor indeed are they found in either
the Arunachali variety of Hindi or in the neighbouring Eastern Indo-Aryan language
Assamese; occasional transcriptions of dentals and retroflexes in the sources would
accordingly seem to be errors.
All Tani languages contrast voiceless and voiced plosives. Affricates pattern with plosives in languages which retain them; in Siang-area languages such as Pugo Galo and
Lower Adi, tɕ historically merges to ɕ and thence s or h. PT contrasted voiced and voiceless fricatives, but this distinction is maintained by very few modern languages other than
Na/Bangni; in most modern languages, fricatives have merged to a two-way distinction
between a glottal h, and a medial fricative, either the conservative ɕ (mostly in northerly
lects) or the innovative, Indo-Aryan-influenced s (towards the south). A small number of
languages, including Apatani, have also innovated a velar fricative x. A breathy/glottal
onset distinction among vowel-initial words is found in some languages, with the breathy
onset ʱ- reconstructible to a subset of PT fricatives, and the glottal onset ˀ- reflecting
vowel-initial morphemes at the PT stage; for example, compare Northwestern Galo ʱáa‘come; set (sun)’ (< PT *vaŋ, cf. Bangni vaa-) with ˀàp- ‘shoot’ (< PT *(ˀ )ap, cf. Bangni
ap-). Neither onset is found word-medially: compare the cognate first and second formatives in Northwestern Galo ʱag-jàa ‘fermented soya bean’ and pej-àk ‘soya bean’. Other
Tani languages, such as lower dialects of Lare Galo and Lower Adi, have merged these
categories completely, retaining only a single fricative s or h.
TANI LANGUAGES
325
TABLE 15.1 PROTO-TANI CONSONANT INVENTORY, BASED ON SUN (1993: 56)
Plosive [-VD]
Plosive [+VD]
Nasal
Fricative [-VD]
Fricative [+VD]
Lateral
Rhotic
Semivowel
Labial
Alveolar
(Alveolo-)Palatal
Velar
Glottal
*p
*b
*m
*f
*v
*t
*d
*n
*tɕ
*dʑ
*ɲ
*ɕ
*ʑ
*k
*g
*ŋ
(*ˀ)
*h
*ɦ
*l
*r
*j
TABLE 15.2 PROTO-TANI VOWEL INVENTORY,
BASED ON SUN (1993: 67)
High
Mid
Low
Front
Central
Back
*i
*e
*ɨ
*ə
*a
*u
*o
Most Tani languages preserve the PT seven vowel inventory (Table 15.2); although
Apatani split and merged PT *ə to the polar vowels (e.g. Apatani já-mù ‘fire’ < PT *mə
‘fire’), the same vowel ə has re-emerged due to a vowel centralization rule in the environment of rhotics (e.g. Apatani tà-də́r ‘worm’ < PT *dor ‘worm’). An unusually large ten
vowel system has been reported by Ou-Yang (1985: 77) for the Bodic-influenced and
possibly mixed language Damu: a, i, u, e, o, ə, ɨ, y, ø, ɿ (= syllabic z); this report remains
unconfirmed.
Although most instances of contrastive vowel length in underlying forms of morphemes may be secondary, vowel length is a robust phonological feature found in nearly
all Tani languages:2 compare Galo banám ‘to vomit’ and baanám ‘to move the head’.
While contrastive word-medial vowel length is usually easy to detect, underlying wordfinal vowel length is more difficult to detect in context-free utterances, and is absent from
the majority of descriptions. However, word-final vowel length can typically be discerned
in phrase-medial contexts: for example, Galo aló gó ‘some salt’ vs alóo gó ‘a day’. True
diphthongs are largely absent from the underlying forms of PT morphemes; diphthongs
in the modern languages are typically either recent loans or contracted compounds.
Tani languages vary greatly in their inventories of syllable codas. The language that
preserves the largest number of the original proto-codas is (Adi) Padam, with -p, -t, -k,
-m, -n, -ŋ, -r, and -l. Milang is the only other language documented that exhibits an -l
coda, albeit usually in Padam loans. The velar nasal *-ŋ and the dental/alveolar stop *-t
codas are most prone to loss, especially in WT. The tendency towards coda attrition is
epitomized in Apatani, where only two PT codas remain: -ʔ (from the original stop codas)
and -r. New final consonants, including -s, -l, and -m, have been imported via recent
Indo-European loans, such as pas ‘five’ (< Indo-Aryan). In Nyishi, Galo and Hill Miri,
non-original codas are created via the clipping of short final vowels from original disyllables, leading to an innovative coda voicing contrast: e.g. Nishi ab ‘father’, tap ‘pumpkin’
(Chhangte 1992: 4); cf. Bengni a-buu ‘father’, ta-pɨɨ ‘pumpkin’.
Phonological words are minimally bimoraic, and tend strongly to be disyllabic in Tani;
among languages in our Tani database for which more than 1,000 lemma have been
326 TIBETO-BURMAN: LANGUAGES OF NE INDIA
recorded, no language has more than 0.5 per cent morphologically simplex, monosyllabic
lexical words. When such words occur, they seem to be idiosyncratic retentions from a
basically monosyllabic proto-language; for example, Galo ɲíi ‘person’ but Minyong ˀa-mí
‘person’ and Apatani mí-yù ‘person’. Rhythm tends strongly to be trochaic in Tani, and
has a number of phonological outcomes, including coda-reductions and syncope processes, post-head clitic incorporation, and perhaps even the overall disyllabification of
the basically monosyllabic proto-lexicon (Post 2006, 2011b).
Phonemic tone is found in most relatively well-described Tani languages, including
Apatani (all dialects), Galo (all dialects), Na/Bangni, Upper Minyong and Tangam, as
well as in Milang. Generally speaking, tones are more robust and have a higher functional
load in WT languages than in ET languages; however, it would appear that both tone and
non-tone languages may be found in each branch. Analysis of tone in Tani languages
requires reference to both the surface phonological word and its underlying morpheme
constituents. In the case of underlying morphemes, no more than two contrastive tones
have been attested for any Tani language: an unmarked H and a marked L. These ‘underlying tone-bearing units (TBUs)’ then interact with syllable and word structure to produce
a surface word tone contour, rules for the derivation of which differ from language to
language. In Galo, a ‘pitch peak’ over an L word is attracted to heavy syllables, thus
interacting with rhythmic processes; for example, r ̀ -dùu ‘do-IPFV’ > [ˈr .́ dù] but r ̀ -dùu-kú
‘do-IPFV-COMPL’ > [r .́ ˈdûu.kù] (here and elsewhere in this chapter, á is a (mid-)high tone,
à is a low tone, and â is a (rising-)high-falling tone). In Apatani, two underlying H and L
tones interact with the structure of word-final syllables to produce three disyllabic word
tone contours: HH or HL for words with final light syllables (depending on the underlying
specifications of constituent morphemes), and LH for words with final heavy syllables
(independent of morpheme constituency). Underlying tones of words with final heavy
syllables in Apatani can be discerned via tone spreading to following articles or suffixes.
For example: ˀámí (kê) ‘(an) elder sister’, ˀámì (kè) ‘(a) tail’, ˀàpı́ ̃ (kê) ‘(some) cooked
rice’, ˀàmı́ ̃ (kè) ‘(a) name’. At least some Tani languages or dialects thereof seemingly
lack lexical tones, including Lower Adi, Mising and Nyishi. Unfortunately, due to an
ongoing lack of adequate description (particularly of ET languages), reconstruction of PT
tone categories is problematic.
Phonological and morphophonological alternations are extensive in Tani, particularly
syllable-structure adjustments or assimilation. The rich allomorphy of the nominal prefix
*a- in Bokar provides a typical example: a-ŋaa ‘child’; i-kii ‘dog’; u-puk ‘arrow’; e-tɕe
‘clothes’; o-ŋoo ‘fish’; ə-jək ‘pig’; ɨ-lɨŋ ‘stone’. Morpheme identification can be tricky without knowing the sandhi processes at work. For example, given Bokar luɣin ‘fingernail’, four
morphophonemic rules have to be undone to retrieve the original shapes of the component
morphemes lok- ‘hand’ and jin- ‘nail’: namely, initial j- simplification (→ lok-in), obstruent
voicing assimilation (→ log-in), spirantization (→ loɣ-in), and vowel assimilation
(→ luɣ-in), compare Bokar a-lok ‘hand’, lə-jin ‘bird’s claw’ (compare also Bengni lak-sin
‘fingernail’, from PT *lak-ʑin). In general, morphophonological processes are more intricate
among WT languages than among ET languages, perhaps reaching an apex in Tangam.
4 MORPHOLOGY
4.1 Diachrony
Tani morphology is best understood against the backdrop of its evolution. Most Tani morphemes are reconstructible as simplex monosyllables, and presumably reflect simplex,
TANI LANGUAGES
327
monosyllabic lexical words at some early stage (whether PT or earlier). Similarly, most
Tani grammatical morphemes are reconstructible to earlier lexical roots; for example,
most tense-aspect morphemes reconstruct to an earlier set of clause-final existential or
auxiliary verbs (e.g. Galo -dùu ‘IPFV’ < PT *duŋ ‘sit; be at (sitting)’, or Apatani -dàʔ ‘COS’
< PT *dak ‘stand; be at (standing)’), and most predicate derivations originated as constituents of earlier serial verb constructions (e.g. Minyong laa-bi ‘take-BEN’ < PT *laŋ-bi
‘take-give’). The modern languages are synthetic and agglutinating to varying degrees;
most nouns and adjectives are disyllabic compounds (or further compounds thereof),
while earlier serial verb constructions have amalgamated into a single, morphologically
complex and expansive predicate word (1).
(1) Galo
tolo
tɕàa-lèn-l ̀ ɨ-máa-dùu
< *tə
*lo *tɕaŋ
*len *lɨŋ
DST.UP.LOC
ascend-OUT-DESD-NEG-IPFV
DST.UP
LOC ascend
exit want
‘(I) don’t want to go up there.’
*maŋ
*duŋ
not.(have) sit/stay
4.2 Word classes
PT appears to have had nouns and verbs, together with classifiers, postpositions, demonstratives, articles, conjunctions and particles. The modern languages have nouns, most
often disyllabic prefixations or compounds of earlier nouns, adjectives, which are nounlike in structure and often derive from deverbal nominalizations, as well as classifiers,
postpositions, demonstratives, articles, ideophones and particles. Adverbs are not
well-represented in either the early or the modern languages; manner adverbials in the
modern languages are typically derived from adjectives via a postposition or suffix *pə
(prob. < PTB nominalizer *pa), as in Lower Adi kampo pə ‘beautiful AVZR’ ‘nicely; beautifully’, while temporal expressions occur as oblique noun phrases, such as Lower Adi
jumə lo ‘in (the) evening’. A few particles and predicate derivations also have adverb-like
meanings and/or functions, such as the Adi ‘versatile’ particle ruŋ ‘certainly; definitely’
or the Adi predicate derivation -man ‘playfully’. Verbs in the modern languages are best
discussed in terms of predicate structure (section 4.3.2).
Nouns are most easily distinguished by their ability to take articles, as in Adi ami ko,
Apatani míyù kè (person IND) ‘a person’. Verb roots must be nominalized to be used in
referential phrases, as in Adi do-nam ko (eat-NZR IND) ‘some food’. Conversely, nouns
cannot usually take predicate morphology; Galo *ɲíi-dùu ‘person-IPFV’ is unacceptable.
In many languages, adjectives can be distinguished from verb roots and nouns by their
ability to occur in both an article-derived copula construction, like nouns, and in an
inflected predicate, like verb roots (2).
(2)
Lower Adi
apeŋ=ə!
ŋo
apee-duŋ.
tired-IPFV
tired=COP 1.SG
‘How tired (I am)!’; ‘I’m tired.’
Singular, dual and plural pronouns occur in three persons in the majority of Tani languages (Milang has an additional clusivity distinction, seemingly absent from Tani
proper). However, only the first and second person forms appear to be reconstructible;
third person forms often reflect earlier *ba-ɦɨ, being a composition of *ɦɨ ‘self’ with an
328 TIBETO-BURMAN: LANGUAGES OF NE INDIA
TABLE 15.3 TANI FIRST AND SECOND PERSON PRONOUNS
1
2
SG
DL
PL
*ŋo
‘I’
*no
‘you’
*ŋo ɲi
‘I + two’
*no ɲi
‘you + two’
*ŋo luŋ
‘I + group’
*no luŋ
‘you + group’
unknown morpheme (as in Pailibo maɨ ‘3.SG’), or a demonstrative *ɦə (as in Tagin ʱə
‘3.SG’) or else ko as in Bokar (possibly < *ko ‘one’). Tangam has an especially unusual
third person singular form nodɨ – the second formative in fact cognate with *ɦɨ ‘self’, but
the first formative oddly homophonous with the second person singular no, and different
from the 3DL and 3PL forms daɲi and datəŋ (Table 15.3).
Classifiers seem to be a PT innovation, not found in Milang (Post and Modi 2011). PT
classifiers are monosyllabic and simplex: e.g. *tɨŋ ‘CLF: GROUP (OF ANIMALS)’, *soŋ ‘CLF:
LONG, SLENDER’, *buŋ ‘CLF: LONG, HOLLOW’ and *tak ‘CLF: FRAGMENT OF SPHERE’. Modern Tani
languages tend to have dozens of classifiers; in some languages, classifier roots are prefixed
in their citation form, as in Galo ˀa-búu ‘CLF: LONG, HOLLOW’, or else compounded to numeral
roots or a small number of adjectival roots, as in Galo búu-ɲí ‘CLF: LONG, HOLLOW-two’ or
búu-tə̀ ‘CLF: LONG, HOLLOW-big’. In other languages, such as Apatani, classifier roots only
occur in compounded forms. In Bokar, classifiers are reported to not occur with numerals
higher than one (Sun et al. 1980: 128). Notably, all known Tani classifier systems lack a
generic classifier (comparable to Mandarin ge), as well as a classifier for human beings.
Instead, humans and unclassifiables are enumerated without the help of a classifier.
Particles are a broad category, expressing a host of modal, evidential and illocutionaryforce meanings. In general, inventories are much larger in WT languages than in ET, sometimes by a factor of six or seven. Some of the best attested Tani particles are *ju(kə) ‘REP’, *dɨ
‘WONDER’, *pə ‘INFR’ and *la ‘CQ’; typically, particles follow the focus of a clause, occurring
clause-finally in the majority of cases, or else after a focused NP in a cleft/focus construction
(section 5.1). Postpositions and articles are reconstructible, and will be discussed in
section 5.2. At least two topographical-deictic distal demonstratives *tə ‘DST.UP(RIVER)’ and
*bə ‘DST.DOWN(RIVER)’ are reconstructible, with a third category ‘DST.SLEV’ also reconstructible, but of uncertain phonetic value (possibly *ə). A proximate *ɕi is also reconstructible;
languages vary in the remainder of their demonstrative inventories, but generally include
either a medial-distal or an addressee-proximate, both of which also track anaphora. Ideophones are found widely in Tani, but remain poorly described. Many have non-prototypical
phonology, and typically occur in construction with the verb ‘say’ (or a cognate suffix).
(3)
Galo
cubúk!
ə́m-làa
sound.of.silent.entry.into.water say-NF
‘He slipped “sploosh!” into the water.’
ó-l ̀ k-káa
fall.from.height-INSERT-PRF
4.3 Word structures
4.3.1 Nouns and adjectives
Tani nouns and adjectives generally have one of the structures [ROOT] or, more often, [PFXor [ROOT-ROOT], as in Galo ɲíi ‘person’, ˀa-d ́ r ‘tired’ and lə̀-cèk ‘numb, of feet
ROOT]
TANI LANGUAGES
329
(< leg-numb)’ or Apatani dʑìi ‘black’, ˀà-míʔ ‘eye’ and mìʔ-láa ‘tear (< eye-juice)’. The most
commonly attested prefixes are *a-, *ɕ(j)a-, *ja-, *ta-, and *pa-;*ɕ(j)a- is attached to roots
denoting higher animals; *ta- is used for lower animals, insects, trees, male proper names
and small objects in general; *ja- is used with colours, diseases, other negative-connotation
items and female proper names; *pa- is confined to bird names and related nouns, e.g.
Bokar pɨ-tɨr ‘chicken carrying basket’. As for *a-, it is found on many basic nouns,
including kinship terms, body parts and basic features of the natural world, as well as on
many basic adjectives; presumably, *a- derives from the early TB nominalizer. The prefixes *ɕ(j)a- and *pa- appear to derive from earlier initial formatives of compounds,
reflecting perhaps PTB *sja ‘flesh/meat/animal’ and *bja ‘bird’ (Benedict 1972: 46, 177).
Compounds of the form [ROOT-ROOT] are common among nouns and adjectives, often
following a classificatory template in which the first formative names a generic class or
‘type’ and the second a specific ‘exemplar’ (Figure 15.2). The exemplar formative may
in turn stand as a new type; the resulting word networks and families can be large
(Table 15.4).
Semi-reduplicative expressive compounds of the form A-B A-C are also common
among Tani nouns, adjectives, and verbs alike, as illustrated by these Bengni examples:
a-tuŋ a-juŋ ‘garbage’, a-bak a-jak ‘thick (liquid)’, dʑi-sit dʑi-mit ‘to pass something
around’, and jip-mii jip-maa ‘sleepy’. The familiar Tibeto-Burman voicing alternation in
Basic Noun ?a-
Type
Exemplar
Type
?a-pòm
?a-móo
‘cluster’
Exemplar
Type
moo-bàa ‘plateau’
Exemplar
tum-pə ‘dryland jungle’
‘place’
moo-tùm ‘jungle’
?a-pə
‘globe’
moo-mèn ‘clearing’
tum-ràa ‘dense jungle’
FIGURE 15.2 CLASSIFICATORY WORD FORMATION IN GALO (FROM POST 2012)
TABLE 15.4 WORD FAMILY BASED ON THE INITIAL ROOT l ̀ ɨ ‘STONE’ (FROM POST 2006)
lɨɨtɕàk
lɨɨm ̀ k
lɨɨmìk
lɨɨtàk
lɨɨkàa
lɨɨpùu
lɨɨtɕ ̀ k
‘pebble’
‘gravel’
‘algae’
‘giant boulder’
‘igneous rock’
‘marble’
‘cooking tripod’
lɨɨnə̀
lɨɨpùm
lɨɨtòr
lɨɨjàa
lɨɨkə̀r
lɨɨtɕùm
lɨɨpə̀
‘boulder’
‘stone pile’
‘hard stone’
‘soft stone’
‘green bead var.’
‘green bead var.’
‘sharpening stone’
330 TIBETO-BURMAN: LANGUAGES OF NE INDIA
the formation of simplex-causative pairs is preserved in some measure, albeit unproductively: for example, Galo d ́ r- ‘break (of a long thing)’ vs t ́ r- ‘break (a long thing)’, or
búk- ‘burst’ vs púk- ‘pop (knuckles)’.
4.3.2 Predicates
Most Tani predicates have the basic structure [V.ROOT(-DERIVATIONS)-INFLECTIONS], an
example of which can be found in (3); adjectives may also head a predicate (e.g. (2)), but
take a smaller set of the available derivations and inflections. Many verb roots are cognate
with noun roots, and are preferentially used together with a preceding cognate nominal
(usually non-referential and unmarked for case), as in the following Bengni examples:
(4)
to lay egg
to spit
to dream
to wear shoes
pɨ-pɨ pɨta-tɕɨr tɕɨrjip-maa maalɨ-kjam kjam-
(cf. pɨ-pɨ egg)
(cf. ta-tɕɨr spittle)
(cf. jip-maa dream)
(cf. lɨ-kjam shoe)
Derivation in the Tani predicate is prolific. Many predicate derivations are semantically
rich, homophonous with verb roots, and in simple stems may be interpretable as compounded lexical roots: for example, Galo ín-mèn ‘go-AS.PLAY’ ‘stroll’. However, predicate
derivations tend to be fully productive, and as many as four or five can be ‘stacked’ onto
a single stem, as in the following Minyong example (5). Predicate derivations encode
meanings related to manner, result, direction, aktionsart, modality, argument structure
change (e.g. applicatives) and word class change (Post 2010).
(5)
ami
ə=kom
gok-ta-kɨ-ram-hɨ-kaa-to=î.
call-INCP-TENT-FRUS-REFL-EXP-PFV=QTAG
person IND=ADD
‘The guy also tried in vain to have a go at calling, eh.’
Nominalization is highly functional in Tani languages, forming relative clause constructions in addition to action and participant nouns (section 5.3). The most widely attested
nominalizers are realis -na ~ -nə ‘subject/actor NMLZ’, -nam ‘action/undergoer NMLZ’ -nana
‘instrumental NMLZ’ and -ko ‘locative NMLZ’; some languages also have irrealis/purposive
nominalizers, such as Galo -há.
Tani is similar to most other eastern Himalayan Tibeto-Burman subgroups, but different from several groups to the east and west, in lacking grammaticalized person-indexing
on the predicate. The relevant inflectional categories of the predicate are aspect and mode,
much as in Tibetan. Some amount of variation is found among Tani inflectional systems;
the most widely attested set of predicate inflections is given in Table 15.5, together with
their lexical source values (when known); a few languages exhibit additional inflections
with uncertain provenance, such as Apatani -cì ‘intentional’ and -ŋé ‘imperative’, and
Milang -kaʎ ‘irrealis’.
In most Tani languages, predicate formation is sensitive to a relationship between subject person and speaker knowledge, comparable in many respects to the ‘egophoric’ systems found in Bodic languages and their neighbours. In general, declarative statements
with a non-first person subject in perfective aspects are unacceptable unless qualified, as
in these Bengni examples:
(6)
a. ŋuu kanoo-pa
1SG hungry-PFV
‘I have become hungry.’
TANI LANGUAGES
331
TABLE 15.5 TANI PREDICATE INFLECTIONS AND THEIR
LEXICAL SOURCES
Domain
PT
Negation
Imperfectives
*maŋ
*duŋ
*do
*dak
*to
*ka
*ku
*rje
*ai
Perfectives
Irrealis
Anterior
Value
Source
NEG
‘not (have)’
‘sit; be (at)’
‘lie down; be (at)’
‘stand; be (at)’
‘have/be associated to’
‘have/exist’
Pre-PT
‘live/exist’
PTB *waj ‘cop’ (?)
IPFV
STAT
COS
PFV
PRF
COMPL
IRR
ANTR
b. tatɨk . kanoo-pɨ-dɨɨ (*kanoo-pa)
Tatik . hungry-PFV-WONDER
‘Tatik has become hungry.’
In some paradigms, as in the Galo ‘direct’ perfective, dedicated suffixes index the
‘egophoric’ (a.k.a. ‘conjunct’, or ‘self-person’) category of first person in declaratives/
second person in interrogatives, and the ‘alterphoric’ (a.k.a. ‘disjunct’ or ‘other-person’)
category of second/third in declaratives, first/third in interrogatives (7a–b). These are
semantic rather than morphosyntactic indices, however, a fact which can be demonstrated by the acceptability of other persons when evidential particles which alter the
construal are employed (Post 2013) (7b). Finally, note that unlike in Tibeto-Burman
languages to the north and west, egophoricity in Galo is not sensitive to agentivity or
volitionality.
(7)
Galo
a. ŋó (*nó, *b ̀ ɨ)
ˀatɕín
dó-tó-bá
1.SG (2.SG, 3.SG) cooked.rice eat-PFV:EGO-PFV:DIR
‘I’ve just had my meal (I experienced it).’
b. nó/b ̀ ɨ (*ŋó)
ˀatɕín
dó-ggée-bá
2.SG/3.SG) (1.SG) cooked.rice eat-PFV:ALTER-PFV:DIR
‘You/he/she just had your/his/her meal (I witnessed it).’
c. b ̀ ɨ
ˀatɕín
dó-tó-bá
jukə̀
cooked.rice eat-PFV:EGO-PFV:DIR REP
3.SG
‘He, it is said, “had my meal.”’
d. ŋó (*nó, *b ̀ ɨ)
ò-lòo-tó-bá
(*o-loo-gee-ba)
1.SG (2.SG, 3.SG) fall-DOWN-PFV:EGO-PFV-DIR fall-DOWN-PFV:ALTER-PFV-DIR
‘I fell down (I experienced it).’
5 SYNTAX
5.1 Clause and phrase types and structures
Tani information structure has the basic order [TOPIC FOCUS]. Syntactically, clauses can be
divided into predicative and appositive subtypes. A predicative clause is headed by an
332 TIBETO-BURMAN: LANGUAGES OF NE INDIA
inflecting predicate (4.3.2), which usually occurs last, in the FOCUS position (7); more
topical noun phrases occur furthest from the predicate, with the most frequent resulting
order among core arguments of multi-argument clauses being agent > recipient > patient.
However, different orders are straightforwardly possible (8):
(8)
Galo (popular song by Marto Kamdak)
nó-m̀ =ne
ŋó… káa-tó-làa
2.SG-ACC=NAGT 1.SG look-PFV-NF
‘I saw you.’
A basic appositive clause consists of two noun phrases arrayed in the order [TOPIC FOCUS].
Appositive clauses generally handle equative and attributive functions. In many if not all
Tani languages, the demonstrative *ɦə ‘DIST/ANAP’ has developed via a definite article into
a copula in this construction, and can predicate an adjectival as well as nominal attributive focus.
(9)
Tangam
a. hi
apoŋ
(e < *ɦə)
PROX
rice.beer (COP)
‘This is rice beer.’
b. ŋo
kenoŋ
e
1.SG hungry COP
‘I’m hungry.’
Posture verbs *duŋ ‘sit’, *dak ‘stand’ and *do(ŋ) ‘lie’ also developed locative existential
copula functions in the same construction; however, languages vary in the extent to which
this construction is retained. Galo appears to have merged this construction into the
predicative clause type – choosing to inflect its locational posture verbs – and some Adi
varieties have generalized *duŋ in all locative-existential functions. However, the full
paradigm is retained in Apatani, in which dàʔ predicates entities with legs, or in a standing position, dó predicates other inanimate entities, and dùu predicates other animate
entities (10).
(10)
Apatani
a. n ́ -kà
pàróʔ
so
dàʔ
kèe
PROX.LOC be.at.LOC(STANDING) POL
2.SG-GEN fowl
‘Your chicken is (standing) here.’
b. mòo-kà
mj ́ ɨ
dùu
be.at.LOC(ANIMATE)
3.SG-GEN wife
‘His wife is here.’
c. ŋ ́ -kà
póosá
dó
1.SG-GEN money be.at.LOC(INANIMATE)
‘I have money (with me).’
Negation in appositive clauses is generally handled by post-modifying the focus with
a negator ma(ŋ) or mo(ŋ) (the latter mainly found in north-central-area languages).
Only Apatani is known to have a suppletive negative copula ɲímá. Illocutionary
force is generally signalled via a wide range of particles. In most cases, particles
occur clause-finally; however, in a marked focus construction, they mark the focus.
When a marked focus construction is based on a predicative (not an appositive)
TANI LANGUAGES
333
clause, and when the predicate is outside the focus, the predicate must either be nominalized, or else is marked by a nominalizer-derived suffix, as in the following Lower
Adi example:
(11)
Lower Adi
no
kolo
la gɨ-ye-n?
2SG where CQ go-IRR-Q(< NMLZ:SBJ)
‘Where are you going?’
Within the noun phrase, compound nominals are typically MODIFIER–HEAD, as in Lower
Adi kalam diitə ‘Mt. Kalam’. Coordinative compounding is also common at this level,
with each noun signifying an exemplar and the whole signifying a superordinate type,
as in Galo donám-tɨɨnám ‘eat.NMLZ imbibe.NMLZ’ ‘provisions’. Possessors are pre-head
(10), while classifiers and numerals follow the head in that order, as in Apatani s ́ ɨ
dór-ɲì ‘cattle CLF-two’, and are followed by relator nouns, articles and/or postpositions, as in Galo ˀaɲ ́ ɨ kook ̀ ɨ lo ‘after a year (year back LOC)’. A striking characteristic
of demonstratives is they can occur on both flanks of the head (e.g. Bengni sii əki sii
‘this here dog’). Nominalization-based relative clauses also occur on both sides of the
head, as in Lower Adi dəm-nam ami ~ ami dəm-nam ‘person who was hit’ (section
5.3). Adjectival modifiers, if formed as relative clauses, in the same way may occur on
both sides; in languages which allow direct adjectival modification (without a nominalizer), the adjective can only post-modify, as in Lower Adi mimum kampo ‘woman
beautiful’.
5.2 Referential and relational marking
Tani noun phrases are marked differently depending on whether they represent generic/
non-referential, indefinite (new, usually focal), or definite (given, usually topical) information, and on whether they are in subject, non-subject core or oblique functions. Indefinite NPs are typically marked by an article *(a)ko (< ‘one’), while definite NPs are
marked either by an appropriate demonstrative, or by a demonstrative-derived article,
usually *ɦə. Generic/non-referential NPs are typically unmarked.
Relational marking follows a basically nominative–accusative pattern: subjects tend to
be definite, but are unmarked for case, whether functioning as the single intransitive argument or as the actor of a transitive verb. It is common to encounter identifications of a
‘nominative case’ marker ə in early descriptions of Tani languages; in fact, this is a reflex
of the definite/topic marker *ɦə (12).
(12)
Lower Adi
a. bɨ
si-kai
3SG die-PRF
‘He died.’
b. bɨ-k
əsu
ə
si-kai
3SG-GEN mithun TOP die-PRF
‘His mithun died.’
Non-subject core arguments are marked by a form of the accusative case when definite.
Objects are distinguished via an unmarked accusative *-m (for pronouns, see (8)) ~ *ɦəm
(for full NPs; see (13b)); note that the latter incorporates an earlier demonstrative *ɦə,
334 TIBETO-BURMAN: LANGUAGES OF NE INDIA
which explains its restriction to definite noun phrases. Non-referential undergoers are
generally predicate-adjacent, and unmarked for case (as in (7)). Indefinite arguments are
never marked for case (13a).
(13)
Bokar
a. ŋoo pətaŋ ako(*-m)
koŋpoŋ-pa
INDEF(*-OBJ) see-PFV
1SG bird
‘I saw a bird.’
b. ŋoo pətaŋ-hə-m koŋpoŋ-pa
1SG bird-DEF-OBJ see-PFV
‘I saw the bird.’
In most languages, there is an additional ‘non-agentive’ object marker usually
descended from *mɨ (possibly itself the source of our pronominal accusative), which
functions to highlight the object status of a highly topical/animate participant; an
example of this is in (8) (using the innovative Galo form nè). Recipients are marked
in the same manner as undergoers, with animacy and context apparently sufficing to
disambiguate. Relational marking of agents has not been attested, except for demoted
agents which are marked by an instrumental postposition in the passive constructions
of some languages (14):
(14)
Minyong
bɨ
hɨmjo kɨŋ do-ko-hɨ-to
INST
eat-PASS-REFL-PFV
3.SG tiger
‘He was eaten by a tiger.’
A genitive case marker *ka is widespread and marks adnominal noun subordination (10),
as well as subjects of both intransitive and transitive nominalized clauses (section 5.3).
The allative cases are often identical to, or built on, the locative; the ablative and comitative are likewise usually derived from the locative, e.g. lokə ‘from’ < LOC=GEN. An instrumental case marker *kɨŋ is found in some though not all languages (see (12)); in languages
which lack it, the instrumental is typically built on the ablative and/or genitive (as in Galo
lokkə̀).
5.3 Complex constructions
Tani languages are particularly rich in the variety of complex constructions they exhibit.
Nominalization-based constructions are especially prominent; complex constructions not
based on synchronic nominalizations include clause-chaining, postpositional and/or
adverbial subordination, and complementation. Serial verb constructions are absent from
the modern languages.
Nominalization-based constructions include action nominalizations, several types of
participant nominalization, internally headed, externally headed and headless relative
clause constructions, cleft/focus constructions ((11) earlier), and clausal nominalizations
(see Post 2011a). In general, a set of three to five ‘primary’ nominalizers (4.3.2) mark the
participant/action and often also the modality status of the nominalized clause. In (15a),
the Galo nominalizer -há produces an irrealis action nominalization, while in (15b) the
same nominalizer produces a gapped externally headed non-subject relative clause construction (note here the genitive subject marking).
TANI LANGUAGES
(15)
335
Galo (Post 2011a: 267–9)
a. bulù-kə̀
ˀagóm=əm porì-há
káa-máa
speech=ACC study-NMLZ:IRR.EVENT have/exist-NEG
3.PL-GEN
‘There would be no (need/cause) to study their language.’
ŋó-kə
dó-há
jaráa=ə
1.SG-GEN
Øi
eat-NMLZ:IRR.NONSBJ goodsi=COP
‘This is the thing that I’ll eat (lit. my to-be-eaten thing).’
b. hɨgɨ
SPRX.IND
Various types of adverbial subordination, including manner, purposive and temporal
clauses are marked by postpositions, typically *pə (< DAT), *lo (< LOC) and *(ɦə)m (< ACC),
and/or by dedicated markers built at least partly on one of them. Similar to nominalized
clauses, many postpositionally subordinated clause types also exhibit a genitive subject
(16a); however, postpositionally subordinated clause heads are often inflected (unlike
most nominalized clause heads), typically lose genitive subject marking when more
advanced in their development, and more generally tend to more closely resemble finite
clauses than do nominalized clauses (16b).
(16)
a. Galo (Post 2011a: 282)
taníi=gə hobə́=ə̀m
pá-dàk=lo
ŋó
ˀín-ɲíi-tó
3.SG=GEN mithun=ACC strike.to.cut-COS=LOC 1.SG GO-ABANDON-PFV
‘I left as Tani killed the mithun (lit. at Tani’s killing of the mithun).’
b. Galo (Post 2009: 84)
nó
hag ̀ ɨ-booló hag ̀ ɨ-rə́
sigh-IRR
2.SG sigh-COND
‘If you sigh, (it too) will sigh.’
Complex and chained events in a thematically united sequence are typically represented
using clause chains, most often marked via a non-final suffix (< additive coordinator *laŋ).
Depending on a variety of factors, non-final predicates may or may not be inflected, and often
exhibit additional suffixes (such as completive or attainment suffixes) which serve to ‘finetune’ the temporal-episodic sequencing of events, as in example (17) from Upper Minyong:
(17)
Upper Minyong
dəl
ammo hɨdla…
də iˀala,
ŋolu məhaalaŋkula…
dəlo
ammo hɨt-la
də i-pa-la
ŋolu mə-haŋ-laŋ-ku-la
ANAP.LOC paddy dibble-NF ANAP DO-ATTN-NF 1.PL
put-UPWARD-NF-COMPL-NF
‘And then we planted paddy…and after doing that, we would leave it to grow…’
Clause chaining and action nominalization are also frequently interwoven to produce
episodic breaks amid thematic continuity, as is widely the case in Tibeto-Burman (18).
(18)
Upper Minyong
də
bɨɲi jə-bo-hɨ-duŋ.
jə-bo-hɨ-la
i-nam=ə…
ANAP 3.DL live-RECP-REFL-IPFV live-RECP-REFL-NF do-NMLZ:RLS.EVENT TOP
‘So, the two of them lived together. Because of that (lit. Living together and that
being so)…’
ADDITIONAL ABBREVIATIONS
ADD
ANTR
Additive
Anterior
ALTER
ATTN
Alterphoric
Attainment
ANAP
AVZR
Anaphoric
Adverbializer
336 TIBETO-BURMAN: LANGUAGES OF NE INDIA
COS
EGO
INCP
NAGT
QTAG
SLEV
STAT
Change of state
Egophoric
Incipient
Non-agentive
Question tag
Same (topographic) level
Stative
CQ
EXP
IND
NF
REP
SPRX
TENT
Content interrogative
Experiential
Individuator
Non-finite/non-final
Reportative
Speaker-proximate
Tentative
DIR
FRUS
INFR
POL
RLS
Direct evidential
Frustrative
Inferential
Polite
Realis
NOTES
1 Simon (1972) and Abraham (1985, 1987) have been field-checked and found to be
incomplete and/or unreliable in very many respects. Apatani Language Development
Committee (2015) is reliable, but far from comprehensive.
2 Kepor Mara, a trained linguist and native Tagin speaker, has indicated that vowel
length may be non-contrastive in Tagin, however this remains unconfirmed.
REFERENCES
Abraham, P. T. (1985). Apatani Grammar. Mysore: Central Institute of Indian Languages.
—— (1987). Apatani–English–Hindi Dictionary. Mysore: Central Institute of Indian
Languages.
Apatani Language Development Commttee (2015). ‘Tanw Aguñ: An Audio Dictionary
of the Apatani Language.’ Available at: language.apatani.org (accessed 15 August
2016).
Benedict, Paul K. (James A. Matisoff, contributing editor). (1972). Sino-Tibetan:
A Conspectus. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Census of India. (2011). A-11: Individual Scheduled Tribe Primary Census Abstract Data
and its Appendix: Arunachal Pradesh and Assam. New Delhi: Government of India.
Available at: www.censusindia.gov.in/2011census/PCA/ST.html (accessed 4 April 2016).
Chhangte, Thangi. (1992). Phonology of some Nishi (Dafla) dialects. Paper presented at
the 25th International Conference on Sino-Tibetan Languages and Linguistics, Berkeley,
University of California.
Huber, Toni. (2012). Micro-migrations of hill peoples in Northern Arunachal Pradesh:
Rethinking methodologies and claims of origin in Tibet. In: Origins and Migrations in
the Extended Eastern Himalaya, ed. by Toni Huber and Stuart Blackburn. Leiden:
Brill, 83–106.
Modi, Yankee. (2005). Emergence and Impact of Hindi as a Lingua Franca in Arunachal
Pradesh. New Delhi: Jawaharlal Nehru University M. Phil Thesis.
Ou-Yang, Jue-Ya. (1985). ⨎ᐤ᯿䈝䀰ㆰᘇ˄ፙቬঊాቄ䈝) [Luobazu Yuyan Jianzhi
(Bengni Boga’eryu)/Brief Description of the Luoba Nationality Language (The BengniBokar Language)]. Beijing: ≁᯿ࠪ⡸⽮ [Minzu Chubanshe/Nationalities Press].
Pegu, Jugendra. (2011). Morpho-syntactic variation in the pagro and sa:jaŋ dialects of the
Mising community. In: North East Indian Linguistics Vol. 3, ed. by G. Hyslop,
S. Morey and M. W. Post. New Delhi: Cambridge University Press India, 155–170.
Post, Mark W. (2006). Compounding and the structure of the Tani lexicon. Linguistics of
the Tibeto-Burman Area 29.41–60.
—— (2007). A Grammar of Galo. Melbourne: La Trobe University PhD Thesis.
—— (2009). The semantics of clause linking in Galo. In: The Semantics of Clause Linking:
A Cross-Linguistic Typology, ed. by R. M. W. Dixon and A. Y. Aikhenvald. Oxford:
Oxford University Press, 75–91.
TANI LANGUAGES
337
—— (2010). Predicate derivations in the Tani languages: Root, suffix, both or neither? In:
North East Indian Linguistics Vol. 2, ed. by S. Morey and M. W. Post. New Delhi:
Cambridge University Press India, 175–197.
—— (2011a). Nominalization and nominalization-based constructions in Galo. In:
Nominalization in Asian Languages: Diachronic and Typological Perspectives, ed. by
F. H. Yap, K. Grunow-Hårsta and J. Wrona. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 255–287.
—— (2011b). Prosody and typological drift in Austroasiatic and Tibeto-Burman: In:
Against ‘Sinosphere’ and ‘Indosphere’. Austroasiatic Studies: Papers from ICAAL4.
Mon-Khmer Studies Special Issue No. 3, ed. by S. Srichampa, P. Sidwell and K. J.
Gregerson. Canberra: Pacific Linguistics, 198–221.
—— (2012). The language, culture, environment and origins of Proto-Tani speakers:
What is knowable, and what is not (yet). In: Origins and Migrations in the Extended
Eastern Himalaya, ed. by T. Huber and S. Blackburn. Leiden: Brill, 153–186.
—— (2013). Person-sensitive TAME marking in Galo: Historical origins and functional
motivation. In: Functional-Historical Approaches to Explanation: In Honor of Scott
DeLancey, ed. by T. Thornes, J. Jansen, G. Hyslop and E. Andvik. Amsterdam: John
Benjamins, 107–130.
—— (2015). Sharing innovations across branches: A case study of the Tani subgroup of
Trans-Himalayan [Tibeto-Burman]. Paper presented at the International Conference
on Historical Linguistics 22. Naples, Italy.
Post, Mark W. and Roger Blench. (2011). Siangic: A new language phylum in North East
India. Paper presented at the 6th International Conference of the North East Indian
Linguistics Society, Tezpur University, Tezpur, Assam, India.
Post, Mark W. and Yankee Modi. (2011). Language contact and the genetic position of
Milang (Eastern Himalaya). Anthropological Linguistics 53.215–258.
Post, Mark W. and Kanno Tage. (2013). Apatani phonology and lexicon, with a special
focus on tone. Himalayan Linguistics 12.17–75.
Simon, Ivan Martin. (1972). An Introduction to Apatani. Gangtok, Sikkim: Government
of India Press.
Sun, Hong-Kai, Ji-Chuan Zhang, Shao-Zun Lu and Jue-Ya Ou-Yang. (1980). 䰘ᐤǃ
⨎ᐤǃ܌ӪⲴ䈝䀰 [Menba, Luoba, Dengren de Yuyan/The Language of the Menba,
Luoba and Dengren] Beijing: ѝഭ⽮Պ、ᆖࠪ⡸⽮ [Zhongguo Shehui Kexue
Chubanshe/Chinese Social Sciences Press].
Sun, Tian-Shin Jackson. (1993). A Historical-Comparative Study of the Tani (Mirish)
Branch of Tibeto-Burman. Berkeley: University of California PhD Dissertation.