Current Psychology
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-019-00552-y
Humor styles across 28 countries
Julie Aitken Schermer 1 & Radosław Rogoza 2 & Maria Magdalena Kwiatkowska 2 & Christopher Marcin Kowalski 1 &
Sibele Aquino 3 & Rahkman Ardi 4 & Henrietta Bolló 5 & Marija Branković 6 & Razieh Chegeni 7 & Jan Crusius 8 &
Marta Doroszuk 9 & Violeta Enea 10 & Thi Khanh Ha Truong 11 & Dzintra Iliško 12 & Tomislav Jukić 13 & Emira Kozarević 14 &
Gert Kruger 15 & Adil Kurtić 14 & Jens Lange 16 & Kadi Liik 17 & Sadia Malik 18 & Samuel Lins 19 & Agim Mamuti 20 &
Laura Martinez-Buelvas 21 & Benjamin Mrkušić 22 & Ginés Navarro-Carrillo 23 & Oscar Oviedo-Trespalacios 24,25 &
Emrah Özsoy 26 & Eva Papazova 27 & Joonha Park 28 & Natalia Pylat 29 & Goran Riđić 30 & Ognjen Riđić 31 & Dženan Skelić 32 &
Chee-Seng Tan 33 & Jorge Torres-Marín 34 & Osman Uslu 26 & Tatiana Volkodav 35 & Anna Włodarczyk 36 & Georg Krammer 37
# Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019
Abstract
Responses to a measure of the four humor styles of affiliative, aggressive, self-enhancing, and self-defeating from the
Humor Styles Questionnaire (HSQ; Martin et al. Journal of Research in Personality, 37(1), 48–75, 2003) were collected
from individuals (N = 8361) in 28 countries encompassing 21 different languages. The purpose of this global collaboration was to examine both differences and similarities of humor styles across nations at the descriptive level. Across the
countries, typically the highest scores were for the affiliative humor style. When each humor style was examined, some
country samples demonstrated differences in mean scores. For example, the samples from Hungary, Indonesia, South
Africa, and Serbia had high self-enhancing scores and Japan scored the lowest. In contrast to mean differences, almost
all of the countries demonstrated positive inter-scale correlations, similar sex differences, and similar correlations with
age, suggesting more similarities than differences. As discussed, some of the samples had low internal consistency
values and poorly fitting factor structures for the humor style scales, suggesting that those results should be interpreted
with caution.
Keywords Humor styles . Cross culture . Adults
Introduction
The Four Humor Styles
This study investigates the possible similarities and differences in four humor styles across 28 countries and 21 languages at the descriptive level. As humor styles are representative of how individuals use humor in their daily life and are
conceptualized as individual difference variables akin to personality (Martin et al. 2003), knowing more about humor
styles across countries adds information to how individuals
interact with each other and use humor both individually and
interpersonally.
Humor styles are representative of how individuals use
humor in their daily life (Martin et al. 2003). The four
humor styles model proposed by Martin et al. (2003) decomposes humor styles into a 2 × 2 design with one dimension representing the focus (or target) of the style (self
versus the group) and the other representing the nature of
the humor (positive or benevolent versus negative or potentially detrimental or malevolent). The two group or outward or other-oriented humor styles are affiliative and aggressive. The affiliative humor style is a positive humor
style that involves using humor to improve the cohesiveness of the group, for example agreeing to the item, “I
enjoy making people laugh” (Martin et al. 2003, p. 58).
The aggressive humor style is a negative style of humor
where the direction is outward and involves belittling and
teasing others, for example agreeing to the item, “If someone makes a mistake, I will often tease them about it”
(Martin et al. 2003, p.58).
* Julie Aitken Schermer
[email protected]
Extended author information available on the last page of the article
Curr Psychol
The two self-oriented humor styles include the selfenhancing and the self-defeating humor styles. The selfenhancing humor style represents a positive self-oriented humor style. People who score highly on the self-enhancing
humor scale use humor to improve their mood even when
alone (can “cheer” themselves), for example, agreeing to the
item, “Even when I’m by myself, I’m often amused by the
absurdities of life” (Martin et al. 2003, p. 58). The selfdefeating humor style is a negative person-oriented style.
Individuals who engage in self-defeating humor use themselves as the target of ridicule and make fun of themselves.
Individuals may use self-defeating humor to try to ingratiate
themselves into a group or to gain acceptance, but the target of
the humor is the self and not a group, for example, agreeing to
the item, “Letting others laugh at me is my way of keeping my
friends and family in good spirits” (Martin et al. 2003, p. 59).
How people use humor styles can provide a greater understanding about individual differences. The four humor styles
have been found to correlate with various mental health variables. For example, positive humor styles correlate positively
with measures of social competence (Fitts et al. 2009), happiness (Ford et al. 2014; Yue et al. 2014), perceived social support, satisfaction with life (Dyck and Holtzman 2013), and
resiliency (Cann and Collette 2014). Negative correlations
are found between the positive humor styles and loneliness
(Schermer et al. 2017), shyness (Fitts et al. 2009), suicidal
ideation (Tucker et al. 2013b), depression (Dyck and
Holtzman 2013; Tucker et al. 2013a), anger-proneness
(Torres-Marín et al. 2018), neuroticism (Dyck and Holtzman
2013), and social anxiety (Tucker et al. 2013a). In contrast, the
negative humor styles correlate negatively with happiness
(Ford et al. 2014) and positively with thwarted belongingness
(Tucker et al. 2013b), depressive symptoms (Tucker et al.
2013b), and loneliness (Schermer et al. 2017). These results
suggest a pattern where negative humor styles are associated
with mental health problems whereas positive humor styles
are associated with better mental health. Many of these correlations with mental health may reflect the relationships also
found between humor styles and personality, especially neuroticism, as described below.
With respect to personality, the four humor styles have
been correlated with the Big Five factors. For example, in a
meta-analysis by Mendiburo-Seguel et al. (2015) combining
the results of 15 studies, the affiliative humor style correlated
positively with extraversion, agreeableness, and openness to
experience. The affiliative humor style had a negative correlation with neuroticism. The aggressive humor style scores
correlated positively with neuroticism and extraversion.
Negative correlations were found between the aggressive humor style and agreeableness, openness to experience, and conscientiousness. The self-enhancing humor style had positive
correlations with extraversion, openness to experience, agreeableness, and conscientiousness and a negative correlation
with neuroticism. Positive correlations were reported between
the self-defeating humor style and neuroticism and openness
to experience. The self-defeating humor style had a positive
correlation with neuroticism and negative correlations with
extraversion, agreeableness, and conscientiousness
(Mendiburo-Seguel et al. 2015).
Polimeni and Reiss (2006) have suggested that humor is an
essential component of human interaction with a strong evolutionary history. Across some behavior genetic studies using
adult twin samples, humor styles have been found to have a
genetic component with heritability estimates ranging from a
low of 5% for self-defeating humor in one sample to a high of
47% for both aggressive and self-defeating humor styles
(Baughman et al. 2012; Schermer et al. 2017; Vernon et al.
2008a, 2008b). Because humor styles have a genetic component and because humor styles correlate with personality and
mental health variables, humor styles provide a unique insight
into individual differences.
Cross-National Studies Using the Humor
Styles Model
Only a few cross-country studies have been conducted with
the humor style model proposed by Martin et al. (2003), and
of those available, only two countries are compared at one
time together. Kalliny et al. (2006) investigated differences
between American and Arab university students on the scale
scores for the humor styles. Although there were no differences between Arabs and Americans in affiliative and aggressive humor styles, Americans did score significantly higher
than the Arab sample on self-enhancing and self-defeating
styles. Kalliny et al. (2006) suggested that these differences
reflected findings that Americans are more individualistic in
nature (as they scored higher on the self-oriented humor
styles) but that there were no differences in the more group
oriented humor styles (aggressive and affiliative). Cruthirds
et al. (2012) examined American versus Mexican television
commercials and categorized the commercials based on the
four humor styles. Cruthirds et al. (2012) reported that
American commercials were twice as likely to use humor
compared to Mexican commercials and that American commercials were scored higher on affiliative, aggressive, and
self-defeating humor styles. In contrast, Mexican commercials
were scored higher on self-enhancing humor style. Chen and
Martin (2007) investigated the differences between Chinese
participants and the Canadian score norms of the Humor Style
Questionnaire (HSQ; Martin et al. 2003) and reported that
Chinese individuals scored lower on all four humor styles,
compared to the Canadians, and especially lower for the
aggressive humor style. Although mean comparisons were
not made on the humor styles, Wang et al. (2018) reported
that positive humor styles reduced the relationship between
Curr Psychol
stress measures over time for Australian employees but not for
Chinese employees. Also reported was that negative humor
styles did not mitigate the correlation between stress over time
for either the Chinese or the Australians (Wang et al. 2018).
The country comparison studies demonstrate that groups of
individuals’ humor styles may have similarities as well as
differences. Expanding upon the number of countries assessed
can add further information in the field of humor research. For
example, Proyer et al. (2009) examined gelotophobia (the fear
of being laughed at) across 73 countries. The scale properties
of the single gelotophobia scale was tested across the samples.
The authors reported that the nation accounted for more variance in item responses than did language. Heintz et al. (2018)
investigated corrective and benevolent humor across 22 countries. For all of the samples, individuals scored higher on benevolent humor than they did on corrective humor. National
differences were also reported. For example, samples of individuals from countries such as Malaysia used corrective humor more than Croatians and Latvians. In general, these large
comparisons of national samples provide an insight into the
similarities and differences across countries with respect to
certain dimensions of humor.
seven-point Likert type scale ranging from 1 (definitely
disagree) to 7 (definitely agree). As each scale consists of
eight items, scale totals can range from a low of 8 to a
maximum value of 56.
In order to conduct our study, we have involved a number
of researchers who not only supervised data collection in their
countries but also developed 16 new native language
(translated) versions of the HSQ (see Table 1). Scales which
were delivered in German (Ruch and Heintz 2016), Hungarian
(Boda-Ujlaky et al. 2017), Polish (Hornowska and Charytonik
2011), Russian (Ivanova et al. 2013), and Spanish (TorresMarín et al. 2018) had been previously translated and published. Each of the new translations were translated and then
back translated prior to administration. Nevertheless, these
new translated scales have not been independently assessed,
and the results in this paper are the first to report upon the scale
properties. Therefore, the results with these new measures
should be interpreted as preliminary (note that each of the
new and unpublished translations of the scales are available
from the associated authors upon request).
Statistical Analyses
Current Study
The aim of the current study was to explore how humor styles
relate to each other across multiple countries as there is empirical evidence on the individual differences for people in
using humor styles across nations, as described above.
Following, the scale properties for each country are examined.
In particular, the internal consistency (reliability), inter-scale
correlations, and confirmatory factor analyses were
established for the sample from each country. Also for descriptive purposes, the profile of the humor style scores for
each country sample is presented.
Participants and Procedure
The study was completed by N = 8361 participants (5238 females, 3107 males, 16 missing sex information) from 28
countries. Participants were mostly young adults, with an average age of 23.23 years (SD = 6.42) and ranging from 17 to
82 (the 17 year old individuals represent first year undergraduate students; see Table 1 for demographic information about
the samples).
Measure
Participants completed the Humor Styles Questionnaire
(HSQ; Martin et al. 2003). The HSQ is comprised of 32
items measuring different behaviors corresponding to the
four humor styles: affiliative, aggressive, self-enhancing,
and self-defeating. Respondents answer the items using
First, we report the descriptive statistics on the manifest
scores, i.e., means, reliability estimates, inter-item correlations, and Pearson’s correlations between the scales.
However, as manifest scores are burdened with measurement error, we further examined the HSQ in the framework of structural equation models (i.e., four correlated
factors model) in order to assess the influence of this error
(Kline 2013). In the assessment of the HSQ factorial
structure, we parcelled the eight items per factor into four
parcels per factor using the item-to-construct balance approach (Little et al. 2002). No correlations between residuals were entered. Given the fact that the CFI tends to be
biased in models with a high number of variables (Kenny
and McCoach 2003), to evaluate model fit, we relied on
the estimates of the Root Mean Square Error of
Approximation (RMSEA), which, according to Byrne
(1994), should be below .08. The computations were carried out in Mplus (v. 7.2. Muthén and Muthén 2012). The
data for this paper are available at: https://osf.io/yb4mj/?
view_only=adee09e66fc24145884c2e7a217270dd
Research Ethics Board approval was obtained from two
host universities before collecting data. Before completing
the questionnaires, each participant was acquainted with the
research procedure and informed about the subject of the
study. All rights of the participant were described in detail
and informed consent for participation in the study was obtained. The study was anonymous, and no sensitive or personal data were collected. The procedure lasted approximately
15–30 min.
Curr Psychol
Table 1 Demographic statistics
for the samples across the 28
nations
Country
Language
Data collection
procedure
N
females
N
males
Age M years
(SD)
Age
range
Bosnia &
Herzegovina
Brazil
Bosnian
Paper-pencil
297
203
20.91 (2.70)
18–43
Portugese
Online
209
95
28.76 (11.38)
18–71
Bulgaria
Canada
Chile
Colombia
Croatia
Estonia
Germany
Hungary
Indonesia
Iran
Japan
Latvia
Malaysia
Pakistan
Poland
Portugal
South Africa
Romania
Bulgarian
English
Spanish
Spanish
Croatian
Estonian
German
Hungarian
Indonesian
Farsi
Japanese
Lativian
English
English
Polish
Portuguese
English
Romanian
Paper-pencil
Online
Online
Online
Online
Paper-pencil
Online
Online
Online
Online/ Paper-pencil
Paper-pencil
Online
Paper-pencil
Paper-pencil
Online
Online
Online
Paper-pencil
128
109
164
142
185
153
258
243
147
172
65
142
94
289
167
375
217
100
131
119
69
114
64
215
75
43
147
156
132
61
106
63
78
94
148
100
19.94 (1.49)
24.30 (5.20)
20.97 (3.10)
21.06 (3.22)
21.35 (2.61)
24.28 (6.96)
26.83 (6.56)
30.11 (11.81)
21.28 (2.51)
28.79 (8.30)
19.64 (1.16)
26.65 (8.36)
21.72 (1.31)
21.20 (1.30)
23.75 (4.43)
22.82 (7.46)
20.71 (3.57)
20.06 (1.14)
17–33
18–55
17–49
18–48
18–34
18–49
17–57
18–73
18–36
17–62
18–24
18–66
20–25
19–26
18–40
17–78
17–45
18–27
Russia
Serbia
South Korea
Spain
Turkey
Ukraine
United States
Russian
Serbian
Korean
Spanish
Turkish
Ukrainian
English
Online
Online
Paper-pencil
Online
Paper-pencil
Online
Online
189
302
96
226
140
270
233
125
102
88
100
62
71
188
19.64 (1.64)
21.73 (4.86)
21.77 (2.13)
23.71 (5.84)
20.40 (2.24)
26.93 (9.82)
26.75 (3.26)
18–29
18–52
18–27
18–55
18–34
17–82
19–55
Vietnam
Vietnamese
Online
126
158
20.22 (1.66)
17–28
Results
Descriptive Statistics and Reliability Estimates
The descriptive statistics and reliability estimates (coefficient
alpha values) for the humor styles for each country are in
Table 2. For the affiliative humor style, the average coefficient
alpha value was .75 with a 95% confidence interval (CI) between .72 and .79. Although these values are acceptable, the
value for the sample from Pakistan was too low at .47. Upon
further investigation, the alpha would only increase to .50 if
the reverse-keyed item, “I rarely make other people laugh by
telling funny stories about myself” was removed. Because of
this low alpha value, the scale descriptives for Pakistan should
be interpreted with caution. The second-lowest alpha value
was .60 for the sample from Malaysia. This value was estimated to rise to .65 with the removal of the reverse-keyed
item, “I usually can’t think of witty things to say when I’m
with other people.” As with Pakistan, the results with
Malaysia should be interpreted with caution. The remaining
countries all had alpha values greater than a liberal alpha value
of .60 for the affiliative humor style scale. Although there is no
definitive “cut-off” for alpha values, a .60 has been characterized as moderate or satisfactory (Taber 2018).
Of the four humor styles, the aggressive humor style had
the lowest coefficient alpha values with a mean of only .59
(95% CI: .55 to .64). As reported in Table 2, 10 of the 28
countries had alpha values less than .60. The lowest alpha
was .32 for the sample from Pakistan. Further examination
suggests that the alpha value may increase to .33 with the
removal of the item, “Sometimes I think of something that is
so funny that I can’t stop myself from saying it, even if it is not
appropriate for the situation”. The alpha is estimated to increase to .34 with the removal of the item, “People are never
offended or hurt by my sense of humor” and increase to .36
with the removal of the item, “I never participate in laughing
at others even if all my friends are doing it”; both of these
items are negatively-keyed. The coefficient alpha for the
Curr Psychol
Table 2
Scale descriptives for the four humor style scales for each country sampled
Country
Affiliative M(SD|α|mean
inter-item correlation)
Aggressive M(SD|α|mean
inter-item correlation)
Self-enhancing M(SD|α|mean
inter-item correlation)
Self-defeating M(SD|α|mean
inter-item correlation)
Bosnia & Herzegovina
Brazil
Bulgaria
Canada
42.34(7.70|.63|.18)
44.47(8.58|.84|.41)
44.48(7.52|.73|.25)
45.82(6.74|.80|.34)
25.70(7.05|.45|.09)
24.17(7.36|.63|.17)
27.43(7.97|.62|.17)
28.78(8.02|.73|.25)
34.72(8.25|.64|.18)
34.11(9.90|.82|.37)
35.32(8.68|.71|.24)
33.92(9.52|.84|.40)
28.25(8.60|.67|.21)
26.75(9.69|.80|.34)
30.02(7.89|.64|.18)
28.12(8.62|.78|.31)
Chile
Colombia
Croatia
Estonia
Germany
Hungary
Indonesia
Iran
Japan
Latvia
Malaysia
Pakistan
Poland
Portugal
South Africa
Romania
Russia
Serbia
42.76(8.31|.82|.37)
41.06(8.30|.78|.31)
42.41(7.44|.70|.23)
44.26(7.13|.80|.35)
43.43(8.23|.85|.42)
42.70(9.15|.83|.40)
43.83(7.14|.83|.39)
40.80(9.28|.81|.36)
37.95(7.68|.80|.33)
34.41(6.27|.61|.16)
32.42(5.23|.60|.16)
34.76(7.20|.47|.10)
42.86(8.52|.85|.41)
43.57(7.68|.81|.35)
42.94(8.04|.74|.26)
43.62(7.13|.65|.20)
41.88(8.40|.80|.34)
46.06(6.59|.75|.29)
25.71(7.94|.71|.24)
24.01(7.51|.67|.21)
27.61(6.34|.42|.08)
29.08(7.56|.71|.24)
26.71(7.22|.66|.20)
24.74(8.09|.72|.25)
28.60(5.92|.51|.11)
22.56(6.99|.57|.16)
28.35(6.84|.67|.20)
28.79(5.84|.51|.11)
31.54(4.38|.41|.08)
28.45(6.55|.32|.05)
27.44(7.79|.72|.25)
24.32(6.73|.61|.17)
25.90(7.66|.58|.15)
28.42(6.63|.47|.10)
29.73(7.57|.63|.18)
24.57(7.42|.64|.19)
35.78(8.90|.81|.35)
35.77(9.22|.81|.35)
32.96(7.88|.69|.22)
35.48(8.13|.78|.31)
34.05(9.16|.83|.37)
36.14(9.88|.82|.37)
37.51(7.68|.79|.32)
31.29(10.70|.84|.39)
29.83(5.78|.52|.12)
33.79(7.09|.76|.29)
31.36(4.06|.33|.06)
34.81(7.50|.57|.15)
34.10(8.44|.80|.33)
33.33(9.65|.83|.37)
37.40(9.29|.76|.28)
35.33(8.09|.68|.21)
34.05(8.49|.74|.26)
36.85(8.86|.76|.26)
27.70(8.46|.75|.27)
23.94(8.21|.77|.29)
25.90(8.02|.73|.26)
28.04(7.76|.75|.28)
25.54(9.56|.85|.42)
25.79(9.18|.76|.29)
31.65(7.69|.76|.28)
24.32(7.60|.68|.22)
31.25(6.23|.63|.18)
29.88(7.57|.78|.31)
31.26(4.08|.36|.07)
30.05(8.09|.62|.17)
29.42(8.72|.79|.33)
23.66(9.46|.83|.38)
26.11(9.09|.72|.25)
25.04(7.47|.64|.18)
28.25(8.03|.70|.23)
27.60(9.17|.79|.32)
South Korea
Spain
Turkey
Ukraine
United States
Vietnam
40.95(6.79|.82|.37)
43.80(7.14|.78|.31)
42.08(7.57|.61|.18)
42.66(8.20|.81|.35)
41.35(8.99|.86|.44)
39.24(7.60|.71|.23)
25.31(6.57|.70|.22)
22.26(7.10|.66|.21)
26.77(7.08|.40|.08)
24.96(6.93|.63|.18)
27.84(7.68|.71|.23)
25.10(6.72|.54|.13)
32.09(6.54|.70|.22)
34.45(8.64|.79|.32)
32.27(10.58|.79|.32)
35.03(8.68|.79|.32)
35.64(9.20|.85|.42)
35.50(7.50|.64|.19)
28.29(7.32|.77|.30)
26.66(8.03|.74|.27)
24.74(8.26|.62|.18)
23.84(8.12|.76|.29)
27.37(9.05|.82|.37)
27.68(8.32|.71|.24)
sample from Turkey was .40 which is also quite low. The
alpha is estimated to increase after removing negativelykeyed items, specifically, removing “People are never
offended or hurt by my sense of humor” is estimated to increase alpha to .42 and removing “I never participate in
laughing at others even if all my friends are doing it” is estimated to increase alpha to .44. The sample from Malaysia also
had a low alpha value of .41. Removing the same negatively
keyed items as suggested for the sample from Turkey increases the alpha estimate to .45 and .46, respectively. In contrast to the negatively keyed item issue, the sample from
Croatia had an alpha of .42 which was estimated to increase
to .51 with the removal of the item, “Sometimes I think of
something that is so funny that I can’t stop myself from saying
it, even if it is not appropriate for the situation”, and increase to
.53 with the removal of the item, “When telling jokes or saying funny things, I am usually not very concerned about how
other people are taking it.” Both of these items are positively
keyed. Removing these two items for the sample from Bosnia
and Herzegovina also increased the coefficient alpha estimate
from .45 to .47 and .48, respectively. Removing the first item
increased the alpha estimate for the sample from Romania
from .47 to .48. These results suggest that the aggressive humor style scale does not perform well across many of the
countries and that the problem does not lie necessarily with
the negative versus positive keyed items. Results based on the
aggressive humor style scores for the 10 countries with low
reliability estimates should be interpreted with caution.
For the self-enhancing humor style scale, the mean coefficient alpha was .74 across the 28 country samples (95% CI of
.69 to .78). Three country samples had alpha values less than
.60: Malaysia at .33, Japan at .52, and Pakistan at .57. Upon
further examination, the alpha for the sample from Malaysia
was found to only increase to an estimated .35 with the deletion of the positively-keyed item, “Even when I’m by myself,
I’m often amused by the absurdities of life”. The alpha value
Curr Psychol
was not found to increase with the deletion of any items for
either the Japanese or Pakistan samples. Because of these low
alpha values, results with these countries should be interpreted
with caution.
The internal consistency value for the self-defeating humor
style scale averaged at .72 (95% CI .68 to .76). Only the
sample from Malaysia had an estimate lower than .60, with
a value of .36. Even with the deletion of the negatively keyed
item, “I don’t often say funny things to put myself down”, the
alpha estimate remained low at .49. Based on this result, the
results for Malaysia should be interpreted with caution.
In general, the mean values in Table 2 suggest that across
countries, the highest scores were for the affiliative humor
style (grand mean = 42.01, SD = 8.33), then the selfenhancing humor style (grand mean = 34.59, SD = 8.82),
followed by the self-defeating humor style (grand mean =
27.24, SD = 8.65), and the lowest for the aggressive humor
style (grand mean = 26.40, SD = 7.45). For descriptive purposes, Figs. 1, 2, 3, 4 provide the manifest mean humor style
scores for the 28 countries for the affiliative, aggressive, selfenhancing, and self-defeating humor style scores, respectively. For the affiliative humor style, the highest scores were
found for the samples from Canada and Serbia and the lowest
score came from the sample from Malaysia. The aggressive
humor style means were generally the lowest values for each
of the 28 samples. As reported above, the aggressive humor
style scale had the lowest internal consistency (coefficient
alpha values) for the humor style scores, therefore these results
should be viewed as descriptive only. The sample from
Malaysia had the highest mean aggressive humor style score
(but also an unacceptably low internal consistency value),
followed by Russia. Samples with low aggressive humor style
scores came from Iran and Spain.
Samples from the countries of Hungary, Indonesia, South
Africa, and Serbia had high self-enhancing humor style scores
and the sample from Japan scored the lowest. For the selfdefeating humor style, the highest means were from the
Fig. 1 Mean affiliative humor
style scores for each country
sample
samples from Indonesia, Japan, and Malaysia (although the
results from Malaysia need to be interpreted with caution due
to the low internal consistency value). The samples with the
lowest means for the self-defeating humor style scores were
from Colombia, Iran, Portugal, and Ukraine.
Sex Differences for each Country
The sex differences for each of the humor style scale scores for
each sample from each of the 28 countries are listed in Table 3.
In their report of the scale properties of the HSQ, Martin et al.
(2003) reported significant sex differences for each of the four
humor style scales and that men scored higher than women for
each comparison. Using the results from all of the participants
in the present study (see bottom of Table 3) as a comparison
basis, men and women did not differ significantly on the
affiliative humor style scores, but men were found to score
higher on the aggressive, the self-enhancing, and the selfdefeating humor style scales. In general, these results align
with the results reported by Martin et al. (2003) except for
the affiliative humor style scores. Women scored significantly
higher than men on the affiliative humor style in the samples
from Croatia and Latvia, but the reverse was the case for the
sample from Iran. For the majority of the country samples,
men scored higher than women on the aggressive humor style
scores. Interestingly, in the sample from Estonia, women
scored significantly higher on the aggressive humor style than
men, the only country to show this significant pattern.
Mean differences for the self-enhancing humor styles were
typically non-significant for each county sample with the exceptions of Canada and the United States where men scored
significantly higher than women. With respect to the selfdefeating humor style, men scored significantly higher than
women in the samples from Colombia, Iran, Latvia, Poland,
Portugal, South Africa, Spain, and Vietnam. In contrast, women in the Estonian and Romanian samples scored significantly
higher than men on the self-defeating humor style scores.
Curr Psychol
Fig. 2 Mean aggressive humor
style scores for each country
sample
Correlations with Age
As reported in Table 1, although all participants were adults,
the age ranges for each sample varied from a six year range for
Japan (18–24) to a 66 year range for the Ukrainian sample
(17–82). To investigate possible correlations between age and
the humor styles, age is included in the inter-scale correlations
for each country sample in Table 4. Using the pattern for all
participants at the bottom of Table 4 as a comparison, the
affiliative humor style had a non-significant correlation with
age, the aggressive humor style had a significant negative
correlation with age, the self-enhancing humor style had a
small but significant positive correlation with age, and the
self-defeating humor style had a significant negative correlation with age. This pattern of correlations was also found for
the sample from Hungary. The sample from Latvia too had the
same pattern of correlations between age and humor styles but
also had a significant positive correlation between age and the
affiliative humor style. In contrast, the samples from Canada,
Estonia, Iran, and Malaysia had significant negative correlations between age and the affiliative humor style. The
Fig. 3 Mean self-enhancing
humor style scores for each
country sample
Brazilian, Serbian, and Ukrainian samples had significant positive correlations between age and the self-enhancing humor
style scores.
In addition to the above, significant negative correlations
were between age and the aggressive humor style scores for
the samples from Brazil, Canada, Iran, Spain, and Ukraine.
Significant negative correlations were also found between the
self-defeating humor style and the samples from Germany,
Iran, Portugal, and Turkey. In general, the results suggest similar generational patterns of humor styles across the samples of
different countries. Only for the correlations with age and the
affiliative humor style scores, did the direction in correlation
change across samples.
Inter-Scale Correlations
The question of whether or not the humor style scores intercorrelated in a similar fashion across the samples from the
countries was examined. As reported by Martin et al. (2003),
the four humor style scores tend to have positive inter-scale
correlations. Using the inter-scale correlations for all
Curr Psychol
Fig. 4 Mean self-defeating
humor style scores for each
country sample
participants at the bottom of Table 4 as a reference, each of the
humor style scores were found to have significant positive
correlations with each other. Of note, the correlations between
the affiliative humor style and both the aggressive and selfdefeating humor styles and between the self-enhancing humor
style and the aggressive humor style were small in magnitude
and significant because of the large sample size. A similar
pattern of correlations is seen in each of the 28 samples with
only four exceptions. Significant negative correlations were
found between the affiliative and aggressive humor styles in
the samples from Croatia, Latvia, and Malaysia. In addition, in
the sample from Latvia, a significant negative correlation was
found between the affiliative and self-defeating humor styles.
Although of potential interest, it must be noted that, as reported in Table 2, the internal consistency values for the aggressive humor style scale for these countries, and the internal
consistency value for the self-defeating humor scale for
Malaysia, were unacceptably low. Therefore these exceptions
to the positive inter-scale correlations should be interpreted
with caution.
Assessment of confirmatory factor analysis
across countries
Overall, the analyzed measurement model tested on all samples simultaneously, did not fit the data well according to the
Confirmatory Fit Index (CFI), but did fit well according to the
R M S E A (χ 2 ( 9 8 ) = 3 2 77 .5 4 ; p < . 00 1 ; C F I = .8 8 2;
RMSEA = .062[.060, .064]). The results for each country are
provided in Table 5. The four-factor structure of the HSQ
reproduced moderately well, according to at least one fit statistic (RMSEA), in most countries, however the estimates of
CFI were mostly below acceptable thresholds. The poorest fit
to the data was found in the samples from Croatia, Iran,
Latvia, Malaysia, Pakistan, Romania, and Turkey, where the
values of CFI were below .800 and/or the values of RMSEA
were above .080. The results for these countries should be
interpreted with caution, as the evidence supporting the fourfactor model is limited.
Discussion
The countries assessed in this study showed some interesting
similarities and differences in scale responses to the four humor styles based on Martin et al.’s (2003) model. The means
(Table 2 and Figs. 1, 2, 3,4) showed variability across the 28
countries. One consistent pattern across all of the countries for
the four humor styles was that each country tended to have
higher affiliative humor style scores. These results are similar
to those reported by Heintz et al. (2018), who found that
benevolent (positive) humor was consistently higher than corrective (negative) humor across their samples from 22 countries. Upon further analysis of the humor style scales, low
internal consistency values were found for three of the four
scales for the samples from Malaysia and Pakistan and for 10
country samples, the aggressive humor style scale had low
internal consistency estimates. Malaysia and Pakistan have
been found to score high on a measure of inconsistent or
careless responding to personality measures (Grau et al.
2019). Possibly careless responding helped to contribute to
the lower internal consistency values for some of the samples.
In addition, as the original Humor Styles Questionnaire (HSQ;
Martin et al. 2003) was developed in Canada, the results based
on the 28 country samples do suggest caution when using the
HSQ in some Asian countries and specifically when measuring aggressive humor across countries.
Examining the scale scores for each country sample demonstrated a poor fit for a correlated four factor solution for
some of the groups, suggesting that for some of the samples,
the results should be interpreted with caution. With respect to
the inter-scale correlations within each sample, the four humor
style scales were typically found to correlate positively together, suggesting a similar inter-scale pattern across the samples.
Curr Psychol
Table 3 Sex differences in
homogeneity of variance
(Levene’s F-tests), means (ttests), and effect sizes (d) for each
humor style scale for each country
sample
Men M(SD)
Women M(SD)
Levene’s F
t
d
42.05 (7.48)
27.07 (6.55)
34.28 (7.72)
28.80 (8.27)
42.54 (7.85)
24.76 (7.25)
35.02 (8.59)
27.87 (8.81)
0.01
5.02
3.63
1.84
−0.71
3.63*
−0.98
1.19
−0.06
0.33
−0.09
0.11
44.16 (9.43)
27.42 (7.94)
33.61 (10.54)
28.00 (10.36)
44.61 (8.19)
22.70 (6.60)
34.33 (9.62)
26.18 (9.34)
0.92
4.30
2.26
0.50
−0.42
5.42*
−0.59
1.52
−0.05
0.67
−0.07
0.19
44.97 (7.73)
25.21 (7.76)
35.50 (9.40)
29.06 (7.90)
43.98 (7.29)
29.70 (7.57)
35.12 (7.91)
31.00 (7.78)
0.03
0.03
6.04
0.04
−0.86
−0.86
−0.02
−0.83
−0.11
−0.11
0.00
−0.10
46.14 (6.58)
31.00 (7.67)
35.55 (9.20)
28.34 (7.60)
45.46 (6.93)
26.37 (7.72)
32.12 (9.59)
27.88 (9.66)
1.28
0.03
0.47
7.42*
0.74
4.37*
2.65*
0.38
0.10
0.58
0.35
0.05
41.94 (7.51)
28.97 (7.73)
43.11 (8.62)
24.34 (7.64)
1.02
0.01
−0.98
4.21*
−0.14
0.60
36.91 (7.58)
29.84 (7.27)
35.30 (9.38)
26.80 (8.78)
3.47
4.95
1.26
2.53
0.18
0.36
42.50 (7.83)
26.54 (7.44)
37.08 (8.56)
26.02 (7.38)
39.90 (8.51)
21.98 (6.96)
34.72 (9.62)
22.27 (8.48)
0.55
0.03
1.18
1.13
2.52
5.06*
2.05
3.72*
0.32
0.64
0.26
0.47
36.48 (6.24)
30.66 (4.66)
30.95 (6.82)
27.75 (6.29)
44.46 (6.70)
26.56 (6.51)
33.66 (8.12)
25.26 (8.45)
1.94
19.87*
2.40
6.90*
−8.36*
5.43*
−2.39
2.48
−1.21
0.79
−0.35
0.36
43.84 (7.37)
27.42 (7.24)
34.60 (8.06)
44.88 (6.73)
31.43 (7.40)
36.77 (8.09)
1.47
0.11
0.98
−1.36
−5.14*
−2.50
−0.14
−0.54
Self-defeating
Germany
27.01 (7.50)
29.57 (7.76)
0.01
−3.14*
−0.26
−0.33
Affiliative
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Hungary
Affiliative
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
43.90 (7.78)
29.56 (6.66)
35.69 (7.87)
26.57 (8.94)
43.30 (8.38)
26.03 (7.12)
33.65 (9.44)
25.38 (9.69)
0.12
0.03
3.27
1.13
0.56
3.79*
1.66
0.94
0.07
0.50
0.22
0.12
44.23 (9.80)
26.79 (8.72)
35.07 (10.19)
42.42 (9.02)
24.38 (7.94)
36.33 (9.84)
0.34
0.09
0.15
0.56
0.76
0.70
0.09
0.13
0.12
Bosnia & Herzegovina
Affiliative
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Brazil
Affiliative
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Bulgaria
Affiliative
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Canada
Affiliative
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Chile
Affiliative
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Colombia
Affiliative
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Croatia
Affiliative
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Estonia
Affiliative
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Curr Psychol
Table 3 (continued)
Self-defeating
Indonesia
Affiliative
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Iran
Affiliative
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Japan
Affiliative
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
South Korea
Affiliative
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Latvia
Affiliative
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Malaysia
Affiliative
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Pakistan
Affiliative
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Poland
Affiliative
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Portugal
Affiliative
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Romania
Affiliative
Aggressive
Men M(SD)
Women M(SD)
Levene’s F
t
d
27.70 (9.44)
25.45 (9.10)
0.05
0.82
0.14
43.48 (7.30)
29.83 (6.02)
37.43 (7.76)
32.46 (7.36)
44.18 (6.98)
27.37 (5.57)
37.58 (7.63)
30.84 (7.94)
0.71
0.51
0.00
1.26
−0.84
3.64*
−0.17
1.81
−0.10
0.42
−0.02
0.21
42.66 (7.95)
24.85 (7.65)
32.35 (10.57)
25.63 (7.64)
39.09 (10.07)
20.47 (5.59)
30.34 (10.75)
23.14 (7.38)
6.73*
29.55*
0.06
0.81
3.56*
5.85*
1.70
3.00*
0.39
0.65
0.19
0.33
38.18 (7.54)
28.98 (7.14)
30.50 (5.91)
30.99 (5.89)
37.48 (8.00)
27.08 (6.04)
28.48 (5.27)
31.79 (6.91)
0.08
2.17
0.32
2.22
0.59
1.83
2.30
−0.83
0.09
0.28
0.35
−0.13
41.08 (7.18)
26.73 (6.89)
33.17 (6.76)
29.52 (6.71)
41.00 (6.48)
24.01 (6.09)
31.16 (6.04)
27.07 (7.79)
2.98
1.47
0.02
2.20
0.08
2.84*
2.12
2.28
0.01
0.42
0.31
0.34
32.97 (3.82)
31.28 (2.76)
32.41 (5.12)
31.75 (5.36)
35.03 (6.98)
27.72 (6.47)
34.39 (7.73)
29.07 (8.23)
26.69*
45.00*
6.57
18.23*
−2.71*
5.48*
−1.83
2.76*
−0.41
0.84
−0.28
0.42
31.70 (4.48)
31.69 (4.34)
31.58 (4.44)
31.87 (3.82)
33.23 (5.89)
31.37 (4.45)
31.09 (3.59)
30.57 (4.27)
4.65
0.02
4.73
0.04
−2.09
0.51
0.85
2.26
−0.30
0.07
0.12
0.32
35.13 (6.57)
30.94 (6.56)
34.36 (7.47)
34.68 (7.34)
27.91 (6.43)
34.91 (7.51)
2.19
0.56
0.09
0.44
3.37*
−0.53
0.06
0.47
−0.07
31.52 (7.12)
29.73 (8.26)
2.51
1.60
0.22
44.45 (6.94)
30.76 (7.90)
35.38 (8.46)
32.52 (8.37)
42.11 (9.08)
25.89 (7.26)
33.50 (8.38)
27.97 (8.53)
2.85
1.10
0.01
0.10
2.01
4.75*
1.64
3.92*
0.28
0.65
0.22
0.54
43.32 (8.17)
28.14 (6.65)
34.84 (8.32)
27.33 (8.26)
43.63 (7.56)
23.36 (6.41)
32.94 (9.92)
22.75 (9.54)
0.66
0.63
5.57
3.21
−0.35
6.41*
1.71
4.27*
−0.04
0.74
0.20
0.49
43.95 (7.16)
27.24 (6.55)
43.29 (7.12)
29.59 (6.52)
0.03
0.50
0.65
−2.54
0.09
−0.36
Curr Psychol
Table 3 (continued)
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
South Africa
Affiliative
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Russia
Affiliative
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Serbia
Affiliative
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Spain
Affiliative
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Turkey
Affiliative
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Ukraine
Affiliative
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
United States
Affiliative
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Vietnam
Affiliative
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
All Participants
Affiliative
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
*p < .01, two-tailed
Men M(SD)
Women M(SD)
Levene’s F
t
d
34.92 (8.03)
23.29 (6.99)
35.74 (8.17)
26.78 (7.55)
0.02
0.12
−0.72
−3.39*
−0.10
−0.48
43.49 (7.94)
27.80 (7.85)
37.14 (9.56)
27.72 (9.18)
42.56 (8.10)
24.61 (7.27)
37.58 (9.12)
25.01 (8.88)
0.00
0.19
0.27
0.14
1.08
3.99*
−0.44
2.81*
0.12
0.43
−0.05
0.30
42.66 (8.57)
31.66 (7.65)
35.13 (8.08)
28.18 (7.79)
41.36 (8.28)
28.45 (7.26)
33.33 (8.69)
28.30 (8.20)
0.19
0.03
1.70
0.35
1.34
3.74*
1.85
0.12
0.15
0.43
0.21
0.01
46.08 (7.29)
28.24 (7.88)
38.12 (9.00)
46.05 (6.35)
23.33 (6.84)
36.43 (8.79)
0.17
2.83
0.08
0.04
6.02*
1.67
0.00
0.69
0.19
29.19 (10.14)
27.06 (8.78)
3.81
2.03
0.23
43.79 (7.87)
25.68 (6.95)
36.20 (7.97)
29.05 (7.57)
43.80 (6.80)
20.74 (6.63)
33.68 (8.83)
25.60 (8.02)
2.83
0.01
2.34
0.31
−0.01
6.11*
2.44
3.64*
0.00
0.73
0.29
0.44
41.10 (6.98)
27.59 (7.60)
32.44 (9.98)
25.31 (6.90)
42.51 (7.80)
26.42 (6.85)
32.21 (10.86)
24.50 (8.78)
1.99
0.25
0.54
3.96
−1.20
1.07
0.14
0.61
−0.18
0.16
0.02
0.09
44.38 (8.57)
27.38 (6.54)
36.25 (9.11)
24.77 (8.98)
42.21 (8.06)
24.33 (6.91)
34.71 (8.55)
23.59 (7.87)
0.00
1.10
0.37
2.45
1.99
3.35*
1.33
1.09
0.27
0.45
0.18
0.15
41.71 (8.84)
29.49 (7.12)
41.06 (9.11)
26.52 (7.87)
0.83
0.02
0.74
4.02*
0.07
0.39
37.84 (8.21)
28.31 (8.11)
33.86 (9.57)
26.61 (9.69)
0.01
0.01
4.52*
1.92
0.44
0.19
38.23 (7.66)
26.82 (6.72)
35.41 (7.56)
29.26 (8.52)
40.52 (7.36)
22.93 (6.08)
35.62 (7.45)
25.70 (7.64)
0.17
1.83
0.02
2.67
−2.54
5.06*
0.23
3.67*
−0.30
0.60
0.03
0.44
41.97 (8.27)
28.23 (7.27)
34.97 (8.49)
28.78 (8.11)
42.03 (8.37)
25.32 (7.34)
34.37 (8.99)
26.51 (8.87)
0.35
7.69*
14.15*
43.38*
−0.29
17.55*
3.03*
10.31*
−0.01
0.40
0.07
0.23
Curr Psychol
Table 4 Inter-correlations between the humor styles and age for each
country sample
Table 4 (continued)
Affiliative Aggressive SelfSelfenhancing defeating
Affiliative Aggressive SelfSelfenhancing defeating
Bosnia & Herzegovina
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Age
Brazil
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Age
Bulgaria
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Age
Canada
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Age
Chile
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Age
Colombia
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Age
Croatia
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Age
Estonia
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Age
Germany
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Age
Hungary
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Age
Indonesia
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
−.07
.40*
.09
−.03
.03
.21*
.01
.12
.41*
.33*
−.06
.02
.26*
−.17*
.12
.38*
.22*
.02
.16
.30*
−.08
.19*
.41*
.02
−.19*
.27*
.32*
−.21*
.12
.43*
.30*
−.06
.17*
.32*
.03
.07
.41*
.26*
−.01
−.03
.28*
.01
−.18*
.43*
−.05
.03
−.07
.21*
.01
.22*
.41*
.16*
−.18*
.15*
.30*
−.12
.20*
.53*
.15*
−.12
.16*
.32*
−.08
.14
.51*
.31*
−.05
.08
.40*
−.28*
.21*
.32*
.37*
.16*
.42*
.33*
.09
.16*
.20*
.33*
.06
.25*
−.06
.41*
.08
.33*
.10
.37*
.03
.22*
.02
.16*
.01
.20*
.20*
.41*
.06
−.12
−.06
.04
.01
.09
.01
−.02
−.17*
−.21*
Age
Iran
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Age
Japan
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Age
South Korea
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Age
Latvia
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Age
Malaysia
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Age
Pakistan
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Age
Poland
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Age
Portugal
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Age
Romania
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Age
South Africa
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Age
Russia
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
−.02
−.02
−.09
−.12
.21*
.41*
.34*
−.15*
.06
.49*
−.32
.16*
.11
−.27*
.14
.32*
.31*
.04
.07
.13
.06
.32*
.08
.14
−.09
.37*
.01
.01
−.08
.24*
−.01
.15
.12
.04
−.26*
.14
−.40*
.30*
−.08
.48*
−.38*
.38*
.20*
−.30*
−.27*
−.02
−.15
−.19*
−.03
.11
.14
.33*
.09
.06
.02
.23*
.05
−.02
−.05
.31*
.02
.31*
.06
−.09
.12
.47*
.07
.07
−.02
.23*
.01
.10
.15
.06
.15*
.40*
.04
−.09
.05
.30*
−.07
−.02
.10
−.23*
.04
.23*
−.10
.09
.25*
.41*
−.02
.32*
.07
.05
.21*
.32*
.18*
−.02
−.01
.19*
.07
.02
−.01
−.01
.29*
.42*
.14
.18*
.27*
.28*
Curr Psychol
Table 4 (continued)
Affiliative Aggressive SelfSelfenhancing defeating
Age
Serbia
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Age
Spain
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Age
Turkey
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Age
Ukraine
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Age
United States
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Age
Vietnam
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Age
All Participants
Aggressive
Self-enhancing
Self-defeating
Age
−.02
.02
−.03
−.01
.14*
.41*
.25*
.03
.08
.22*
−.08
.34*
.15
−.03
−.02
.38*
.21*
−.02
.14*
.25*
−.18*
.40*
.14
−.01
.12
.32*
.14
−.05
.23*
.31*
−.03
.27*
.03
−.21*
.16*
.46*
.12
.01
.14*
.26*
−.18*
.09
.17*
−.11
.04
.57*
.02
.04
.06
.35*
−.02
.13*
.09
−.06
−.09
.28*
−.09
−.08
−.08
.40*
.12
.22*
−.01
.10
.05*
.39*
.09*
−.01
.07*
.32*
−.13*
.21*
.07*
−.12*
*p < .01, two-tailed
Sex differences for each humor style for each sample were
examined. Martin et al. (2003) reported that in their sample,
men scored higher than women for each of the four humor
style scales. In general, the present study results demonstrated
non-significant sex differences in the affiliative humor style
scores except for the samples from Croatia and Latvia where
women scored higher than men and in the sample from Iran
where men scored higher than women. Men scored higher
than women on the aggressive humor style scores except for
the sample from Estonia. Only the samples from Canada and
the United States had significant sex differences in the selfenhancing humor style scores with men scoring higher than
women. For the self-defeating humor style scores, the sex
difference direction depended on which country sample was
Table 5 Results of the confirmatory factor analysis of the HSQ
measurement model across 28 country samples
Country
χ2(98)
CFI
RMSEA
Bosnia
Brazil
Bulgaria
Canada
361.39
238.84
202.36
212.52
.784
.905
.868
.915
.073
.069
.064
.071
Chile
Colombia
Croatia
Estonia
Germany
Hungary
Indonesia
Iran
Japan
Korea
Latvia
Malaysia
Pakistan
Poland
Portugal
Romania
South Africa
Russia
215.81
226.88
266.47
255.82
275.63
225.77
197.27
434.03
210.54
227.03
307.18
206.26
290.62
258.78
368.82
222.90
245.00
314.76
.900
.885
.789
.897
.902
.912
.920
.786
.799
.810
.740
.663
.682
.876
.876
.756
.873
.826
.072
.072
.083
.064
.074
.068
.059
.102
.076
.084
.103
.074
.075
.082
.077
.080
.064
.084
Serbia
Spain
Turkey
Ukraine
US
Vietnam
271.77
240.18
229.53
315.01
386.56
268.75
.884
.891
.720
.849
.881
.775
.066
.067
.090
.080
.084
.078
examined. Men scored significantly higher than women in the
samples from Colombia, Iran, Latvia, Poland, Portugal, South
Africa, Spain, and Vietnam. In contrast, women in the
Estonian and Romanian samples scored significantly higher
than men on the self-defeating humor style scores, with the
remaining sample differences not reaching significance.
Also examined were the possible correlates with age and
the four humor style scores. For almost every sample, the
youngest participants were 17 or 18 years old. The upper limit
for the age range varied greatly from 24 years in the Japanese
sample to 82 years in the sample from the Ukraine. For the
affiliative humor style, age either had a non-significant or
negative correlation for each sample except for the sample
from Latvia which was a moderate positive correlation. In
contrast, the aggressive humor style scores were negatively
correlated with age and reached significance for the samples
from Brazil, Canada, Hungary, Iran, Latvia, Spain, and
Ukraine. Across the country samples, the self-enhancing
Curr Psychol
humor style scores were positively correlated with age and
reached significance for the samples from Brazil, Hungary,
Latvia, Serbia, and Ukraine. The self-defeating humor style
scores were negatively correlated with age and reached significance for the samples from Germany, Hungary, Iran, Latvia,
Portugal, and Turkey. In general, the pattern of correlations
between age and humor style scores suggested more generational similarities than differences across the nation samples.
Limitations
Although the data represent multiple national samples, the
various languages may limit the study as well as the two
different data collection procedures (paper versus online).
These factors may have resulted in some of the lower coefficient alpha values found (see Table 1). For example, for
the affiliative humor style, although most of the country
samples had alpha values around .75, suggesting that the
scale was fairly consistent, the values did range from .47
for Pakistan to .86 for the United States of America. In
addition, the aggressive humor style scale performed poorly in 10 of the 28 countries. Greater scale analysis and
possible translation improvements are required in the future, especially for those 16 scales which were translated
and created for this study. Nevertheless, the results may
point to country differences in the aggressive humor style.
For example, possibly the content of the aggressive humor
style scale was too confusing for some of the participants
from certain countries/languages to respond consistently or
the aggressive humor style may not be readily applicable in
the same way for individuals across countries due to different normative behaviours for people in those countries.
We also did not examine the influence of factors such as
social desirable responding. Possibly there are national differences in how individuals perceive the social attractiveness of each humor style and respond accordingly.
Following, future research may want to examine the influence of social desirability responding on the four humor
style scales.
Conclusions
In general, the results add descriptive knowledge about humor
styles across diverse samples across countries and 21 languages. Typically, humor style comparison studies have concentrated on comparing only two countries at one time. By
examining 28 diverse country samples, the results here demonstrate some fascinating similarities and dissimilarities, and
as has been found in a recent review of humor use and perception across countries (Jiang et al. 2019) and a report of
benevolent and corrective humor in 25 countries (Heintz
et al. 2019), the present study found more similarities than
differences across the countries in terms of ranks of humor
style scores, sex differences, correlations with age, and interscale correlations. As humor styles correlate with personality,
mental health dimensions (Heintz 2017; Kuiper and McHale
2009; Martin 2007; Schermer et al. 2015, 2017; Tucker et al.
2013b) as well as behaviour in organizations/work environments (Romero and Arendt 2011), it is of interest to note that
for the samples included in this study, there appeared to be
greater similarities, such as correlations with age, sex effects,
and inter-scale correlations, than there were differences.
Acknowledgements Henrietta Bolló, Eötvös Loránd University,
Hungary: ÚNKP-17-3 New National Excellence Program of the
Ministry of Human Capacities.
Jan Crusius, University of Cologne, Germany: University of Cologne
– Advanced PostDoc Grant.
Maria M. Kwiatkowska, Cardinal Stefan Wyszyński University in
Warsaw, Poland: The work of Maria M. Kwiatkowska was supported
by the Polish Ministry of Science and Higher Education under the
Diamond Grant program [grant number 0101/DIA/2017/46.
Ha Truong Khanh, Vietnam National University, Vietnam: grants
501.01-2016.02 from the Vietnam National Foundation for Science and
Technology Development (NAFOSTED).
Radosław Rogoza, National Science Center, Poland [grant number
2015/19/N/HS6/00685].
Compliance with Ethical Standards
Ethical Approval “All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the
institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964
Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical
standards.”
Conflict of Interest The authors do not have a conflict of interest.
References
Baughman, H. M., Giammarco, E. A., Veselka, L., Schermer, J. A.,
Martin, N. G., Lynskey, M., & Vernon, P. A. (2012). A behavioral
genetic study of humor styles in an Australian sample. Twin
Research and Human Genetics, 15(5), 663–667. https://doi.org/10.
1017/thg.2012.23.
Boda-Ujlaky, J., Séra, L., Köteles, F., & Szabo, A. (2017). Validation of
the Hungarian version of the humor styles questionnaire (HSQ-H).
Mentálhigiéné és Pszichoszomatika, 18, 301–319. https://doi.org/
10.1556/0406.18.2017.013.
Byrne, B. M. (1994). Structural equation modeling with EQS and EQS/
windows. Thousand Oaks: Sage.
Cann, A., & Collette, C. (2014). Sense of humor, stable affect, and psychological well-being. Europe's Journal of Psychology, 10(3), 464–
479. https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v10i3.746.
Chen, G., & Martin, R. A. (2007). A comparison of humor styles, coping
humor, and mental, health between Chinese and Canadian university
students. Humor, 20(3), 215–234. https://doi.org/10.1515/HUMOR.
2007.011.
Cruthirds, K. W., Wang, V. L., Wang, Y. J., & Wei, J. (2012). A comparison of humor styles in US and Mexican television commercials.
Marketing Intelligence & Planning, 30(4), 384–401. https://doi.org/
10.1108/02634501211231856.
Curr Psychol
Dyck, K. T. H., & Holtzman, S. (2013). Understanding humor styles and
well-being: The importance of social relationships and gender.
Personality and Individual Differences, 55, 53–58. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.paid.2013.01.023.
Fitts, S. D., Sebby, R. A., & Zlokovich, M. S. (2009). Humor styles as
mediators of the shyness-loneliness relationship. North American
Journal of Psychology, 11(2), 257–272.
Ford, T. E., McCreight, K. A., & Richardson, K. (2014). Affective style,
humor styles and happiness. Europe's Journal of Psychology, 10(3),
451–463. https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v10i3.766.
Grau, I., Ebbeler, C., & Banse, R. (2019). Cultural differences in careless
responding. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 50(3), 336–357.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022119827379.
Heintz, S. (2017). Putting a spotlight on daily humor behaviors:
Dimensionality and relationships with personality, subjective wellbeing, and humor styles. Personality and Individual Differences,
104, 407–412. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.08.042.
Heintz, S., Ruch, W., Platt, T., Pang, D., Carretero-Dios, H., Dionigi, A.,
et al. (2018). Psychometric comparisons of benevolent and corrective humor across 22 countries: The virtue gap in humor goes international. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 92. https://doi.org/10.3389/
fpsyg.2018.00092.
Heintz, S., Ruch, W., Aykan, S., Brdar, I., Brzozowska, D., CarreteroDios, H., Chen, H. C., Chłopicki, W., Choi, I., Dionigi, A., Ďurka,
R., Ford, T. E., Güsewell, A., Isler, R. B., Ivanova, A., Laineste, L.,
Lajčiaková, P., Lau, C., Lee, M., Măda, S., Martin-Krumm, C.,
Mendiburo-Seguel, A., Migiwa, I., Mustafi, N., Oshio, A., Platt,
T., Proyer, R. T., Quiroga-Garza, A., Ramis, T. S., Săftoiu, R.,
Saklofske, D. H., Shcherbakova, O. V., Slezackova, A., Stalikas,
A., Stokenberga, I., Torres-Marín, J., & Wong, P. S. O. (2019).
Benevolent and corrective humor, life satisfaction, and broad humor
dimensions: Extending the nomological network of the BenCor
across 25 countries. Journal of Happiness Studies, 1–20. https://
doi.org/10.1007/s10902-019-00185-9.
Hornowska, E., & Charytonik, J. (2011). Polska adaptacja kwestionariusa
stylów humoru (HSQ) R. Martina, P. Puhlik-Doris, G. Larsena, J.
Gray I K.Weir. Studia Psychologiczne, 49(4), 5–22. https://doi.org/
10.2478/v10167-010-0032x.
Ivanova, E. M., Mitina, O. V., Zaitseva, A., Stefanenko, E. A., &
Yenekov, S. N. (2013). Russian adaptation of the Martin humor style
questionnaire. Theoretical and Experimental Psychology, 6(2), 71–
85 http://tepjournal.ru/images/pdf/2013/2/07.pdf.
Jiang, T., Li, H., & Hou, Y. (2019). Cultural differences in humor perception, usage, and implications. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 123.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00123.
Kalliny, M., Cruthirds, K. W., & Minor, M. S. (2006). Differences between American, Egyptian and Lebanese humor styles.
International Journal of Cross Cultural Management, 6(1), 121–
134. https://doi.org/10.1177/1470595806062354.
Kenny, D. A., & McCoach, D. B. (2003). Effect of the number of variables on measures of fit in structural equation modeling. Structural
Equation Modeling, 10, 333–351. https://doi.org/10.1207/
S15328007SEM1003_1.
Kline, R. B. (2013). Principles and practice of structural equation
modeling. New York: The Guilford Press.
Kuiper, N. A., & McHale, N. (2009). Humor styles as mediators between
self-evaluative standards and psychological well-being. The Journal
of Psychology, 143(4), 359–376. https://doi.org/10.3200/JRLP.143.
4.359-376.
Little, T. D., Cunningham, W. A., Shahar, G., & Widaman, K. F. (2002).
To parcel or not to parcel: Exploring the question, weighing the
merits. Structural Equation Modeling, 9, 151–173. https://doi.org/
10.1207/S15328007SEM0902_1.
Martin, R. A. (2007). The psychology of humor: An integrative approach.
Burlington: Elsevier.
Martin, R. A., Puhlik-Doris, P., Larsen, G., Gray, J., & Weir, K. (2003).
Individual differences in uses of humor and their relation to psychological well-being: Development of the Humor Styles
Questionnaire. Journal of Research in Personality, 37(1), 48–75.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00534-2.
Mendiburo-Seguel, A., Páez, D., & Martínez-Sánchez, F. (2015). Humor
styles and personality: A meta-analysis of the relation between humor styles and the Big Five personality traits. Scandinavian Journal
of Psychology, 56, 355–340. https://doi.org/10.1111/sjop.12209.
Muthén, L., & Muthén, B. (2012). Mplus user's guide (7th edition). Los
Angeles: Muthén & Muthén.
Polimeni, J., & Reiss, J. P. (2006). The first joke: Exploring the evolutionary origins of humor. Evolutionary Psychology, 4(1), 347–366.
https://doi.org/10.1177/147470490600400129.
Proyer, R. T., Ruch, W., Ali, N. S., Al-Olimat, H. S., Amemiya, T., Adal,
T. A., et al. (2009). Breaking ground in cross-cultural research on the
fear of being laughed at (gelotophobia): A multi-national study involving 73 countries. Humor, 22(1/2), 253–279. https://doi.org/10.
1515/HUMR.2009.012.
Romero, E. J., & Arendt, L. A. (2011). Variable effects of humor styles on
organizational outcomes. Psychological Reports, 108(2), 649–659.
https://doi.org/10.2466/07.17.20.21.PRO.108.2.649-659.
Ruch, W., & Heintz, S. (2016). The German version of the humor styles
questionnaire: Psychometric properties and overlap with other styles
of humor. Europe's Journal of Psychology, 12, 434–455. https://doi.
org/10.5964/ejop.v12i3.1116.
Schermer, J. A., Martin, R. A., Martin, N. G., Lynskey, M. T., Trull, T. J.,
& Vernon, P. A. (2015). Humor styles and borderline personality.
Personality and Individual Differences, 87, 158–161. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.07.043.
Schermer, J. A., Martin, R. A., Vernon, P. A., Martin, N. G., Conde, L. C.,
Statham, D., & Lynskey, M. T. (2017). Lonely people tend to make
fun of themselves: A behavior genetic analysis of humor styles and
loneliness. Personality and Individual Differences, 117, 71–73.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.05.042.
Taber, K. S. (2018). The use of Cronbach’s alpha when developing and
reporting research instruments in science education. Research in
Science Education, 48, 1273–1296. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s11165-016-9602-2.
Torres-Marín, J., Navarro-Carrillo, G., & Carretero-Dios, H. (2018). Is
the use of humor associated with anger management? The assessment of individual differences in humor styles in Spain. Personality
and Individual Differences, 120, 193–201. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
paid.2017.08.040.
Tucker, R. P., Judah, M. R., O'Keefe, V. M., Mills, A. C., Lechner, W. V.,
Davidson, C. L., & Wingate, L. R. (2013a). Humor styles impact the
relationship between symptoms of social anxiety and depression.
Personality and Individual Differences, 55(7), 823–827. https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.07.008.
Tucker, R. P., Wingate, L. R., O’Keefe, V. M., Slish, M. L., Judah, M. R.,
& Rhoades-Kerswill, S. (2013b). The moderating effect of humor
style on the relationship between interpersonal predictors of suicide
and suicidal ideation. Personality and Individual Differences, 54,
610–615.
Vernon, P. A., Martin, R. A., Schermer, J. A., Cherkas, L. F., & Spector, T.
D. (2008a). Genetic and environmental contributions to humor
styles: A replication study. Twin Research and Human Genetics,
11(1), 44–47. https://doi.org/10.1375/twin.11.1.44.
Vernon, P. A., Martin, R. A., Schermer, J. A., & Mackie, A. (2008b). A
behavioral genetic investigation of humor styles and their correlations with the big five personality dimensions. Personality and
Individual Differences, 44(5), 1116–1125. https://doi.org/10.1016/
j.paid.2007.11.003.
Wang, R., Chan, D. K. S., Goh, Y. W., Penfold, M., Harper, T., &
Weltewitz, T. (2018). Humor and workplace stress: A longitudinal
comparison between Australian and Chinese employees. Asia
Curr Psychol
Pacific Journal of Human Resources, 56, 175–195. https://doi.org/
10.1111/1744-7941.12157.
Yue, X. D., Liu, K. W., Jiang, F., & Hiranandani, N. A. (2014). Humor
styles, self-esteem, and subjective happiness. Psychological
Reports, 115(2), 517–525. https://doi.org/10.2466/07.02.PR0.
115c18z6.
Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Affiliations
Julie Aitken Schermer 1 & Radosław Rogoza 2 & Maria Magdalena Kwiatkowska 2 & Christopher Marcin Kowalski 1 &
Sibele Aquino 3 & Rahkman Ardi 4 & Henrietta Bolló 5 & Marija Branković 6 & Razieh Chegeni 7 & Jan Crusius 8 &
Marta Doroszuk 9 & Violeta Enea 10 & Thi Khanh Ha Truong 11 & Dzintra Iliško 12 & Tomislav Jukić 13 & Emira Kozarević 14 &
Gert Kruger 15 & Adil Kurtić 14 & Jens Lange 16 & Kadi Liik 17 & Sadia Malik 18 & Samuel Lins 19 & Agim Mamuti 20 &
Laura Martinez-Buelvas 21 & Benjamin Mrkušić 22 & Ginés Navarro-Carrillo 23 & Oscar Oviedo-Trespalacios 24,25 &
Emrah Özsoy 26 & Eva Papazova 27 & Joonha Park 28 & Natalia Pylat 29 & Goran Riđić 30 & Ognjen Riđić 31 & Dženan Skelić 32 &
Chee-Seng Tan 33 & Jorge Torres-Marín 34 & Osman Uslu 26 & Tatiana Volkodav 35 & Anna Włodarczyk 36 & Georg Krammer 37
1
Department of Psychology, Faculty of Social Science, University of
Western Ontario, London, ON N6A 5C2, Canada
20
University Mother Theresa, Skopje, North Macedonia
21
Universidad Tecnológica de Bolívar, Cartagena, Colombia
2
Cardinal Stefan Wyszyński University in Warsaw, Warszawa, Poland
22
3
Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Rio de Janeiro, Rio de
Janeiro, Brazil
International University of Sarajevo, Sarajevo, Bosnia and
Herzegovina
23
Universidad Loyola Andalucía, Sevilla, Spain
4
Airlangga University, Surabaya, Indonesia
24
Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane City, Australia
5
Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Budapest, Hungary
25
Universidad del Norte, Barranquilla, Colombia
6
Singidunum University, Beograd, Serbia
26
Sakarya University, Serdivan, Turkey
7
University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway
27
IPHS-Bulgarian Academy of Sciences, Sofia, Bulgaria
8
University of Cologne, Köln, Germany
28
NUCB Business School, Aichi, Japan
9
Jagiellonian University, Kraków, Poland
29
Ukrainian Catholic University, Lviv, Ukraine
10
Alexandru Ioan Cuza University, Iași, Romania
30
University of Applied Management Studies, Mannheim, Germany
11
Vietnam National University, Hanoi, Vietnam
31
12
Daugavpils University, Daugavpils, Latvia
International University of Sarajevo, Sarajevo, Bosnia and
Herzegovina
13
University Josip Juraj Strossmayer, Osijek, Republic of Croatia
32
University of Zenica, Zenica, Bosnia and Herzegovina
University of Tuzla, Tuzla, Bosnia and Herzegovina
33
Universiti Tunku Abdul Rahman, Perak Campus, Malaysia
University of Johannesburg, Johannesburg, Republic of South
Africa
34
University of Granada, Granada, Spain
35
Kuban State University, Krasnodar, Russia
14
15
16
University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
36
Universidad Católica del Norte, Antofagasta, Chile
17
Tallinn University, Tallinn, Estonia
37
University College of Teacher Education Styria, Graz, Austria
18
University of Sargodha, Sargodha, Pakistan
19
University of Porto, Porto, Portugal
View publication stats