BJDP 440—28/5/2008—SHANMUGAM—300030
1
British Journal of Developmental Psychology (2008), in press
q 2008 The British Psychological Society
The
British
Psychological
Society
www.bpsjournals.co.uk
Just a talking book? Word learning from watching
baby videos
Michael B. Robb*, Rebekah A. Richert and Ellen A. Wartella
University of California, Riverside, California, USA
This study examined the relationship between viewing an infant DVD and expressive
and receptive language outcomes. Children between 12 and 15 months were randomly
assigned to view Baby Wordsworth, a DVD highlighting words around the house
marketed for children beginning at 12 months of age. Viewings took place in home
settings over 6 weeks. After every 2 weeks and five exposures to the DVD, children
were assessed on expressive and receptive communication measures. Results indicated
there was no increased growth on either outcome for children who had viewed the
DVD as compared to children in the control group, even after multiple exposures. After
controlling for age, gender, cognitive developmental level, income, and parent
education, the most significant predictor of vocabulary comprehension and production
scores was the amount of time children were read to.
Targeting children as young as 1 month old, a growing number of videos and DVDs are
available and marketed to children in the first 2 years of life, including Baby Einstein,
Brainy Superstar, Brainy Baby, Jumpstart, and T.V. Teach Me (Garrison & Christakis,
2005). In 2006, BabyFirstTV, a 24-hour channel featuring programmes targeted to
infants, premiered on cable and satellite providers. As of 2005, the Baby Einstein brand
alone sold over $200 million worth of products with indications that the market would
continue to rise (Bronson & Merryman, 2006). Currently, children under 2 are estimated
to spend 1–2 hours a day viewing television (Christakis, Zimmerman, DiGiuseppe, &
McCarty, 2004; Rideout, Hamel, & Kaiser Family Foundation, 2006; Rideout,
Vandewater, Wartella, & Kaiser Family Foundation, 2003). Although there have been a
small number of reports and studies exploring the effect of this screen time on
development (e.g. Anderson & Pempek, 2005; Rideout et al., 2003, 2006), there is no
clear understanding of potential links between media exposure at this early age and
developmental outcomes, especially with regards to content produced specifically for
children under 2.
Given the rapid proliferation of media directed to children in the beginning years,
parents are often caught in the middle of conflicting advice. Some academics and groups
* Correspondence should be addressed to Michael B. Robb, Department of Psychology, University of California, Riverside, CA
92521, USA (e-mail:
[email protected]).
DOI:10.1348/026151008X320156
BJDP 440—28/5/2008—SHANMUGAM—300030
2 Michael B. Robb et al.
like the American Academy of Pediatrics (1999) favour very restricted or zero exposure
to television for very young children. The concern is that these hours would otherwise
be spent in the kinds of social interaction and high-quality activities critical for brain
development.
In contrast, media producers market their products as having developmental
benefits for young viewers. Despite the slim body of research on learning effects from
infant-directed television and DVDs, teaching appears to be an implicit goal of many
media producers. Garrison and Christakis (2005) found that 76 out of the top 100 bestselling baby DVDs on Amazon.com made educational claims. However, attempts to
demonstrate learning from television and videos in children under 2 have yielded mixed
results. With this discrepancy in mind, a closer examination of learning effects in very
young children is appropriate. This paper specifically addresses how (or if) children
under 2 are able to utilize information from screen media and, specifically, how screen
media might impact children’s language development.
Because television and DVDs are significant environmental inputs for young children,
they might have an impact on children’s language learning. Both areas of language
development, expressive and receptive, are relevant to this concern. Receptive language
pertains to the comprehension of spoken language, whereas expressive language
pertains to communicative behaviours, including vocalization, gestures, and word
production (McCarthy, 1960; Owens, 1996). Research has demonstrated that word
comprehension generally precedes word production (Owens, 1996). Although children
may not be able to articulate a particular word, they can often demonstrate their
understanding of the referent word. For example, a 13-month-old who does not yet say
the word ‘dog’ may still point or direct their attention to the family dog when a parent uses
the word. Learning a word indicates that a child has retained its sound pattern and
meaning and is able to link the two in a contextually appropriate manner. Whereas first
spoken words typically begin around a child’s first birthday, a child’s receptive vocabulary
may include far more words. The MacArthur communicative development inventories
(CDIs) are often used to demonstrate the growth of language from infancy through
toddlerhood and show vocabulary comprehension scores of 20–26 words for the average
12-month-old child (Fenson et al., 2000). Other estimates for children at 12 months
approach 50 words (Golinkoff & Hirsh-Pasek, 1999).
Although language development is considered to be a fairly resilient process in
early childhood, vocabulary acquisition appears to be less resilient and more
influenced by the environment than other language skills. Deviations in the
environment can affect the quantity and quality of linguistic input children receive.
For example, the amount of talk mothers direct to their children is strongly related to
children’s vocabulary growth (Hart & Risley, 1995). Related to young children’s
exposure to screen media, it is unclear whether television and DVDs can provide a
similar level of environmental support as interactions with live caregivers, especially in
light of parents’ generally favourable opinions about the educational uses of screen
media (Rideout et al., 2003).
Learning from books
The benefits of joint book reading for language development have been welldocumented. Although storybook reading is often used primarily as source of enjoyment
for parents and children, it is also important in children’s developing vocabularies and
acquisition of word knowledge (Sénéchal & LeFevre, 2001; Sénéchal, LeFevre, Thomas,
BJDP 440—28/5/2008—SHANMUGAM—300030
Word learning
3
& Daley, 1998). Storybook reading often involves language that is richer and more varied
than other contexts for language interaction, such as dressing or mealtime (HoffGinsberg, 1991). It also provides a context in which children can imitate verbal
behaviours that are demonstrated by the parent (Snow & Goldfield, 1983). Interaction
with very young children often involves the parents drawing attention to and labelling
pictures on a page (Sénéchal, Cornell, & Broda, 1995). These behaviours may be
repeated as parents read books multiple times. The joint attention interaction in book
reading emerges as the parent or caregiver asks the infant questions such ‘What is this?’
or ‘Where is the (desired referent object)?’ or by simply drawing attention to certain
pictures (Ninio, 1983). In doing so, the parent provides a context in which the child can
demonstrate recognition of a word or to produce the word.
Learning from television and DVDs
Given these findings, it is interesting to note that at the beginning of the Baby
Wordsworth DVD, the founder of Baby Einstein encourages parents to use the DVD like
a book. As with storybook reading, the literature on older children’s ability to learn
language from television is substantial, especially regarding high-quality educational
programmes such as Sesame Street, which has been associated with many positive
educational benefits, including increased school readiness (Fisch & Truglio, 2001;
Wright & Huston, 1995; Wright et al., 2001). Sesame Street viewing was a significant
predictor of vocabulary scores at age 5 for children who viewed starting at 3- to 312-yearsold, even after controlling for parental education, gender, family size, and parental
attitudes towards television. The relationship was also found without parental
co-viewing, meaning children could learn without parents viewing with them (Rice,
Huston, Truglio, & Wright, 1990). Other research suggests children older than 2 can
readily acquire vocabulary from television (Rice et al., 1990).
Rice and Woodsmall (1988) looked at 3- to 5-year-olds’ ability to learn novel words
from television. In their experiment, children watched a 15-minute television programme
containing 20 novel words. After viewing, children were given a comprehension test to
assess learning. Four pictures were arrayed on a page, and children were asked to point to
the picture depicting a target word from the programme. Although the experimental
groups performed better than the control groups, there were significant age differences.
The 5-year-olds were more accurate than the 3-year-olds in identifying the correct words.
Rice and Woodsmall concluded that children have the ability to ‘fast map’ new words
from television; in other words, children were able to gain an understanding of a word
after a single exposure to the televised stimulus. In a compelling anecdotal study, Sachs,
Bard, and Johnson (1981) reported a case study of two hearing children raised by deaf
parents who apparently learned spoken language from television viewing, providing
additional evidence that television could be a useful input for vocabulary development.
Although compelling, these results do not necessarily inform discussions about the
impact of media on younger children’s language development because this past research
has focused on programmes targeted to children in the preschool years. In contrast,
critics’ concerns with infant-directed media reflect an assumption that media exposure
in the first 2 years of life might detract from quality adult interactions that support
normal linguistic development. A reasonable hypothesis might be that children who
frequently watch infant DVDs spend less time in play and thus do not have as many playrelated language learning possibilities. At least one recent study suggests that infants
BJDP 440—28/5/2008—SHANMUGAM—300030
4 Michael B. Robb et al.
who watch more baby videos and DVDs have fewer words, regardless of whether a
parent was viewing with the child (Zimmerman, Christakis, & Meltzoff, 2007).
Some research has compared young children’s ability to learn from a televised model
to their ability to learn from a live model. The limited research available for children
younger than 3 suggests evidence of a video deficit, or a lag between teaching with a live
model and a televised model (Anderson & Pempek, 2005). Although the video deficit
model has traditionally been used in research on imitation (Barr, Chavez, Fujimoto,
Garcia, & Muentener, 2007; Muentener, Price, Garcia, & Barr, 2004), it may apply to the
language domain as well, reflecting developmental difficulties in learning language from
screen media. Krcmar, Grela, and Lin (2007) conducted a study examining the fast
mapping abilities of young children. The authors presented words to 15- to 24-monthold children with a live adult model, an adult model on television, and through a clip
from Teletubbies. Much higher gains were found with a live model as compared to the
video of the adult or the Teletubbies clip, although the ability to learn from video
improved as children approached 24 months. Infants under 2 seemed to struggle with
fast mapping from the televised stimulus, especially in comparison to a live stimulus,
leading the authors to conclude that children are more likely to learn the meanings of
new words when instructed by a live adult and are less likely to learn them via television.
Although compelling, these kinds of experiments have generally been conducted
outside of young children’s natural viewing experience.
Other data suggest there may be some linguistic benefit to watching content-specific
programming. Using a survey approach, Linebarger and Walker (2005) found that
differences in expressive language production were related to the specific programmes
viewed. Viewing Barney and Friends, Clifford, Arthur, Dragon Tales, Blue’s Clues, and
Dora the Explorer were all related to higher expressive language scores, whereas
viewing Sesame Street and Teletubbies were related to smaller expressive language
skills. The authors speculated that the higher scores might be attributed to the narrative
or storybook-like natures of the programmes, which afford opportunities for vocabulary
words to be heard, visualized, and embedded in character interactions.
In order to explore whether children learn from viewing the videos produced for
them, the study reported in this paper focuses on a DVD produced by the Baby
Einstein Company called Baby Wordsworth. The DVD highlights 30 vocabulary words
and combines short puppet skits with live footage of children and parents playing and
interacting around the house. One of the most important factors to consider about
children’s potential word learning from Baby Wordsworth is that although words are
paired with pictures of the named object, they are not embedded in a narrative or
within spoken sentences, which may not be an ideal screen environment (Linebarger
& Walker, 2005).
However, there is evidence that children are effectively able to learn words
presented in isolation. In a study on mothers and their 9-month-old infants, Brent and
Siskind (2001) reported that an average of 9% of maternal utterances consisted of
isolated words. Additionally, the frequency with which a child heard a word in
isolation was a significant predictor of whether the child had acquired that word at 12
and 15 months. Bird and Chapman (1998) investigated whether children 13- to 16months old could fast map a word after four or eight exposures to an object with an
unfamiliar label. Results showed that children as young as 13 months could fast map a
new word after as few as four exposures. Thus, it may be possible that repeated
exposure to words heard in isolation, even on DVD, could contribute to infants’
receptive language.
BJDP 440—28/5/2008—SHANMUGAM—300030
Word learning
5
The present study examines the impact of viewing a Baby Wordsworth DVD on
12- to 15-month-olds’ expressive and receptive language skills. Learning was compared
between a group of children who viewed the DVD over multiple time points and a
group of children who were not instructed to view a particular DVD at all. Although
giving the control group a different DVD to view may also have been an appropriate
comparison group, the goal of this study was to compare children who viewed our
target DVD with children who went about their daily routines uninterrupted. Given the
design of the experiment, which ensured that children would be exposed to the DVD
multiple times in order to counteract the potential video deficit effect, it was believed
that children who viewed the DVD would understand and produce more words than the
control group. In order to enhance the ecological validity of the experiment, all
viewings of the DVD occurred in children’s homes.
Method
Participants
Participants were 45 infants (M ¼ 59:38 weeks at first visit, SD ¼ 5:44 weeks) and their
parents from Riverside County and surrounding communities in southern California. All
infants were between 12 and 15 months old. There were 26 male and 19 female infants,
representing Caucasian/White (60.00%), Latino/Hispanic (20.00%), African-American
(8.89%), Asian-American (2.22%), and other multi-ethnic/unknown groups (8.89%).
Participants were recruited through direct mailings, local advertisements, and referrals
from local pediatrician’s offices. All participants were compensated $25 for each visit to
the laboratory and were given a free DVD at the conclusion of their participation.
Measures
Demographic questionnaire
All parents filled out a questionnaire about their ethnic background, income, and
education level.
Media survey
All parents completed a survey regarding their household’s media environment,
attitudes towards media, and media habits. The survey was derived from a previous
version of a media survey utilized in an earlier study assessing home media
environments of children ages 0–6 (Rideout et al., 2003). Parents were asked to reflect
on their own media exposure and habits for their last ‘typical’ day. An example question
included: ‘How long, on a typical day, is your child exposed to television or a DVD that is
on in the background?’ The survey also asked how many minutes their child spent
watching television and DVDs and how many minutes they were read to.
Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development, 3rd edition
The Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development, 3rd edition (BSID-III; Bayley,
2005) is used to assess physical, motor, cognitive, language, and social-emotional
development for children. In this study, only the cognitive subtest was used, which
incorporates play tasks, memory items, problem-solving tasks, and measures
of information-processing speed. Internal consistency reliability estimates for 12- to
BJDP 440—28/5/2008—SHANMUGAM—300030
6 Michael B. Robb et al.
15-month-old children range from r ¼ :83 to :89. The BSID-III was administered only
during the first testing session.
Vocabulary questionnaire
All parents were given a list of 30 words that were featured in the Baby Wordsworth
DVD, such as ‘ball’, ‘puzzle’, and ‘couch.’ For each word, parents were asked to indicate
whether they believed their child understood the word and whether their child could
say the word. Parents were instructed to mark that their child could say a word if it was
recognizable and phonetically similar to the actual world. For example, saying ‘dah’ for
‘dog’ would be accepted, but ‘woof–woof’ would not. The total number of words
Q1 understood (WU) and words said (WS) at each time point were used in the analysis. This
method of parental report is similar to the widely used MacArthur CDIs (Fenson et al.,
1993). A full list of words can be found in the Appendix A. The vocabulary questionnaire
was filled out by parents during each visit.
Materials
Baby Wordsworth: First words around the house DVD
Part of the Baby Einstein series of programmes, Baby Wordsworth is a 39-minute DVD
advertised for children 12 months and older. Set to classical music, the DVD highlights
30 target words of objects and rooms found inside the house. As demonstrated in
Figure 1, the text for each word is displayed with a picture of the item, simultaneously
with an adult woman or female child demonstrating the American Sign Language gesture
for the word. Interspersed with the target words are short skits featuring puppets and
musical montages that incorporate the vocabulary words, for example a child playing on
a toy piano or a child stacking cups (italicized words are highlighted in the DVD).
Procedure
Upon arriving for the first testing session, parents and their infants were brought into
the laboratory, where parents signed the consent form and were informed about the
purpose of the study. All parents were told that the purpose of the study was to
investigate the impact of baby DVDs on very young children. Parents were not informed
about any specific outcome measures. After the consent form was signed, the parent,
child, and experimenter sat on the floor together with toys and played for a few minutes
to help the child warm up to the experimenter. During this time, parents filled out
consent forms, questionnaires, and survey information. When the child seemed
comfortable in the new setting, he or she was tested with the BSID-III.
At the conclusion of the first session, a randomly selected subset of the participants
(M ¼ 60:00 weeks, SD ¼ 5:36, N ¼ 20) received a Baby Wordsworth DVD to take
Person
Object
Text
Figure 1. Sample display of vocabulary word on Baby Wordsworth DVD.
BJDP 440—28/5/2008—SHANMUGAM—300030
Word learning
7
home with them and a time diary to keep track of their viewing. The viewing group
included 9 female and 11 male infants. Five participants whose parents had indicated
that their child had previously seen the Baby Wordsworth DVD were placed in this
group to avoid contaminating the results. Parents were instructed to have their infant
watch the DVD five times every 2 weeks for the next 6 weeks. Parent viewings were
verified with a time diary that they kept indicating their viewing times. All other
participants (M ¼ 58:88 weeks, SD ¼ 5:57, N ¼ 25) were not given specific
instructions, other than to follow their typical routines. The non-viewing group
included 10 female and 15 male infants. Every 2 weeks for 6 weeks, all parents and
children returned to the laboratory for follow-up testing sessions. At each of the
remaining three testing sessions, parents filled out the vocabulary questionnaire.
Results
Average means and standard deviation at each time point can be seen in Table 1.
Examination of t tests revealed no significant differences between the DVD and control
groups on WU or WS scores at any of the time points. Although there were no significant
differences in scores at the group level, the effect of the DVD may have contributed to
individual differences in learning curves over time.
Table 1. Means and standard deviations by group
Words understood
DVD
No
Mean
N
SD
Yes
Mean
N
SD
Total
Mean
N
SD
Words said
T1
T2
T3
T4
T1
T2
T3
T4
11.16
19
6.78
12.27
22
8.03
14.20
20
8.02
17.21
19
7.88
1.05
19
1.39
1.55
22
1.73
1.35
20
1.42
1.95
19
1.61
10.50
12
6.05
12.53
17
6.73
14.50
16
7.37
17.27
15
7.17
1.08
12
1.56
0.88
17
1.05
1.75
16
2.08
3.93
15
4.11
10.90
31
6.41
12.38
39
7.40
14.33
36
7.63
17.24
34
7.46
1.06
31
1.44
1.26
39
1.50
1.53
36
1.73
2.82
34
3.010
In order to take advantage of the longitudinal nature of the data, multi-level
modelling was employed for analysis. Although often used to examine nested
organizations (e.g. students within schools, etc.), multi-level modelling is also useful for
examining longitudinal data, which has time points nested within individuals (Singer,
1998). A significant advantage to this approach is the ability to study multiple levels of
analysis without violating the independence assumptions of linear multiple regression
(Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007).
Before analysis, square root transformations were performed on the word
Q1 understanding (WU), words spoken (WS), and income variables to adjust for extreme
skew and kurtosis. In order to minimize potential third variable associations and to ensure
BJDP 440—28/5/2008—SHANMUGAM—300030
8 Michael B. Robb et al.
that the effects of predictors of interest were reflected appropriately in the outcomes,
controls for age, cognitive developmental level, parent education, and income were
included in all analyses. Additionally, as there was an unequal gender breakdown in our
groups and because gender differences in word learning are sometimes found in children
under 2, gender was also included as a control (Bauer, Goldfield, & Reznick, 2002; Fenson
et al., 1994; Huttenlocher, Haight, Bryk, Seltzer, & Lyons, 1991). All models included three
Level-2 predictors: minutes read to; combined minutes viewing television and
videos/DVDs; and exposure to background television, which is defined as the amount
of time the television was on, even if no one was watching.
Impact of DVD
Analysis technique
Two-level hierarchical models were used to assess the effects of the Baby Wordsworth
DVD on expressive language (as measured by the WU section of the vocabulary
questionnaire) and receptive language (as measured by the WS section of the vocabulary
questionnaire). Level-1 is a linear individual growth model (the ‘within-person’ model),
and Level-2 expresses variations in the Level-1 model as random effects (the ‘betweenperson’ model). To begin, unconditional linear growth models examining variation in
WS and WU were fitted and compared against models specifying no growth. In both
analyses, a Level-1 predictor, age, was included, indicating each child’s score was related
to his or her age. In addition to a random effect representing variation within persons,
random effects for the intercept and in the slopes for age were also included. Assuming
significant variation was found in the intercepts and slopes, investigation would
continue to examine covariates impacting intercept and slope. In all analyses, the
variable age was centred to make the results more interpretable, so that the intercept
would represent the average score for a child across the full sample controlling for age.
Expressive language
The unconditional linear growth model for WS provided a significantly better fit than a
model specifying no growth (Dx2 ½3 ¼ 231:9 2 207:7 ¼ 24:2, p , :0001) (see Table 2).
The unconditional linear growth model revealed that at 59.38 weeks, the average child
could say 2.04 words (out of 30) and gained about 0.14 words per week. Although there
was significant variation in the intercept, there was no significant variance in the slope,
meaning that Level-2 (between-person) covariates could only be used to explain
intercept (i.e. mean score) variation and not slope. With no slope variation to explain,
no analyses on the impact of the Baby Wordsworth DVD were valid. Thus, analysis
shifted to examining Level-2 covariates affecting the intercept (see Table 3).
A final model of the WS score, with all controls present in the model, indicated that the
children in the sample had a parent-reported vocabulary of 0.59 words (out of a possible 30)
that they could say. Although time spent viewing television and videos/DVDs was not a
significant individual predictor, the model fit was significantly improved beyond a controlsonly model by keeping this variable in the model (Dx2 ½3 ¼ 114:5 2 102:8 ¼ 11:7,
p , :01). There was a significant positive relationship between time being read to and
average WS such that for every 15 minutes a child was read to on a typical day, the child’s
average score increased by 0.61 words. There was also a very small, but significant positive
relationship between exposure to background television and WS such that for every 30
minutes of television exposure in the background, the child’s average score increased by
BJDP 440—28/5/2008—SHANMUGAM—300030
Table 2. Estimates of multi-level model fixed effects and random effects variances
Parameter estimates
Random effects
Model
WU – Unconditional means
modela
WU – Unconditional growth
modela
WS – Unconditional means
modela
WS – Unconditional growth
modela
a
t00 variance
in means
t01 covariance
between means
Fixed effects
t11 variance
in slopes
0.76
0.70
0.0043
0
0.18
0.12
0.0055
0
Residual
g00 intercept
0.34
3.65
0.25
3.38
0.20
1.55
0.17
1.43
g01 average slope
Fit statistics
Dx2(df )
p
0.10
34.2 (3)
,.0001
0.046
24.2 (3)
,.0001
Values based on square root transformation.
Word learning
9
Fixed effect estimates
Intercepta
Words understood –
controls only
Words understood –
final model
Words said –
control only
Words said –
final model
3.50 (0.16)***
2.29 (0.53)***
1.41 (0.082)***
0.77 (0.26)**
Ageb
0.091
(0.018)***
0.097
(0.017)***
0.047
(0.011)***
0.053
(0.010)***
Genderb
0.12 (0.32)
0.61 (0.32)
20.16 (0.16)
0.12 (0.16)
BSIDb
0.020
(0.076)
0.012
(0.066)
0.017
(0.38)
0.011
(0.032)
Note. Standard deviations in parentheses. *p , :05; **p , :01; ***p , :001.
Square-root transformed.
b
Centred.
a
Fit statistics
Parent
educationb Incomea,b
20.053
(0.17)
0.15
(0.19)
20.026
(0.085)
0.13
(0.098)
0.0031
(0.0040)
0.0074
(0.0037)
20.0012
(0.0020)
0.0012
(0.0018)
Minutes
viewed
Background TV
exposure
Minutes
read to
22LL
Dx2(df )
191.1
20.0039
(0.0048)
0.0028
(0.0014)*
0.038
(0.12)**
181.8
9.3 (3)*
114.5
20.0028
(0.0024)
0.0015
(0.00070)*
0.022
(0.0062)**
102.8
11.7 (3)**
BJDP 440—28/5/2008—SHANMUGAM—300030
10 Michael B. Robb et al.
Table 3. Fixed effects models
BJDP 440—28/5/2008—SHANMUGAM—300030
Word learning
11
4.00
3.50
3.00
Age
WS
2.50
2.00
Age + 30 minutes
background TV
Age + 15 minutes reading
1.50
Age + 15 minutes reading +
30 minutes background TV
1.00
0.50
0.00
50 52 54 56 58 60 62 64 66 68 70 72 74
Weeks old
Figure 2. Growth curve predicting words said as a function of age, minutes of exposure to television in
the background, and minutes read to.
0.069 words (see Figure 2). This model accounted for approximately 59% of the total
explainable variation in the intercept, compared to the controls-only model, which
accounted for about 34% of the variation.
Receptive language
The unconditional linear growth model for WU provided a significantly better fit than a
no-growth unconditional means model (Dx2 ½3 ¼ 337:8 2 303:6 ¼ 34:2, p , :0001).
The unconditional linear growth model revealed that at 59.38 weeks, the average child
could understand 11.42 words (out of 30) and gained about 0.69 words per week.
Although there was significant variation in the intercept, there was no variance in the
slope, meaning that Level-2 (between-person) covariates could only be used to explain
intercept variation and not slope. Again, with no slope variation to explain, no analyses
on the impact of the Baby Wordsworth DVD were valid. Analysis shifted to examining
Level-2 covariates affecting the intercept (see Table 2).
A final model of the WU score, with all controls present, indicated that the children
in the sample had a parent-reported receptive vocabulary of 5.24 words (out of a
possible 30). Again, time spent viewing television and videos/DVDs was not a uniquely
significant predictor, but the model fit was significantly improved beyond a controlsonly model by keeping this variable in the model (Dx2 ½3 ¼ 191:1 2 181:8 ¼ 9:3,
p , :05). There was a significant positive relationship between time being read to and
average WU such that for every 15 minutes a child was read to on a typical day, the
child’s average score increased by 3.01 words. There was also a small, but significant
positive relationship between exposure to background television and WU such that for
every 30 minutes of television exposure in the background, the child’s average score
increased by 0.40 words (see Figure 3). This model accounted for approximately 33% of
the total explainable variation in the intercept, compared to the controls-only model,
which accounted for about 6% of the variation.
BJDP 440—28/5/2008—SHANMUGAM—300030
12 Michael B. Robb et al.
20.00
18.00
16.00
14.00
Age
Age + 30 minutes
background TV
Age + 15 minutes reading
WU
12.00
10.00
Age + 15 minutes reading +
30 minutes background TV
8.00
6.00
4.00
2.00
0.00
50 52 54 56 58 60 62 64 66 68 70 72 74
Weeks old
Figure 3. Growth curve predicting words understood as a function of age, minutes of exposure to
television in the background, and minutes read to.
To summarize, results indicated that participants in the viewing group and the
control group increased at the same rate on WU and WS. However, there were
differences in what variables predicted participants’ average scores on the measures.
There were significant relationships between the amount of time a child was read to and
outcome scores. Additionally, there were small, but significant relationships between
outcome scores and having television exposure in the background.
Discussion
This experiment examined potential word learning from viewing a DVD produced with
an emphasis on teaching specific words to children 12 months and older. Although this
study is not meant to be a product evaluation, part of its strength lies in the utilization of
professionally produced stimuli reflecting real-world use. Given past research
suggesting young children can learn words from television after repeated exposures
(Krcmar et al., 2007), it was hypothesized that multiple exposures to the DVD could
have a measurable impact on children’s receptive and expressive vocabulary growth.
In contrast to the hypothesis, this experiment found no relationship between
viewing an infant-directed DVD and increases on parent-reported scores of receptive
and expressive knowledge of words highlighted in the DVD. Multilevel modelling
revealed no significant variation in the slopes of the 12- to 15-month-old participants; in
other words, there was no differential growth in any of the outcome measures for those
participants who watched the DVD. Slopes across individuals increased at the same rate,
regardless of condition; the only significant variation occurred in the participants’
average scores (the intercepts), a finding that will be discussed below.
Although the findings suggest that children at this young age cannot learn words
from a DVD of this nature, it is possible that this particular DVD was not well-suited to
BJDP 440—28/5/2008—SHANMUGAM—300030
Word learning
13
teaching words. The structure of the DVD was such that a picture of target word was
displayed on-screen in conjunction with a female voice saying the word, the text of the
word, and an adult female signing the word. This was re-emphasized later with the
same picture, but a child’s voice saying the word and a female child signing it.
This presentation does not mimic the situations in which infants are most likely to learn
words. In particular, infants demonstrate the ability to learn new words and object labels
in joint attention scenarios, where parents establish joint attention on an object with the
infant and produce the desired word (Woodward & Hoyne, 1999). This DVD may not
provide an appropriate context for joint attention learning in two respects: parents may
not watch the DVD with their children and the on-screen adult does not look at the
target object in the DVD.
Another possibility is that the Baby Wordsworth DVD did not attract infants’
attention. For example, the written text and adult signer may have been too distracting,
providing too many things to attend to at once. In addition after each word, there were
multiple examples of the object in use, for example a child playing with a toy telephone
or tapping on keys on his mother’s computer. It could be that there was too much
information on the screen and the child did not attend to parts that would increase the
likelihood of remembering the word.
Given the importance that attention plays in learning, some research has found high
levels of looking by children 12- to 15-months old when watching videos specifically for
babies, in this case the video Baby Mozart (Barr, Zack, Muentener, & Garcia, in press).
Presumably, videos for this age group maintain a simple enough structure to promote
comprehensibility to very young children and make adequate use of formal features that
are attractive to this age group. However, the Baby Mozart video is marketed to
1-month-old children, whereas Baby Wordsworth is marketed to children at 12-months.
Barr and her colleagues may have been able to elicit high levels of looking because their
video was more comprehensible to the children in their sample, a view supported by
Anderson and Lorch’s (1983) idea that attention is driven by understanding. Baby
Wordsworth, with its frequent breaks to highlight words, might be too difficult to follow
and thus less likely to be attended to.
A third reasonable possibility may be that televised exposure is not sufficient to teach
words. In a related argument, Schmitt and Anderson (2002) suggested there may be too
many informational-processing issues involved for children to adequately use television
as a source of information. They believe that the television’s representation of 3-D space
is too degraded to give children the kinds of perceptually salient information needed for
encoding certain types of information. Considering the assumption of limited cognitive
resources, the tasks of understanding what is on the television, identifying the objects,
and perceiving the nature of televised space may utilize cognitive processes that might
otherwise be used to learn new words.
Lastly, the DVD is marketed to children 12 months and older. Children’s first
words are often simple, monosyllabic words that are well-related to their everyday
environments and interactions. In other words, a child may have a lot of contact with
a ball, hear the word ‘ball’, and get to manipulate a ball, contributing to the learning
of that word. Although many of the words highlighted in the DVD fit this
categorization, several others were more complex polysyllabic words that may have
been encountered less frequently by young children, such as ‘refrigerator’ and ‘livingroom’. It seems less likely that a parent would use the word ‘refrigerator’ frequently
when speaking to a 12-month-old child. More age-appropriate vocabulary choices
may facilitate word learning, although follow-up research could help determine
BJDP 440—28/5/2008—SHANMUGAM—300030
14 Michael B. Robb et al.
whether children who did hear complex words on the DVD acquired them earlier
than those who were not exposed.
Although some may find it discouraging that the DVD had no impact on language
learning even after 15 exposures, the finding corroborates other research on infantdirected media demonstrating very young children’s difficulty in learning from televised
stimuli (Barr & Hayne, 1999; Krcmar et al., 2007; Schmitt & Anderson, 2002; Troseth &
DeLoache, 1998). However, other findings from this study could be used to guide future
research. First, reading matters. Children in families who were read to for longer periods
of time had higher reports of words they could understand and say, even after
controlling for age, gender, income, parent education, and cognitive development level.
Given the research pointing to the positive academic outcomes of children with larger
vocabularies and language skills, this is still a valuable (though not surprising) reminder
that reading remains one of the most useful and beneficial practices available to parents
from the very beginning (Neuman, 1999; Shonkoff & Phillips, 2000). A wide literature
indicates how reading to children provides the type of joint-referent, interactive
structure that is critical for learning new words and object labels (Lemish & Rice, 1986;
Ninio, 1983; Ninio & Bruner, 1978).
In this sense, the findings of this research mirror other findings suggesting DVD talk
is not equivalent to parent-talk for children in this age range, a phenomenon most
strongly demonstrated in research testing infants’ phonemic discrimination. In contrast
to older children and adults, new-born infants have the ability to discriminate all
phonemes in all languages, regardless of the language being spoken around them, until
about 8 months of age, at which time they can only discriminate the phonemes that
occur within their native tongue (Eilers, Gavin, & Wilson, 1979; Werker, Gilbert,
Humphrey, & Tess, 1981). Research has demonstrated that repeated exposure to a
non-native language through a book-reading interaction increases the length of time for
which infants can continue to distinguish particular non-native phonemes (Kuhl, Tsao,
& Liu, 2003). This effect was not found when the exposure was through a DVD.
Contrary to what was expected, children in households where the television was on
in the background for longer periods of time had higher reported scores on both
receptive and productive vocabulary measures. Follow-up analyses showed that parents
who believed television was more helpful to children’s learning tended to live in homes
with more background television, perhaps reflecting parents’ positive attitudes about
television. Follow-up also showed a significant negative correlation between time being
read to and background television, a finding supported by Vandewater and colleagues
(2005). Given that there is less time reading, which was already linked to increased
scores, it is unclear why background television might positively influence words
learned. It is possible that the background television variable is serving as a proxy for
another variable, such as having older siblings, which might increase the amount of
television viewed in the household. The presence of siblings might provide additional
environmental opportunities for children to hear and use language with a live,
interactive model (Dunn & Kendrick, 1982; Oshima-Takane, Goodz, & Derevensky,
1996; Sachs & Devin, 1976). However, follow-up analyses did not reveal any differences
in receptive or productive vocabulary between children with older siblings and those
without. It is also possible that children who hear television speech in the background
have greater linguistic input and thus may have larger vocabularies, but this is a tenuous
explanation and requires additional investigation. Since the added benefit to language
was small, it would be irresponsible to recommend leaving the television on as a useful
strategy for developing language, especially considering background television’s role in
BJDP 440—28/5/2008—SHANMUGAM—300030
Word learning
15
displacing other potentially valuable activities and disrupting children’s play (Anderson
& Pempek, 2005; Vandewater et al., 2005).
It is also interesting to note that total time spent viewing television and DVDs was
not a significant unique predictor in any model. Research in media often uses total media
exposure as a predictor of outcomes from aggression to academic outcomes to attention
problems (Anderson & Hanson, 2007). As Anderson and Hanson argue, this can be a
misleading strategy. They liken television exposure to a diet – what is eaten is ultimately
more important than how much; in other words, the kinds of media that children are
exposed to will have a far greater impact than the amount viewed. As in Linebarger and
Walker’s (2005) work, examining a breakdown of the shows children watch may lead to
a more revealing trend.
A final interesting finding to note is that gender, which has been found to contribute
to language differences in other studies, was not a significant predictor in any of
the models. This is somewhat surprising considering research showing that girls have
slightly higher receptive and productive language than boys of the same age (Bauer et al.,
2002; Fenson et al., 1994). However, as Fenson and colleagues (1994) note, gender
accounts for only a very small portion of the variance in early language, especially
compared to the much greater variability that exists within gender.
Several limitations of this study should be considered. Considering the incredible
diversity in infants’ individual differences and rates of growth (Shonkoff & Phillips,
2000), examining language development up to 2 years of age and beyond would provide
valuable insight into the differing role of infant DVDs at different ages. However, as Baby
Wordsworth is marketed to children starting at 12 months, it was an appropriate starting
time point for this study.
Additionally, in a traditional causal experiment, scientists have full control of the
environment in which the experiment takes place to reduce the possibility of external
interference and unwanted variance that could lead to incorrect or misunderstood
conclusions (Cook, Campbell, & Day, 1979). However, in this study, infants viewed the
DVDs in their own homes in a manner that was similar to how to they usually watch in
order to enhance the ecological validity of the findings. The benefit of this approach is that
this study provides results suggesting that these types of DVDs may not be effective in the
ways they are naturally used or for the audience to whom they are targeted. Closer
examination of home factors, such as co-viewing behaviours, will be helpful in
determining if there are differential outcomes for children in diverse home environments,
as has been shown to be the case in other studies of television effects (Desmond, Singer,
Singer, Calam, & Colimore, 1985; Dorr, Kovaric, & Doubleday, 1989; Rice et al., 1990;
Singer & Singer, 1998).
A final potential limitation lies in the way vocabulary scores were measured.
As stated before, the vocabulary questionnaire is similar in nature to the widely used
CDIs (Fenson et al., 1993); and there is reason to believe that there is a significant
correspondence between parental reports of child language and child performance
(Ring & Fenson, 2000), but there is still a potential for a social desirability bias. Such
a bias could inflate scores. One might expect that parents who had the DVD in their
homes and viewed it 15 times (or at least were partially exposed to it) would be
more likely to believe that their child had learned some of the words highlighted in
the DVD. But this is not what occurred – there was no vocabulary gain associated
with watching the DVD. Although this does not rule out that there was a social
desirability bias in score reporting, it does not seem likely that it played an important
role in this study.
BJDP 440—28/5/2008—SHANMUGAM—300030
16 Michael B. Robb et al.
In conclusion, there is no evidence to support implicit claims that Baby
Wordsworth, or infant media generally, can teach children between 12- to 15-monthsold to understand or speak more words solely from viewing in their homes. Although
time being read to and background television exposure were significant predictors of
children’s average scores, the added gains were very small, leading to questions about
their overall meaningfulness. How much can be generalized from results from one DVD
is open to debate; however, in the absence of other research supporting this type of
learning, and considering the many developmental difficulties young children face
in learning from televised stimuli, there is still no evidence that children at this young
age learn language effectively from viewing a DVD. Future research examining
alternative televised presentations of vocabulary words, such as embedding them in
narratives and using them in sentences, would be useful in determining whether this
limitation is a result of media production choices or is reflective of more general
cognitive limitations.
Acknowledgements
This research was funded by Grant # 0623821 from the National Science Foundation.
References
American Academy of Pediatrics (1999). Media education. Pediatrics, 104(2), 341–343.
Anderson, D., & Hanson, K. (2007). Children, media, and methodology. Manuscript submitted for
publication.
Q2
Anderson, D. R., & Lorch, E. P. (1983). Looking at television: Action or reaction. In J. Bryant &
D. R. Anderson (Eds.), Children’s understanding of television: Research on attention and
comprehension (pp. 1–33). New York: Academic Press.
Anderson, D. R., & Pempek, T. A. (2005). Television and very young children. American
Behavioral Scientist, 48(5), 505–522.
Barr, R., Chavez, V., Fujimoto, M., Garcia, A., & Muentener, P. (2007). The effect of repetition on
imitation from television during infancy. Developmental Science, 49(2), 196–207.
Barr, R., & Hayne, H. (1999). Developmental changes in imitation from television during infancy.
Child Development, 70(5), 1067–1081.
Barr, R., Zack, E., Muentener, P., & Garcia, A. (in press). Parent–infant interactions during exposure
to infant-directed programming in 6 - to 18 -month-olds.
Q2
Bauer, D., Goldfield, B., & Reznick, J. (2002). Alternative approaches to analyzing individual
differences in the rate of early vocabulary development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23(3),
313–335.
Bayley, N. (2005). Bayley scales of infant and toddler development (3rd ed.). San Antonio, TX:
Harcourt Assessment.
Bird, E. K. R., & Chapman, R. (1998). Partial representations and phonological selectivity in the
comprehension of 13- to 16-month-olds. First Language, 18(52), 105.
Bjorklund, D. F. (2005). Children’s thinking: Cognitive development and individual differences
(4th ed.). Belmont, CA: Thomson/Wadsworth.
Q3
Brent, M. R., & Siskind, J. M. (2001). The role of exposure to isolated words in early vocabulary
development. Cognition, 81(2), B33–B44.
Bronson, P., & Merryman, A. (2006). Baby Einstein vs. Barbie [Electronic Version]. Time. Retrieved
February 21, 2008, from http://www.time.com/time/nation/article/0,8599,1538507,00.html
Christakis, D. A., Zimmerman, F. J., DiGiuseppe, D. L., & McCarty, C. A. (2004). Early television
exposure and subsequent attentional problems in children. Pediatrics, 113(4), 708–713.
BJDP 440—28/5/2008—SHANMUGAM—300030
Word learning
17
Cook, T. D., Campbell, D. T., & Day, A. (1979). Quasi-experimentation: Design and analysis
issues for field settings. Chicago, IL: Rand McNally College Publishing.
Desmond, R. J., Singer, J. L., Singer, D. G., Calam, R., & Colimore, K. (1985). Family mediation
patterns and television viewing: Young children’s use and grasp of the medium. Human
Communications Research, 11(4), 461–480.
Dorr, A., Kovaric, P., & Doubleday, C. (1989). Parent–child coviewing of television. Journal of
Broadcasting and Electronic Media, 33(1), 33–51.
Dunn, J., & Kendrick, C. (1982). The speech of two-and three-year-olds to infant siblings. Journal
of Child Language, 9(3), 579–595.
Eilers, R. E., Gavin, W., & Wilson, W. R. (1979). Linguistic experience and phonemic perception in
infancy: A crosslinguistic study. Child Development, 50(1), 14–18.
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. J., & Pethick, S. J. (1994). Variability in early
communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
Development, 59(5), 1–173.
Q5
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartung, J. P., et al. (1993). Macarthur
communicative development inventories: User’s guide and technical manual. Baltimore,
MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing.
Fenson, L., Pethick, S., Renda, C., Cox, J. L., Dale, P. S., & Reznick, J. S. (2000). Short-form versions
of the MacArthur communicative development inventories. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21,
95–115.
Fisch, S. M. & Truglio, E. (Eds.). (2001). ‘G’ if for growing: Thirty years of research on children and
Sesame Street. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Garrison, M., & Christakis, D. A. (2005). A teacher in the living room? Educational media for
babies, toddlers, and preschoolers. Menlo Park, CA: Kaiser Family Foundation.
Golinkoff, R. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (1999). How babies talk: The magic and mystery of language
in the first three years of life. New York: Dutton.
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young
American children. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing.
Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1991). Mother–child conversation in different social classes and communicative
settings. Child Development, 62, 782–796.
Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M., & Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth:
Relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology, 27(2), 236–248.
Krcmar, M., Grela, B., & Lin, K. (2007). Can toddlers learn vocabulary from television?
An experimental approach. Media Psychology, 10(1), 41–63.
Kuhl, P. K., Tsao, F. M., & Liu, H. M. (2003). Foreign-language experience in infancy: Effects of
short-term exposure and social interaction on phonetic learning. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences, 100(15), 9096–9101.
Lemish, D., & Rice, M. (1986). Television as a talking picture book: A prop for language
acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 13(2), 251–274.
Linebarger, D. L., & Walker, D. (2005). Infants’ and toddlers’ television viewing and language
outcomes. American Behavioral Scientist, 48(5), 624–645.
McCarthy, D. (1960). Language development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
Development, 25(3), 5–14.
Muentener, P., Price, K., Garcia, A., & Barr, R. (2004). Transferring the representation: Infants can
imitate from television. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Eastern Psychological
Association, Washington, DC.
Neuman, S. B. (1999). Books make a difference: A study of access to literacy. Reading Research
Quarterly, 34(3), 286–311.
Ninio, A. (1983). Joint book reading as a multiple vocabulary acquisition device. Developmental
Psychology, 19(3), 445–451.
Ninio, A., & Bruner, J. (1978). The achievement and antecedents of labelling. Journal of Child
Language, 5(1), 1–15.
BJDP 440—28/5/2008—SHANMUGAM—300030
18 Michael B. Robb et al.
Oshima-Takane, Y., Goodz, E., & Derevensky, J. L. (1996). Birth order effects on early language
development: Do secondborn children learn from overheard speech? Child Development,
67(2), 621–634.
Owens, R. E. (1996). Language development: An introduction (4th ed.). Needham Heights, MA:
Allyn and Bacon.
Rice, M. L., Huston, A. C., Truglio, R., & Wright, J. (1990). Words from Sesame Street: Learning
vocabulary while viewing. Developmental Psychology, 26, 421–428.
Rice, M. L., & Woodsmall, L. (1988). Lessons from television: Children’s word learning when
viewing. Child Development, 59(2), 420–429.
Rideout, V. J., Hamel, E., & Kaiser Family Foundation. (2006). The media family: Electronic media
in the lives of infants, toddlers, preschoolers and their parents. Menlo Park, CA: Henry
J. Kaiser Family Foundation.
Rideout, V. J., Vandewater, E. A., Wartella, E. A., & Kaiser Family Foundation. (2003). Zero to six:
Electronic media in the lives of infants, toddlers and preschoolers. Menlo Park, CA: Henry
J. Kaiser Family Foundation.
Ring, E. D., & Fenson, L. (2000). The correspondence between parent report and child
performance for receptive and expressive vocabulary beyond infancy. First Language, 20(59),
141–159.
Sachs, J., Bard, B., & Johnson, M. L. (1981). Language learning with restricted input: Case studies of
two hearing children of deaf parents. Applied Psycholinguistics, 2(1), 33–54.
Sachs, J., & Devin, J. (1976). Young children’s use of age-appropriate speech styles in social
interaction and role-playing. Journal of Child Language, 3(1), 81–98.
Schmitt, K. L., & Anderson, D. R. (2002). Television and reality: Toddlers’ use of visual information
from video to guide behavior. Media Psychology, 4(1), 51–76.
Sénéchal, M., Cornell, E. H., & Broda, L. S. (1995). Age-related changes in the organization of
parent–infant interactions during picture-book reading. Early Childhood Research Quarterly,
10, 317–337.
Sénéchal, M., & LeFevre, J.-A. (2001). Storybook reading and parent teaching: Links to language
and literacy development. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 92, 39–52.
Sénéchal, M., LeFevre, J.-A., Thomas, E. M., & Daley, K. E. (1998). Differential effects of home
literacy experiences on the development of oral and written language. Reading Research
Quarterly, 33(1), 96–116.
Shonkoff, J. P. & Phillips, D. A. (Eds.). (2000). From neurons to neighbourhoods: The science of
early childhood development. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
Singer, J. D. (1998). Using SAS PROC MIXED to fit multilevel models, hierarchical models, and
individual growth models. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 23(4), 323–355.
Singer, J., & Singer, D. (1998). Barney and friends as entertainment and education: Evaluating the
quality and effectiveness of a television series for preschool children. In J. Asamen & G. Berry
(Eds.), Research paradigms, television, and social behavior. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Q4
Snow, C. E., & Goldfield, B. A. (1983). Turn the page please: Situation-specific language
acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 10, 551–569.
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). Boston, MA:
Pearson and Allyn and Bacon.
Troseth, G. L., & DeLoache, J. S. (1998). The medium can obscure the message: Young children’s
understanding of video. Child Development, 69(4), 950–965.
Vandewater, E. A., Bickham, D. S., Lee, J. H., Cummings, H. M., Wartella, E. A., & Rideout, V. J.
(2005). When the television is always on: Heavy television exposure and young children’s
development. American Behavioral Scientist, 48(5), 562–577.
Werker, J. F., Gilbert, J. H., Humphrey, K., & Tess, R. D. (1981). Developmental aspects of crosslanguage speech perception. Child Development, 52, 349–355.
Woodward, A. L., & Hoyne, K. L. (1999). Infants’ learning about words and sounds in relation to
objects. Child Development, 70(1), 65–77.
BJDP 440—28/5/2008—SHANMUGAM—300030
Word learning
19
Wright, J. C., & Huston, A. C. (1995). Effects of educational TV viewing of lower income
preschoolers on academic skills, school readiness, and school adjustment one to three years
later: A report to Children’s Television Workshop. Lawrence, KS: University of Kansas Center
for Research on the Influences of Television on Children.
Wright, J. C., Huston, A. C., Murphy, K. C., Peters, M. S., Pinon, M., Scantlin, R. M., et al. (2001).
The relations of early television viewing to school readiness and vocabulary of children from
low income families: The early window project. Child Development, 72, 1347–1366.
Zimmerman, F. J., Christakis, D. A., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2007). Television and DVD/video viewing in
children younger than 2 years. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 161(5),
473–479.
Received 30 November 2007; revised version received 11 April 2008
Appendix A
Baby Wordsworth vocabulary items
Kitchen
Refrigerator
Bowl
Cup
Chair
Table
Playroom
Telephone
Computer
Puzzle
Blocks
Book
Yard
Dog
Ball
Tree
Flower
Swing
Living room
Cat
Window
Couch
Lamp
Piano
Bedroom
Bear
Clock
Mirror
Blanket
Bed
BJDP 440—28/5/2008—SHANMUGAM—300030
20 Michael B. Robb et al.
Author Queries
JOB NUMBER: 440
JOURNAL: BJDP
Q1
The same acronyms ‘WU and WS’ have been provided for two different
expansions ‘words understood and words said’ and ‘word understanding and
words spoken’. Please check and confirm the appropriate acronyms.
Q2
Please update the references Anderson and Hanson (2007) and Barr, Zack,
Muentener, and Garcia (in press).
Reference Bjorklund (2005) is provided in the list but not cited in the text.
Please supply citation details or delete the reference from the reference list.
Q3
Q4
Q5
Please supply page range for Singer and Singer (1998).
We have inserted page range for this reference Fenson et al. (1994). Please
check and approve.