RobeRt G. bednaRik - a shoRt ethnoGRaphy of cupules
Bednarik, R. G. 2010. A short ethnography of cupules. In R. Querejazu Lewis and R. G. Bednarik
(eds), Mysterious cup marks: proceedings of the First International Cupule Conference, pp.
109-114. BAR International Series 2073, Archaeopress, Oxford.
A SHORT ETHNOGRAPHY OF CUPULES
Robert G. bednarik
Abstract. Globally we have quite limited ethnographic information about petroglyphs, about
their emic meaning or purpose. such information is even more limited for cupules, amounting
to just a few fragmentary and very isolated accounts. the currently known examples are cited,
their scientific veracity is discussed, and it is demonstrated through them that the endeavours of
archaeologists to determine the meanings or significance of cupules are generally misguided.
such etic interpretation is impossible, and where it is attempted it is scientifically irrelevant,
except for the cognitive scientist in studying the cognition of the interpreter.
Keywords: cupule, ethnography, interpretation, central australia, california, india, africa, nepal
Most of the thousands of publications about cupules
comprise some commentary on their meaning or cultural
role — their significance to those who made or used them.
almost invariably, these speculations lack any scientific
veracity; they are simply notions of the observers who
completely lack any emic access to meaning, and in
most cases have no idea of the ages of the rock art, or its
provenience or cultural affiliation. here we find a mother
lode of archaeological humbug still awaiting detailed
mining. let us be quite blunt on this point: archaeology
has not presented a scientifically based, or even plausible,
explanation or interpretation of the rather unusual
behaviour pattern manifested in cupules. that behaviour
is documented in most of the world’s rock art regions, and
in some of them over enormous time spans. cupules were
still made in the early 20th century, in a very few places,
and yet the ethnographically sound information collected
about them is extremely sparse.
four of these explanations could at best only account
for horizontal cupules and can therefore be excluded
for all others, or at least vertical ones. Moreover, they
are proposed without the facility of falsification, i.e. no
evidence for them is presented, they are simply guesses. the
explanation as patterns of heavenly bodies is particularly
popular in china and parts of europe, and is also offered
without any tangible evidence. star constellations, we can
reasonably assume, are entirely random features, and it
is then not surprising that they resemble other random or
fortuitous arrangements. large groupings of cupules tend
to be cumulative, i.e. the marks constituting them were
made singly and at greatly different times. that renders this
explanation highly unlikely, and in all cases i am aware of,
the resemblance with star constellations is only vague. for
the vast majority of cupule constellations, no corresponding
star charts have been proposed, and this notion appears to
be without empirical basis as well as being unfalsifiable. it
therefore is not a scientific proposition.
the most commonly mentioned archaeological interpretations of cupules could be grouped into a number of
classes, based on these purported uses (but see more detailed
discussion in the chapter on interpretation, this volume):
the explanation of random cupule groups as maps,
popular in the alpine regions of europe, falls into the same
category. it is untestable, has no ethnographic support, and
is a priori unlikely unless all cupules were made at the
same time. it is also reminiscent of other endeavours in
seeking rock art explanations, in which various patterns
are thought to be pre-historic maps, apparently without
justification. for instance, it has been proposed that the
spatial configurations of french caves resemble the local
topography surrounding those caves, and the pleistocene
animals depicted refer to their former distribution in the
vicinity of the site. such attempts to interpret the sites and
their rock art lack any empirical basis.
1. the preparation of paints.
2. unspecified or specified cultic or magic rituals.
3. the pounding of medicines (mineral or plant), pigments
or spices.
4. the placement of offerings (‘Opferschalen’), including
human blood and semen.
5. the depiction of star constellations.
6. the map-like depiction of topographic elements of
nearby landscapes.
7. board games.
8. a symbolism that is no longer recoverable.
i am only aware of a few sound ethnographic explanations
109
MysteRious cup MaRks: pRoceedinGs of the fiRst inteRnational cupule confeRence
Figure 2. The cupules of the Tunalili increase site
(photograph by C. P. Mountford).
Figure 1. The Tukalili increase site near Nantaguna
springs, Northern Territory, Australia. The precise
emic significance of the cupules has been recorded
(1940 photograph by Charles P. Mountford).
animal or natural force the site is associated with, through
its release by an appropriate ceremony. it then increases the
supply or strength of that entity, which can range from a
plague of head lice to bring down on one’s enemies to the
supply of an edible tree gum. it has also been suggested
(taçon et al. 1997: 947) that some cupule sites near the
Mann River in eastern arnhem land are related to Green
plum dreaming ceremonies but there is no evidence that
this was their original use.
of cupules in the world literature, of which one or two are
probably ‘derived’ interpretations, and another two are of
little help in formulating anything approaching a generic
explanation. only Mountford’s (1976) observation of
1940 and perhaps a few american examples meet the
strict requirements of a scientific interpretation, and it is
limited to a very small number of cupule locations. the
first case concerns the story of the death of tukalili, the
cockatoo-woman, a creation myth collected in the northern
territory of australia (figures 1 and 2). her totemic body,
a large boulder near nantaguna springs, bears in a recess
around sixteen horizontal cupules. they are the result
of pulkarin rituals conducted to cause the pink cockatoo
(Cacatua leadbeateri) to lay more eggs (figure 3). this
is accomplished through the mineral powder rising into
the air as the cupules are pounded. the dust represents the
kuranita of the rock and, as it is thus released, it fertilises
the female cockatoos. Kuranita (life essence) can rise like
a mist into the air from any ‘increase
site’, impregnating a specific plant,
Figure 3. The pink cockatoo
(cacatua leadbeateri).
in Mountford’s example the cupules are clearly not the
intended result of the exercise; the fertilising dust is the
crucial element (figure 4). the cupules are an incidental
but the only surviving consequence of the ritual activity
in question, and what we need to be most aware of is that
this authentic interpretation of cupules could never be
determined archaeologically. this example is not just one
of the very few scientific explanations of any cupules in the
world, it also shows the general impotence of archaeology
in explaining archaeological phenomena. Without the
Figure 4. Stone powder from increase site being used for body decoration.
110
RobeRt G. bednaRik - a shoRt ethnoGRaphy of cupules
Figure 5. Renewal/reuse of a cupule on a lithophone or rock gong, Pola Bhata, Madhya Pradesh, India, in 2004.
recorded ethnographic observation, an archaeologist could
never expect to formulate the authentic explanation. all
correct interpretations of the residue that archaeologists
chose to call archaeological remains are just as remote
and unfathomable (i.e. emic) as is the interpretation of the
cupules at tukalili’s site.
certainly an interesting observation, but it can easily elicit
unwarranted extrapolation to other sites. other ethnographic
indications in the western united states provide very
different explanations. the klamath of southern oregon
are said to have renewed cupules in order to summon the
wind to change the weather (spier 1930: 21). similarly, the
shasta of california sought to influence the weather: they
incised straight parallel grooves into selected ‘rain rocks’ to
increase or decrease snowfall, and they pounded cupules to
induce rainfall and wind (heizer 1953). this also brings to
mind the northern australian custom of cutting sub-parallel
grooves into bedrock to ‘make old Man Rain bleed’ (arndt
1962: 171). again, it is evident how similar cultural practices
can be developed independently, without any contact.
parkman (1992: 367) speculates that the percussion sound
of pounding cupules could have been intended to ‘attract or
replace thunder’. he notes, in support of this contention, that
‘among the kashaya pomo, women grinding acorns in their
mortars took special precautions to prevent unwanted rain’.
apparently they prepared shelters to muffle the sound, so as
not to summon rain unintentionally (alvarez and peri 1987:
12). similarly, the shasta covered their rain rocks in order
to prevent rain (heizer 1953). parkman (1988) offers one
further explanation for cupules, in describing rock slabs at
takimitlding and Medilding, california, as hupa ‘calendar
stones’. it appears from his description that contemporary
hupa believe the stones to have had some astronomical
role, but the consultants were unable to explain the actual
function of these features and the interpretation, like others
listed here, cannot be regarded as secure.
a second ethnographic explanation of cupules on a limited
number of specific rocks comes from california and was
recorded early in the 20th century by barrett (1908: 164–
165, 1952: 385–387; see also loeb 1926: 247; Gifford and
kroeber 1937: 186; heizer 1953; Grant 1967: 106; hedges
1983a, 1983b). specific boulders bearing collections of
cupules were visited by pomo women to conduct fertility
ceremonies. these rituals, intended to lead to conception,
involved the collection of the ‘fertilising’ dust created in
pounding the cupules. the rock is either steatite or chlorite
schist, the powder was made into a paste which was usually
applied to the woman’s skin, or, in one case recorded, was
inserted into her vagina to achieve pregnancy through the
rock’s magical essence. however, the cupules at these
sites tend to be outnumbered by incised grooves, and
hedges (1983b) rightly emphasises that the ethnographic
explanation of the pomo ‘baby rocks’, as they are called,
should not be extended to other cupule sites (McGowan
1982). nevertheless, one cupule site used in fertility rituals
has also been reported from new Mexico, Mother Rock
on to’wa yäl’länne (corn Mountain) near Zuni pueblo
(stevenson 1887: 539–540; also fewkes 1891: 9–10). there,
the pregnant woman would collect the mineral powder ‘into
a tiny vase made for the purpose’ and deposit it in a wall
cavity, if she desired a daughter (stevenson 1904: 295).
another correct ethnographic interpretation of cupules
i can offer is illustrated in figure 5. here, a properly
knowledgeable person demonstrates the use of a cupule,
one of several dozen at the site that were still being renewed
the parallel development of the concept of a fertilising effect
of the mineral powder resulting from pounding cupules is
111
MysteRious cup MaRks: pRoceedinGs of the fiRst inteRnational cupule confeRence
used to render a barren woman
fertile. lombry also mentions the
preparation of war paints (nbuka
vura) in congolese cupules.
Figure 6. Kebaroti site 1, southern Kenya. Photograph by O. Odak.
a further, but different ethnographic
interpretation concerns the
kebaroti site complex and the
lanet site in southern kenya. here,
odak (1992) reported a number of
cupule pavements which the local
kuria people have interpreted
to him as boa game boards. it
appears, however, that the cupules
predate these people and that their
interpretation is not that of the
makers, but is one imposed on preexisting rock art (figure 6).
the notion of the use of some
cupules in board games is, however, promising elsewhere.
in nne congo, lombry (2008) observed the use of cupule
arrangements in a pursuit game called mangara as recently
as in the 1950s. in relation to geometrically arranged cupules
in nepal, pohle (2000: 199–202) discusses the possibility
that they were used in uluk and rama rildok games, and
she accepts that many of the cupule arrangements relate to
the latter game (pohle 2000: tafeln 1.1, 14–16, 18.1, 28.2).
Rama rildok is a mancala game, to which bandini-könig
(1999) also attributes cupules at hodar, in the uppermost
indus valley, and fu (1989: 179) mentions the same for
chinese sites. cupules supposedly or actually used in
board games form geometric alignments or groupings (cf.
lombry 2008: fig. 4) and occur on horizontal rock panels.
the ethnographic foundation of interpretations as elements
of board games requires further investigation, but it appears
to have been demonstrated in some cases.
recently. the elongate quartzite rock he squats on is a
lithophone, the use and purpose of which were explained
and demonstrated to me. in this instance, the cupule is again
incidental, and — as was the case in the previous examples
— its relative position to other cupules is irrelevant; it does
not represent astronomical observations or whatever else
ethnocentric observers like to invent. another ethnographic
interpretation of cupules as marking lithophones is reported
from burkina faso (formerly upper Volta, western africa;
trost 1993: 94). on the other hand, there is anecdotic
information suggesting that along the Ganges, especially
in punjab, indian women desiring to become pregnant pour
sacred water into cupules, once again linking the rock art to
fertility. a similar observation has been made by lombry
(2008) who reports from the congo the use of cupules to
mix red paint made from haematite powder derived from a
mangwa gumba (a polished haematite axe), which was then
Figure 7. Cupules at Moda Bhata, Rajasthan, India, examined by
microerosion analysis in 2002.
112
there is scientific evidence from numerous sites
that cupules were re-used after they were first
created (steinbring and lanteigne 1991; huber
1995), sometimes many millennia later. for
instance, one specific cupule at Moda bhata,
india (figure 7), which was initially pounded
about 9000 years ago was briefly re-worked
about 7200 years later (bednarik et al. 2005:
182). Many similar examples are known, and
it is clear that pre-existing cupules were often
incorporated in the beliefs or practices of later
people. this raises yet another warning, namely
that it would be premature to equate the perceived
‘age’ of a cupule with its full antiquity: many
cupules were no doubt initially created long
before their most recent retouch event, and if
the latter is extensive enough, no traces of earlier
surfaces are likely to remain within the cupule.
it is therefore then wrong to refer to the age of
such marks, it is better to think of it in terms of
its most recent use evidence, and in terms of this
RobeRt G. bednaRik - a shoRt ethnoGRaphy of cupules
being a minimum age. Many cupules, especially the oldest
known in the world, occur on particularly erosion-resistant
rock types, such as quartzite, gneissic granite and even
crystalline quartz (bednarik et al. 2005).
painting and rock engravings. Oceania 32: 163–177.
bandini-köniG, d. 1999. Die Felsbildstation Hodar. Materialien
zur archäologie der nordgebiete pakistans 3, Mainz.
baRRett, s. a. 1908. the ethnogeography of the pomo and
neighboring indians. University of California Publications in
American Archaeology and Ethnology 6(1): 1–332.
baRRett, s. a. 1952. Material aspects of Pomo culture. bulletin
of the public Museum of the city of Milwaukee 20 (parts 1
and 2), aMs press, new york.
bednaRik, R. G., G. kuMaR, a. WatchMan and R. G. RobeRts
2005. preliminary results of the eip project. Rock Art Research
22(2): 147–197.
feWkes, J. W. (ed.) 1891. A journal of American ethnology and
archaeology. houghton, Mifflin, new york.
fu, c. Z. 1989. The rock arts of China (in chinese). the Jiang
photographic art press, beijing.
G iffoRd , e. W. and a. l. k RoebeR 1937. culture element
distributions: iV. pomo. University of California Publications
in American Archaeology and Ethnology 37: 117–254.
GRant, c. 1967. Rock art of the American Indian. promontory
press, new york.
h edGes , k. 1983a. a re-examination of pomo baby rocks.
American Indian Rock Art 9: 10–21.
hedGes, k. 1983b. the cloverdale petroglyphs. Rock Art Papers
1: 57–64.
heiZeR, R. f. 1953. sacred rain-rocks of northern california.
university of california archaeological survey, Report 22.
Reports of the University of California Archaeological Survey
20: 33–38.
hubeR, a. 1995. die fussabdrücke am schalenstein auf der
schöneben. Mitteilungen der Anisa 16: 52–62.
huWileR, k. 1998. Zeichen und Felsen. freemedia, Germany.
loeb, e. M. 1926. pomo folkways. University of California
Publications in American Archaeology and Ethnology 19(2):
149–405.
loMbRy, G. e. 2008. congolese uses of cupules. Rock Art Research
25: 207–208.
M c G oWan , c. 1982. Ceremonial fertility sites in southern
California. san diego Museum papers 14, san diego.
MountfoRd, c. p. 1976. Nomads of the Australian desert. Rigby,
adelaide.
odak, o. 1992. cup-marks patterns as an interpretation strategy in
some southern kenyan petroglyphs. in M. lorblanchet (ed.),
Rock art of the Old World: papers presented in Symposium A
of the AURA Congress, Darwin (Australia) 1988, pp. 49–60.
iGnca Rock art series 1, indira Gandhi national centre for
the arts, new delhi.
paRkMan, e. b. 1988. the hupa calendar stones at takimitlding
and Medilding, north-western california. Rock Art Research
5: 72–74.
paRkMan, e. b. 1992. toward a proto-hokan ideology. in s.
Goldsmith, s. Garvie, d. selin and J. smith (eds), Ancient
images, ancient thought: the archaeology of ideology, pp. 365–
370. proceedings of the 23rd annual chacmool conference,
university of calgary, calgary.
pohle, p. 2000. Historisch-geographische Untersuchungen im
tibetanischen Himalaya. Giessener Geographische schriften
76/1and 76/2, institut für Geographie der Justus-liebiguniversität, Gießen.
spieR, l. 1930. klamath ethnography. University of California
Publications in American Archaeology and Ethnologie 30:
1–338.
steinbRinG, J. and M. lanteiGne 1991. the petroglyphs of West
yorkshire: explorations in analysis and interpretation. Rock
Art Research 8: 13–28.
steVenson, M. c. 1904. the Zuni indians: their mythology, esoteric
one more ethnographic interpretation of cupules is available
from Zimbabwe. several granite lithophones are described
by huwiler (1998: 148), who reports that they are locally
called mujejeje. these occur near burial places and were
still used recently to communicate with ancestors interred
in the vicinity.
of these very few ethnographic interpretations of cupules,
only the australian example, some of those from the
western united states, and those regarding lithophones
can be regarded as fully secure. interpretations as game
boards may be plausible in some cases but need to be
investigated more comprehensively. other than that, we
lack adequate ethnographic information to establish the
former meanings of cupules, and all those explanations
formulated by archaeologists lack scientific credibility.
in summary, we have limited ethnographic information
that in some of the tens of thousands of cultural traditions
that can be said to have existed since the first known
cupules were made, they served for purposes related to
fertility and to increase rituals, and we know with certainty
that many cupules designate lithophones (see chapter on
these phenomena). however, faced by the immensity of
numbers of cupules ever made (very probably many times
their surviving number) and of the enormous time span
accounting for them, it is obvious that these glimpses are of
very limited value in explaining the general phenomenon.
Rather, these snippets of explanations appear to be
incidental to some other principle. in particular, they raise
unanswered questions that imply some unknown cultural
dimension in these extremely limited cases we have
reasonable explanations for. in all the secure ethnographic
interpretations, there is no obvious need for the marks to
assume precisely the very specific form of cupules. there
is some merit in the assumption that, for lithophonic
cupules, impact was focused on a very specific point
because it yielded the best sound. however, even this is
limited to some specimens, whereas on most lithophones
there are numerous markings, all consisting of perfectly
formed cupules, i.e. percussion was not just focused in their
production, but was highly focused and quite intentionally
so. none of the rare ethnographic explanations we have
offers a reason for cupules to take the specific shape they
have, or the fact that many or most of them seem to follow
the principle of achieving greatest depth with smallest
possible diameter. in short, the available ethnography of
cupules does not explain the phenomenon satisfactorily.
REFERENCES
alVaReZ, s. h. and d. W. peRi 1987. acorns: the stuff of life. News
from Native California 1(4): 10–14.
aRndt, W. 1962. the interpretation of the delamere lightning
113
MysteRious cup MaRks: pRoceedinGs of the fiRst inteRnational cupule confeRence
cupule engravings from Jinmium-Granilpi (northern australia
and beyond: exploration of a widespread and enigmatic class
of rock markings. Antiquity 71: 942–965.
tRost, f. 1993. Ethnoarchäologie in Südwest-Burkina Faso. I. Das
Fundmaterial. akademische druck-u. Verlagsanstalt, Graz.
fraternities, and ceremonies. Twenty-third Annual Report of the
Bureau of American Ethnology, pp. 3–634. Washington
steVenson, t. e. 1887. the religious life of the Zuni child. Fifth
Annual Report of the Bureau of American Ethnology, pp.
533–555. Washington.
taçon, p. s. c., R. fullaGaR, s. ouZMan and k. MulVaney 1997.
114