418
Microcircuits in visual cortex
Kevan AC Martin
The microcircuity of the neocortex is bewildering in its
anatomical detail, but seen through the filters of physiology,
some simple circuits have been suggested. Intensive
investigations of the cortical representation of orientation,
however, show how difficult it is to achieve any consensus on
what the circuits are, how they develop, and how they work.
New developments in modeling allied with powerful experimental
tools are changing this. Experimental work combining optical
imaging with anatomy and physiology has revealed a rich local
cortical circuitry. Whereas older models of cortical circuits have
concentrated on simple ‘feedforward’ circuits, newer theoretical
work has explored more the role of the recurrent cortical circuits,
which are more realistic representations of the actual circuits
and are computationally richer.
Addresses
Institute of NeuroInformatics, University of Zurich and Eidgenössische
Technische Hochschule (ETH), Zurich, Winterthurerstrasse 190,
8057 Zurich, Switzerland; e-mail:
[email protected]
Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2002, 12 :418–425
0959-4388/02/$ —see front matter
© 2002 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.
Published online 12 July 2002
Abbreviations
2D
two-dimensional
3D
three-dimensional
LGN
lateral geniculate nucleus
Introduction
Francis Crick once advised: ‘if you do not make headway
studying a complex system, study its structure and knowledge of its function will follow automatically’ (cited in [1]).
Explorers of neocortical microcircuits have traditionally
chosen the reverse strategy: they study function and use it
to infer structure. How successful have they been? Very —
if textbooks and computational models of cortical microcircuits are any indication. Despite the recent flowering of
anatomical studies associated with physiological recordings
in slices of cortex maintained in vitro, these new studies
have had remarkably little influence on modern ideas of
microcircuits in visual cortex. This is quite unlike the
central significance that anatomy has had for concepts of
hierarchical processing and for notions of feedforward and
feedback processing between cortical areas.
It could be argued that the relative lack of impact of
anatomical discoveries in cortical slices is because the work
in slices has been directed to questions of neuronal biophysics and synaptic physiology and not to the structural
basis of the functional microcircuits. Also, most cortical
slice studies use rodent somatomotor cortex and not the
visual cortex of the cat or monkey, which have been the
major models for investigations of cortical circuits. A more
fundamental reason may be that most of the progress in
our understanding of cortical microcircuits has come about
through the approach deployed with effortless brilliance
by Hubel and Wiesel [2]. They first constructed a hierarchical model (Figure 1a) of the microcircuit in visual
cortex on the basis of the receptive fields and of the lamina
in which neurons were recorded [2]. The power of their
approach was that it mattered not whether one or one hundred
cell types were involved, whether the synapses depressed
or potentiated, or what glutamate receptor subtypes and
ion channels were present: the baffling complexity of
microanatomy and microphysiology was simply irrelevant.
Thus, despite its ripe age, their model continues to be
remarkably agile and it remains the textbooks’ favorite. It
has had the good fortune to be insoluble by past and
current techniques and thus it continues to tantalize
successive generations of experimentalists. Most modelers
of cortical microcircuits have followed suit and taken the
first stage of the hierarchy — the generation of orientationselective ‘simple cells’ from a row of relay cells in the
lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) of the thalamus — as
their test case.
With this history as ambience, it is no surprise to discover
that recent publications on cortical microcircuits maintain
this bias of approach and interest. According to the dictum,
‘anatomy tells you what could be, physiology tells you
what is’ (JA Movshon, personal communication), in vivo
physiology and modeling continue to be the main tools
used to solve the structure of cortical microcircuits. Where
real structural studies are made in vivo, it is usually at the
level of populations of labeled neurons observed through
the light microscope. Thus, although the anatomical basis
of the visual field map (retinotopy) and the segregated
inputs of left and right eyes (ocular dominance) appear to
be fully explained by the distribution of the eye-specific
thalamic inputs to layer 4 [3], to date, no anatomical
circuits have been demonstrated for the cortical properties
of orientation tuning, binocular disparity, direction selectivity
and contrast adaptation, amongst others. Yet, this apparent
lack of progress is deceptive, for many of the elements
required for a new synthesis are already here. The new
experimental and theoretical evidence reviewed here
indicates that feedforward inputs to cortical layers from the
thalamus are not the sole determinant of the specificity of
neurons, even in the orientation domain. Rather, local
recurrent cortical circuits (Figure 1b) play an important
role in the organization of such specificity at the level of
single neurons and at the level of cortical maps (Figure 2).
Wiring the cortex
On the basis of the retinotopic map derived from the
mapping of thalamic afferents, the primary visual cortex
elaborates an impressive range of spatial and temporal
Microcircuits in visual cortex Martin
419
Figure 1
(a)
(b)
L4
E
–
E
+
L3
LGN
+
L3
I
E
L4
I
E
LGN
Current Opinion in Neurobiology
Schematics of the vertical columns of visual cortex. (a) The hierarchical
model of Hubel and Wiesel. It has two stages: neurons of the LG N
with their concentric centre-surround receptive fields converge on
layer 4 (L4) neurons to form ‘simple’ receptive fields. The layer 4
neurons then converge on layer 3 (L3) neurons to form ‘complex’
receptive fields. Both simple and complex cells lie within the same
orientation column, indicated by cylinder. The cardinal characteristic of
the complex cells is their position invariance – an optimal oriented
stimulus placed anywhere in the receptive field gives the same
response. Responses of simple cells are highly position-dependent
and sign-dependent. Only feedforward excitatory connections are
considered in their model. Lefthand column indicates schematic of
circuitry, righthand columns the receptive field structure of neurons at
the three stages of the hierarchy. (b) An alternative hierarchy. Layers 3
and 4 contain excitatory (E) and inhibitory (I) neurons that are
recurrently connected in every possible combination. Most recurrent
connections to any neuron are from neighbors. It is out of this
collective computation that the functional maps are formed.
properties. Because these properties are generated by
cortical microcircuits, it is important to discover the rules
and constraints governing the layout of the wiring in the
microcircuits. However, such investigations are relatively
rare. Stevens [4•] proposed that amongst visual cortical
properties, it is only the need to represent orientation with
the same fidelity as the retinotopic map that drives the
significant increase in the total number of cortical neurons
relative to lateral geniculate relay neurons. In general, any
additional parameter that needs to be represented in visual
cortex means additional constraints on the way in which
the neurons wire themselves together; thus, it is crucial to
understand the nature of the cortical representations of
dimensions such as space, motion, and orientation. It is a
fortuitous coincidence that, thanks to optical recordings of
the intrinsic signals in the visual cortex, we know more
about the representation of the orientation of stimuli than
we do about any visual property other than retinotopy.
This attention to the two-dimensional (2D) organization of
the orientation domain, as it is expressed in superficial
cortical layers, has resulted in many delightful papers. The
beautiful false-color images used to illustrate the orientation maps have seduced many editors searching for an
eye-catching cover picture for their journals (Figure 2).
However, the 2D maps themselves are intriguing because
they contain large regions where orientation preferences of
neurons change slowly, interspersed with point singularities,
called ‘pinwheels’ or ‘orientation centers’, where orientation
preferences change rapidly. Such images have been the
inspiration for models of cortical wiring, where the goal is
to discover whether the maps are in some way optimal
solutions to the constraint of reducing wiring length [5,6•].
The significance of exploring this domain is that it places
important constraints on the 2D intracortical connections
and offers experimentally testable predictions.
Algorithms for self-organizing maps have been successful
in describing the basic features of cortical maps [7–9]. One
issue they have addressed is the relationship between the
orientation map and the retinotopic and eye dominance
maps [10]. The basic assumption of these models is that
computations in cortex are local; thus, optimally, all the
neurons that represent the different parameters in a given
part of the visual field should be neighbors and have the
shortest interconnections. In visual cortex, the two dimensions of retinotopic space, and one each of orientation and
ocular dominance, have to be represented on a 2D cortical
surface. In reducing the dimensions, distortions are
introduced to the map. In the case of near optimal solutions
(i.e. those requiring the minimum ‘wire’) the most rapid
420
Sensory systems
Figure 2
results by adding a variant of Hebbian synaptic learning to
the Kohonen self-organizing algorithm [7]. In Kohonen’s
algorithm, synapses in a patch of the map are strengthened
regardless of whether the neurons they contact are also
active. This is sometimes called ‘volume learning’ and the
Hebbian variant requires the additional constraint that
synapses are only strengthened when the postsynaptic
neurons are also sufficiently active. Mitchison and
Swindale’s [12] modifications were only partially successful,
because the correlation between changes in orientation
and changes in retinal position were weak, albeit positive.
Worse still, they found that rapid changes in orientation
were also correlated with abrupt changes in ocular dominance in their model. This does not occur experimentally
[10], which argues against purely Hebbian learning
mechanisms being involved in map formation. For good
measure, Mitchison and Swindale cautioned against a
thorough overhaul of models before species other than the
cat were examined. Sure enough, the ferret [13••] and
the tree shrew [14] do not have distorted maps; thus, on
the basis of the present data, the cat’s visual cortex seems
to be anomalous. However, new models have concentrated
on explaining the cat data [15,16•]. Surprisingly, a consensus
has emerged that the orientation map reflects the architecture
and activity patterns of lateral connections within the
cortical circuits, and not simply the feedforward pattern of
thalamic connections [11,12,13••].
s
Thinking laterally
p
c
i
Current Opinion in Neurobiology
Schematic of connectivity within the orientation map of visual cortex.
Optical recordings of intrinsic signals rendered in false color provide
striking images of the tangential organization of the cortical orientation
map. The maps are generated by presenting stimuli at different
orientations and recording the ‘intrinsic’ optical signals associated with
metabolic or electrical activity in the superficial layers of the cortex. The
intrinsic signals associated with the different orientation are then colorcoded and presented in a composite orientation map. The maps have
orientation centers, or pinwheels, indicated by the four donuts,
separated by relatively linear iso-orientation zones where slow changes
in orientation preference occur across the iso-orientation bands.
Injections of anterograde and retrograde tracers (dark circle on the
bottom left donut) into the orientation centers produce symmetric
labeling of inhibitory (indicated by white dots) and excitatory (black
dots) axons and/or cells. Tracer injections into the linear zones (dark
circle towards the center/right) produce labeling distributed along the
visual field axis corresponding to the cells’ preferred orientation. The
star indicates orientation preferences of the optical map. Visual space
is also mapped in 2D on the cortical surface. Visual fields: s, superior;
i, inferior; c, central; p, peripheral. Not shown to scale.
changes in orientation occur in places where the retinal
position changes most slowly. Inconveniently, the opposite
was found in one experimental study in the cat: the most
rapid changes in orientation were associated with the most
rapid changes in retinal position [11]. Mitchison and
Swindale [12] attempted to accommodate these new
In the feedfoward model of orientation selectivity, a row of
LGN neurons converges on a single cortical neuron to
create a cortical receptive field that is longer than it is wide
(Figure 1a). This anisotropy in the LGN connections to
layer 4 neurons is the means by which orientation selectivity
is generated by the feedforward connections. One
alternate view to feedforward patterning is that lateral connections of cortical neurons generate the spatial anisotropy
inherent in orientation preferences [17•,18,19]. In the
monkey, the axons of the non-orientation-selective layer
4cβ cells have few lateral connections, whereas the
orientation-sensitive layer 4cα cells have strong lateral
projections [19]. In cat layer 4, spiny stellate cells are
orientation selective and have extensive lateral connections
[20]. Layer 3 neurons in the cat also extend their axons
along the retinotopic axis of the cortical map that
corresponds to their orientation preference [18]. Previous
attempts in macaque monkeys to discover an orientationrelated anisotropy of intracortical connections outside
layer 4 foundered, because of the high degree of anisotropy
generated when the visual fields of the left and right eyes
are mapped into separate ‘ocular dominance’ stripes. By
contrast, the tree shrew, which separates the left and right
eye input to cortex into sublayers rather than bands
[21,22], provides a particularly florid expression of the
anatomical pattern seen in the cat [23] (Figure 2).
The confounding ocular dominance system is more weakly
expressed in New World monkeys than in the Old World
Microcircuits in visual cortex Martin
macaque, which means that, locally, the retinotopic map is
almost isotropic. A similar elongation of axon collaterals is
found in the New World primate as exists in the cat and
the tree shrew [17•]. However, the anisotropy is small,
∼1.7 times long as it is wide. The long axis is ∼3 mm, which
means that any neuron could connect monosynaptically to
another neuron with adjacent but non-overlapping receptive
fields. However, it must be noted that this extent of
collaterals may be an overestimate, because the tracer used
in this study might also label the local axon collaterals of
distant neurons whose axons transport the label retrogradely. A related study in the cat [24••], using similar
methods, explored the topography of lateral connections
in layer 3, to and from the pinwheel or orientation centers
— the points in the orientation map upon which all
orientations converge [10]. Unlike the connections made
in iso-orientation domains [25], the pattern of labeling to
and from the centers was circularly symmetric (∼1.6 mm in
diameter). This resembles the pattern seen in layer 4 of
the cat, where the lateral extent of labeling was ∼1–2 mm,
with little clustering or asymmetry [26]. The interpretations of these anatomical studies are somewhat hampered
by the fact that the putative inhibitory and excitatory
connections are not always differentiated.
Bold attempts were made previously to identify the lateral
connections of inhibitory and excitatory neurons within
the orientation map in adult ferret visual cortex [27]; the
latest such attempts are as technically demanding [28••].
Roerig and Chen [28••] made optical recordings of the
orientation maps in ferret cortex in vivo: they then sliced
the cortex horizontally and used photo-released glutamate
to stimulate presumed monosynaptic inhibitory and
excitatory inputs onto single pyramidal cells in known
orientation domains in superficial and deep layers (orientation tuning is poor or absent in layer 4 neurons in ferret
[29]). About 90% of the recorded inputs originated from
neurons lying within 500 µm of the recorded pyramidal
cell. Inhibitory potentials were more common close to the
recorded cell, whereas excitatory connections were more
common at longer distances and had a stronger preference
for the iso-orientation domains than the inhibitory
connections. This is similar to the pattern seen in the cat,
although anatomical connections in the ferret extend twice
as far as in the cat [24••,26,30•]. The excitatory currents
originating from iso-orientation domains were also larger in
amplitude on average than those originating from neurons
in other orientation domains. These currents arise from
clusters of neurons, so this amplitude may indicate that
relatively more neurons connect from iso-orientation
domains. However, overall, the individual neurons
received <50% of their input from neurons whose
orientation preference was close (within 30°) to theirs.
Simply on the basis of random connectivity, it would
be expected that inhibitory neurons, which make dense
local axonal arbors, would connect on average more to
their neighboring cells, which have similar orientation
preferences [30•] (Figure 1b).
421
Taken together, these studies appear to rule out explanations
of orientation that rely on high specificity of connections
between cortical neurons. Of course, even if local connections
were highly specific for orientation, we would be none the
wiser as to the mechanisms underlying the generation of
orientation specificity. Nevertheless, these studies have
brought a new degree of technical sophistication to experimental studies of cortical microcircuitry.
Wetware
The task of elucidating the circuits for basic functions such
as orientation and direction selectivity might seem simpler
in the cat than in the ferret, tree shrew, or monkey — all
species where the orientation and direction selectivity of
neurons in layer 4, the major thalamorecipient layer, is
weak or absent [22,29,31]. The simplification offered by
the model of Hubel and Wiesel [2] is that the pattern of
convergence of thalamic connections on layer 4 neurons
generates the receptive field properties of the first stage of
the hierarchy. Thus, local connections between cortical
neurons are ignored in favor of a feedfoward circuit
(Figure 1a). Cross-correlating the activity of the presynaptic
and postsynaptic neurons allows an exploration of the rules
governing the thalamic connections to orientation-sensitive neurons in layer 4 of the cat. This electro-anatomical
method assumes that a monosynaptic connection exists
between two neurons when a spike in one cell is followed,
after a delay of a few milliseconds, by a spike in a second
cell. However, even in optimal conditions, where the
monosynaptic connection is strong, cross-correlation shows
only the physiological expression of a part of the underlying
circuit under the given stimulus conditions, not the whole
circuit itself. Nonetheless, cross-correlation studies have
made strong claims in support of the Hubel and Wiesel
model and have recently extended their simple cell model
by claiming that the feedforward input is not only spatially
specific, but also specific in terms of timing, strength of
connection, and size of thalamic input [32•]. Cross-correlation
techniques further show that monosynaptic connections
exist between layer 4 simple cells and layer 3 complex cells
within the same orientation column [33•]. These latter
results ‘provide strong support for one of the main tenants
(sic) of the hierarchical model’ [33•]. Such cross-correlation
studies do not explain the operation of a cortical circuit of
the observed three-dimensional (3D) organization and
richness of behavior of the visual cortex, but what they do
support is a strong claim that connections between cortical
neurons are highly specific. If true, how does such connectional specificity develop?
Map-making
Although orientation specificity is present at birth in many
species, one view is that it arises not through epigenesis,
but through activity-dependent learning. Here, theorists
continue to have a field day [16•,34•,35•] but experimentalists who share their devotion to nurture as an organizing
principle have found it hard to obtain unequivocal
evidence that neural activity is the primary factor driving
422
Sensory systems
the development of orientation selectivity. The main
paradigm for activity-dependent learning involves rearing
animals in restricted environments (e.g. in the dark, or
with both eyes closed, in stroboscopic lighting, or ‘striperearing’) where the animals live in a pop-art world of high
contrast vertical or horizontal stripes. The conventional
assumption is that the changes such deprivation induces
are due to some form of Hebbian learning. In the most
recent account in ferrets, the particular form of the deprivation had a marked influence on visual maps [36••]. The
maps in animals with both eyes closed during development were far more rudimentary than in eye-open animals
reared in the dark. The interpretation offered is that light
scattering through the closed eyelids disrupts the normal
orientation-based correlations in activity that lead to a
sharpening of the orientation circuits by a Hebbian process
[36••]. Both forms of deprivation produced marked reductions in the extent and in the degree of clustering of lateral
connections of layer 3.
microcircuit is, even if there are still strong differences of
opinion concerning its function. For example, one persistent misconception about feedback or recurrent circuits is
that they are inherently much slower than feedforward
circuits and that the selectivity they generate must sharpen
over time [46]. In fact, models of recurrent circuits indicate
that sharp tuning is present from the first spike, despite a
broadly tuned feedforward input [44,47••]. Thus, local
recurrent processing doesn’t necessarily slow interareal
transmission [48•]. Recordings with a voltage-sensitive dye
in the recurrent circuits of the cat’s visual cortex have now
tracked the time course of orientation-tuned responses
[49••]. As with intracellular experiments [46], these recordings
reveal that orientation tuning does not sharpen with time,
but that the response to the non-preferred (orthogonal)
orientation is suppressed relative to the preferred. Thus,
instead of ‘sharpening’ the orientation-tuning curve, the
suppression (inhibition) appeared to increase the difference between the preferred and non-preferred responses.
Immaculate timing is crucial for Hebbian synaptic modification, whether it be to potentiate or depress a synapse.
Recent in vivo studies use protocols that provide far better
temporal control of presynaptic and postsynaptic activity
than restricted rearing experiments [37••,38,39]. These
studies produce conditioned shifts in the orientation of
neurons. One important discovery in the context of the
present discussion is that the orientation preferences of
layer 4 neurons are not modifiable [37••]. This is reminiscent
of the finding that layer 4 neurons do not respond rapidly
to monocular deprivation, whereas neurons in other layers
do [40,41••]. The synapses in the major thalamorecipient
layer, it seems, are not so ‘plastic’. Nevertheless, recent
models [35•] are still strongly centered on specificity
‘learned’ at geniculocortical synapses in layer 4. Work on
hierarchical networks that use temporal learning rules and
natural stimuli to learn complex receptive fields may
indicate another route of understanding the development
of specificity [42•]. Indeed, over the years, the thought has
occured to a number of prominent investigators that the
key features that we regard as ‘cortical’ may truly be an
invention of the recurrent circuits of cortex itself and not
solely due to an ingenious construction of feedforward
thalamic connections [11,17•,23].
Although recurrent models nominally include more of the
actual cortical circuitry than feedforward models, they have
remained minimalist with regard to cell types, 3D organization and dynamics. As to the architecture, most assume that
the lateral connections of the cortex are isotropic and that
inhibitory connections form the surround and excitatory
connections dominate the center [15,16•,34•,44,50]. This
Mexican Hat pattern is the inverse of the actual structure
(Figure 2). For reasons of stability, inhibition dominates
model recurrent networks, although this seems at odds with
the cortical microcircuits in which only 25% of neurons are
inhibitory and ∼80% of the synapses made by excitatory
cells are with other excitatory cells. Even modelers trying to
use a more realistic connectivity make the inhibitory
synapses so much stronger than the excitatory ones that
they effectively generate a Mexican Hat [51,52•]. With a
Mexican Hat connectivity, neurons located in the orientation centers have an advantage, theoretically at least,
because all orientations provide them with inhibition and
this generates a more sharply tuned output from a broadly
tuned input than does iso-orientation or cross-orientation
inhibition. This access to all orientations may help explain
the marked short-term orientation plasticity seen in neurons near orientation centers [53•].
Food for thought
Conclusions
To be fair, feedforward models have evolved. So much so
that they are reminiscent of the fabled ‘stone soup’, in
which the more supplementary ingredients added to the
basic recipe of boiled stone, the better the taste. As more
of the known cortical microcircuits have been included in
computational models, they have come to exhibit a richer
range of cortical-like behaviors. Indeed, in matter, if not
yet in the minds of their inventors, the current crop of
feedforward circuits is indistinguishable from earlier
versions of recurrent circuits founded on observed cortical
anatomy [43–45]. This convergence offers the enticing
prospect of consensus on what the structure of the basic
Work on cortical microcircuits is being done in the conceptual
framework that is 40 years old and based on investigations
in cat area 17 [2]. This framework offers a simple feedforward
hierarchy to explain the formation of specific receptive
fields (Figure 1a). Thus, papers on cat microcircuits still
begin with sentences such as: ‘Although separated by a
single synapse, thalamic cells and cortical simple cells have
very different response properties’ [32•]. This focus on the
thalamocortical synapse obscures the truth that, even in
layer 4 of the cat, 95% of the synapses on a simple cell
come from other cortical neurons, which are themselves
connected in a multisynaptic network [54]. It is this
Microcircuits in visual cortex Martin
collective computation that needs to be understood, not
just the pattern of feedfoward thalamic connections that
contribute a few percent of cortical synapses and a small
fraction of the total synaptic drive [43,44,54]. Similarly,
layer 3, the second tier in the hierarchical model (Figure 1a,b),
receives only ∼20% of its excitatory connections from
layer 4. Approximately 75% of its connections arise from its
neighbors in layer 3 (T Binzegger, R Douglas, KAC Martin,
unpublished data). Nevertheless, conceptually and experimentally, it has proved difficult to go beyond the
thalamocortical synapse and to get to grips with the essential
multilayered recurrence of the local cortical circuit. Many
modelers still select details of the cortical wiring in the
hope that they will eventually describe physiological
phenomena, rather than attempting to discover the principles
by which cortical microcircuits are built and operate. The
devil, it seems, is still in the details.
It is clear that we simply do not understand much of the
detailed structure of cortical microcircuits or their relation
to function. Our present models are highly abstracted versions of the known fragments of the microcircuits. Perhaps
because we are overawed by the structure of cortical microcircuits, we have not yet learned to think about them in
their own terms — as highly successful adaptations that
solve the difficult problems of vision in the natural world.
Nevertheless, experimentalists and theoreticians increasingly
share the same brain and this offers new hope for rapid
progress on the hard problems of cortical microcircuitry.
Many of the clues we need are already available. In particular, we certainly cannot continue to neglect the details of
microstructure and physiology that have been gathered in
recent years from in vitro experiments. But, whatever the
model, the simple truth still remains: ‘what chiefly distinguishes cerebral cortex from other parts of the nervous
system is the great diversity of its cell types and interconnections. It would be astonishing if such a structure did not
profoundly modify the response patterns of the fibres
coming into it’ [2].
Acknowledgements
I thank the Human Frontier Science Program and the European Union for
support, members of Information Networking Institute for critical readings,
and John Anderson for artwork.
References and recommended reading
Papers of particular interest, published within the annual period of review,
have been highlighted as:
• of special interest
•• of outstanding interest
423
number of lateral geniculate neurons. The additional neurons are explained
by the need to represent another dimension — orientation — at the same
spatial resolution as the visual field map.
5.
Swindale NV: Keeping the wires short: a singularly difficult
problem. Neuron 2001, 29 :316-317.
6.
•
Koulakov AA, Chklovskii DB: Orientation preference patterns in
mammalian visual cortex: a wire length minimization problem.
Neuron 2001, 29 :519-527.
These authors suggest that the orientation centers that arise in the orientation maps of cat, ferret, tree shrew and monkey might simply be a consequence of mechanisms that minimize the connections between neurons. This
model gives a reason for the existence of these singularities in the orientation map and provides insights into the possible factors that lead to the
absence of the singularities in rodents.
7.
Kohonen T: Self-organized formation of topologically correct
feature maps. Biol Cybern 1982, 43 :59-69.
8.
Durbin R, Willshaw DJ: An analogue approach to the traveling
salesman problem using an elastic net method. Nature 1987,
326 :688-691.
9.
Durbin R, Mitchison G: A dimension reduction framework for
understanding cortical maps. Nature 1990, 343 :644-647.
10. Bartfeld E, Grinvald A: Relationships between orientation
preference pinwheels, cytochrome oxidase blobs and ocular
dominance columns in primate striate cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci
USA 1992, 89 :11905-11909.
11. Das A, Gilbert CD: Distortions of visuotopic map match orientation
singularities in primary visual cortex. Nature 1997, 343 :594-598.
12. Mitchison GJ, Swindale NV: Can Hebbian volume learning explain
discontinuities in cortical maps? Neural Comput 1999, 11:1519-1526.
13. White LE, Bosking W H, Fitzpatrick D: Consistent mapping of
•• orientation preference across irregular functional domains in
ferret visual cortex. Vis Neurosci 2001, 18 :65-76.
Theoreticians have yet to catch up with the twists and turns in the experimental evidence from different species as to the relationship between the
orientation and retinotopic maps. The ferret is apparently different from its
carnivorous cousin, the cat, in which rapid changes in orientation area are
associated with rapid shifts in retinal position. These new data are important
for understanding how the different parameters represented in cortex are
best mapped onto a 2D surface.
14. Bosking W H, Crowley JC, Fitzpatrick D: Fine structure of the map of
visual space in the tree shrew striate cortex revealed by optical
imaging. Soc Neurosci Abs 1997, 23 :1945.
15. Ernst U, Pawelzik K, Tsodyks M, Sejnowski TJ: Relation between
retinotopical and orientation maps in visual cortex. Neural Comput
1999, 11 :375-379.
16. Ernst UA, Pawelzik KR, Sahar-Pikielny C, Tsodyks MV: Intracortical
•
origin of visual maps. Nat Neurosci 2001, 4 :431-436.
As an extension to their previous work [15], Ernst et al. assume a nonHebbian learning rule to generate cortical maps that explain the retinotopy–orientation distortion in cat [11]. This provides an alternative to widely
used Hebbian learning models to explain the development of cortical maps.
17. Sincich LC, Blasdel GG: Oriented axon projections in primary
•
visual cortex of the monkey. J Neurosci 2001, 21 :4416-4426.
The orientation of the axons of layer 3 neurons in area 17 of New World
monkeys is slightly elongated in the axis of preferred orientation. The authors
of this paper suggest that these axons may form a recurrent excitatory circuit
that generates length summation.
18. Schmidt KE, Geobel R, Löwel S, Singer W : The perceptual grouping
criterion of colinearity is reflected by anisotropies of connections
in the primary visual cortex. Eur J Neurosci 1997, 9 :1083-1089.
1.
Ramachandran VS: The neurobiology of perception. Perception
1985, 14 :97-103.
19. Fitzpatrick D, Lund JS, Blasdel GG: Intrinsic connections of the
macaque striate cortex: afferent and efferent connections of
lamina 4C. J Neurosci 1985, 5 :3329-3349.
2.
Hubel DH, Wiesel TN: Receptive fields, binocular interaction and
functional architecture in the cat’s visual cortex. J Physiol 1962,
160 :106-154.
20. Martin KAC, Whitteridge D: Form, function and intracortical
projections of spiny neurones in the striate visual cortex of the
cat. J Physiol 1984, 353 :463-504.
3.
Hubel DH, Wiesel TN: Anatomical demonstration of columns in the
monkey striate cortex. Nature 1969, 221 :747-750.
21. Fitzpatrick D: The functional organization of local circuits in visual
cortex: insights from the study of tree shrew striate cortex. Cereb
Cortex 1996, 6 :329-341.
4. Stevens CF: An evolutionary scaling law for the primate visual
•
system and its basis in cortical function. Nature 2001, 10 :193-195.
This paper provides a compact explanation of the observation that the
number of neurons in primate area 17 increases as a 3/2 power of the
22. Muly EC, Fitzpatrick D: The morphological basis for binocular and
ON/ OFF convergence in tree shrew striate cortex. J Neurosci
1992, 12 :1319-1334.
424
Sensory systems
23. Bosking W H, Zhang Y, Scofield B, Fitzpatrick D: Orientation
selectivity and the arrangement of horizontal connections in tree
shrew visual cortex. J Neurosci 1997, 17 :2112-2127.
24. Yousel T, Toth E, Rausch M, Eysel UT, Kisvarday ZF: Topography of
•• orientation centre connections in the primary visual cortex of the
cat. Neuroreport 2001, 12 :1693-1699.
This group continues their fine experimental work on the microarchitecture of
visual cortex directed at the structural basis of the orientation system. Here,
they show that connections to orientation centers are different from
connections to outside centers.
25. Kisvarday ZF, Toth E, Rausch M, Eysel UT: Orientation-specific
relationship between populations of excitatory and inhibitory
lateral connections in the visual cortex of the cat. Cereb Cortex
1997, 7 :605-618.
26. Yousef T, Bonhoeffer T, Kim DS, Eysel UT, Toth E, Kisvarday ZF:
Orientation topography of layer 4 lateral networks revealed by
optical imaging in cat visual cortex (area 18). Eur J Neurosci 1999,
11 :4291-4308.
27.
Weliky M, Kandler K, Fitzpatrick D, Katz LC: Patterns of excitation
and inhibition evoked by horizontal connections in visual cortex
share a common relationship to orientation columns. Neuron
1995, 15 :541-552.
28. Roerig B, Chen B: Relationships of local inhibitory and excitatory
•• circuitry to orientation preference maps in ferret visual cortex.
Cereb Cortex 2001, 12 :197-198.
An understated study of the spatial distribution of inhibitory and excitatory
connections in identified orientation columns. The authors claim that isoorientation inhibition dominates the short-range connections and isoorientation excitation dominates the long-range connections in area 17.
29. Chapman B, Stryker MP: Development of orientation selectivity in
ferret visual cortex and effects of deprivation. J Neurosci 1993,
13 :5251-5262.
30. Buzas P, Eysel UT, Adorjan P, Kisvarday ZF: Axonal topography of
•
cortical basket cells in relation to orientation, direction, and ocular
dominance maps. J Comp Neurol 2001, 437 :259-285.
This study provides a rare view of the tangential projections of individual
inhibitory neurons within different cortical maps.
31. Hubel DH, Wiesel TN: Receptive fields and functional architecture
of monkey striate cortex. J Physiol 1968, 195 :215-243.
32. Alonso J-M, Usrey W M, Reid RC: Rules of connectivity between
•
geniculate cells and simple cells in cat primary visual cortex.
J Neurosci 2001, 21 :4002-4015.
The authors if this paper present the most advanced account of thalamocortical cross-correlation studies. They document both temporal and spatial
relations between the LG N and layer 4 simple cells while adroitly skirting the
vexed issue of how the two major types of LG N neurons, the X and Y cells,
project to the simple cells in area 17.
33. Martinez LM, Alonso JM: Construction of complex receptive fields
•
in cat primary visual cortex. Neuron 2001, 32 :515-525.
A cross-correlation study of connections between layer 4 and layer 3 in the
cat visual cortex. The authors show that γ-amino butyric acid (GABA)-inactivation of the A-layer in the lateral geniculate nucleus silences connected
neurons both in the thalamus and in the cortex. Their results directly contradict earlier findings of Malpeli et al. [55], who found that layer 3 neurons
remained active after LG N A-layers were inactivated. Although they claim
that the findings strongly support the hierarchical model, the method
is inherently unable to demonstrate that complex cell responses are due
solely to simple cell input, as the hierarchical model requires.
Accompanying panygeric [56].
34. Burger T, Lang EW : Self-organization of local circuits and
•
cortical orientation maps: a nonlinear Hebbian model of the visual
cortex with adaptive lateral couplings. Z Naturforsch 2001,
56 :464-478.
Burger and Lang discover that when left (nearly) to their own devices, local
neocortical circuits create Difference of Gaussian (DOG) patterns of
connectivity. This model does not explain the mechanisms that lead to the
alternate architecture seen in the brain.
35. Kayser AS, Miller KD: Opponent inhibition: a developmental
•
model of layer 4 in the neocortical circuit. Neuron 2002,
33 :131-142.
Here, the authors further explore the ability of Hebb’s eponymous synapses
to generate opponent inhibition in which inhibition is evoked by stimuli that
are anticorrelated with those that excite the cell. The claim is that opponent
inhibition is the mechanism whereby cortical neurons respond specifically,
regardless of the contrast of the stimulus.
36. White LE, Coppola DM, Fitzpatrick D: The contribution of sensory
•• experience to the maturation of orientation selectivity in ferret
visual cortex. Nature 2001, 411 :1049-1052.
In this study, the authors indicate the involvement of Hebbian learning
mechanisms in the development of the orientation map in the visual cortex.
Simple eyelid closure has a far more catastrophic effect on the map than
dark-rearing —a result with curiously Beckettian resonance [57].
37.
••
Schuett S, Bonhoeffer T, Hübener M: Pairing-induced changes of
orientation maps in cat visual cortex. Neuron 2001,
32 :325-337.
Schuett et al. apply a Hebbian paradigm to induce acute changes in the
orientation maps of anaesthetized kittens.
38. Yao H, Dan Y: Stimulus timing-dependent plasticity in cortical
processing of orientation. Neuron 2001, 32 :315-323.
39. Sur M, Schummers J, Dragoi V: Cortical plasticity: time for a
change. Curr Biol 2002, 12 :R168-R170.
40. Trachtenberg JT, Trepel C, Stryker MP: Rapid extragranular
plasticity in the absence of thalamocortical plasticity in the
developing primary visual cortex. Science 2000,
287 :2029-2032.
41. Trachtenberg JT, Stryker MP: Rapid anatomical plasticity of
•• horizontal connection in the developing visual cortex. J Neurosci
2001, 21 :3476-3482.
This fascinating study shows rapid changes in horizontal connections in
superficial layers of visual cortex following monocular eye closure in kittens.
A complement to [39].
42. Einhäuser W, Kayser C, König P, Körding KP: Learning the
•
invariance properties of complex cells from their responses to
natural stimuli. Eur J Neurosci 2002, 15 :1-14.
A new attempt to explain how, in a feedforward circuit, complex cells could
acquire their property of spatial invariance from simple cells by exploiting the
temporal structure of natural images.
43. Douglas RJ, Martin KAC, Whitteridge D: A canonical microcircuit for
neocortex. Neural Comput 1989, 1 :480-488.
44. Somers DC, Nelson SB, Sur M: An emergent model of orientation
selectivity in cat visual cortical simple cells. J Neurosci 1995,
15 :5448-5465.
45. Miller KD, Pinto DJ, Simons DJ: Processing in layer 4 of the
neocortical circuit: new insights from visual and somatosensory
cortex. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2001, 11 :488-497.
46. Gillespie DC, Lampl I, Anderson JS, Ferster D: Dynamics of the
orientation-tuned membrane potential response in cat primary
visual cortex. Nat Neurosci 2001, 4 :1014-1019.
47.
••
Lui SC, Kramer J, Indiveri G, Delbrück T, Burg T, Douglas R:
Orientation-selective aVLSI spiking neurons. Neural Netw 2001,
14 :629-643.
The authors present an analogue Very Large Scale Integrated (VLSI) that
emulates a visual system of retina and cortex, using integrate-and-fire
neurons and a soft winner-take-all cortical microcircuit. Here, a VLSI system
is used to compare spatial and temporal dynamics of feedforward and
feedback models in real-time computations. Sharp orientation selectivity
is present from the first spike in recurrent circuits, despite a broadly
oriented input.
48. Panzeri S, Rolls ET, Battaglia F, Lavis R: Speed of feedforward and
•
recurrent processing in multilayer networks of integrate-and-fire
neurons. Network 2001, 12 :423-440.
Panzeri et al. simulate interareal transmission with the surprising result that
recurrent networks add little to the processing time needed by purely
feedforward networks.
49. Sharon D, Grinvald A: Dynamics and constancy in cortical
•• spatiotemporal patterns of orientation processing. Science 2002,
295 :512-515.
The authors use voltage-sensitive dyes with high temporal resolution to
study the evolution of the orientation response in superficial layers of cat
cortex. The study contains a timely plea from the savant of Rehovot to
combine feedforward and recurrent models of orientation selectivity.
50. Douglas RJ, Mahowald MA, Martin KAC: Hybrid analog–digital
architectures for neuromorphic systems. IEEE Trans Neural
Networks 1994, 5 :1848-1853.
51. McLaughlin D, Shapley R, Shelley M, Wielaard DJ: A neuronal
network model of macaque monkey primary visual cortex (V1):
orientation selectivity and dynamics in the input layer 4Cα. Proc
Natl Acad Sci USA 2001, 97 :8087-8092.
Microcircuits in visual cortex Martin
52. Wielaard DJ, Shelley M, McLaughlin D, Shapley R: How simple cells
•
are made in a nonlinear network model of the visual cortex.
J Neurosci 2001, 21 :5203-5211.
An extension of their earlier model of simple cells [51] that points to
the potential importance of phase-insensitive inhibition generated by intracortical circuits.
53. Dragoi V, Rivadulla C, Sur M: Foci of orientation plasticity in visual
•
cortex. Nature 2001, 411 :80-86.
Here, the authors find that short-term adaptation is stronger in neurons located
in orientation centers than in neurons located in iso-orientation domains.
425
54. Ahmed B, Anderson JC, Douglas RJ, Martin KA, Nelson JC:
Polyneuronal innervation of spiny stellate neurons in cat visual
cortex. J Comp Neurol 1994, 341 :39-49.
55. Malpeli JG: Activity of cells in area 17 of the cat in the absence of
input from layer A of lateral geniculate nucleus. J Neurophysiol
1983, 49 :595-610.
56. Callaway EM: Neural mechanism for the generation of visual
complex cells. Neuron 2001, 32 :378-380.
57.
Beckett S: Watt. John Calder: London; 1976:231.